1
|
Zhang Y, Leng B. Performance of Elite Women's Singles Badminton Players: The Influence of Left-Handed Players. J Hum Kinet 2024; 92:239-249. [PMID: 38736595 PMCID: PMC11079930 DOI: 10.5114/jhk/172783] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/23/2023] [Accepted: 09/22/2023] [Indexed: 05/14/2024] Open
Abstract
The purpose of the study was to analyze the influence of left-handed athletes on the opponent (right-handed players) of elite badminton women's singles. The study selected a total of 40 women's singles matches played by elite female players: twenty matches (42 games, n = 42) were played between left-handed and right-handed players and twenty matches (44 games, n = 44) were played between two right-handed players. There were significant (p < 0.05) differences in hitting positions, techniques, routes and landing points. No significant (p > 0.05) differences were found in scores per game and frequency distribution of rally outcomes. In conclusion, the details of technical and tactical application were different in two game forms, the main impact of the left-handed player on the opponent's (right-handed player) game was a decrease in the opponent's stroke in the overhead, an increase in the number of drives, predominance of small slashes and a decrease in big slashes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yibo Zhang
- College of Physical Education, Taiyuan University of Technology, Taiyuan Shanxi, China
| | - Bo Leng
- College of Competitive Sport, Beijing Sport University, Beijing, China
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
No Sex Differences in the Attentional Bias for the Right Side of Human Bodies. Symmetry (Basel) 2023. [DOI: 10.3390/sym15020466] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/12/2023] Open
Abstract
Ambiguous silhouettes representing human individuals which perform unimanual actions are interpreted more often as right-handed. Such a preference might reflect a perceptual frequency effect, due to the fact that most social interactions occur with right-handers. As a consequence, observers would preferentially attend to the region in which others’ dominant hand usually falls, thus increasing the efficiency in monitoring both aggressive and communicative acts. Given that men can be more dangerous compared with women, the right-hand bias should be larger when observing male rather than female individuals, and given that aggressive interactions involve men more frequently than women, it should be larger in male rather than female observers. However, previous studies did not specifically test whether: (i) male—compared with female—observers pay more attention to the right side of others (regardless of the observed individuals’ sex), or (ii) observers (regardless of their sex) pay more attention to the right side of male—compared with female—individuals. Therefore, in the present study we used ambiguous human silhouettes rotating about their vertical axis with one arm extended in order to determine whether the rightward bias is larger for male rather than female figures and/or in male rather than female participants. According to our data, the bias toward the right side of human bodies was not significantly associated with either the figure’s or the participant’s sex.
Collapse
|
3
|
Tsorlakis N, Koidou I, Grigoropoulou M, Grouios G. Limb Preference and Limb Bone Mineral Density: an Overview of Empirical Findings. Clin Rev Bone Miner Metab 2022. [DOI: 10.1007/s12018-022-09284-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/23/2022]
|
4
|
Ben Kahla A, Elghoul Y, Ammar A, Masmoudi L, Trabelsi K, Glenn JM, Dahmen R. Bilateral Training Improves Agility and Accuracy for Both Preferred and Non-Preferred Legs in Young Soccer Players. Percept Mot Skills 2022; 129:1804-1825. [PMID: 36054086 DOI: 10.1177/00315125221124373] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
As laterality of the lower limbs is regarded as a relevant influence on soccer performance, we assessed whether a bilateral training program for both the preferred leg (PL) and non-preferred leg (NPL) would improve soccer players' lateral asymmetry, agility, and accuracy. Sixty right-foot-dominant young soccer players were randomly assigned to either an experimental group (EXP) that underwent bilateral training or a control group (CONT) that performed their usual training schedule without bilateral exercise. We assessed the players' lateral asymmetries before and after training on four soccer skill tasks: a zigzag test with and without a ball, a receiving and direct volley-shooting accuracy test, and a passing accuracy test. Results showed post-test versus pre-test performance improvements for the EXP group on the index of technical skills and agility (p < .001). A post-hoc analysis further revealed a pre-test to post-test performance improvement on shooting and passing accuracy with both legs only for the EXP group (p < .001). These results support the role of bilateral practice in improving lateral asymmetries, agility, and accuracy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ahmed Ben Kahla
