1
|
Levy K, Barnea A, Tauber E, Ayali A. Crickets in the spotlight: exploring the impact of light on circadian behavior. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 2024; 210:267-279. [PMID: 38252321 PMCID: PMC10994875 DOI: 10.1007/s00359-023-01686-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/16/2023] [Revised: 12/07/2023] [Accepted: 12/15/2023] [Indexed: 01/23/2024]
Abstract
Crickets serve as a well-established model organism in biological research spanning various fields, such as behavior, physiology, neurobiology, and ecology. Cricket circadian behavior was first reported over a century ago and prompted a wealth of studies delving into their chronobiology. Circadian rhythms have been described in relation to fundamental cricket behaviors, encompassing stridulation and locomotion, but also in hormonal secretion and gene expression. Here we review how changes in illumination patterns and light intensity differentially impact the different cricket behaviors as well as circadian gene expression. We further describe the cricket's circadian pacemaker. Ample anatomical manipulations support the location of a major circadian pacemaker in the cricket optic lobes and another in the central brain, possibly interconnected via signaling of the neuropeptide PDF. The cricket circadian machinery comprises a molecular cascade based on two major transcriptional/translational negative feedback loops, deviating somewhat from the canonical model of Drosophila and emphasizing the significance of exploring alternative models. Finally, the nocturnal nature of crickets has provided a unique avenue for investigating the repercussions of artificial light at night on cricket behavior and ecology, underscoring the critical role played by natural light cycles in synchronizing cricket behaviors and populations, further supporting the use of the cricket model in the study of the effects of light on insects. Some gaps in our knowledge and challenges for future studies are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Keren Levy
- School of Zoology, Tel Aviv University, 6997801, Tel-Aviv, Israel.
| | - Anat Barnea
- Department of Natural Sciences, The Open University of Israel, 4353701, Ra'anana, Israel
| | - Eran Tauber
- Department of Evolutionary and Environmental Biology, Institute of Evolution, University of Haifa, 3103301, Haifa, Israel
| | - Amir Ayali
- School of Zoology, Tel Aviv University, 6997801, Tel-Aviv, Israel.
- Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, 6997801, Tel-Aviv, Israel.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Yilmaz A, Belušič G, J Foster J, Tocco C, Khaldy L, Dacke M. Polarisation vision in the dark: green-sensitive photoreceptors in the nocturnal ball-rolling dung beetle Escarabaeus satyrus. J Exp Biol 2024; 227:jeb246374. [PMID: 38284763 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.246374] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/30/2023] [Accepted: 01/19/2024] [Indexed: 01/30/2024]
Abstract
Many insects utilise the polarisation pattern of the sky to adjust their travelling directions. The extraction of directional information from this sky-wide cue is mediated by specialised photoreceptors located in the dorsal rim area (DRA). While this part of the eye is known to be sensitive to the ultraviolet, blue or green component of skylight, the latter has only been observed in insects active in dim light. To address the functional significance of green polarisation sensitivity, we define the spectral and morphological adaptations of the DRA in a nocturnal ball-rolling dung beetle-the only family of insects demonstrated to orient to the dim polarisation pattern in the night sky. Intracellular recordings revealed polarisation-sensitive green photoreceptors in the DRA of Escarabaeus satyrus. Behavioural experiments verified the navigational relevance of this finding. To quantify the adaptive value of green sensitivity for celestial orientation at night, we also obtained the polarisation properties of the night sky in the natural habitat of the beetle. Calculations of relative photon catch revealed that under a moonlit sky the green-sensitive DRA photoreceptors can be expected to catch an order of magnitude more photons compared with the UV-sensitive photoreceptors in the main retina. The green-sensitive photoreceptors - which also show a range of morphological adaptations for enhanced sensitivity - provide E. satyrus with a highly sensitive system for the extraction of directional information from the night sky.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ayse Yilmaz
- Lund Vision Group, Department of Biology, Lund University, 223 62 Lund, Sweden
| | - Gregor Belušič
- Department of Biology, Biotechnical Faculty, University of Ljubljana, Večna pot 111, 1000 Ljubljana, Slovenia
| | - James J Foster
- Lund Vision Group, Department of Biology, Lund University, 223 62 Lund, Sweden
- Neurobiology, University of Konstanz, Universitätsstr. 10, 78464 Konstanz, Germany
| | - Claudia Tocco
- Lund Vision Group, Department of Biology, Lund University, 223 62 Lund, Sweden
| | - Lana Khaldy
- Lund Vision Group, Department of Biology, Lund University, 223 62 Lund, Sweden
| | - Marie Dacke
- Lund Vision Group, Department of Biology, Lund University, 223 62 Lund, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Supple JA, Varennes-Phillit L, Gajjar-Reid D, Cerkvenik U, Belušič G, Krapp HG. Generating spatiotemporal patterns of linearly polarised light at high frame rates for insect vision research. J Exp Biol 2022; 225:275926. [PMID: 35708202 PMCID: PMC9339910 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.244087] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/31/2022] [Accepted: 06/13/2022] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Polarisation vision is commonplace among invertebrates; however, most experiments focus on determining behavioural and/or neurophysiological responses to static polarised light sources rather than moving patterns of polarised light. To address the latter, we designed a polarisation stimulation device based on superimposing polarised and non-polarised images from two projectors, which can display moving patterns at frame rates exceeding invertebrate flicker fusion frequencies. A linear polariser fitted to one projector enables moving patterns of polarised light to be displayed, whilst the other projector contributes arbitrary intensities of non-polarised light to yield moving patterns with a defined polarisation and intensity contrast. To test the device, we measured receptive fields of polarisation-sensitive Argynnis paphia butterfly photoreceptors for both non-polarised and polarised light. We then measured local motion sensitivities of the optic flow-sensitive lobula plate tangential cell H1 in Calliphora vicina blowflies under both polarised and non-polarised light, finding no polarisation sensitivity in this neuron. Summary: Design of a versatile visual stimulation device for presenting moving patterns of polarised light, and demonstration of its use to characterise polarisation sensitivity in butterfly photoreceptors and blowfly motion-sensitive interneurons.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jack A Supple
- Department of Bioengineering, Imperial College London, Royal School of Mines, Exhibition Road, London, SW7 2AZ, UK
| | - Léandre Varennes-Phillit
- Department of Bioengineering, Imperial College London, Royal School of Mines, Exhibition Road, London, SW7 2AZ, UK
| | - Dexter Gajjar-Reid
- Department of Bioengineering, Imperial College London, Royal School of Mines, Exhibition Road, London, SW7 2AZ, UK
| | - Uroš Cerkvenik
- Department of Biology, Biotechnical Faculty, University of Ljubljana, 1000 Ljubljana, Slovenia
| | - Gregor Belušič
- Department of Biology, Biotechnical Faculty, University of Ljubljana, 1000 Ljubljana, Slovenia
| | - Holger G Krapp
- Department of Bioengineering, Imperial College London, Royal School of Mines, Exhibition Road, London, SW7 2AZ, UK
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Storms M, Jakhar A, Mitesser O, Jechow A, Hölker F, Degen T, Hovestadt T, Degen J. The rising moon promotes mate finding in moths. Commun Biol 2022; 5:393. [PMID: 35484191 PMCID: PMC9051113 DOI: 10.1038/s42003-022-03331-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/17/2021] [Accepted: 03/30/2022] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
To counteract insect decline, it is essential to understand the underlying causes, especially for key pollinators such as nocturnal moths whose ability to orientate can easily be influenced by ambient light conditions. These comprise natural light sources as well as artificial light, but their specific relevance for moth orientation is still unknown. We investigated the influence of moonlight on the reproductive behavior of privet hawkmoths (Sphinx ligustri) at a relatively dark site where the Milky Way was visible while the horizon was illuminated by distant light sources and skyglow. We show that male moths use the moon for orientation and reach females significantly faster with increasing moon elevation. Furthermore, the choice of flight direction depended on the cardinal position of the moon but not on the illumination of the horizon caused by artificial light, indicating that the moon plays a key role in the orientation of male moths. The experimental release of male moths show that moon presence, location, and elevation affect their finding of mates.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mona Storms
- Biocenter of the University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany
| | - Aryan Jakhar
- School of Biology, Indian Institute of Science Education and Research Thiruvananthapuram, Thiruvananthapuram, India
| | | | - Andreas Jechow
- Leibniz Institute of Freshwater Ecology and Inland Fisheries, Berlin, Germany
| | - Franz Hölker
- Leibniz Institute of Freshwater Ecology and Inland Fisheries, Berlin, Germany
| | - Tobias Degen
- Biocenter of the University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Performance of polarization-sensitive neurons of the locust central complex at different degrees of polarization. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 2022; 208:387-403. [PMID: 35157117 PMCID: PMC9123078 DOI: 10.1007/s00359-022-01545-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/08/2021] [Revised: 01/18/2022] [Accepted: 01/25/2022] [Indexed: 10/29/2022]
Abstract
The polarization pattern of the sky is exploited by many insects for spatial orientation and navigation. It derives from Rayleigh scattering in the atmosphere and depends directly on the position of the sun. In the insect brain, the central complex (CX) houses neurons tuned to the angle of polarization (AoP), that together constitute an internal compass for celestial navigation. Polarized light is not only characterized by the AoP, but also by the degree of polarization (DoP), which can be highly variable, depending on sky conditions. Under a clear sky, the DoP of polarized sky light may reach up to 0.75 but is usually much lower especially when light is scattered by clouds or haze. To investigate how the polarization-processing network of the CX copes with low DoPs, we recorded intracellularly from neurons of the locust CX at different stages of processing, while stimulating with light of different DoPs. Significant responses to polarized light occurred down to DoPs of 0.05 indicating reliable coding of the AoP even at unfavorable sky conditions. Moreover, we found that the activity of neurons at the CX input stage may be strongly influenced by nearly unpolarized light, while the activity of downstream neurons appears less affected.
