1
|
Uchiyama M, Mishima K, Yagi T, Yoshihara T, Eto T, Muehlan C, Togo O, Inoue Y. Pharmacokinetics, safety, and efficacy of daridorexant in Japanese subjects: Results from phase 1 and 2 studies. J Sleep Res 2024:e14302. [PMID: 39118245 DOI: 10.1111/jsr.14302] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/17/2024] [Revised: 06/25/2024] [Accepted: 07/22/2024] [Indexed: 08/10/2024]
Abstract
Daridorexant is a dual orexin receptor antagonist for the treatment of insomnia. We report results from the first two randomised, double-blind clinical studies of daridorexant in Japanese subjects. In the Phase 1 study, daridorexant (10, 25, 50 mg) or placebo were administered in the morning for 4 days in 24 young (mean age 26.9 years) and 24 older (mean age 69.7 years) healthy Japanese adults. Daridorexant reached a peak plasma concentration within 1.0 h across every dose and age group. For all doses, the mean plasma concentration of daridorexant showed a similar change between the age groups. Exposure parameters increased dose-dependently with minimal/no accumulation upon repeated dosing. The terminal half-life was ~8 h. In the Phase 2, four-period, four-way crossover study, 47 Japanese subjects (mean age 50.4 years) with insomnia disorder were randomised to receive four treatments (daridorexant 10, 25, 50 mg, placebo) during four treatment periods, each consisting of two treatment nights (5-12 day washout between treatment periods). Subjects continued their fourth treatment for 12 further days. A statistically significant dose-response relationship (multiple-comparison procedure-modelling, p < 0.0001) was found in the reduction of polysomnography-measured wake after sleep onset (WASO; primary endpoint) and latency to persistent sleep (secondary endpoint) from baseline to days 1/2. Statistically significant dose-response relationships were also observed for secondary subjective endpoints from baseline to days 1/2 (sWASO, latency to sleep onset). All daridorexant doses were well tolerated, with no treatment discontinuations and no next-morning residual effects. These results supported further investigation of daridorexant in Japanese patients with insomnia disorder.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Makoto Uchiyama
- Department of Psychiatry, Nihon University School of Medicine, Tokyo, Japan
- Tokyo Adachi Hospital, Tokyo, Japan
| | - Kazuo Mishima
- Department of Neuropsychiatry, Akita University Graduate School of Medicine, Akita, Japan
| | - Tomoko Yagi
- Kurume University School of Medicine, Fukuoka, Japan
| | - Tatsuya Yoshihara
- SOUSEIKAI Fukuoka Mirai Hospital Clinical Research Center, Fukuoka, Japan
| | | | - Clemens Muehlan
- Clinical Pharmacology, Idorsia Pharmaceuticals Ltd, Allschwil, Switzerland
| | - Osamu Togo
- Data Management & Biometry, Nxera Pharma Japan Co., Ltd, Tokyo, Japan
| | | |
Collapse
|
2
|
Kakutani-Hatayama M, Kadoya M, Morimoto A, Miyoshi A, Kosaka-Hamamoto K, Kanzaki A, Konishi K, Kusunoki Y, Syoji T, Koyama H. Excessive daytime napping independently associated with decreased insulin sensitivity in cross-sectional study - Hyogo Sleep Cardio-Autonomic Atherosclerosis cohort study. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne) 2023; 14:1211705. [PMID: 38027100 PMCID: PMC10656607 DOI: 10.3389/fendo.2023.1211705] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/25/2023] [Accepted: 10/17/2023] [Indexed: 12/01/2023] Open
Abstract
Background Although excessive daytime napping has been shown to be involved in diabetes occurrence, its impact on insulin secretion and sensitivity has not been elucidated. It is speculated that excessive napping disrupts the sleep-wake rhythm and increases sympathetic nerve activity during the day, resulting in decreased insulin sensitivity, which may be a mechanism leading to development of diabetes. We previously conducted a cross-sectional study that showed an association of autonomic dysfunction with decreased insulin sensitivity, though involvement of autonomic function in the association between napping and insulin sensitivity remained unclear. Furthermore, the effects of napping used to supplement to short nighttime sleep on insulin secretion and sensitivity are also unknown. In the present cross-sectional study, we examined the relationships of daytime nap duration and autonomic function with insulin secretion and sensitivity in 436 subjects enrolled in the Hyogo Sleep Cardio-Autonomic Atherosclerosis (HSCAA) Cohort Study who underwent a 75-g oral glucose tolerance test (75-g OGTT), after excluding those already diagnosed with diabetes. Methods Daytime nap duration was objectively measured using actigraphy, with the subjects divided into the short (≤1 hour) and long (>1 hour) nap groups. Insulin secretion and sensitivity were determined using 75-g OGTT findings. Standard deviation of normal to normal R-R interval (SDNN), a measure of autonomic function, was also determined based on heart rate variability. Subgroup analysis was performed for the associations of napping with insulin secretion and sensitivity, with the results stratified by nighttime sleep duration of less or greater than six hours. Results Subjects in the long nap group exhibited lower insulin sensitivity parameters (QUICKI: β=-0.135, p<0.01; Matsuda index: β=-0.119, p<0.05) independent of other clinical factors. In contrast, no associations with insulin secretion were found in either group. Furthermore, the association of long nap duration with insulin sensitivity was not confounded by SDNN. Specific subgroup analyses revealed more prominent associations of long nap habit with lower insulin sensitivity in subjects with a short nighttime sleep time (β=-0.137, p<0.05). Conclusion Long daytime nap duration may be a potential risk factor for decreased insulin sensitivity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Manabu Kadoya
- Department of Diabetes, Endocrinology and Clinical Immunology, School of Medicine, Hyogo Medical University, Nishinomiya, Hyogo, Japan
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
3
|
Ou S, Cao Y, Xie T, Jiang T, Li J, Luo W, Ma N. Effect of homeostatic pressure and circadian arousal on the storage and executive components of working memory: Evidence from EEG power spectrum. Biol Psychol 2023; 184:108721. [PMID: 37952693 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2023.108721] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/22/2023] [Revised: 09/08/2023] [Accepted: 11/08/2023] [Indexed: 11/14/2023]
Abstract
Diurnal fluctuations in working memory (WM) performance, characterized by task-specific peaks and troughs, are likely attributed to the differential regulation of WM subcomponents by interactions between circadian and homeostatic processes. The current study aimed to investigate the independent effects of circadian and homeostatic processes on the storage and executive subcomponents of WM. We assessed the change in frontal-midline theta (FMT) power supporting WM executive component and posterior alpha/beta power supporting WM storage during N-back tasks in the morning, midafternoon with and without a nap from 31 healthy adults. The results suggested that when the accumulated sleep homeostasis was alleviated in the midafternoon by a daytime nap, higher ACC, less number of omissions, and a stronger increase in FMT power from the no nap to nap conditions. Compared to the morning, a stronger decrease in posterior alpha power, and posterior beta power (only in the 3-back task), was observed in the no-nap condition because of circadian arousal regulation. These findings suggest that the circadian process primarily influences the storage aspect of WM supported by posterior alpha and beta activity, while sleep homeostasis has a greater impact on the execution aspect supported by FMT activity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Simei Ou
- Philosophy and Social Science Laboratory of Reading and Development in Children and Adolescents (South China Normal University), Ministry of Education, Guangzhou 510631, China; Center for Sleep Research, Center for Studies of Psychological Application, Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health & Cognitive Science, School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China
| | - Yixuan Cao
- Philosophy and Social Science Laboratory of Reading and Development in Children and Adolescents (South China Normal University), Ministry of Education, Guangzhou 510631, China; Center for Sleep Research, Center for Studies of Psychological Application, Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health & Cognitive Science, School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China
| | - Tian Xie
- Philosophy and Social Science Laboratory of Reading and Development in Children and Adolescents (South China Normal University), Ministry of Education, Guangzhou 510631, China; Center for Sleep Research, Center for Studies of Psychological Application, Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health & Cognitive Science, School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China
| | - Tianxiang Jiang
- Philosophy and Social Science Laboratory of Reading and Development in Children and Adolescents (South China Normal University), Ministry of Education, Guangzhou 510631, China; Center for Sleep Research, Center for Studies of Psychological Application, Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health & Cognitive Science, School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China
| | - Jiahui Li
- Philosophy and Social Science Laboratory of Reading and Development in Children and Adolescents (South China Normal University), Ministry of Education, Guangzhou 510631, China; Center for Sleep Research, Center for Studies of Psychological Application, Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health & Cognitive Science, School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China
| | - Wei Luo
- Philosophy and Social Science Laboratory of Reading and Development in Children and Adolescents (South China Normal University), Ministry of Education, Guangzhou 510631, China; Center for Sleep Research, Center for Studies of Psychological Application, Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health & Cognitive Science, School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China
| | - Ning Ma
- Philosophy and Social Science Laboratory of Reading and Development in Children and Adolescents (South China Normal University), Ministry of Education, Guangzhou 510631, China; Center for Sleep Research, Center for Studies of Psychological Application, Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health & Cognitive Science, School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Xu JJ, Lin GJ, Fang F, Yu J. Relationship between self-reported sleep and cognitive function: a specification curve analysis. Cogn Process 2023; 24:451-462. [PMID: 36952054 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-023-01133-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/06/2022] [Accepted: 03/13/2023] [Indexed: 03/24/2023]
Abstract
The relationship between self-reported sleep and cognitive function is complex; it is unclear whether self-reported sleep is a robust correlate of people's cognitive function. We address this gap by using a comprehensive large-scale dataset (N = 1054) coupled with a novel modeling approach, specification curve analysis (SCA), to test the association between self-reported sleep and cognitive function. The results of the SCA showed robust correlations between self-reported sleep and cognitive function, with poorer sleep associated with worse cognitive function. Furthermore, the correlations between sleep components and cognitive function were heterogeneous, with differences emerging across cognitive measures and domains. Specifically, daytime dysfunction was associated with the strongest effect on subjective cognitive function, whereas sleep duration and sleep efficiency had the strongest effect on objective cognitive function. Therefore, the relationship between self-reported sleep and cognition depends largely on what and how cognitive function is measured. Our findings guide measurement and domain selection for future research on the role of sleep in cognitive function.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jia-Jie Xu
- Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China
| | - Guo-Jun Lin
- Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China
| | - Fang Fang
- University-Town Hospital of Chongqing Medical University, Chongqing, China.