- Education, Motor Skills, Sports and Health (EM2S), LR19JS01, High Institute of Sport and Physical Education of Sfax, 479822University of Sfax, Tunisia.,High Institute of Sport and Physical Education of Sfax, 479822University of Sfax, Tunisia
| | - Yousri Elghoul
- Education, Motor Skills, Sports and Health (EM2S), LR19JS01, High Institute of Sport and Physical Education of Sfax, 479822University of Sfax, Tunisia.,High Institute of Sport and Physical Education of Sfax, 479822University of Sfax, Tunisia
| | - Achraf Ammar
- Department of Training and Movement Science, Institute of Sport Science, Johannes Gutenberg-University Mainz, Mainz, Germany.,Interdisciplinary Laboratory in Neurosciences, Physiology and Psychology, Physical Activity, Health and Learning (LINP2), UPL, UFR STAPS (Faculty of Sport Sciences), Paris Nanterre University, Nanterre, France
| | - Liwa Masmoudi
- Education, Motor Skills, Sports and Health (EM2S), LR19JS01, High Institute of Sport and Physical Education of Sfax, 479822University of Sfax, Tunisia.,High Institute of Sport and Physical Education of Sfax, 479822University of Sfax, Tunisia
| | - Khaled Trabelsi
- Education, Motor Skills, Sports and Health (EM2S), LR19JS01, High Institute of Sport and Physical Education of Sfax, 479822University of Sfax, Tunisia.,High Institute of Sport and Physical Education of Sfax, 479822University of Sfax, Tunisia
| | - Jordan M Glenn
- Exercise Science Research Center, Department of Health, Human Performance and Recreation, University of Arkansas, Fayetteville, AR, USA
| | - Riadh Dahmen
- Education, Motor Skills, Sports and Health (EM2S), LR19JS01, High Institute of Sport and Physical Education of Sfax, 479822University of Sfax, Tunisia.,High Institute of Sport and Physical Education of Sfax, 479822University of Sfax, Tunisia
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Rodway P, Schepman A. Who goes where in couples and pairs? Effects of sex and handedness on side preferences in human dyads. Laterality 2022; 27:415-442. [PMID: 35729774 DOI: 10.1080/1357650x.2022.2090573] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/24/2023]
Abstract
There is increasing evidence that inter-individual interaction among conspecifics can cause population-level lateralization. Male-female and mother-infant dyads of several non-human species show lateralised position preferences, but such preferences have rarely been examined in humans. We observed 430 male-female human pairs and found a significant bias for males to walk on the right side of the pair. A survey measured side preferences in 93 left-handed and 92 right-handed women, and 96 left-handed and 99 right-handed men. When walking, and when sitting on a bench, males showed a significant side preference determined by their handedness, with left-handed men preferring to be on their partner's left side and right-handed men preferring to be on their partner's right side. Women did not show significant side preferences. When men are with their partner they show a preference for the side that facilitates the use of their dominant hand. We discuss possible reasons for the side preference, including males prefering to occupy the optimal "fight ready" side, and the influence of sex and handedness on the strength and direction of emotion lateralization.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paul Rodway
- School of Psychology, University of Chester, Chester, United Kingdom
| | - Astrid Schepman
- School of Psychology, University of Chester, Chester, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Abstract
As shown by a series of previous studies, ambiguous human bodies performing unimanual or unipedal actions tend to be perceived more frequently as right-handed or right-footed rather than left-handed or left-footed, which indicates a perceptual and attentional bias toward the right side of others’ body. However, none of such studies assessed whether the relative strength of such a bias differs between the upper and lower limbs. Indeed, given that the prevalence of right-handedness is slightly larger than that of right-footedness, and given that hands provide more information than feet as regards both communicative and aggressive acts, it is plausible that the bias toward the right side of human bodies should be stronger for the hand than for the foot. We performed three experiments in each of which participants had to indicate the rotating direction (revealing the perceived handedness/footedness) of ambiguous human figures with either one limb (arm or leg) or two limbs (one arm and the contralateral leg) extended. The hypothesized advantage of the right hand over the right foot was found in both the second and the third experiment.