Collapse
|
6
|
Khaldy L, Foster JJ, Yilmaz A, Belušič G, Gagnon Y, Tocco C, Byrne MJ, Dacke M. The interplay of directional information provided by unpolarised and polarised light in the heading direction network of the diurnal dung beetle Kheper lamarcki. J Exp Biol 2022; 225:274310. [PMID: 35037692 PMCID: PMC8918814 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.243734] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/30/2021] [Accepted: 01/11/2022] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
The sun is the most prominent source of directional information in the heading direction network of the diurnal, ball-rolling dung beetle Kheper lamarcki. If this celestial body is occluded from the beetle's field of view, the distribution of the relative weight between the directional cues that remain shifts in favour of the celestial pattern of polarised light. In this study, we continue to explore the interplay of the sun and polarisation pattern as directional cues in the heading direction network of K. lamarcki. By systematically altering the intensity and degree of the two cues presented, we effectively change the relative reliability of these directional cues as they appear to the dung beetle. The response of the ball-rolling beetle to these modifications allows us to closely examine how the weighting relationship of these two sources of directional information is influenced and altered in the heading direction network of the beetle. We conclude that the process in which K. lamarcki relies on directional information is very likely done based on Bayesian reasoning, where directional information conveying the highest certainty at a particular moment is afforded the greatest weight.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lana Khaldy
- Lund Vision Group, Department of Biology, Lund University, Sölvegatan 35, 223 62 Lund, Sweden
| | - James J Foster
- Zoology II, Biozentrum, University of Würzburg, Am Hubland, 97074 Würzburg, Germany
| | - Ayse Yilmaz
- Lund Vision Group, Department of Biology, Lund University, Sölvegatan 35, 223 62 Lund, Sweden
| | - Gregor Belušič
- Department of Biology, Biotechnical Faculty, University of Ljubljana, SI-1000 Ljubljana, Slovenia
| | - Yakir Gagnon
- Lund Vision Group, Department of Biology, Lund University, Sölvegatan 35, 223 62 Lund, Sweden
| | - Claudia Tocco
- Lund Vision Group, Department of Biology, Lund University, Sölvegatan 35, 223 62 Lund, Sweden
| | - Marcus J Byrne
- School of Animal, Plant and Environmental Sciences, University of the Witswatersrand, 1 Jan Smuts Avenue, Braamfontein, Johannesburg 2000, South Africa
| | - Marie Dacke
- Lund Vision Group, Department of Biology, Lund University, Sölvegatan 35, 223 62 Lund, Sweden.,School of Animal, Plant and Environmental Sciences, University of the Witswatersrand, 1 Jan Smuts Avenue, Braamfontein, Johannesburg 2000, South Africa
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Yadav P, Shein-Idelson M. Polarization vision in invertebrates: beyond the boundaries of navigation. CURRENT OPINION IN INSECT SCIENCE 2021; 48:50-56. [PMID: 34628060 DOI: 10.1016/j.cois.2021.09.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/12/2021] [Revised: 08/16/2021] [Accepted: 09/21/2021] [Indexed: 05/05/2023]
Abstract
Invertebrates possess the unique ability to see polarized light. This allows them to exploit the rich polarization information embedded in their natural environments: patterns in plants, high contrast on water surfaces, distinctive signatures of conspecifics, and the celestial polarization pattern around the sun. From this wide repertoire of polarization signals, studies have primarily focused on understanding how celestial polarization information is converted into an internal compass. This review highlights several studies which suggest that spatio-temporal polarization information is utilized by insects for additional functions, such as signaling, detection, contrast enhancement, and host assessment. It concludes by evaluating recent technological advances for uncovering the full repertoire of polarization-sensitivity in invertebrates.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pratibha Yadav
- Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, Israel; School of Zoology, Tel Aviv University, Israel
| | - Mark Shein-Idelson
- Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, Israel; School of Neurobiology, Biochemistry, and Biophysics, Tel Aviv University, Israel.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Matsubara N, Okada R, Sakura M. Possible Role of Polarized Light Information in Spatial Recognition in the Cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. Zoolog Sci 2021; 38:297-304. [PMID: 34342949 DOI: 10.2108/zs200081] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/28/2020] [Accepted: 04/20/2021] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Many insects are able to use skylight e-vector patterns to deduce their heading direction. Crickets have been well known to orient themselves to certain e-vector orientations to keep their walking direction. However, it is still unknown if crickets are able to utilize polarized light information for spatial recognition. Using an experimental paradigm similar to the Morris water maze for rodents, here we examine the possibility that the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus can utilize polarized light information to find the target place. Crickets were placed in a round arena with a heated floor, a portion of which was cooled, and a cross-shaped e-vector pattern was presented from the top of the arena so that the cricket could find the cool spot by walking along the e-vector direction. When the arrangement of the e-vector pattern and the cool spot were fixed throughout the experiments, the time and the walking distance to find the cool spot were significantly decreased with increasing trials, but not when the e-vector pattern was rotated between each trial. Moreover, a model selection indicated that the visual stimulus contributed to the decrease in time and distance. To investigate the cricket's exploration patterns in the arena, a test trial in which the whole floor was uniformly heated was performed before and after the training trials. In the test trial, the crickets trained with the positionally fixed e-vector pattern showed wall-following behavior for a significantly longer time than those untrained and those trained with random e-vector patterns.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nobuaki Matsubara
- Department of Biology, Graduate School of Science, Kobe University, Kobe 657-8501, Japan
| | - Ryuichi Okada
- Department of Biology, Graduate School of Science, Kobe University, Kobe 657-8501, Japan.,School of Human Science and Environment, University of Hyogo, Himeji 670-0092, Japan
| | - Midori Sakura
- Department of Biology, Graduate School of Science, Kobe University, Kobe 657-8501, Japan,
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Temple SE, How MJ, Powell SB, Gruev V, Marshall NJ, Roberts NW. Thresholds of polarization vision in octopuses. J Exp Biol 2021; 224:238090. [PMID: 33602676 PMCID: PMC8077535 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.240812] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/16/2020] [Accepted: 02/11/2021] [Indexed: 12/16/2022]
Abstract
Polarization vision is widespread in nature, mainly among invertebrates, and is used for a range of tasks including navigation, habitat localization and communication. In marine environments, some species such as those from the Crustacea and Cephalopoda that are principally monochromatic, have evolved to use this adaptation to discriminate objects across the whole visual field, an ability similar to our own use of colour vision. The performance of these polarization vision systems varies, and the few cephalopod species tested so far have notably acute thresholds of discrimination. However, most studies to date have used artificial sources of polarized light that produce levels of polarization much higher than found in nature. In this study, the ability of octopuses to detect polarization contrasts varying in angle of polarization (AoP) was investigated over a range of different degrees of linear polarization (DoLP) to better judge their visual ability in more ecologically relevant conditions. The ‘just-noticeable-differences’ (JND) of AoP contrasts varied consistently with DoLP. These JND thresholds could be largely explained by their ‘polarization distance’, a neurophysical model that effectively calculates the level of activity in opposing horizontally and vertically oriented polarization channels in the cephalopod visual system. Imaging polarimetry from the animals’ natural environment was then used to illustrate the functional advantage that these polarization thresholds may confer in behaviourally relevant contexts. Summary: Octopuses are highly sensitive to small changes in the angle of polarization (<1 deg contrast), even when the degree of polarization is low, which may confer a functional advantage in behaviourally relevant contexts.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shelby E Temple