| | - Jing Yu
- Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China.
- Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Adra N, Dümmer LW, Paixao L, Tesh RA, Sun H, Ganglberger W, Westmeijer M, Da Silva Cardoso M, Kumar A, Ye E, Henry J, Cash SS, Kitchener E, Leveroni CL, Au R, Rosand J, Salinas J, Lam AD, Thomas RJ, Westover MB. Decoding information about cognitive health from the brainwaves of sleep. Sci Rep 2023; 13:11448. [PMID: 37454163 PMCID: PMC10349883 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-37128-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/30/2022] [Accepted: 06/16/2023] [Indexed: 07/18/2023] Open
Abstract
Sleep electroencephalogram (EEG) signals likely encode brain health information that may identify individuals at high risk for age-related brain diseases. Here, we evaluate the correlation of a previously proposed brain age biomarker, the "brain age index" (BAI), with cognitive test scores and use machine learning to develop and validate a series of new sleep EEG-based indices, termed "sleep cognitive indices" (SCIs), that are directly optimized to correlate with specific cognitive scores. Three overarching cognitive processes were examined: total, fluid (a measure of cognitive processes involved in reasoning-based problem solving and susceptible to aging and neuropathology), and crystallized cognition (a measure of cognitive processes involved in applying acquired knowledge toward problem-solving). We show that SCI decoded information about total cognition (Pearson's r = 0.37) and fluid cognition (Pearson's r = 0.56), while BAI correlated only with crystallized cognition (Pearson's r = - 0.25). Overall, these sleep EEG-derived biomarkers may provide accessible and clinically meaningful indicators of neurocognitive health.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Noor Adra
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), Boston, MA, USA
- Clinical Data Animation Center (CDAC), MGH, Boston, MA, USA
- Henry and Allison McCance Center for Brain Health, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), 55 Fruit Street, Boston, MA, 02114, USA
| | - Lisa W Dümmer
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), Boston, MA, USA
- Clinical Data Animation Center (CDAC), MGH, Boston, MA, USA
- University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
| | - Luis Paixao
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), Boston, MA, USA
- Clinical Data Animation Center (CDAC), MGH, Boston, MA, USA
- Department of Neurology, Washington University School of Medicine in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA
| | - Ryan A Tesh
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), Boston, MA, USA
- Clinical Data Animation Center (CDAC), MGH, Boston, MA, USA
- Henry and Allison McCance Center for Brain Health, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), 55 Fruit Street, Boston, MA, 02114, USA
| | - Haoqi Sun
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), Boston, MA, USA
- Clinical Data Animation Center (CDAC), MGH, Boston, MA, USA
- Henry and Allison McCance Center for Brain Health, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), 55 Fruit Street, Boston, MA, 02114, USA
| | - Wolfgang Ganglberger
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), Boston, MA, USA
- Clinical Data Animation Center (CDAC), MGH, Boston, MA, USA
- Sleep and Health Zurich, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Mike Westmeijer
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), Boston, MA, USA
- Clinical Data Animation Center (CDAC), MGH, Boston, MA, USA
- Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Madalena Da Silva Cardoso
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), Boston, MA, USA
- Clinical Data Animation Center (CDAC), MGH, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Anagha Kumar
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), Boston, MA, USA
- Clinical Data Animation Center (CDAC), MGH, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Elissa Ye
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), Boston, MA, USA
- Clinical Data Animation Center (CDAC), MGH, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Jonathan Henry
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), Boston, MA, USA
- Clinical Data Animation Center (CDAC), MGH, Boston, MA, USA
- Henry and Allison McCance Center for Brain Health, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), 55 Fruit Street, Boston, MA, 02114, USA
| | - Sydney S Cash
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), Boston, MA, USA
- Clinical Data Animation Center (CDAC), MGH, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Erin Kitchener
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), Boston, MA, USA
- Clinical Data Animation Center (CDAC), MGH, Boston, MA, USA
- Henry and Allison McCance Center for Brain Health, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), 55 Fruit Street, Boston, MA, 02114, USA
| | | | - Rhoda Au
- Boston University Chobanian and Avedisian School of Medicine, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Jonathan Rosand
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), Boston, MA, USA
- Henry and Allison McCance Center for Brain Health, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), 55 Fruit Street, Boston, MA, 02114, USA
| | - Joel Salinas
- New York University Grossman School of Medicine, New York, NY, USA
| | - Alice D Lam
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), Boston, MA, USA
- Clinical Data Animation Center (CDAC), MGH, Boston, MA, USA
- Henry and Allison McCance Center for Brain Health, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), 55 Fruit Street, Boston, MA, 02114, USA
| | - Robert J Thomas
- Division of Pulmonary, Critical Care, and Sleep, Department of Medicine, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center (BIDMC), Boston, MA, USA
| | - M Brandon Westover
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), Boston, MA, USA.
- Clinical Data Animation Center (CDAC), MGH, Boston, MA, USA.