Collapse
|
7
|
Connor JD, Mann DL, Gomez MA, Leicht AS, Doma K. Performance Advantages of Left-Handed Cricket Batting Talent. Front Psychol 2020; 11:1654. [PMID: 32849010 PMCID: PMC7424046 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01654] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/20/2020] [Accepted: 06/17/2020] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
The purpose of this study was to examine performance advantages associated with batting stance, in the form of left- vs. right-handed dominant stance, and orthodox vs. reverse stance, of talented junior cricket batters within age-restricted competitions. Data were sourced from the national male younger age competition (YAC; Under-17; n = 237) and older age competition (OAC; Under-19; n = 302), as well as female YAC (Under-15; n = 234) and OAC (Under-18; n = 260) over a 4-year period. Left-hand dominant (LHD) batters were consistently overrepresented in the male YAC (Right: 69.2%; Left: 30.8%) and OAC (Right: 68.2%; Left: 31.8%) compared with the expected general population distribution. Male LHD batters exhibited a significantly (p < 0.05) higher batting aggregate (YAC: 116.82 ± 84.75 vs. 137.84 ± 89.74; OAC: 117.07 ± 89.00 vs. 146.28 ± 95.99), scored more runs (YAC: 19.65 ± 12.32 vs. 23.96 ± 14.71; OAC: 19.27 ± 12.61 vs. 23.98 ± 14.15), spent more time batting (YAC: 45.33 ± 25.89 min vs. 54.59 ± 28.62 min; OAC: 39.80 ± 21.79 min vs. 49.33 ± 27.41 min), and scored more boundary-4s per game (YAC: 1.83 ± 1.40 vs. 2.44 ± 1.87; OAC: 1.76 ± 1.32 vs. 2.19 ± 1.83), across both YAC and OAC groups with small effect sizes. No overrepresentation was present for either female group (YAC, Right: 88.5%/Left: 11.5%; OAC, Right: 90.0%/Left: 10.0%). Female LHD batters exhibited significantly higher batting aggregate (68.97 ± 53.17 vs. 102.96 ± 73.48), batting average (13.24 ± 10.88 vs. 17.75 ± 12.28), and spent more time batting per game (25.52 ± 15.08 vs. 37.75 ± 26.76 min), but only at the OAC level with small-moderate effects sizes. Finally, there were few performance advantages identified to batting with a reverse stance, with further work needed to clarify any potential biomechanical benefits. Team selection practices may exploit the left-handed advantage by over-selecting talented left-handed junior cricketers. Practical implications for coaches include creating practice environments that negate the negative frequency-dependent selection, such as providing more practice opportunities for their players against left-handed opponents.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jonathan D Connor
- Sport and Exercise Science, James Cook University, Townsville, QLD, Australia
| | - David L Mann
- Department of Human Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
| | - Miguel-Angel Gomez
- Faculty of Physical Activity and Sports Sciences, Polytechnic University of Madrid, Madrid, Spain
| | - Anthony S Leicht
- Sport and Exercise Science, James Cook University, Townsville, QLD, Australia
| | - Kenji Doma
- Sport and Exercise Science, James Cook University, Townsville, QLD, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Gualdi-Russo E, Rinaldo N, Pasini A, Zaccagni L. Hand Preference and Performance in Basketball Tasks. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF ENVIRONMENTAL RESEARCH AND PUBLIC HEALTH 2019; 16:ijerph16224336. [PMID: 31703270 PMCID: PMC6887724 DOI: 10.3390/ijerph16224336] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/14/2019] [Revised: 11/02/2019] [Accepted: 11/06/2019] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
Abstract