- Ecology of Vision Group, School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK.,Azul Optics Ltd, Henleaze, Bristol BS9 4QG, UK
| | - Martin J How
- Ecology of Vision Group, School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK
| | - Samuel B Powell
- Sensory Neurobiology Group, Queensland Brain Institute, University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD 4072, Australia
| | - Viktor Gruev
- Biosensors Lab, Electrical and Computer Engineering, University of Illinois, Urbana, IL 61801, USA
| | - N Justin Marshall
- Sensory Neurobiology Group, Queensland Brain Institute, University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD 4072, Australia
| | - Nicholas W Roberts
- Ecology of Vision Group, School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Kobayashi N, Okada R, Sakura M. Orientation to polarized light in tethered flying honeybees. J Exp Biol 2020; 223:jeb228254. [PMID: 33106299 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.228254] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/04/2020] [Accepted: 10/16/2020] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Many insects exploit the partial plane polarization of skylight for visual compass orientation and/or navigation. In the present study, using a tethering system, we investigated how flying bees respond to polarized light stimuli. The behavioral responses of honeybees (Apis mellifera) to a zenithal polarized light stimulus were observed using a tethered animal in a flight simulator. Flight direction of the bee was recorded by monitoring the horizontal movement of its abdomen, which was strongly anti-correlated with its torque. When the e-vector orientation of the polarized light was rotated clockwise or counterclockwise, the bee responded with periodic right-and-left abdominal movements; however, the bee did not show any clear periodic movement under the static e-vector or depolarized stimulus. The steering frequency of the bee was well coordinated with the e-vector rotation frequency of the stimulus, indicating that the flying bee oriented itself to a certain e-vector orientation, i.e. exhibited polarotaxis. The percentage of bees exhibiting clear polarotaxis was much smaller under the fast stimulus (3.6 deg s-1) compared with that under a slow stimulus (0.9 or 1.8 deg s-1). Bees did not demonstrate any polarotactic behavior after the dorsal rim area of the eyes, which mediates insect polarization vision in general, was bilaterally covered with black paint. Preferred e-vector orientations under the clockwise stimulus varied among individuals and distributed throughout -90 to 90 deg. Some bees showed similar preferred e-vector orientations for clockwise and counterclockwise stimuli whereas others did not. Our results strongly suggest that flying honeybees utilize the e-vector information from the skylight to deduce their heading orientation for navigation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Norihiro Kobayashi
- Department of Biology, Graduate School of Science, Kobe University, Rokkodai 1-1, Nada-ku, Kobe, Hyogo 657-8501, Japan
| | - Ryuichi Okada
- Department of Biology, Graduate School of Science, Kobe University, Rokkodai 1-1, Nada-ku, Kobe, Hyogo 657-8501, Japan
- School of Human Science and Environment, University of Hyogo, 1-1-12 Shinzaike-Honcho, Himeji, Hyogo 670-0092, Japan
| | - Midori Sakura
- Department of Biology, Graduate School of Science, Kobe University, Rokkodai 1-1, Nada-ku, Kobe, Hyogo 657-8501, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Mathejczyk TF, Wernet MF. Modular assays for the quantitative study of visually guided navigation in both flying and walking flies. J Neurosci Methods 2020; 340:108747. [PMID: 32339523 DOI: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2020.108747] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/13/2020] [Revised: 04/16/2020] [Accepted: 04/17/2020] [Indexed: 01/12/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND The quantitative study of behavioral responses to visual stimuli provides crucial information about the computations executed by neural circuits. Insects have long served as powerful model systems, either when walking on air suspended balls (spherical treadmill), or flying while glued to a needle (virtual flight arena). NEW METHOD Here we present detailed instructions for 3D-printing and assembly of arenas optimized for visually guided navigation, including codes for presenting both celestial and panorama cues. These modular arenas can be used either as virtual flight arenas, or as spherical treadmills and consist entirely of commercial and 3D-printed components placed in a temperature and humidity controlled environment. COMPARISON TO EXISTING METHOD(S) Previous assays often include a combination of rather cost-intensive and technically complex, custom-built mechanical, electronic, and software components. Implementation amounts to a major challenge when working in an academic environment without the support of a professional machine shop. RESULTS Robust optomotor responses are induced in flyingDrosophila by displaying moving stripes in a cylinder surrounding the magnetically tethered fly. Similarly, changes in flight heading are induced by presenting changes in the orientation of linearly polarized UV light presented from above. Finally, responses to moving patterns are induced when individual flies are walking on an air-suspended ball. CONCLUSION These modular assays allow for the investigation of a diverse combination navigational cues (sky and panorama) in both flying and walking flies. They can be used for the molecular dissection of neural circuitry in Drosophila and can easily be rescaled for accommodating other insects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas F Mathejczyk
- Freie Universität Berlin, Fachbereich Biologie, Chemie und Pharmazie, Institut für Biologie - Neurobiologie, Königin-Luise Strasse 1-3, 14195 Berlin, Germany
| | - Mathias F Wernet
- Freie Universität Berlin, Fachbereich Biologie, Chemie und Pharmazie, Institut für Biologie - Neurobiologie, Königin-Luise Strasse 1-3, 14195 Berlin, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Heading choices of flying Drosophila under changing angles of polarized light. Sci Rep 2019; 9:16773. [PMID: 31727972 PMCID: PMC6856357 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-53330-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/30/2019] [Accepted: 10/30/2019] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Many navigating insects include the celestial polarization pattern as an additional visual cue to orient their travels. Spontaneous orientation responses of both walking and flying fruit flies (Drosophila melanogaster) to linearly polarized light have previously been demonstrated. Using newly designed modular flight arenas consisting entirely of off-the-shelf parts and 3D-printed components we present individual flying flies with a slow and continuous rotational change in the incident angle of linear polarization. Under such open-loop conditions, single flies choose arbitrary headings with respect to the angle of polarized light and show a clear tendency to maintain those chosen headings for several minutes, thereby adjusting their course to the slow rotation of the incident stimulus. Importantly, flies show the tendency to maintain a chosen heading even when two individual test periods under a linearly polarized stimulus are interrupted by an epoch of unpolarized light lasting several minutes. Finally, we show that these behavioral responses are wavelength-specific, existing under polarized UV stimulus while being absent under polarized green light. Taken together, these findings provide further evidence supporting Drosophila’s abilities to use celestial cues for visually guided navigation and course correction.