- Henry and Allison McCance Center for Brain Health, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), 55 Fruit Street, Boston, MA, 02114, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Wofford N, Ceballos N, Elkins G, Westerberg CE. A brief nap during an acute stressor improves negative affect. J Sleep Res 2022; 31:e13701. [PMID: 35851731 PMCID: PMC9786543 DOI: 10.1111/jsr.13701] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/30/2022] [Revised: 06/27/2022] [Accepted: 06/29/2022] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
Overnight sleep can reduce perceived stress, and improve associated cognitive disruptions and negative affect after an acute stressor. Whether a brief nap can also bestow these benefits in a non-sleep-restricted population is currently unknown. In this study that used a between-subjects design, stress was triggered by administering a modified Trier Social Stress Test to two groups of participants (nap [n = 29], wake [n = 41]). All participants were instructed they would give a speech during the study but the topic would be withheld until later, and then completed a math task. After a 40-min break in which participants watched a neutral video or took a nap monitored with electroencephalography, stress was reinforced by presenting the speech topics and giving participants a 10-min preparation period. Next, instead of giving a speech, the study ended and participants were debriefed. Negative affect, perceived stress and working memory were measured at multiple time points before and after the break. Both groups showed lower perceived stress and improved working memory after the break than before, but a nap did not confer additional benefits for perceived stress or working memory beyond taking a break. However, the nap group exhibited lower negative affect after the break than the wake group, and only the nap group showed a reduction in negative affect compared with initial negative affect levels. These results indicate a nap can improve negative emotions accompanying a stressor to a greater extent than taking a break, and suggest that brief naps may be a useful way to improve mood while experiencing an acute stressor.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nathan Wofford
- Department of PsychologyTexas State UniversitySan MarcosTexasUSA,Department of Psychology and NeuroscienceBaylor UniversityWacoTexasUSA
| | - Natalie Ceballos
- Department of PsychologyTexas State UniversitySan MarcosTexasUSA
| | - Gary Elkins
- Department of Psychology and NeuroscienceBaylor UniversityWacoTexasUSA
| | | |
Collapse
|
7
|
Chen PC, Zhang J, Thayer JF, Mednick SC. Understanding the roles of central and autonomic activity during sleep in the improvement of working memory and episodic memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2022; 119:e2123417119. [PMID: 36279428 PMCID: PMC9636982 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2123417119] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
The last decade has seen significant progress in identifying sleep mechanisms that support cognition. Most of these studies focus on the link between electrophysiological events of the central nervous system during sleep and improvements in different cognitive domains, while the dynamic shifts of the autonomic nervous system across sleep have been largely overlooked. Recent studies, however, have identified significant contributions of autonomic inputs during sleep to cognition. Yet, there remain considerable gaps in understanding how central and autonomic systems work together during sleep to facilitate cognitive improvement. In this article we examine the evidence for the independent and interactive roles of central and autonomic activities during sleep and wake in cognitive processing. We specifically focus on the prefrontal-subcortical structures supporting working memory and mechanisms underlying the formation of hippocampal-dependent episodic memory. Our Slow Oscillation Switch Model identifies separate and competing underlying mechanisms supporting the two memory domains at the synaptic, systems, and behavioral levels. We propose that sleep is a competitive arena in which both memory domains vie for limited resources, experimentally demonstrated when boosting one system leads to a functional trade-off in electrophysiological and behavioral outcomes. As these findings inevitably lead to further questions, we suggest areas of future research to better understand how the brain and body interact to support a wide range of cognitive domains during a single sleep episode.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pin-Chun Chen
- Department of Neurosurgery, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104
| | - Jing Zhang
- Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California, Irvine, CA 92697
| | - Julian F. Thayer
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of California, Irvine, CA 92697
| | - Sara C. Mednick
- Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California, Irvine, CA 92697
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Leong RLF, Lo JC, Chee MWL. Systematic review and meta-analyses on the effects of afternoon napping on cognition. Sleep Med Rev 2022; 65:101666. [PMID: 36041284 DOI: 10.1016/j.smrv.2022.101666] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/02/2021] [Revised: 06/09/2022] [Accepted: 06/21/2022] [Indexed: 10/15/2022]
Abstract
Naps are increasingly considered a means to boost cognitive performance. We quantified the cognitive effects of napping in 60 samples from 54 studies. 52 samples evaluated memory. We first evaluated effect sizes for all tests together, before separately assessing their effects on memory, vigilance, speed of processing and executive function. We next examined whether nap effects were moderated by study features of age, nap length, nap start time, habituality and prior sleep restriction. Naps showed significant benefits for the total aggregate of cognitive tests (Cohen's d = 0.379, CI95 = 0.296-0.462). Significant domain specific effects were present for declarative (Cohen's d = 0.376, CI95 = 0.269-0.482) and procedural memory (Cohen's d = 0.494, CI95 = 0.301-0.686), vigilance (Cohen's d = 0.610, CI95 = 0.291-0.929) and speed of processing (Cohen's d = 0.211, CI95 = 0.052-0.369). There were no significant moderation effects of any of the study features. Nap effects were of comparable magnitude across subgroups of each of the 5 moderators (Q values = 0.009 to 8.572, p values > 0.116). Afternoon naps have a small to medium benefit over multiple cognitive tests. These effects transcend age, nap duration and tentatively, habituality and prior nocturnal sleep.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ruth L F Leong
- Centre for Sleep and Cognition, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, Singapore.
| | - June C Lo
- Centre for Sleep and Cognition, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, Singapore
| | - Michael W L Chee
- Centre for Sleep and Cognition, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, Singapore.
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Ru T, Qian L, Chen Q, Sun H, Zhou G. Effects of an afternoon nap on sustained attention and working memory: The role of physiological arousal and sleep variables. Int J Psychophysiol 2022; 179:21-29. [PMID: 35753563 DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2022.06.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/07/2021] [Revised: 06/10/2022] [Accepted: 06/20/2022] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Taking a short midday nap has been associated with higher alertness and better cognitive task performance. Yet, the mechanisms associated with nap-dependent performance enhancement are unclear. The current study was conducted to explore the impact of physiological arousal during cognitive task and sleep architecture during a pre-task nap on post-nap behavioral outcomes. A within-subjects design (N = 18) was employed, in which participants either took a nap or remained awake for 40 min during the post-lunch period. The psychomotor vigilance test (PVT) and n-back task were administered to assess sustained attention and working memory, respectively, with each task including one block of easy trials and one block of difficult trials. Results showed that a short midday nap improved sustained attention but not working memory. In addition, a midday nap induced lower physiological arousal during the performance on both cognitive tasks, with relatively higher delta and lower beta activity. The relative power of theta and alpha were positively correlated with performance on the easy PVT, whereas the alpha power was negatively correlated with performance on the difficult PVT, and the theta power was negatively correlated with reaction speed in the n-back task regardless of the task difficulty. Meanwhile, the shorter total sleep time and longer time of wake after sleep onset were associated with the faster overall reaction speed in PVT easy trials. These findings suggested that both changes in physiological arousal and sleep variables might account for changes in task performance after a short midday nap.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Taotao Ru
- National Center for International Research on Green Optoelectronics, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510006, China; Lab of Light and Physio-psychological Health, School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China.
| | - Liu Qian
- National Center for International Research on Green Optoelectronics, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510006, China; Lab of Light and Physio-psychological Health, School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China
| | - Qingwei Chen
- National Center for International Research on Green Optoelectronics, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510006, China; Lab of Light and Physio-psychological Health, School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China
| | - Hailing Sun
- Lab of Light and Physio-psychological Health, School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China; Guangdong Provincial Key Laboratory of Optical Information Materials and Technology & Institute of Electronic Paper Displays, South China Academy of Advanced Optoelectronics, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510006, China
| | - Guofu Zhou
- Lab of Light and Physio-psychological Health, School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China; Guangdong Provincial Key Laboratory of Optical Information Materials and Technology & Institute of Electronic Paper Displays, South China Academy of Advanced Optoelectronics, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510006, China
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Au J, Smith-Peirce RN, Carbone E, Moon A, Evans M, Jonides J, Jaeggi SM. Effects of Multisession Prefrontal Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation on Long-term Memory and Working Memory in Older Adults. J Cogn Neurosci 2022; 34:1015-1037. [PMID: 35195728 PMCID: PMC9836784 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_01839] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/14/2023]
Abstract
Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) is a noninvasive form of electrical brain stimulation popularly used to augment the effects of working memory (WM) training. Although success has been mixed, some studies report enhancements in WM performance persisting days, weeks, or even months that are actually more reminiscent of consolidation effects typically observed in the long-term memory (LTM) domain, rather than WM improvements per se. Although tDCS has been often reported to enhance both WM and LTM, these effects have never been directly compared within the same study. However, given their considerable neural and behavioral overlap, this is a timely comparison to make. This study reports results from a multisession intervention in older adults comparing active and sham tDCS over the left dorsolateral pFC during training on both an n-back WM task and a word learning LTM task. We found strong and robust effects on LTM, but mixed effects on WM that only emerged for those with lower baseline ability. Importantly, mediation analyses showed an indirect effect of tDCS on WM that was mediated by improvements in consolidation. We conclude that tDCS over the left dorsolateral pFC can be used as an effective intervention to foster long-term learning and memory consolidation in aging, which can manifest in performance improvements across multiple memory domains.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jacky Au
- School of Education, University of California, Irvine, Irvine CA, 92697, USA
| | | | - Elena Carbone
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, 35131, Italy
| | - Austin Moon
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside, Riverside CA, 92521, USA
| | - Michelle Evans
- Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor MI, 48109, USA
| | - John Jonides
- Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor MI, 48109, USA
| | - Susanne M. Jaeggi
- School of Education, University of California, Irvine, Irvine CA, 92697, USA
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Mekky JF, Yousof S, Elsayed I, Elsemelawy R, Mahmoud H, Elweshahi H. Assessment of the cognitive function in adult Egyptian patients with obstructive sleep apnea using the Montreal Cognitive Assessment: a retrospective large-scale study. J Clin Sleep Med 2022; 18:721-729. [PMID: 34605391 PMCID: PMC8883109 DOI: 10.5664/jcsm.9704] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022]
Abstract
STUDY OBJECTIVES Sleep apnea is a chronic disorder associated with multiple recognized comorbidities. Only a few studies focus on evaluating the cognitive profile in patients diagnosed with sleep apnea. The aim of the study was to assess the cognitive functions in this population using the Montreal Cognitive Assessment. METHODS The study cohort was 1,445 adult patients who were referred for overnight polysomnography, 764 cases and 681 healthy controls. All participants' clinical data and comorbidities were taken, and they all performed overnight polysomnography and Montreal Cognitive Assessment. RESULTS A significantly higher proportion (57.5%) of sleep apnea groups were males; 15.7% were illiterate compared to the non-sleep apnea group. Hypertension and diabetes mellitus were significantly more prevalent among studied patients with sleep apnea, and the mean total score for the Montreal Cognitive Assessment scale was significantly lower among those with sleep apnea at P < .001. Those with no sleep apnea showed a significantly higher function in all attributes compared to patients with sleep apnea-namely, language, orientation, abstraction, naming, attention, and recall (P < .05). CONCLUSIONS Logistic regression analysis was conducted to investigate predictors for occurrence of cognitive impairment (Montreal Cognitive Assessment score < 26) among the studied sample (n = 1,445). The overall model was significant at P < .001. Variables that showed significance in univariate analysis were entered in the model. Significant predictors for cognitive impairment were being male, older age, diabetic, hypertensive, and with a lower level of education and having sleep apnea. CITATION Mekky JF, Yousof S, Elsayed I, Elsemelawy R, Mahmoud H, Elweshahi H. Assessment of the cognitive functions in adult Egyptian patients with obstructive sleep apnea using the Montreal Cognitive Assessment: a retrospective large-scale study. J Clin Sleep Med. 2022;18(3):721-729.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jaidaa F. Mekky
- Department of Neuropsychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Alexandria University, Alexandria, Egypt,Address correspondence to: Jaidaa Farouk Mekky, MD, Department of Neuropsychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Alexandria University, 18, Mina Street, Kafr Abdou 21311 Alexandria, Egypt; Tel: 01065606664;
| | - Shimaa Yousof
- Department of Medical Physiology, Faculty of Medicine, Suez Canal University, Suez Canal, Egypt
| | - Inas Elsayed
- Department of Pulmonology, Faculty of Medicine, Alexandria University, Alexandria, Egypt
| | | | | | - Heba Elweshahi
- Department of Community Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, Alexandria University, Alexandria, Egypt
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Whitehurst LN, Subramoniam A, Krystal A, Prather AA. Links between the brain and body during sleep: implications for memory processing. Trends Neurosci 2022; 45:212-223. [PMID: 35074220 DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2021.12.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/09/2021] [Revised: 11/30/2021] [Accepted: 12/21/2021] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
Sleep is intimately related to memory processes. The established view is that the transformation of experiences into long-term memories is linked to sleep-related CNS function. However, there is increasing evidence that the autonomic nervous system (ANS), long recognized to modulate cognition during waking, can impact memory processing during sleep. Here, we review human research that examines the role of autonomic activity and sleep in memory formation. We argue that autonomic activity during sleep may set the stage for the CNS dynamics associated with sleep and memory stability and integration. Further, we consider how the link between ANS activity and polysomnographic markers of sleep may help elucidate both healthy and pathological cognitive aging in humans.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Andrew Krystal
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Aric A Prather
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Du J, Wang Y, Xu S, Huang Y, Zhang R, Xiao L, Xu J, Wang H, Su T, Tang Y. Structural Model of Napping Motivation Among Chinese College Students Based on Self-Rating: Evidence from an Exploratory Factor Analysis. Nat Sci Sleep 2022; 14:843-853. [PMID: 35529049 PMCID: PMC9075905 DOI: 10.2147/nss.s349013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/16/2021] [Accepted: 04/13/2022] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
PURPOSE Previous epidemiological and psychological studies have assessed the effects of napping on individual performance and health. However, these studies did not distinguish between spontaneous and passive napping due to sleep disorders. Therefore, this study aimed to explore the potential motivation for napping among Chinese college students and to assess the relationship between different nap motivations and sleep. PATIENTS AND METHODS This cross-sectional study was conducted at a university in Shanghai in March 2021 using convenience sampling. A total of 564 Chinese college students with self-reported napping habits participated in this study. Exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was employed to analyze 34 self-rated motivations for napping to derive a potential structural model of napping motivation. Correlation and multiple linear regression analyses were performed to determine the influence of nap motivation on sleep behavior. RESULTS Chinese college students mainly take naps to reduce fatigue, usually in the form of frequent and long naps (6.25 ± 1.24 days; 64.62 ± 23.70 min). A structural model of nap motivation named the I-DREAM model was proposed, consisting of six factors: restorative, induced, mindful, appetitive, dysregulative, and exercise naps. There were also sex differences in nap motivation, as women preferred restorative naps and appetitive naps, while men preferred exercise naps (p < 0.01, p < 0.05). Furthermore, there was a significant correlation between nap motivation on nap duration, frequency and nighttime sleep quality (R2 = 2.70-18.9%). CONCLUSION The proposed I-DREAM model implies that there are population differences in napping motivation. Different motivations also have different relationships with napping patterns and nighttime sleep. In understanding the relationship between naps and health status, nap motivation may be an extremely important influencing factor.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jing Du
- Department of Medical Psychology, Naval Medical University, Shanghai, People's Republic of China
| | - Yajing Wang
- Department of Medical Psychology, Naval Medical University, Shanghai, People's Republic of China
| | - Shuyu Xu
- Department of Medical Psychology, Naval Medical University, Shanghai, People's Republic of China
| | - Yujia Huang
- Department of Medical Psychology, Naval Medical University, Shanghai, People's Republic of China
| | - Ruike Zhang
- Department of Medical Psychology, Naval Medical University, Shanghai, People's Republic of China
| | - Lei Xiao
- Department of Medical Psychology, Naval Medical University, Shanghai, People's Republic of China
| | - Jingzhou Xu
- Department of Medical Psychology, Naval Medical University, Shanghai, People's Republic of China
| | - Hao Wang
- Department of Medical Psychology, Naval Medical University, Shanghai, People's Republic of China
| | - Tong Su
- Department of Medical Psychology, Naval Medical University, Shanghai, People's Republic of China
| | - Yunxiang Tang
- Department of Medical Psychology, Naval Medical University, Shanghai, People's Republic of China
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Competitive dynamics underlie cognitive improvements during sleep. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2021; 118:2109339118. [PMID: 34903651 PMCID: PMC8713802 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2109339118] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 11/02/2021] [Indexed: 12/02/2022] Open
Abstract
Sleep facilitates both long-term episodic memory consolidation and short-term working memory functioning. However, the mechanism by which the sleeping brain performs both complex feats and which sleep features are associated with these processes remain unclear. Using a pharmacological approach, we demonstrate that long-term and working memory are served by distinct offline neural mechanisms and that these mechanisms are mutually antagonistic. We propose a sleep switch model in which the brain toggles between the two memory processes via a complex interaction at the synaptic, systems, and mechanistic level with implications for research on cognitive disturbances observed in neurodegenerative disorders such as Alzheimer’s and Parkinson's disease, both of which involve the decline of sleep. We provide evidence that human sleep is a competitive arena in which cognitive domains vie for limited resources. Using pharmacology and effective connectivity analysis, we demonstrate that long-term memory and working memory are served by distinct offline neural mechanisms that are mutually antagonistic. Specifically, we administered zolpidem to increase central sigma activity and demonstrated targeted suppression of autonomic vagal activity. With effective connectivity, we determined the central activity has greater causal influence over autonomic activity, and the magnitude of this influence during sleep produced a behavioral trade-off between offline long-term and working memory processing. These findings suggest a sleep switch mechanism that toggles between central sigma-dependent long-term memory and autonomic vagal-dependent working memory processing.