The aims of this study were to develop and validate an instrument to quantitatively assess the handedness of basketballers in basketball tasks (Basketball Handedness Inventory, BaHI) and to compare it with their handedness in daily activities by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (EHI). The participants were 111 basketballers and 40 controls. All subjects completed the EHI and only basketballers filled in the BaHI. To validate the BaHI, a voluntary subsample of basketballers repeated the BaHI. Exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses supported a two-factor model. Our results show that: (i) Handedness score (R) in daily actions did not differ between basketball players (R by EHI = 69.3 ± 44.6) and the control group (R by EHI = 64.5 ± 58.6); (ii) basketballers more frequently favored performing certain sport tasks with the left hand or mixed hands (as highlighted by R by BaHI = 50.1 ± 47.1), although their choice was primarily the right hand in everyday gestures; and (iii) this preference was especially true for athletes at the highest levels of performance (R by BaHI of A1 league = 38.6 ± 58.3) and for those playing in selected roles (point guard's R = 29.4 ± 67.4). Our findings suggest that professional training induces handedness changes in basketball tasks. The BaHI provides a valid and reliable measure of the skilled hand in basketball. This will allow coaches to assess mastery of the ball according to the hand used by the athlete in the different tasks and roles.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emanuela Gualdi-Russo
- Department of Biomedical and Specialty Surgical Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, Pharmacy and Prevention, University of Ferrara, 44121 Ferrara, Italy; (E.G.-R.); (N.R.)
| | - Natascia Rinaldo
- Department of Biomedical and Specialty Surgical Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, Pharmacy and Prevention, University of Ferrara, 44121 Ferrara, Italy; (E.G.-R.); (N.R.)
| | - Alba Pasini
- Department of Biomedical and Specialty Surgical Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, Pharmacy and Prevention, University of Ferrara, 44121 Ferrara, Italy; (E.G.-R.); (N.R.)
- Correspondence: (A.P.); (L.Z.); Tel.: +39-0532-293792 (A.P.); +39-0532-293717 (L.Z.)
| | - Luciana Zaccagni
- Department of Biomedical and Specialty Surgical Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, Pharmacy and Prevention, University of Ferrara, 44121 Ferrara, Italy; (E.G.-R.); (N.R.)
- Centre for Exercise Science and Sport, University of Ferrara, 44123 Ferrara, Italy
- Correspondence: (A.P.); (L.Z.); Tel.: +39-0532-293792 (A.P.); +39-0532-293717 (L.Z.)
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Richardson T, Gilman RT. Left-handedness is associated with greater fighting success in humans. Sci Rep 2019; 9:15402. [PMID: 31659217 PMCID: PMC6817864 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-51975-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/12/2019] [Accepted: 10/07/2019] [Indexed: 12/01/2022] Open
Abstract
Left-handedness is a costly, sexually dimorphic trait found at low frequencies in all human populations. How the handedness polymorphism is maintained is unclear. The fighting hypothesis argues that left-handed men have a negative frequency-dependent advantage in violent intrasexual competition giving them a selective advantage. In support of this, many studies have found that left-handed men are overrepresented among modern professional fighters, but studies typically find no difference in fighting success between left and right-handed fighters. We studied over 13,800 professional boxers and mixed martial artists of varying abilities in three of the largest samples to test this hypothesis to date, finding robust evidence that left-handed fighters have greater fighting success. This held for both male and female fighters, and for both percentage of fights won and an objective measure of fighting ability. We replicated previous results showing that left-handed fighters are strongly overrepresented in professional combat sports, but left-handed fighters did not show greater variance in fighting ability, a hypothesis suggested in previous studies. Overall we find strong evidence consistent with the fighting hypothesis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas Richardson
- School of Earth and Environmental Sciences, University of Manchester, Manchester, United Kingdom.