Collapse
|
13
|
Gkanias E, Risse B, Mangan M, Webb B. From skylight input to behavioural output: A computational model of the insect polarised light compass. PLoS Comput Biol 2019; 15:e1007123. [PMID: 31318859 PMCID: PMC6638774 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1007123] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/05/2018] [Accepted: 05/22/2019] [Indexed: 01/30/2023] Open
Abstract
Many insects navigate by integrating the distances and directions travelled on an outward path, allowing direct return to the starting point. Fundamental to the reliability of this process is the use of a neural compass based on external celestial cues. Here we examine how such compass information could be reliably computed by the insect brain, given realistic constraints on the sky polarisation pattern and the insect eye sensor array. By processing the degree of polarisation in different directions for different parts of the sky, our model can directly estimate the solar azimuth and also infer the confidence of the estimate. We introduce a method to correct for tilting of the sensor array, as might be caused by travel over uneven terrain. We also show that the confidence can be used to approximate the change in sun position over time, allowing the compass to remain fixed with respect to 'true north' during long excursions. We demonstrate that the compass is robust to disturbances and can be effectively used as input to an existing neural model of insect path integration. We discuss the plausibility of our model to be mapped to known neural circuits, and to be implemented for robot navigation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Evripidis Gkanias
- School of Informatics, The University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
| | - Benjamin Risse
- Faculty of Mathematics and Computer Science, University of Münster, Münster, Germany
| | - Michael Mangan
- Department of Computer Science, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, United Kingdom
| | - Barbara Webb
- School of Informatics, The University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Iwatani Y, Ogawa H, Shidara H, Sakura M, Sato T, Hojo MK, Honma A, Tsurui-Sato K. Markerless visual servo control of a servosphere for behavior observation of a variety of wandering animals. Adv Robot 2019. [DOI: 10.1080/01691864.2019.1570334] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Yasushi Iwatani
- Department of Science and Technology, Hirosaki University, Hirosaki, Japan
| | - Hiroto Ogawa
- Department of Biological Science, Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan
| | - Hisashi Shidara
- Department of Biological Science, Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan
| | | | - Takuya Sato
- Department of Biology, Kobe University, Kobe, Japan
| | - Masaru K. Hojo
- Department of Bioscience, Kwansei Gakuin University, Sanda, Japan
| | - Atsushi Honma
- Okinawa Prefectural Plant Protection Center, Naha, Japan
- Graduate School of Agriculture, University of the Ryukyus, Nishihara, Japan
| | - Kaori Tsurui-Sato
- Center for Strategic Research Project, University of the Ryukyus, Nishihara, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Swimming behaviour tunes fish polarization vision to double prey sighting distance. Sci Rep 2019; 9:944. [PMID: 30700806 PMCID: PMC6353921 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-018-37632-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/30/2018] [Accepted: 12/07/2018] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
The analysis of the polarization of light expands vision beyond the realm of colour and intensity and is used for multiple ecological purposes among invertebrates including orientation, object recognition, and communication. How vertebrates use polarization vision as part of natural behaviours is widely unknown. In this study, I tested the hypothesis that polarization vision improves the detection of zooplankton prey by the northern anchovy, Engraulis mordax, the only vertebrate with a demonstrated photoreceptor basis explaining its polarization sensitivity. Juvenile anchovies were recorded free foraging on zooplankton under downwelling light fields of varying percent polarization (98%, 67%, 19%, and 0% - unpolarized light). Analyses of prey attack sequences showed that anchovies swam in the horizontal plane perpendicular, on average, to the polarization direction of downwelling light and attacked prey at pitch angles that maximized polarization contrast perception of prey by the ventro-temporal retina, the area devoted to polarization vision in this animal. Consequently, the mean prey location distance under polarized light was up to 2.1 times that under unpolarized conditions. All indicators of polarization vision mediated foraging were present under 19% polarization, which is within the polarization range commonly found in nature during daylight hours. These results demonstrate: (i) the first use of oriented swimming for enhancing polarization contrast detection of prey, (ii) its relevance to improved foraging under available light cues in nature, and (iii) an increase in target detection distance that is only matched by polarization based artificial systems.
Collapse
|
16
|
Foster JJ, Kirwan JD, El Jundi B, Smolka J, Khaldy L, Baird E, Byrne MJ, Nilsson DE, Johnsen S, Dacke M. Orienting to polarized light at night - matching lunar skylight to performance in a nocturnal beetle. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2019; 222:jeb.188532. [PMID: 30530838 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.188532] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/10/2018] [Accepted: 11/29/2018] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
For polarized light to inform behaviour, the typical range of degrees of polarization observable in the animal's natural environment must be above the threshold for detection and interpretation. Here, we present the first investigation of the degree of linear polarization threshold for orientation behaviour in a nocturnal species, with specific reference to the range of degrees of polarization measured in the night sky. An effect of lunar phase on the degree of polarization of skylight was found, with smaller illuminated fractions of the moon's surface corresponding to lower degrees of polarization in the night sky. We found that the South African dung beetle Escarabaeus satyrus can orient to polarized light for a range of degrees of polarization similar to that observed in diurnal insects, reaching a lower threshold between 0.04 and 0.32, possibly as low as 0.11. For degrees of polarization lower than 0.23, as measured on a crescent moon night, orientation performance was considerably weaker than that observed for completely linearly polarized stimuli, but was nonetheless stronger than in the absence of polarized light.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- James J Foster
- Lund Vision Group, Department of Biology, Lund University, Sölvegatan 35, 223 62 Lund, Sweden
| | - John D Kirwan
- Lund Vision Group, Department of Biology, Lund University, Sölvegatan 35, 223 62 Lund, Sweden
| | - Basil El Jundi
- Biocenter (Zoology II), University of Würzburg, Am Hubland, 97074 Würzburg, Germany
| | - Jochen Smolka
- Lund Vision Group, Department of Biology, Lund University, Sölvegatan 35, 223 62 Lund, Sweden
| | - Lana Khaldy
- Lund Vision Group, Department of Biology, Lund University, Sölvegatan 35, 223 62 Lund, Sweden
| | - Emily Baird
- Lund Vision Group, Department of Biology, Lund University, Sölvegatan 35, 223 62 Lund, Sweden
| | - Marcus J Byrne
- School of Animal, Plant and Environmental Sciences, University of the Witwatersrand, Wits 2050, South Africa
| | - Dan-Eric Nilsson
- Lund Vision Group, Department of Biology, Lund University, Sölvegatan 35, 223 62 Lund, Sweden
| | - Sönke Johnsen
- Biology Department, Duke University, 130 Science Drive, Durham, NC 27708, USA
| | - Marie Dacke
- Lund Vision Group, Department of Biology, Lund University, Sölvegatan 35, 223 62 Lund, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Zhao H, Xu W, Zhang Y, Li X, Zhang H, Xuan J, Jia B. Polarization patterns under different sky conditions and a navigation method based on the symmetry of the AOP map of skylight. OPTICS EXPRESS 2018; 26:28589-28603. [PMID: 30470033 DOI: 10.1364/oe.26.028589] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/03/2018] [Accepted: 09/20/2018] [Indexed: 06/09/2023]
Abstract
The polarization states of a skylight can be used for navigation by a lot of insects as well as by human beings. However, the polarization patterns of skylight are greatly influenced by the atmospheric conditions. This paper studied the polarization patterns of skylight under different sky conditions by polarized imaging measurements, in which the AOT (Aerosol Optical Thickness) and clouds were taken into account. The results showed that both the aerosol and cloud disturbed the polarization patterns of the skylight, but the patterns of AOP (Angle of Polarization) showed great robustness. The symmetry of the AOP images was found to be quite steady under most of the sky conditions. We proposed a navigation method by finding out the solar meridian according to the symmetry of an AOP map. The results showed that the solar meridian can be identified accurately under different situations. The calculation errors slightly fluctuated along with the aerosol and cloud.