Collapse
|
15
|
Au J, Katz B, Moon A, Talati S, Abagis TR, Jonides J, Jaeggi SM. Post-training stimulation of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex impairs working memory training performance. J Neurosci Res 2021; 99:2351-2363. [PMID: 33438297 PMCID: PMC8273206 DOI: 10.1002/jnr.24784] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/03/2020] [Accepted: 12/23/2020] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Research investigating transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) to enhance cognitive training augments both our understanding of its long-term effects on cognitive plasticity as well as potential applications to strengthen cognitive interventions. Previous work has demonstrated enhancement of working memory training while applying concurrent tDCS to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). However, the optimal stimulation parameters are still unknown. For example, the timing of tDCS delivery has been shown to be an influential variable that can interact with task learning. In the present study, we used tDCS to target the right DLPFC while participants trained on a visuospatial working memory task. We sought to compare the relative efficacy of online stimulation delivered during training to offline stimulation delivered either immediately before or afterwards. We were unable to replicate previously demonstrated benefits of online stimulation; however, we did find evidence that offline stimulation delivered after training can actually be detrimental to training performance relative to sham. We interpret our results in light of evidence suggesting a role of the right DLPFC in promoting memory interference, and conclude that while tDCS may be a promising tool to influence the results of cognitive training, more research and an abundance of caution are needed before fully endorsing its use for cognitive enhancement. This work suggests that effects can vary substantially in magnitude and direction between studies, and may be heavily dependent on a variety of intervention protocol parameters such as the timing and location of stimulation delivery, about which our understanding is still nascent.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jacky Au
- School of Education, University of California, Irvine, Irvine, CA, 92697, USA
| | - Benjamin Katz
- Department of Human Development and Family Science, Virginia Tech, Blacksburg, VA, 24061, USA
| | - Austin Moon
- School of Education, University of California, Irvine, Irvine, CA, 92697, USA
| | - Sheebani Talati
- Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
| | - Tessa R. Abagis
- Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
| | - John Jonides
- Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
| | - Susanne M. Jaeggi
- School of Education, University of California, Irvine, Irvine, CA, 92697, USA
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Exploring Individual Differences as Predictors of Performance Change During Dual-N-Back Training. JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE ENHANCEMENT 2021; 5:480-498. [DOI: 10.1007/s41465-021-00216-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
17
|
Okuda M, Noda A, Mabuchi S, Iwamoto K, Banno M, Miyata S, Yasuma F, Ozaki N. Sleep fragmentation and working memory in healthy adults. Sleep Sci 2021; 14:111-117. [PMID: 35082979 PMCID: PMC8764943 DOI: 10.5935/1984-0063.20200088] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/03/2020] [Accepted: 12/08/2020] [Indexed: 12/05/2022] Open
Abstract
Introduction Sleep is essential for performing cognitive function in humans. We have hypothesized that sleep fragmentation compared to sleep efficiency may have a negative impact on the working memory. Material and Methods Twenty-eight healthy adults (18 males and 10 females; mean age 27.8±15.5 years) were enrolled in this study. We measured the total sleep time (TST), sleep efficiency, %stage wakefulness (W), %stage rapid eye movement (REM), %stage N1, %stage N2, %stage N3, wake after sleep onset (WASO), and arousal index using polysomnography. Working memory, executive function, and sustained attention of three domains of cognitive function were evaluated with the number of back task (N-back task), Wisconsin card sorting test (WCST), and continuous performance test-identical pairs (CPT-IP), respectively. Results The percentage of correct answers on the 2-back task was significantly correlated with %stage REM, %stage N1, and %stage N2 (%stage REM: r=0.505, p=0.006; %stage N1: r=-0.637, p<0.001; %stage N2: r=0.670, p<0.001), and multiple regression analysis including the stepwise forward selection method revealed that %stage N2 was the most significant factor (%stage N2: β=0.670, p<0.001). The percentage of correct answers on the 2-back task was also significantly correlated with TST, sleep efficiency, WASO, and arousal index (TST: r=0.492, p=0.008; sleep efficiency: r=0.622, p<0.001; WASO: r=-0.721, p<0.001; arousal index: r=-0.656, p<0.001), and WASO was the significant factor (β=-2.086, p=0.007). The WCST category achievement and CPT-IP d-prime score were correlated with none of the sleep variables. Conclusion Increased WASO and a decrease in %stage N2 were associated with worse working memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Masato Okuda
- Chubu University Graduate School of Life and Health Sciences, Department of Biomedical Sciences - Kasugai - Aichi - Japan
| | - Akiko Noda
- Chubu University Graduate School of Life and Health Sciences, Department of Biomedical Sciences - Kasugai - Aichi - Japan
| | - Sho Mabuchi
- Chubu University Collage of Life and Health Sciences, Department of Biomedical Sciences -Kasugai - Aichi- Japan
| | - Kunihiro Iwamoto
- Nagoya University Graduate School of Medicine, Department of Psychiatry - Nagoya - Aichi -Japan
| | - Masahiro Banno
- Nagoya University Graduate School of Medicine, Department of Psychiatry - Nagoya - Aichi -Japan.,Seichiryo Hospital, Department of Psychiatry - Nagoya - Aichi - Japan
| | - Seiko Miyata
- Nagoya University Graduate School of Medicine, Department of Psychiatry - Nagoya - Aichi -Japan
| | - Fumihiko Yasuma
- National Hospital Organization Suzuka Hospital, Department of Internal Medicine - Suzuka - Mie -Japan
| | - Norio Ozaki
- Nagoya University Graduate School of Medicine, Department of Psychiatry - Nagoya - Aichi -Japan
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Mark Lawrence W, Esther Yuet Ying L, Yeuk Ching L, Benjamin R, Chia-Huei T, Tatia Mei Chun L, Yun Kwok W. The protective effect of daytime sleep on planning and risk-related decision-making in emerging adults. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2020; 15:1228-1237. [PMID: 33064803 PMCID: PMC7745149 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsaa140] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/25/2019] [Revised: 08/20/2020] [Accepted: 10/13/2020] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
We assessed the effect of a daytime sleep opportunity on planning and risk-related decision-making in emerging adults using multiple neurobehavioral assessments. A total of 136 healthy emerging adults (20.0 ± 1.5 years), 65% female, performed the Risky-Gains Task and the Tower of London test twice. Between these assessments, they were randomized to either have a sleep opportunity monitored by polysomnography (Sleep group, n = 101) or to stay awake (Wake group, n = 35). During Test 2, in comparison to the Sleep group, the Wake group showed increased sleepiness, worse planning ability and more decrease in reaction times when selecting risky choices. Changes in Tower of London test steps used and Risky-Gains Task response time correlated with the number of central and frontal fast sleep spindles, respectively. These results indicate that among emerging adults who commonly have poor sleep patterns, a daytime sleep opportunity was related to better planning ability, better psychomotor vigilance and stable response speeds in risk-related decision-making. Changes in planning and risk-related decision-making correlated with the number of sleep spindles during the nap, supporting a specific role for sleep in modulating planning and potentially other higher-order cognitive functions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Lau Esther Yuet Ying
- Department of Psychology, The Education University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong.,Centre for Psychosocial Health, The Education University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong.,Centre for Religious and Spirituality Education, The Education University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
| | - Lam Yeuk Ching
- Department of Psychology, The Education University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong.,Centre for Psychosocial Health, The Education University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
| | - Rusak Benjamin
- Department of Psychiatry, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada.,Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada
| | - Tseng Chia-Huei
- Research Institute of Electrical Communication, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan
| | | | - Wing Yun Kwok
- Sleep Assessment Unit, Department of Psychiatry, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Chen PC, Whitehurst LN, Naji M, Mednick SC. Autonomic Activity during a Daytime Nap Facilitates Working Memory Improvement. J Cogn Neurosci 2020; 32:1963-1974. [PMID: 32530384 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_01588] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/21/2022]
Abstract
Recent investigations have implicated the parasympathetic branch of the autonomic nervous system in higher-order executive functions. These actions are purported to occur through autonomic nervous system's modulation of the pFC, with parasympathetic activity during wake associated with working memory (WM) ability. Compared with wake, sleep is a period with substantially greater parasympathetic tone. Recent work has reported that sleep may also contribute to improvement in WM. Here, we examined the role of cardiac parasympathetic activity during sleep on WM improvement in healthy young adults. Participants were tested in an operation span task in the morning and evening, and during the intertest period, participants experienced either a nap or wake. We measured high-frequency heart rate variability as an index of cardiac, parasympathetic activity during both wake and sleep. Participants showed the expected boost in parasympathetic activity during nap, compared with wake. Furthermore, parasympathetic activity during sleep, but not wake, was significantly correlated with WM improvement. Together, these results indicate that the natural boost in parasympathetic activity during sleep may benefit gains in prefrontal executive function in young adults. We present a conceptual model illustrating the interaction between sleep, autonomic activity, and prefrontal brain function and highlight open research questions that will facilitate understanding of the factors that contribute to executive abilities in young adults as well as in cognitive aging.
Collapse
|
20
|
Chen PC, Whitehurst LN, Naji M, Mednick SC. Autonomic/central coupling benefits working memory in healthy young adults. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2020; 173:107267. [PMID: 32535198 DOI: 10.1016/j.nlm.2020.107267] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/15/2020] [Revised: 05/13/2020] [Accepted: 06/08/2020] [Indexed: 02/01/2023]
Abstract
Working memory (WM) is an executive function that can improve with training. However, the precise mechanism for this improvement is not known. Studies have shown greater WM gains after a period of sleep than a similar period of wake, and correlations between WM improvement and slow wave activity (SWA; 0.5-1 Hz) during slow wave sleep (SWS). A different body of literature has suggested an important role for autonomic activity during wake for WM. A recent study from our group reported that the temporal coupling of Autonomic/CentralEvents (ACEs) during sleep was associated with memory consolidation. We found that heart rate bursts (HR bursts) during non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep are accompanied by increases in SWA and sigma (12-15 Hz) power, as well as increases in the high-frequency (HF) component of the RR interval, reflecting vagal rebound. In addition, ACEs predict long-term, episodic memory improvement. Building on these previous results, we examined whether ACEs also contribute to gains in WM. We tested 104 young adults in an operation span task (OSPAN) in the morning and evening, with either a nap (n = 53; with electroencephalography (EEG) and electrocardiography (ECG)) or wake (n = 51) between testing sessions. We identified HR bursts in the ECG and replicated the increases in SWA and sigma prior to peak of the HR burst, as well as vagal rebound after the peak. Furthermore, we showed sleep-dependent WM improvement, which was predicted by ACE activity. Using regression analyses, we discovered that significantly more variance in WM improvement could be explained with ACE variables than with overall sleep activity not time-locked with ECG. These results provide the first evidence that coordinated autonomic and central events play a significant role in sleep-related WM improvement and implicate the potential of autonomic interventions during sleep for cognitive enhancement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pin-Chun Chen
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, Irvine USA
| | | | - Mohsen Naji
- Department of Medicine, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA
| | - Sara C Mednick
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, Irvine USA.