| | - R Tucker Gilman
- School of Earth and Environmental Sciences, University of Manchester, Manchester, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Marcori AJ, Monteiro PHM, Okazaki VHA. Changing handedness: What can we learn from preference shift studies? Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2019; 107:313-319. [PMID: 31521700 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.09.019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/11/2019] [Revised: 07/08/2019] [Accepted: 09/11/2019] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
Handedness is a dynamic and complex aspect of human behavior. Changing it through practice, either willingly or obliged by some reason, requires a considerable amount of effort. Analyzing studies that presented handedness shifts may expand our comprehension of this phenomenon, since knowing how to change it might provide insights into how it develops. Therefore, we reviewed the outcomes of handedness shifts. The results suggest that neural asymmetries related to handedness are likely a consequence of lateralized practice since they correlate with modifications in the behavioral patterns. Clearly, practice is not the only factor influencing handedness development, but it seems to play a significant role in the formation and consolidation of neural and behavioral asymmetries. Another key finding of our review is the suggestion of a ceiling effect for the capacity to change handedness direction and degree, considering none of the reviewed studies reported complete shifts in behavioral measures and brain activation patterns.
Collapse
|
11
|
van der Feen FE, Zickert N, Groothuis TG, Geuze RH. Does hand skill asymmetry relate to creativity, developmental and health issues and aggression as markers of fitness? Laterality 2019; 25:53-86. [DOI: 10.1080/1357650x.2019.1619750] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Fleur E. van der Feen
- Clinical and Developmental Neuropsychology, University of Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands
| | - Nele Zickert
- Behavioral Biology, the Groningen Institute for Evolutionary Life Sciences, University of Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands
| | - Ton G.G. Groothuis
- Behavioral Biology, the Groningen Institute for Evolutionary Life Sciences, University of Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands
| | - Reint H. Geuze
- Clinical and Developmental Neuropsychology, University of Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Rico-Guevara A, Hurme KJ. Intrasexually selected weapons. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 2019; 94:60-101. [PMID: 29924496 DOI: 10.1111/brv.12436] [Citation(s) in RCA: 78] [Impact Index Per Article: 15.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/20/2017] [Revised: 05/14/2018] [Accepted: 05/18/2018] [Indexed: 01/24/2023]
Abstract
We propose a practical concept that distinguishes the particular kind of weaponry that has evolved to be used in combat between individuals of the same species and sex, which we term intrasexually selected weapons (ISWs). We present a treatise of ISWs in nature, aiming to understand their distinction and evolution from other secondary sex traits, including from 'sexually selected weapons', and from sexually dimorphic and monomorphic weaponry. We focus on the subset of secondary sex traits that are the result of same-sex combat, defined here as ISWs, provide not previously reported evolutionary patterns, and offer hypotheses to answer questions such as: why have only some species evolved weapons to fight for the opposite sex or breeding resources? We examined traits that seem to have evolved as ISWs in the entire animal phylogeny, restricting the classification of ISW to traits that are only present or enlarged in adults of one of the sexes, and are used as weapons during intrasexual fights. Because of the absence of behavioural data and, in many cases, lack of sexually discriminated series from juveniles to adults, we exclude the fossil record from this review. We merge morphological, ontogenetic, and behavioural information, and for the first time thoroughly