Collapse
|
18
|
Warren TL, Weir PT, Dickinson MH. Flying Drosophilamelanogaster maintain arbitrary but stable headings relative to the angle of polarized light. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2018; 221:jeb.177550. [PMID: 29593084 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.177550] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/16/2018] [Accepted: 03/19/2018] [Indexed: 01/27/2023]
Abstract
Animals must use external cues to maintain a straight course over long distances. In this study, we investigated how the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster selects and maintains a flight heading relative to the axis of linearly polarized light, a visual cue produced by the atmospheric scattering of sunlight. To track flies' headings over extended periods, we used a flight simulator that coupled the angular velocity of dorsally presented polarized light to the stroke amplitude difference of the animals' wings. In the simulator, most flies actively maintained a stable heading relative to the axis of polarized light for the duration of 15 min flights. We found that individuals selected arbitrary, unpredictable headings relative to the polarization axis, which demonstrates that D. melanogaster can perform proportional navigation using a polarized light pattern. When flies flew in two consecutive bouts separated by a 5 min gap, the two flight headings were correlated, suggesting individuals retain a memory of their chosen heading. We found that adding a polarized light pattern to a light intensity gradient enhanced flies' orientation ability, suggesting D. melanogaster use a combination of cues to navigate. For both polarized light and intensity cues, flies' capacity to maintain a stable heading gradually increased over several minutes from the onset of flight. Our findings are consistent with a model in which each individual initially orients haphazardly but then settles on a heading which is maintained via a self-reinforcing process. This may be a general dispersal strategy for animals with no target destination.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Timothy L Warren
- Division of Biology and Biological Engineering, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, California 91125, USA.,Institute of Neuroscience, University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon 97401, USA.,Department of Botany and Plant Pathology, Oregon State University, Corvallis, Oregon 97331, USA
| | - Peter T Weir
- Division of Biology and Biological Engineering, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, California 91125, USA.,Data Science, Yelp, San Francisco, CA, 94111, USA
| | - Michael H Dickinson
- Division of Biology and Biological Engineering, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, California 91125, USA
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Libersat F, Kaiser M, Emanuel S. Mind Control: How Parasites Manipulate Cognitive Functions in Their Insect Hosts. Front Psychol 2018; 9:572. [PMID: 29765342 PMCID: PMC5938628 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00572] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/04/2018] [Accepted: 04/04/2018] [Indexed: 12/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Neuro-parasitology is an emerging branch of science that deals with parasites that can control the nervous system of the host. It offers the possibility of discovering how one species (the parasite) modifies a particular neural network, and thus particular behaviors, of another species (the host). Such parasite-host interactions, developed over millions of years of evolution, provide unique tools by which one can determine how neuromodulation up-or-down regulates specific behaviors. In some of the most fascinating manipulations, the parasite taps into the host brain neuronal circuities to manipulate hosts cognitive functions. To name just a few examples, some worms induce crickets and other terrestrial insects to commit suicide in water, enabling the exit of the parasite into an aquatic environment favorable to its reproduction. In another example of behavioral manipulation, ants that consumed the secretions of a caterpillar containing dopamine are less likely to move away from the caterpillar and more likely to be aggressive. This benefits the caterpillar for without its ant bodyguards, it is more likely to be predated upon or attacked by parasitic insects that would lay eggs inside its body. Another example is the parasitic wasp, which induces a guarding behavior in its ladybug host in collaboration with a viral mutualist. To exert long-term behavioral manipulation of the host, parasite must secrete compounds that act through secondary messengers and/or directly on genes often modifying gene expression to produce long-lasting effects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Frederic Libersat
- Department of Life Sciences and Zlotowski Center for Neurosciences, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva, Israel
| | - Maayan Kaiser
- Department of Life Sciences and Zlotowski Center for Neurosciences, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva, Israel
| | - Stav Emanuel
- Department of Life Sciences and Zlotowski Center for Neurosciences, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Foster JJ, Temple SE, How MJ, Daly IM, Sharkey CR, Wilby D, Roberts NW. Polarisation vision: overcoming challenges of working with a property of light we barely see. THE SCIENCE OF NATURE - NATURWISSENSCHAFTEN 2018; 105:27. [PMID: 29589169 PMCID: PMC5871655 DOI: 10.1007/s00114-018-1551-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/28/2017] [Revised: 02/27/2018] [Accepted: 03/01/2018] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
In recent years, the study of polarisation vision in animals has seen numerous breakthroughs, not just in terms of what is known about the function of this sensory ability, but also in the experimental methods by which polarisation can be controlled, presented and measured. Once thought to be limited to only a few animal species, polarisation sensitivity is now known to be widespread across many taxonomic groups, and advances in experimental techniques are, in part, responsible for these discoveries. Nevertheless, its study remains challenging, perhaps because of our own poor sensitivity to the polarisation of light, but equally as a result of the slow spread of new practices and methodological innovations within the field. In this review, we introduce the most important steps in designing and calibrating polarised stimuli, within the broader context of areas of current research and the applications of new techniques to key questions. Our aim is to provide a constructive guide to help researchers, particularly those with no background in the physics of polarisation, to design robust experiments that are free from confounding factors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- James J Foster
- Vision Group, Department of Biology, Lund University, Sölvegatan 35, 223 62, Lund, Sweden.
| | - Shelby E Temple
- Ecology of Vision Laboratory, School of Biological Sciences, Life Sciences Building, University of Bristol, Tyndall Avenue, Bristol, BS8 1TQ, UK
- Azul Optics Ltd., 7 Bishop Manor Road, Westbury-On-Trym, Bristol, BS10 5BD, UK
| | - Martin J How
- Ecology of Vision Laboratory, School of Biological Sciences, Life Sciences Building, University of Bristol, Tyndall Avenue, Bristol, BS8 1TQ, UK
| | - Ilse M Daly
- Ecology of Vision Laboratory, School of Biological Sciences, Life Sciences Building, University of Bristol, Tyndall Avenue, Bristol, BS8 1TQ, UK
| | - Camilla R Sharkey
- Department of Physiology, Development and Neuroscience, Cambridge University, Cambridge, CB2 3EG, UK
| | - David Wilby
- Ecology of Vision Laboratory, School of Biological Sciences, Life Sciences Building, University of Bristol, Tyndall Avenue, Bristol, BS8 1TQ, UK
| | - Nicholas W Roberts
- Ecology of Vision Laboratory, School of Biological Sciences, Life Sciences Building, University of Bristol, Tyndall Avenue, Bristol, BS8 1TQ, UK
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Heinloth T, Uhlhorn J, Wernet MF. Insect Responses to Linearly Polarized Reflections: Orphan Behaviors Without Neural Circuits. Front Cell Neurosci 2018; 12:50. [PMID: 29615868 PMCID: PMC5870057 DOI: 10.3389/fncel.2018.00050] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/19/2017] [Accepted: 02/15/2018] [Indexed: 12/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The e-vector orientation of linearly polarized light represents an important visual stimulus for many insects. Especially the detection of polarized skylight by many navigating insect species is known to improve their orientation skills. While great progress has been made towards describing both the anatomy and function of neural circuit elements mediating behaviors related to navigation, relatively little is known about how insects perceive non-celestial polarized light stimuli, like reflections off water, leaves, or shiny body surfaces. Work on different species suggests that these behaviors are not mediated by the “Dorsal Rim Area” (DRA), a specialized region in the dorsal periphery of the adult compound eye, where ommatidia contain highly polarization-sensitive photoreceptor cells whose receptive fields point towards the sky. So far, only few cases of polarization-sensitive photoreceptors have been described in the ventral periphery of the insect retina. Furthermore, both the structure and function of those neural circuits connecting to these photoreceptor inputs remain largely uncharacterized. Here we review the known data on non-celestial polarization vision from different insect species (dragonflies, butterflies, beetles, bugs and flies) and present three well-characterized examples for functionally specialized non-DRA detectors from different insects that seem perfectly suited for mediating such behaviors. Finally, using recent advances from circuit dissection in Drosophila melanogaster, we discuss what types of potential candidate neurons could be involved in forming the underlying neural circuitry mediating non-celestial polarization vision.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tanja Heinloth
- Division of Neurobiology, Institut für Biology, Fachbereich Biologie, Chemie & Pharmazie, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Juliane Uhlhorn
- Division of Neurobiology, Institut für Biology, Fachbereich Biologie, Chemie & Pharmazie, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Mathias F Wernet
- Division of Neurobiology, Institut für Biology, Fachbereich Biologie, Chemie & Pharmazie, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Abstract
The visual world is rich in linearly polarized light stimuli, which are hidden from the human eye. But many invertebrate species make use of polarized light as a source of valuable visual information. However, exploiting light polarization does not necessarily imply that the electric (e)-vector orientation of polarized light can be perceived as a separate modality of light. In this Review, I address the question of whether invertebrates can detect specific e-vector orientations in a manner similar to that of humans perceiving spectral stimuli as specific hues. To analyze e-vector orientation, the signals of at least three polarization-sensitive sensors (analyzer channels) with different e-vector tuning axes must be compared. The object-based, imaging polarization vision systems of cephalopods and crustaceans, as well as the water-surface detectors of flying backswimmers, use just two analyzer channels. Although this excludes the perception of specific e-vector orientations, a two-channel system does provide a coarse, categoric analysis of polarized light stimuli, comparable to the limited color sense of dichromatic, 'color-blind' humans. The celestial compass of insects employs three or more analyzer channels. However, that compass is multimodal, i.e. e-vector information merges with directional information from other celestial cues, such as the solar azimuth and the spectral gradient in the sky, masking e-vector information. It seems that invertebrate organisms take no interest in the polarization details of visual stimuli, but polarization vision grants more practical benefits, such as improved object detection and visual communication for cephalopods and crustaceans, compass readings to traveling insects, or the alert 'water below!' to water-seeking bugs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas Labhart
- Institute of Molecular Life Sciences, University of Zurich, Winterthurerstrasse 190, Zürich CH 8057, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Zhao H, Xu W. A Bionic Polarization Navigation Sensor and Its Calibration Method. SENSORS 2016; 16:s16081223. [PMID: 27527171 PMCID: PMC5017388 DOI: 10.3390/s16081223] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/09/2016] [Revised: 07/24/2016] [Accepted: 07/24/2016] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
The polarization patterns of skylight which arise due to the scattering of sunlight in the atmosphere can be used by many insects for deriving compass information. Inspired by insects' polarized light compass, scientists have developed a new kind of navigation method. One of the key techniques in this method is the polarimetric sensor which is used to acquire direction information from skylight. In this paper, a polarization navigation sensor is proposed which imitates the working principles of the polarization vision systems of insects. We introduce the optical design and mathematical model of the sensor. In addition, a calibration method based on variable substitution and non-linear curve fitting is proposed. The results obtained from the outdoor experiments provide support for the feasibility and precision of the sensor. The sensor's signal processing can be well described using our mathematical model. A relatively high degree of accuracy in polarization measurement can be obtained without any error compensation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Huijie Zhao
- School of Instrument Science and Opto-Electronics Engineering, Beihang University, No. 37 Xueyuan RD, Haidian, Beijing 100191, China.