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Kordestani-Moghadam P, Nasehi M, Khodagholi F, Vaseghi S, Zarrindast MR, Khani M. The fluctuations of metabotropic glutamate receptor subtype 5 (mGluR5) in the amygdala in fear conditioning model of male Wistar rats following sleep deprivation, reverse circadian and napping. Brain Res 2020; 1734:146739. [PMID: 32087111 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2020.146739] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/23/2019] [Revised: 02/15/2020] [Accepted: 02/18/2020] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
Abstract
Sleep is involved in metabolic system, mental health and cognitive functions. Evidence shows that sleep deprivation (SD) negatively affects mental health and impairs cognitive functions, including learning and memory. Furthermore, the metabotropic glutamate receptor subtype 5 (mGluR5) is a metabolic biomarker, which is affected by various conditions, including stress, sleep deprivation, and cognitive and psychiatric disorders. In this research, we investigated the effect of SD and reverse circadian (RC), and two models of napping (continuous and non-continuous) combined with SD or RC on fear-conditioning memory, anxiety-like behavior and mGluR5 fluctuations in the amygdala. 64 male Wistar rats were used in this study. The water box apparatus was used to induce SD/RC for 48 h, and fear-conditioning memory apparatus was used to assess fear memory. The results showed, fear-conditioning memory was impaired following SD and RC, especially in contextual stage. However, anxiety-like behavior was increased. Furthermore, mGluR5 was increased in the left amygdala more than the right amygdala. Additionally, continuous napping significantly improved fear-conditioning memory, especially freezing behavior. In conclusion, following SD and RC, fear-conditioning memory in contextual stage is more vulnerable than in auditory stage. Furthermore, increase in anxiety-like behavior is related to increase in the activity of left amygdala and mGluR5 receptors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Mohammad Nasehi
- Cognitive and Neuroscience Research Center (CNRC), Amir-Almomenin Hospital, Tehran Medical Sciences, Islamic Azad University, Tehran, Iran.
| | - Fariba Khodagholi
- Neuroscience Research Center, Shahid Beheshti University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| | - Salar Vaseghi
- Cognitive and Neuroscience Research Center (CNRC), Amir-Almomenin Hospital, Tehran Medical Sciences, Islamic Azad University, Tehran, Iran
| | - Mohammad-Reza Zarrindast
- Department of Pharmacology, School of Medicine, Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran; Department of Neuroendocrinology, Endocrinology and Metabolism Research Institute, Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran; Institute for Cognitive Science Studies (ICSS), Tehran, Iran
| | - Mojgan Khani
- Cognitive and Neuroscience Research Center (CNRC), Amir-Almomenin Hospital, Tehran Medical Sciences, Islamic Azad University, Tehran, Iran
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Lau EYY, Lau KNT, Chan CS, Tseng CH, Lam YC, Tse DMS, Cheng WY, Chung KF, Wing YK. Effects of REM sleep during a daytime nap on emotional perception in individuals with and without depression. J Affect Disord 2020; 260:687-694. [PMID: 31550615 DOI: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.09.058] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/15/2019] [Revised: 09/03/2019] [Accepted: 09/11/2019] [Indexed: 02/01/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND It has been theoretically proposed that alteration in sleep physiology may contribute to the development of biased emotional processing featured in depression. The current study investigated the role of sleep and especially REM in modulating perception of emotional faces in depressed versus non-depressed individuals using a napping paradigm. METHODS Forty-six individuals with major depressive disorder and 66 age- and education-matched healthy controls completed an emotional face perception task before and after random assignment to one of the three intention-to-treat (ITT) conditions, namely 30-min-nap, 90-min-nap and wake. To delineate the effects of REM, as-treated (30-min-nap, 90-min-REM-nap, 90-min-noREM-nap and wake) analyses were also conducted. RESULTS Repeated measures multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) showed a significant Time *Group *Condition interaction on angry faces for both analyses of ITT (p = .017) and AT (p = .027). Pairwise comparison with Bonferroni corrections revealed a significant increase in the intensity rating of angry faces only after 90-min-REM-nap in the depressed group. Correlational analyses convergingly showed that the increase of intensity rating of angry faces was associated with the proportion of REM sleep in the depressed group, p = .035. LIMITATIONS The observed effect of REM sleep during daytime napping may not represent the effect of nighttime REM sleep in depression. CONCLUSIONS We provide the first evidence of the association of daytime sleep, particularly REM sleep, with a more negative perception of angry faces exclusively in individuals with depression. The differentiated impact of sleep observed may contribute to the development of altered emotional processing in depression.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Esther Yuet Ying Lau
- Department of Psychology, The Education University of Hong Kong, 10 Lo Ping Road, Tai Po, New Territories, Hong Kong; Centre for Psychosocial Health, The Education University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong; Department of Psychiatry, Queen Mary Hospital, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong.
| | - Kristy Nga Ting Lau
- Clinical Psychological Services, Hong Kong Children & Youth Services, Hong Kong
| | - Christian S Chan
- Department of Psychology, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
| | - Chia-Huei Tseng
- Research Institute of Electrical Communication, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan
| | - Yeuk Ching Lam
- Department of Psychology, The Education University of Hong Kong, 10 Lo Ping Road, Tai Po, New Territories, Hong Kong; Centre for Psychosocial Health, The Education University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
| | - Desiree Man-Sik Tse
- Department of Psychology, The Education University of Hong Kong, 10 Lo Ping Road, Tai Po, New Territories, Hong Kong; Centre for Psychosocial Health, The Education University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
| | - Wing Yee Cheng
- Department of Psychology, The Education University of Hong Kong, 10 Lo Ping Road, Tai Po, New Territories, Hong Kong; Centre for Psychosocial Health, The Education University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
| | - Ka-Fai Chung
- Department of Psychiatry, Queen Mary Hospital, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
| | - Yun Kwok Wing
- Sleep Assessment Unit, Department of Psychiatry, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Lau EYY, Wong ML, Rusak B, Lam YC, Wing YK, Tseng CH, Lee TMC. The coupling of short sleep duration and high sleep need predicts riskier decision making. Psychol Health 2019; 34:1196-1213. [PMID: 30966760 DOI: 10.1080/08870446.2019.1594807] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/20/2023]
Abstract
Objective: To examine how risk-related decision making might be associated with habitual sleep variables, including sleep variability, sleep duration and perceived sleep need in young adults cross-sectionally and longitudinally. Design: 166 participants completed a 7-day protocol with sleep and risk-related decision-making measures at baseline (T1) and 12 months later (T2). Results: Habitual short sleep duration (averaging < 6 h nightly) was identified in 11.0% in our sample. After controlling for baseline demographic factors and risk-taking measures, self-reported sleep need at T1 interacted with habitual short sleep in predicting risk taking at follow-up (F8,139=9.575, adjusted R2=.431, p<.001). T1 greater perceived sleep need predicted more risk taking among short sleepers, but decreased risk taking among normal sleepers at T2. Variable sleep timing was cross-sectionally correlated with making more Risky choices at baseline and fewer Safe choices after loss at follow up. Conclusions: Young adults with variable sleep timing and those with short sleep duration coupled with high perceived sleep need were more likely to take risks. The moderating effects of perceived sleep need suggest that individual differences may alter the impact of sleep loss and hence should be measured and accounted for in future studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Esther Yuet Ying Lau
- Department of Psychology, The Education University of Hong Kong , Tai Po , Hong Kong.,Centre for Religious and Spirituality Education, The Education University of Hong Kong , Tai Po , Hong Kong
| | - Mark Lawrence Wong
- Department of Clinical Psychology, Pamela Youde Nethersole Eastern Hospital , Chai Wan , Hong Kong
| | - Benjamin Rusak
- Department of Psychiatry, Dalhousie University , Halifax, Nova Scotia , Canada.,Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Dalhousie University , Halifax, Nova Scotia , Canada
| | - Yeuk Ching Lam
- Department of Psychology, The Education University of Hong Kong , Tai Po , Hong Kong.,Centre for Psychosocial Health, The Education University of Hong Kong , Tai Po , Hong Kong
| | - Yun Kwok Wing
- Department of Psychiatry, The Chinese University of Hong Kong , Shatin , Hong Kong
| | - Chia-Huei Tseng
- Research Institute of Electrical Communication, Tohoku University , Sendai , Japan
| | - Tatia Mei Chun Lee
- Department of Psychology, The University of Hong Kong , Pokfulam , Hong Kong
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Langille JJ. Remembering to Forget: A Dual Role for Sleep Oscillations in Memory Consolidation and Forgetting. Front Cell Neurosci 2019; 13:71. [PMID: 30930746 PMCID: PMC6425990 DOI: 10.3389/fncel.2019.00071] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/27/2018] [Accepted: 02/13/2019] [Indexed: 12/20/2022] Open
Abstract