review the tree of life to identify separate evolution of ISWs. We found that ISWs are only found in bilateral animals, appearing independently in nematodes, various groups of arthropods, and vertebrates. Our review sets a reference point to explore other taxa that we identify with potential ISWs for which behavioural or morphological studies are warranted. We establish that most ISWs come in pairs, are located in or near the head, are endo- or exoskeletal modifications, are overdeveloped structures compared with those found in females, are modified feeding structures and/or locomotor appendages, are most common in terrestrial taxa, are frequently used to guard females, territories, or both, and are also used in signalling displays to deter rivals and/or attract females. We also found that most taxa lack ISWs, that females of only a few species possess better-developed weapons than males, that the cases of independent evolution of ISWs are not evenly distributed across the phylogeny, and that animals possessing the most developed ISWs have non-hunting habits (e.g. herbivores) or are faunivores that prey on very small prey relative to their body size (e.g. insectivores). Bringing together perspectives from studies on a variety of taxa, we conceptualize that there are five ways in which a sexually dimorphic trait, apart from the primary sex traits, can be fixed: sexual selection, fecundity selection, parental role division, differential niche occupation between the sexes, and interference competition. We discuss these trends and the factors involved in the evolution of intrasexually selected weaponry in nature.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alejandro Rico-Guevara
- Department of Integrative Biology, University of California, Berkeley, 3040 Valley Life Sciences Building, Berkeley, CA, 94720, U.S.A.,Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Connecticut, 75 N. Eagleville Rd, Unit 3043, Storrs, CT, 06269, U.S.A.,Instituto de Ciencias Naturales, Universidad Nacional de Colombia, Código Postal 11001, Bogotá DC, Colombia
| | - Kristiina J Hurme
- Department of Integrative Biology, University of California, Berkeley, 3040 Valley Life Sciences Building, Berkeley, CA, 94720, U.S.A.,Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Connecticut, 75 N. Eagleville Rd, Unit 3043, Storrs, CT, 06269, U.S.A
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Functional Asymmetry and Fingerprint Features of Left-Handed and Right-Handed Young Yakuts (Mongoloid Race, North-Eastern Siberia). Symmetry (Basel) 2018. [DOI: 10.3390/sym10120728] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
An ethnically homogeneous group of Yakuts (Mongoloid race, Northeast Asia), aged 18–31, was studied to characterize the diversity of particular features between left- and right-handed individuals. A total of 52 left-handed (32 women and 20 men) and 100 right-handed (50 women and 50 men) individuals were studied. Testing included two sets of questions and tasks, dynamometry of the right and left hand, and fingerprint analysis. Left-handed and right-handed people were found to differ in functional asymmetry of psychophysiological and motor reactions. Right-handers were characterized by higher intragroup similarity, while, among left-handers, greater dispersion of these traits was observed. Asymmetry in hand grip strength was less pronounced in the left-handed people than in the right-handed; this difference was statistically significant, and the difference was greater in men than in women. This suggests that the non-dominant hand in the left-handed people was subjected to a greater load and indicates the forced adaptation of the left-handed people to “dextrastress”. No significant difference between sexes was found when analyzing fingerprint patterns. Left-handers had arches significantly more often than right-handers. Radial loops were most often found on the index finger, and, in the left-handers, their occurrence was significantly higher on three to five fingers of the left hand compared with the right-handers. The levels of fluctuating asymmetry in left-handers and right-handers were similar.