| | - Wujian Xu
- School of Instrument Science and Opto-Electronics Engineering, Beihang University, No. 37 Xueyuan RD, Haidian, Beijing 100191, China.
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Temple SE, McGregor JE, Miles C, Graham L, Miller J, Buck J, Scott-Samuel NE, Roberts NW. Perceiving polarization with the naked eye: characterization of human polarization sensitivity. Proc Biol Sci 2016; 282:rspb.2015.0338. [PMID: 26136441 PMCID: PMC4528539 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2015.0338] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Like many animals, humans are sensitive to the polarization of light. We can detect the angle of polarization using an entoptic phenomenon called Haidinger's brushes, which is mediated by dichroic carotenoids in the macula lutea. While previous studies have characterized the spectral sensitivity of Haidinger's brushes, other aspects remain unexplored. We developed a novel methodology for presenting gratings in polarization-only contrast at varying degrees of polarization in order to measure the lower limits of human polarized light detection. Participants were, on average, able to perform the task down to a threshold of 56%, with some able to go as low as 23%. This makes humans the most sensitive vertebrate tested to date. Additionally, we quantified a nonlinear relationship between presented and perceived polarization angle when an observer is presented with a rotatable polarized light field. This result confirms a previous theoretical prediction of how uniaxial corneal birefringence impacts the perception of Haidinger's brushes. The rotational dynamics of Haidinger's brushes were then used to calculate corneal retardance.We suggest that psychophysical experiments, based upon the perception of polarized light, are amenable to the production of affordable technologies for self-assessment and longitudinal monitoring of visual dysfunctions such as age-related macular degeneration.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shelby E. Temple
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK
- e-mail:
| | | | - Camilla Miles
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK
| | - Laura Graham
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK
| | - Josie Miller
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK
| | - Jordan Buck
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
25
|
Wernet MF, Perry MW, Desplan C. The evolutionary diversity of insect retinal mosaics: common design principles and emerging molecular logic. Trends Genet 2015; 31:316-28. [PMID: 26025917 PMCID: PMC4458154 DOI: 10.1016/j.tig.2015.04.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 57] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/03/2015] [Revised: 04/13/2015] [Accepted: 04/14/2015] [Indexed: 12/21/2022]
Abstract
Independent evolution has resulted in a vast diversity of eyes. Despite the lack of a common Bauplan or ancestral structure, similar developmental strategies are used. For instance, different classes of photoreceptor cells (PRs) are distributed stochastically and/or localized in different regions of the retina. Here, we focus on recent progress made towards understanding the molecular principles behind patterning retinal mosaics of insects, one of the most diverse groups of animals adapted to life on land, in the air, under water, or on the water surface. Morphological, physiological, and behavioral studies from many species provide detailed descriptions of the vast variation in retinal design and function. By integrating this knowledge with recent progress in the characterization of insect Rhodopsins as well as insight from the model organism Drosophila melanogaster, we seek to identify the molecular logic behind the adaptation of retinal mosaics to the habitat and way of life of an animal.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mathias F Wernet
- New York University Abu Dhabi, Abu Dhabi, 129188 Saadiyat Island, United Arab Emirates
| | - Michael W Perry
- Department of Biology, New York University, New York, NY 10003, USA
| | - Claude Desplan
- New York University Abu Dhabi, Abu Dhabi, 129188 Saadiyat Island, United Arab Emirates; Department of Biology, New York University, New York, NY 10003, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
An evaluation of skylight polarization patterns for navigation. SENSORS 2015; 15:5895-913. [PMID: 25763652 PMCID: PMC4435192 DOI: 10.3390/s150305895] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/23/2014] [Revised: 12/15/2014] [Accepted: 02/28/2015] [Indexed: 12/02/2022]
Abstract
Skylight polarization provides a significant navigation cue for certain polarization-sensitive animals. However, the precision of the angle of polarization (AOP) of skylight for vehicle orientation is not clear. An evaluation of AOP must be performed before it is utilized. This paper reports an evaluation of AOP of skylight by measuring the skylight polarization patterns of clear and cloudy skies using a full-sky imaging polarimetry system. AOP measurements of skylight are compared with the pattern calculated by the single-scattering Rayleigh model and these differences are quantified. The relationship between the degree of polarization (DOP) and the deviation of AOP of skylight is thoroughly studied. Based on these, a solar meridian extracted method is presented. The results of experiments reveal that the DOP is a key parameter to indicate the accuracy of AOP measurements, and all the output solar meridian orientations extracted by our method in both clear and cloudy skies can achieve a high accuracy for vehicle orientation.