It has been known since the time of patient H. M. and Karl Lashley's equipotentiality studies that the hippocampus and cortex serve mnestic functions. Current memory models maintain that these two brain structures accomplish unique, but interactive, memory functions. Specifically, most modeling suggests that memories are rapidly acquired during waking experience by the hippocampus, before being later consolidated into the cortex for long-term storage. Sleep has been shown to be critical for the transfer and consolidation of memories in the cortex. Like memory consolidation, a role for sleep in adaptive forgetting has both historical precedent, as Francis Crick suggested in 1983 that sleep was for "reverse-learning," and recent empirical support. In this article I review the evidence indicating that the same brain activity involved in sleep replay associated memory consolidation is responsible for sleep-dependent forgetting. In reviewing the literature, it became clear that both a cellular mechanism for systems consolidation and an agreed upon general, as well as cellular, mechanism for sleep-dependent forgetting is seldom discussed or is lacking. I advocate here for a candidate cellular systems consolidation mechanism wherein changes in calcium kinetics and the activation of consolidative signaling cascades arise from the triple phase locking of non-rapid eye movement sleep (NREMS) slow oscillation, sleep spindle and sharp-wave ripple rhythms. I go on to speculatively consider several sleep stage specific forgetting mechanisms and conclude by discussing a notional function of NREM-rapid eye movement sleep (REMS) cycling. The discussed model argues that the cyclical organization of sleep functions to first lay down and edit and then stabilize and integrate engrams. All things considered, it is increasingly clear that hallmark sleep stage rhythms, including several NREMS oscillations and the REMS hippocampal theta rhythm, serve the dual function of enabling simultaneous memory consolidation and adaptive forgetting. Specifically, the same sleep rhythms that consolidate new memories, in the cortex and hippocampus, simultaneously organize the adaptive forgetting of older memories in these brain regions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jesse J Langille
- Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Saylor J, Ji X, Calamaro CJ, Davey A. Does sleep duration, napping, and social jetlag predict hemoglobin A1c among college students with type 1 diabetes mellitus? Diabetes Res Clin Pract 2019; 148:102-109. [PMID: 30641174 PMCID: PMC7274839 DOI: 10.1016/j.diabres.2019.01.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/15/2018] [Revised: 12/12/2018] [Accepted: 01/04/2019] [Indexed: 12/11/2022]
Abstract
AIMS The first aim examined the relationship between sleep behaviors (duration, napping, and social jetlag) and hemoglobin A1c (HbA1c) among emerging young adults (EYAs) with T1DM between 18 and 25 years old, who are living on a college campus. The second aim characterized the gender differences in glucose management, sleep behaviors, caffeine intake, and nighttime technology. METHODS A cross-sectional study of eligible participants used a convenience sample of eligible participants. Using Research Electronic Data Capture (REDCap), participants completed surveys about diabetes management, caffeine intake, nighttime technology use, and sleep-related behaviors. Data were analyzed using correlation and multiple linear regression to predict HbA1c from sleep behaviors, adjusting for covariates. RESULTS Participants (N = 76) average years with T1DM was 10.25 ± 5.70. Compared to females, males had a longer sleep duration lower HbA1c levels. HbA1c levels were negatively correlated with weekday sleep (r = -0.24, p = 0.03) and positively correlated with napping (r = 0.34, p = 0.003). After adjusting for covariates, participants who napped had a higher HbA1c level (β = 0.74, p = 0.03) compared with non-nappers. CONCLUSIONS Higher HbA1c levels were found among EYAs with T1DM in college who were nappers and had a longer sleep duration. Modifying sleep behaviors may be an appropriate target to improve glycemic control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jennifer Saylor
- University of Delaware, Tower at STAR, 5th Floor, 100 Discovery Blvd, Newark, DE 19713, USA.
| | - Xiaopeng Ji
- University of Delaware, Tower at STAR, 5th Floor, 100 Discovery Blvd, Newark, DE 19713, USA.
| | | | - Adam Davey
- University of Delaware, Carpenter Sports Building, 26 North College Avenue, Newark, DE 19713, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Abstract
Objective/Background: The impact of midday napping on neurocognitive function in adolescents has not been well established. This study aimed to investigate the relationship between self-reported midday-napping behaviors and neurocognitive function in early adolescents. Participants: The sample was comprised of 363 early adolescents (12.00 ± 0.38 years old) from Jintan, China. Methods: Midday napping, nighttime sleep duration, and sleep quality were measured by self-reported questionnaires. Neurocognitive function was measured by the Penn Computerized Neurocognitive Battery (accuracy and reaction times). Generalized linear regression was used to analyze the relationships. Results: Sixty-four percent of our sample took more than 3 naps per week, and 70.11% reported nap durations of over 30 min. Participants with higher frequencies or longer durations of midday napping reported significantly better nighttime sleep quality (p < 0.05). Adjusted models showed that frequent nappers (5-7d/week) were significantly associated with heightened accuracy on tasks that measured sustained attention and nonverbal reasoning and faster reaction times on spatial memory compared with other frequency groups (ps < 0.05). For napping duration subgroups, early adolescents who took naps of any length were estimated to have faster reaction speeds on the sustained attention task compared with participants who never napped (ps < 0.05). However, only nappers with a moderate duration (31-60 min) tended to achieve both faster speeds (β = -38.28, p = 0.02) and better accuracy (β = 3.90, p = 0.04) on the sustained attention task. Conclusions: Our findings suggest that there is an association between habitual midday napping and neurocognitive function in early adolescents, especially in China, where midday napping is a cultural practice.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xiaopeng Ji
- School of Nursing University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA,College of Health Sciences, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, USA
| | - Junxin Li
- School of Nursing University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA,Center for Sleep and Circadian Neurobiology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Jianghong Liu
- School of Nursing University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA,Correspondence: Jianghong Liu, PhD, RN, FAAN, Associate Professor of Nursing, School of Nursing and School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, 418 Curie Blvd., Room 426, Claire M. Fagin Hall, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 19104-6096, Tel: (215) 898-8293; Fax: (215) 573-7492,
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Anastasopoulos E, Haidich AB, Coleman AL, Wilson MR, Harris A, Yu F, Koskosas A, Pappas T, Keskini C, Kalouda P, Karkamanis G, Topouzis F. Risk factors for Age-related Macular Degeneration in a Greek population: The Thessaloniki Eye Study. Ophthalmic Epidemiol 2018; 25:457-469. [PMID: 30265203 DOI: 10.1080/09286586.2018.1512634] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
Abstract
PURPOSE To assess the association of potential risk factors with early and late age-related macular degeneration (AMD) in the Thessaloniki Eye Study (TES) population Design: Population-based, cross-sectional study of subjects over age of 60 living in Thessaloniki, Greece Methods: Subjects without any AMD features and subjects with early and late AMD (neovascular AMD or geographic atrophy) were identified in the TES cohort using standardized procedures and masked grading of stereo color fundus photos. Demographic, lifestyle, systemic and other ophthalmic covariates were also collected during a detailed examination process. Their association with AMD was investigated using univariate and multivariate adjusted logistic regression models. RESULTS Among the 2108 participants with gradable photos, the grading process identified 1204 subjects with no AMD, 848 subjects with early AMD, and 56 subjects with late AMD (24 with geographic atrophy and 32 with neovascular AMD). In multivariate analysis, compared to no AMD, late AMD was positively associated with older age (OR:1.16; 95%CI:1.10-1.22 per year of age), current smoking (smoking vs. never smoking, OR:2.34; 95%CI:1.12-4.90), prior cataract surgery (cataract surgery vs. no cataract surgery OR:2.06; 95%CI:0.96-4.40), marital status (divorced/separated vs. married, OR:3.10; 95%CI:1.08-8.93) and with 60% lower odds when sleeping in the afternoon (yes vs. no, OR:0.40; 95%CI:0.22-0.72). Early AMD was positively associated with older age (OR: 1.03; 95%CI:1.01-1.05 per year of age) and negatively with higher pulse pressure (OR:0.99; 95%CI:0.98-0.99 per mmHg). CONCLUSIONS In TES, apart for well-known risk factors for AMD like age, smoking, and cataract surgery, two novel behavioral risk factors for prevalent late AMD were suggested. Sleeping in the afternoon was associated with 60% decreased odds for late AMD and 67% decreased odds for neovascular AMD. Being divorced/separated compared to married was associated with 3-fold higher odds for late AMD. Large longitudinal population-based studies will be necessary to further establish the potential late AMD risk effects of these two novel factors, to demonstrate potential implications of underlying pathogenetic mechanisms, and to explore preventive measures and therapeutic targets.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eleftherios Anastasopoulos
- a Laboratory of Research and Clinical Applications in Ophthalmology (LARCAO), Department of Ophthalmology, School of Medicine , Aristotle University of Thessaloniki , Thessaloniki , Greece
| | - Anna Bettina Haidich
- b Department of Epidemiology, School of Medicine , Aristotle University of Thessaloniki , Thessaloniki , Greece
| | - Anne Louise Coleman
- c Stein Eye Institute, David Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA , University of California Los Angeles , Los Angeles , CA , USA
| | | | - Alon Harris
- e Eugene and Marilyn Glick Eye Institute, Department of Ophthalmology , Indiana University School of Medicine , Indianapolis , Indiana , USA
| | - Fei Yu
- f Department of Biostatistics , UCLA Fielding School of Public Health , Los Angeles , CA , USA
| | - Archimides Koskosas
- a Laboratory of Research and Clinical Applications in Ophthalmology (LARCAO), Department of Ophthalmology, School of Medicine , Aristotle University of Thessaloniki , Thessaloniki , Greece
| | - Theofanis Pappas
- a Laboratory of Research and Clinical Applications in Ophthalmology (LARCAO), Department of Ophthalmology, School of Medicine , Aristotle University of Thessaloniki , Thessaloniki , Greece
| | - Christina Keskini
- a Laboratory of Research and Clinical Applications in Ophthalmology (LARCAO), Department of Ophthalmology, School of Medicine , Aristotle University of Thessaloniki , Thessaloniki , Greece
| | - Pelagia Kalouda
- a Laboratory of Research and Clinical Applications in Ophthalmology (LARCAO), Department of Ophthalmology, School of Medicine , Aristotle University of Thessaloniki , Thessaloniki , Greece
| | - Georgia Karkamanis
- a Laboratory of Research and Clinical Applications in Ophthalmology (LARCAO), Department of Ophthalmology, School of Medicine , Aristotle University of Thessaloniki , Thessaloniki , Greece
| | - Fotis Topouzis
- a Laboratory of Research and Clinical Applications in Ophthalmology (LARCAO), Department of Ophthalmology, School of Medicine , Aristotle University of Thessaloniki , Thessaloniki , Greece
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
A daytime nap enhances visual working memory performance and alters event-related delay activity. COGNITIVE AFFECTIVE & BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE 2018; 18:1105-1120. [DOI: 10.3758/s13415-018-0625-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/07/2023]
|
29
|
Lau EYY, McAteer S, Leung CNW, Tucker MA, Li C. Beneficial effects of a daytime nap on verbal memory in adolescents. J Adolesc 2018; 67:77-84. [PMID: 29929055 DOI: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2018.06.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/16/2018] [Revised: 06/06/2018] [Accepted: 06/09/2018] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
This study aimed to examine the sleep-dependent memory consolidation of verbal declarative memory in Chinese adolescents in a naturalistic experimental setting. Thirty-nine healthy boarding school students (ages 15-18, 70% female) were randomized to either a one-hour afternoon nap or wake group between the baseline and the retest sessions of three verbal declarative memory tasks: a Prose Stories Recall task, a Word Pair Associates task, and Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test. Results showed that the nap group performed better than the no-nap group on both the Prose Stories Recall task and the Word Pair Associates task, but not on list learning. Our findings suggest that napping is beneficial to verbal declarative memory in adolescents, providing ecologically-valid empirical support for the sleep-dependent memory consolidation hypothesis using a napping paradigm in participants' naturalistic habitat. Our results demonstrate the potential importance of napping as a practical mnemonic intervention/compensatory strategy for student populations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Esther Yuet Ying Lau
- Department of Psychology, The Education University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong; Centre for Psychosocial Health, The Education University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong.
| | - Susan McAteer
- Department of Psychology, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
| | | | | | - Cheng Li
- Department of Psychology, The Education University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong; Centre for Psychosocial Health, The Education University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Effects of Afternoon Nap Deprivation on Adult Habitual Nappers' Inhibition Functions. BIOMED RESEARCH INTERNATIONAL 2018; 2018:5702646. [PMID: 29765985 PMCID: PMC5885336 DOI: 10.1155/2018/5702646] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/31/2017] [Revised: 11/24/2017] [Accepted: 02/05/2018] [Indexed: 12/04/2022]
Abstract
Multiple studies have established the effects of afternoon naps on cognition. However, relatively few studies have investigated the domain of executive functions. Moreover, the effects of napping on inhibition are far from conclusive. The present study employed adult habitual nappers to investigate the effects of afternoon nap deprivation on response-based inhibition assessed by a Go/No-go task and stimulus-based inhibition assessed by a Flanker task and on alertness assessed by a psychomotor vigilance test (PVT) and the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS). The results showed that afternoon nap deprivation significantly decreased participants' accuracy and reaction speed for the Go/No-go task but not for the Flanker task. In addition, participants' alertness was significantly impaired after nap deprivation in terms of increased subjective sleepiness and worse PVT performance. Task-specific effects of napping on inhibition were demonstrated. The implications of the results are discussed.
Collapse
|
31
|
Nakagawa S, Takeuchi H, Taki Y, Nouchi R, Sekiguchi A, Kotozaki Y, Miyauchi CM, Iizuka K, Yokoyama R, Shinada T, Yamamoto Y, Hanawa S, Araki T, Kunitoki K, Sassa Y, Kawashima R. Sex-Related Differences in the Effects of Sleep Habits on Verbal and Visuospatial Working Memory. Front Psychol 2016; 7:1128. [PMID: 27516751 PMCID: PMC4963384 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01128] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/25/2016] [Accepted: 07/14/2016] [Indexed: 01/16/2023] Open
Abstract
Poor sleep quality negatively affects memory performance, and working memory in particular. We investigated sleep habits related to sleep quality including sleep duration, daytime nap duration, nap frequency, and dream content recall frequency (DCRF). Declarative working memory can be subdivided into verbal working memory (VWM) and visuospatial working memory (VSWM). We hypothesized that sleep habits would have different effects on VWM and VSWM. To our knowledge, our study is the first to investigate differences between VWM and VSWM related to daytime nap duration, nap frequency, and DCRF. Furthermore, we tested the hypothesis that the effects of duration and frequency of daytime naps and DCRF on VWM and VSWM differed according to sex. We assessed 779 healthy right-handed individuals (434 males and 345 females; mean age: 20.7 ± 1.8 years) using a digit span forward and backward VWM task, a forward and backward VSWM task, and sleep habits scales. A correlation analysis was used to test the relationships between VWM capacity (VWMC) and VSWM capacity (VSWMC) scores and sleep duration, nap duration, nap frequency, and DCRF. Furthermore, multiple regression analyses were conducted to identify factors associated with VWMC and VSWMC scores and to identify sex-related differences. We found significant positive correlations between VSWMC and nap duration and DCRF, and between VWMC and sleep duration in all subjects. Furthermore, we found that working memory capacity (WMC) was positively correlated with nap duration in males and with sleep duration in females, and DCRF was positively correlated with VSWMC in females. Our finding of sex-related differences in the effects of sleep habits on WMC has not been reported previously. The associations between WMC and sleep habits differed according to sex because of differences in the underlying neural correlates of VWM and VSWM, and effectiveness of the sleep habits in males and females.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Seishu Nakagawa
- Department of Human Brain Science, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku UniversitySendai, Japan; Department of Psychiatry, Tohoku Medical and Pharmaceutical UniversitySendai, Japan
| | - Hikaru Takeuchi
- Division of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku University Sendai, Japan
| | - Yasuyuki Taki
- Division of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku UniversitySendai, Japan; Division of Medical Neuroimaging Analysis, Department of Community Medical Supports, Tohoku Medical Megabank Organization, Tohoku UniversitySendai, Japan; Department of Nuclear Medicine and Radiology, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku UniversitySendai, Japan
| | - Rui Nouchi
- Creative Interdisciplinary Research Division, Frontier Research Institute for Interdisciplinary Science, Tohoku UniversitySendai, Japan; Human and Social Response Research Division, International Research Institute of Disaster Science, Tohoku UniversitySendai, Japan; Smart Ageing International Research Center, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku UniversitySendai, Japan
| | - Atsushi Sekiguchi
- Department of Human Brain Science, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku UniversitySendai, Japan; Division of Medical Neuroimaging Analysis, Department of Community Medical Supports, Tohoku Medical Megabank Organization, Tohoku UniversitySendai, Japan; Department of Adult Mental Health, National Institute of Mental Health, National Center of Neurology and PsychiatryTokyo, Japan
| | - Yuka Kotozaki
- Smart Ageing International Research Center, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku University Sendai, Japan
| | - Carlos M Miyauchi
- Department of Human Brain Science, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku UniversitySendai, Japan; Department of General Systems Studies, Graduate School of Arts and Sciences, The University of TokyoTokyo, Japan
| | - Kunio Iizuka
- Department of Human Brain Science, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku UniversitySendai, Japan; Department of Psychiatry, Tohoku University Graduate School of MedicineSendai, Japan
| | - Ryoichi Yokoyama
- School of Medicine, Kobe UniversityKobe, Japan; Japan Society for the Promotion of ScienceTokyo, Japan
| | - Takamitsu Shinada
- Department of Human Brain Science, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku University Sendai, Japan
| | - Yuki Yamamoto
- Department of Human Brain Science, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku University Sendai, Japan
| | - Sugiko Hanawa
- Department of Human Brain Science, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku University Sendai, Japan
| | | | | | - Yuko Sassa
- Division of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku University Sendai, Japan
| | - Ryuta Kawashima
- Division of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku UniversitySendai, Japan; Smart Ageing International Research Center, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku UniversitySendai, Japan; Advanced Brain Science, Institute of Development, Aging and Cancer, Tohoku UniversitySendai, Japan
| |
Collapse
|