Collapse
|
27
|
Blaho M, Herczeg T, Kriska G, Egri A, Szaz D, Farkas A, Tarjanyi N, Czinke L, Barta A, Horvath G. Unexpected attraction of polarotactic water-leaving insects to matt black car surfaces: mattness of paintwork cannot eliminate the polarized light pollution of black cars. PLoS One 2014; 9:e103339. [PMID: 25076137 PMCID: PMC4116178 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0103339] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/05/2014] [Accepted: 06/27/2014] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
The horizontally polarizing surface parts of shiny black cars (the reflection-polarization characteristics of which are similar to those of water surfaces) attract water-leaving polarotactic insects. Thus, shiny black cars are typical sources of polarized light pollution endangering water-leaving insects. A new fashion fad is to make car-bodies matt black or grey. Since rough (matt) surfaces depolarize the reflected light, one of the ways of reducing polarized light pollution is to make matt the concerned surface. Consequently, matt black/grey cars may not induce polarized light pollution, which would be an advantageous feature for environmental protection. To test this idea, we performed field experiments with horizontal shiny and matt black car-body surfaces laid on the ground. Using imaging polarimetry, in multiple-choice field experiments we investigated the attractiveness of these test surfaces to various water-leaving polarotactic insects and obtained the following results: (i) The attractiveness of black car-bodies to polarotactic insects depends in complex manner on the surface roughness (shiny, matt) and species (mayflies, dolichopodids, tabanids). (ii) Non-expectedly, the matt dark grey car finish is much more attractive to mayflies (being endangered and protected in many countries) than matt black finish. (iii) The polarized light pollution of shiny black cars usually cannot be reduced with the use of matt painting. On the basis of these, our two novel findings are that (a) matt car-paints are highly polarization reflecting, and (b) these matt paints are not suitable to repel polarotactic insects. Hence, the recent technology used to make matt the car-bodies cannot eliminate or even can enhance the attractiveness of black/grey cars to water-leaving insects. Thus, changing shiny black car painting to matt one is a disadvantageous fashion fad concerning the reduction of polarized light pollution of black vehicles.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Miklos Blaho
- Environmental Optics Laboratory, Department of Biological Physics, Physical Institute, Eötvös University, Budapest, Hungary
| | - Tamas Herczeg
- Environmental Optics Laboratory, Department of Biological Physics, Physical Institute, Eötvös University, Budapest, Hungary
| | - Gyorgy Kriska
- Group for Methodology in Biology Teaching, Biological Institute, Eötvös University, Budapest, Hungary
- Danube Research Institute, Centre for Ecological Research, Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Budapest, Hungary
| | - Adam Egri
- Environmental Optics Laboratory, Department of Biological Physics, Physical Institute, Eötvös University, Budapest, Hungary
| | - Denes Szaz
- Environmental Optics Laboratory, Department of Biological Physics, Physical Institute, Eötvös University, Budapest, Hungary
| | - Alexandra Farkas
- Environmental Optics Laboratory, Department of Biological Physics, Physical Institute, Eötvös University, Budapest, Hungary
| | - Nikolett Tarjanyi
- Danube Research Institute, Centre for Ecological Research, Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Budapest, Hungary
| | - Laszlo Czinke
- Environmental Optics Laboratory, Department of Biological Physics, Physical Institute, Eötvös University, Budapest, Hungary
- Group for Methodology in Biology Teaching, Biological Institute, Eötvös University, Budapest, Hungary
| | - Andras Barta
- Environmental Optics Laboratory, Department of Biological Physics, Physical Institute, Eötvös University, Budapest, Hungary
| | - Gabor Horvath
- Environmental Optics Laboratory, Department of Biological Physics, Physical Institute, Eötvös University, Budapest, Hungary
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Velez MM, Gohl D, Clandinin TR, Wernet MF. Differences in Neural Circuitry Guiding Behavioral Responses to Polarized light Presented to Either the Dorsal or Ventral Retina inDrosophila. J Neurogenet 2014; 28:348-60. [DOI: 10.3109/01677063.2014.922556] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/27/2023]
|
29
|
Biron DG, Loxdale HD. Host–parasite molecular cross-talk during the manipulative process of a host by its parasite. J Exp Biol 2013; 216:148-60. [DOI: 10.1242/jeb.073825] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/25/2022]
Abstract
Summary
Many parasite taxa are able to alter a wide range of phenotypic traits of their hosts in ways that seem to improve the parasite’s chance of completing its life cycle. Host behavioural alterations are classically seen as compelling illustrations of the ‘extended phenotype’ concept, which suggests that parasite genes have phenotype effects on the host. The molecular mechanisms and the host–parasite cross-talk involved during the manipulative process of a host by its parasite are still poorly understood. In this Review, the current knowledge on proximate mechanisms related to the ‘parasite manipulation hypothesis’ is presented. Parasite genome sequences do not themselves provide a full explanation of parasite biology nor of the molecular cross-talk involved in host–parasite associations. Recently, first-generation proteomics tools have been employed to unravel some aspects of the parasite manipulation process (i.e. proximate mechanisms and evolutionary convergence) using certain model arthropod-host–parasite associations. The pioneer proteomics results obtained on the manipulative process are here highlighted, along with the many gaps in our knowledge. Candidate genes and biochemical pathways potentially involved in the parasite manipulation are presented. Finally, taking into account the environmental factors, we suggest new avenues and approaches to further explore and understand the proximate mechanisms used by parasite species to alter phenotypic traits of their hosts.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David G. Biron
- Clermont Université, Université Blaise Pascal, Laboratoire ‘Microorganismes: Génome et Environnement’, BP 10448, F-63000 Clermont-Ferrand, France
- CNRS, UMR 6023, LMGE, F-63177 Aubiere, France
| | - Hugh D. Loxdale
- Royal Entomological Society, Chiswell Green Lane, St Albans AL2 3NS, UK
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Henze MJ, Dannenhauer K, Kohler M, Labhart T, Gesemann M. Opsin evolution and expression in arthropod compound eyes and ocelli: insights from the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. BMC Evol Biol 2012; 12:163. [PMID: 22935102 PMCID: PMC3502269 DOI: 10.1186/1471-2148-12-163] [Citation(s) in RCA: 52] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/17/2012] [Accepted: 08/24/2012] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Opsins are key proteins in animal photoreception. Together with a light-sensitive group, the chromophore, they form visual pigments which initiate the visual transduction cascade when photoactivated. The spectral absorption properties of visual pigments are mainly determined by their opsins, and thus opsins are crucial for understanding the adaptations of animal eyes. Studies on the phylogeny and expression pattern of opsins have received considerable attention, but our knowledge about insect visual opsins is still limited. Up to now, researchers have focused on holometabolous insects, while general conclusions require sampling from a broader range of taxa. We have therefore investigated visual opsins in the ocelli and compound eyes of the two-spotted cricket Gryllus bimaculatus, a hemimetabolous insect. Results Phylogenetic analyses place all identified cricket sequences within the three main visual opsin clades of insects. We assign three of these opsins to visual pigments found in the compound eyes with peak absorbances in the green (515 nm), blue (445 nm) and UV (332 nm) spectral range. Their expression pattern divides the retina into distinct regions: (1) the polarization-sensitive dorsal rim area with blue- and UV-opsin, (2) a newly-discovered ventral band of ommatidia with blue- and green-opsin and (3) the remainder of the compound eye with UV- and green-opsin. In addition, we provide evidence for two ocellar photopigments with peak absorbances in the green (511 nm) and UV (350 nm) spectral range, and with opsins that differ from those expressed in the compound eyes. Conclusions Our data show that cricket eyes are spectrally more specialized than has previously been assumed, suggesting that similar adaptations in other insect species might have been overlooked. The arrangement of spectral receptor types within some ommatidia of the cricket compound eyes differs from the generally accepted pattern found in holometabolous insect taxa and awaits a functional explanation. From the opsin phylogeny, we conclude that gene duplications, which permitted differential opsin expression in insect ocelli and compound eyes, occurred independently in several insect lineages and are recent compared to the origin of the eyes themselves.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Miriam J Henze
- Department of Biology, Lund University, Sölvegatan 35, 22362 Lund, Sweden.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
31
|
Homberg U, Heinze S, Pfeiffer K, Kinoshita M, el Jundi B. Central neural coding of sky polarization in insects. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2011; 366:680-7. [PMID: 21282171 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0199] [Citation(s) in RCA: 160] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Many animals rely on a sun compass for spatial orientation and long-range navigation. In addition to the Sun, insects also exploit the polarization pattern and chromatic gradient of the sky for estimating navigational directions. Analysis of polarization-vision pathways in locusts and crickets has shed first light on brain areas involved in sky compass orientation. Detection of sky polarization relies on specialized photoreceptor cells in a small dorsal rim area of the compound eye. Brain areas involved in polarization processing include parts of the lamina, medulla and lobula of the optic lobe and, in the central brain, the anterior optic tubercle, the lateral accessory lobe and the central complex. In the optic lobe, polarization sensitivity and contrast are enhanced through convergence and opponency. In the anterior optic tubercle, polarized-light signals are integrated with information on the chromatic contrast of the sky. Tubercle neurons combine responses to the UV/green contrast and e-vector orientation of the sky and compensate for diurnal changes of the celestial polarization pattern associated with changes in solar elevation. In the central complex, a topographic representation of e-vector tunings underlies the columnar organization and suggests that this brain area serves as an internal compass coding for spatial directions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Uwe Homberg
- Department of Biology, Animal Physiology, University of Marburg, 35032 Marburg, Germany.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
32
|
Lerner A, Sabbah S, Erlick C, Shashar N. Navigation by light polarization in clear and turbid waters. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2011; 366:671-9. [PMID: 21282170 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0189] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Certain terrestrial animals use sky polarization for navigation. Certain aquatic species have also been shown to orient according to a polarization stimulus, but the correlation between underwater polarization and Sun position and hence the ability to use underwater polarization as a compass for navigation is still under debate. To examine this issue, we use theoretical equations for per cent polarization and electric vector (e-vector) orientation that account for the position of the Sun, refraction at the air-water interface and Rayleigh single scattering. The polarization patterns predicted by these theoretical equations are compared with measurements conducted in clear and semi-turbid coastal sea waters at 2 m and 5 m depth over sea floors of 6 m and 28 m depth. We find that the per cent polarization is correlated with the Sun's elevation only in clear waters. We furthermore find that the maximum value of the e-vector orientation angle equals the angle of refraction only in clear waters, in the horizontal viewing direction, over the deeper sea floor. We conclude that navigation by use of underwater polarization is possible under restricted conditions, i.e. in clear waters, primarily near the horizontal viewing direction, and in locations where the sea floor has limited effects on the light's polarization.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Amit Lerner
- Department of Atmospheric Sciences, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem 91904, Israel.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
33
|
Polarization-sensitive descending neurons in the locust: connecting the brain to thoracic ganglia. J Neurosci 2011; 31:2238-47. [PMID: 21307260 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.3624-10.2011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/19/2023] Open
Abstract
Many animal species, in particular insects, exploit the E-vector pattern of the blue sky for sun compass navigation. Like other insects, locusts detect dorsal polarized light via photoreceptors in a specialized dorsal rim area of the compound eye. Polarized light information is transmitted through several processing stages to the central complex, a brain area involved in the control of goal-directed orientation behavior. To investigate how polarized light information is transmitted to thoracic motor circuits, we studied the responses of locust descending neurons to polarized light. Three sets of polarization-sensitive descending neurons were characterized through intracellular recordings from axonal fibers in the neck connectives combined with single-cell dye injections. Two descending neurons from the brain, one with ipsilaterally and the second with contralaterally descending axon, are likely to bridge the gap between polarization-sensitive neurons in the brain and thoracic motor centers. In both neurons, E-vector tuning changed linearly with daytime, suggesting that they signal time-compensated spatial directions, an important prerequisite for navigation using celestial signals. The third type connects the suboesophageal ganglion with the prothoracic ganglion. It showed no evidence for time compensation in E-vector tuning and might play a role in flight stabilization and control of head movements.
Collapse
|
34
|
Ponton F, Otálora-Luna F, Lefèvre T, Guerin PM, Lebarbenchon C, Duneau D, Biron DG, Thomas F. Water-seeking behavior in worm-infected crickets and reversibility of parasitic manipulation. Behav Ecol 2011; 22:392-400. [PMID: 22476265 PMCID: PMC3071748 DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arq215] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/10/2010] [Revised: 12/02/2010] [Accepted: 12/02/2010] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
One of the most fascinating examples of parasite-induced host manipulation is that of hairworms, first, because they induce a spectacular "suicide" water-seeking behavior in their terrestrial insect hosts and, second, because the emergence of the parasite is not lethal per se for the host that can live several months following parasite release. The mechanisms hairworms use to increase the encounter rate between their host and water remain, however, poorly understood. Considering the selective landscape in which nematomorph manipulation has evolved as well as previously obtained proteomics data, we predicted that crickets harboring mature hairworms would display a modified behavioral response to light. Since following parasite emergence in water, the cricket host and parasitic worm do not interact physiologically anymore, we also predicted that the host would recover from the modified behaviors. We examined the effect of hairworm infection on different behavioral responses of the host when stimulated by light to record responses from uninfected, infected, and ex-infected crickets. We showed that hairworm infection fundamentally modifies cricket behavior by inducing directed responses to light, a condition from which they mostly recover once the parasite is released. This study supports the idea that host manipulation by parasites is subtle, complex, and multidimensional.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Fleur Ponton
- GEMI/UMR CNRS-IRD 2724, Equipe: "Evolution des Systèmes Symbiotiques", IRD, 911 Avenue Agropolis, B.P. 5045, 34032 Montpellier Cedex 1, France
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
35
|
Pfeiffer K, Negrello M, Homberg U. Conditional Perception Under Stimulus Ambiguity: Polarization- and Azimuth-Sensitive Neurons in the Locust Brain Are Inhibited by Low Degrees of Polarization. J Neurophysiol 2011; 105:28-35. [DOI: 10.1152/jn.00480.2010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Sensory perception often relies on the integration and matching of multisensory inputs. In the brain of desert locusts, identified neurons that signal the sun's direction relative to the animal's head integrate information about the polarization pattern of the sky with information on the color and intensity contrast of the sky. The cloudless blue sky exhibits a gradient from unpolarized sunlight to strongly polarized light at 90° from the sun. Therefore the percentage of polarized light in the sky is highest at dusk and dawn and lowest when the sun is in the zenith. We investigated the effect of different degrees of polarization on neurons of the anterior optic tubercle of the desert locust through intracellular recordings. Whereas dorsal presentation of strongly polarized light largely excited the neurons, weakly polarized light, i.e., a blend of polarized light of many orientations, led to inhibition. The data suggest that the polarization input to these neurons is inhibited within a radius of 50° around the sun, thereby avoiding conflicting input from the polarization and direct sunlight channels. These properties can be regarded as sensory filters to avoid ambiguous signaling during sky compass orientation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Keram Pfeiffer
- Department of Biology, Animal Physiology, University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany; and
| | - Mario Negrello
- Okinawa Institute of Science and Technology, Onna, Onna-Son, Kunigami, Okinawa, Japan
| | - Uwe Homberg
- Department of Biology, Animal Physiology, University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany; and
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Vickerstaff RJ, Cheung A. Which coordinate system for modelling path integration? J Theor Biol 2009; 263:242-61. [PMID: 19962387 DOI: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2009.11.021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/01/2009] [Revised: 11/19/2009] [Accepted: 11/22/2009] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Path integration is a navigation strategy widely observed in nature where an animal maintains a running estimate, called the home vector, of its location during an excursion. Evidence suggests it is both ancient and ubiquitous in nature, and has been studied for over a century. In that time, canonical and neural network models have flourished, based on a wide range of assumptions, justifications and supporting data. Despite the importance of the phenomenon, consensus and unifying principles appear lacking. A fundamental issue is the neural representation of space needed for biological path integration. This paper presents a scheme to classify path integration systems on the basis of the way the home vector records and updates the spatial relationship between the animal and its home location. Four extended classes of coordinate systems are used to unify and review both canonical and neural network models of path integration, from the arthropod and mammalian literature. This scheme demonstrates analytical equivalence between models which may otherwise appear unrelated, and distinguishes between models which may superficially appear similar. A thorough analysis is carried out of the equational forms of important facets of path integration including updating, steering, searching and systematic errors, using each of the four coordinate systems. The type of available directional cue, namely allothetic or idiothetic, is also considered. It is shown that on balance, the class of home vectors which includes the geocentric Cartesian coordinate system, appears to be the most robust for biological systems. A key conclusion is that deducing computational structure from behavioural data alone will be difficult or impossible, at least in the absence of an analysis of random errors. Consequently it is likely that further theoretical insights into path integration will require an in-depth study of the effect of noise on the four classes of home vectors.
Collapse
|
37
|
Sakura M, Lambrinos D, Labhart T. Polarized skylight navigation in insects: model and electrophysiology of e-vector coding by neurons in the central complex. J Neurophysiol 2007; 99:667-82. [PMID: 18057112 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00784.2007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 82] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Many insects exploit skylight polarization for visual compass orientation or course control. As found in crickets, the peripheral visual system (optic lobe) contains three types of polarization-sensitive neurons (POL neurons), which are tuned to different ( approximately 60 degrees diverging) e-vector orientations. Thus each e-vector orientation elicits a specific combination of activities among the POL neurons coding any e-vector orientation by just three neural signals. In this study, we hypothesize that in the presumed orientation center of the brain (central complex) e-vector orientation is population-coded by a set of "compass neurons." Using computer modeling, we present a neural network model transforming the signal triplet provided by the POL neurons to compass neuron activities coding e-vector orientation by a population code. Using intracellular electrophysiology and cell marking, we present evidence that neurons with the response profile of the presumed compass neurons do indeed exist in the insect brain: each of these compass neuron-like (CNL) cells is activated by a specific e-vector orientation only and otherwise remains silent. Morphologically, CNL cells are tangential neurons extending from the lateral accessory lobe to the lower division of the central body. Surpassing the modeled compass neurons in performance, CNL cells are insensitive to the degree of polarization of the stimulus between 99% and at least down to 18% polarization and thus largely disregard variations of skylight polarization due to changing solar elevations or atmospheric conditions. This suggests that the polarization vision system includes a gain control circuit keeping the output activity at a constant level.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Midori Sakura
- Institute of Zoology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
| | | | | |
Collapse
|