Collapse
|
14
|
Jang H, Lee JY, Lee KI, Park KM. Are there differences in brain morphology according to handedness? Brain Behav 2017; 7:e00730. [PMID: 28729936 PMCID: PMC5516604 DOI: 10.1002/brb3.730] [Citation(s) in RCA: 63] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/25/2017] [Revised: 03/31/2017] [Accepted: 04/10/2017] [Indexed: 01/19/2023] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE This study aimed to investigate the differences in brain morphology according to handedness. MATERIALS AND METHODS Forty-two healthy subjects were enrolled (21 right-handers and 21 nonright-handers). The two groups were classified according to the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory. Measures of cortical morphology, such as thickness, surface area, volume, and curvature, and the volumes of subcortical structures, such as the amygdala, caudate, hippocampus, globus pallidus, putamen, and thalamus, were compared between the groups according to handedness using whole-brain 3D T1-weighted MRI. In addition, we investigated the white matter differences between the groups using diffusion tensor imaging. Moreover, we quantified correlations between the handedness scales of the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory and each measure of different brain morphologies. RESULTS The volumes of the right putamen and left globus pallidus in nonright-handed participants were significantly larger than those who were right-handed (0.3559 vs. 0.3155%, p = .0028; 0.1101 vs. 0.0975%, p = .0025; respectively). Moreover, the volumes of the right putamen and left globus pallidus were negatively correlated with the handedness scales of the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (r = -.392, p = .0101; r = -.361, p = .0189; respectively). However, the cortex morphology and the other subcortical volumes were not significantly different between the two groups. In addition, we did not find any white matter differences between the groups. CONCLUSIONS We demonstrated that there were significant differences in brain morphology between right-handers and nonright-handers, especially in the basal ganglia, which could produce differences in motor control according to handedness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Han Jang
- Inje University College of Medicine Haeundae-gu Busan Korea
| | - Jae Youn Lee
- Inje University College of Medicine Haeundae-gu Busan Korea
| | - Kang Il Lee
- Inje University College of Medicine Haeundae-gu Busan Korea
| | - Kang Min Park
- Inje University College of Medicine Haeundae-gu Busan Korea
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
|
16
|
Tran US, Voracek M. Footedness Is Associated with Self-reported Sporting Performance and Motor Abilities in the General Population. Front Psychol 2016; 7:1199. [PMID: 27559326 PMCID: PMC4978716 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01199] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/23/2016] [Accepted: 07/28/2016] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Left-handers may have strategic advantages over right-handers in interactive sports and innate superior abilities that are beneficial for sports. Previous studies relied on differing criteria for handedness classification and mostly did not investigate mixed preferences and footedness. Footedness appears to be less influenced by external and societal factors than handedness. Utilizing latent class analysis and structural equation modeling, we investigated in a series of studies (total N > 15300) associations of handedness and footedness with self-reported sporting performance and motor abilities in the general population. Using a discovery and a replication sample (ns = 7658 and 5062), Study 1 revealed replicable beneficial effects of mixed-footedness and left-footedness in team sports, martial arts and fencing, dancing, skiing, and swimming. Study 2 (n = 2592) showed that footedness for unskilled bipedal movement tasks, but not for skilled unipedal tasks, was beneficial for sporting performance. Mixed- and left-footedness had effects on motor abilities that were consistent with published results on better brain interhemispheric communication, but also akin to testosterone-induced effects regarding flexibility, strength, and endurance. Laterality effects were only small. Possible neural and hormonal bases of observed effects need to be examined in future studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ulrich S Tran
- Department of Basic Psychological Research and Research Methods, School of Psychology, University of Vienna Vienna, Austria
| | - Martin Voracek
- Department of Basic Psychological Research and Research Methods, School of Psychology, University of Vienna Vienna, Austria
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Life on the Left. Laterality 2016. [DOI: 10.1016/b978-0-12-801239-0.00010-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
|
18
|
|
19
|
Petit L, Zago L, Mellet E, Jobard G, Crivello F, Joliot M, Mazoyer B, Tzourio‐Mazoyer N. Strong rightward lateralization of the dorsal attentional network in left-handers with right sighting-eye: an evolutionary advantage. Hum Brain Mapp 2015; 36:1151-64. [PMID: 25409934 PMCID: PMC6869826 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.22693] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/26/2014] [Revised: 10/22/2014] [Accepted: 11/10/2014] [Indexed: 11/11/2022] Open
Abstract
Hemispheric lateralization for spatial attention and its relationships with manual preference strength and eye preference were studied in a sample of 293 healthy individuals balanced for manual preference. Functional magnetic resonance imaging was used to map this large sample while performing visually guided saccadic eye movements. This activated a bilateral distributed cortico-subcortical network in which dorsal and ventral attentional/saccadic pathways elicited rightward asymmetrical activation depending on manual preference strength and sighting eye. While the ventral pathway showed a strong rightward asymmetry irrespective of both manual preference strength and eye preference, the dorsal frontoparietal network showed a robust rightward asymmetry in strongly left-handers, even more pronounced in left-handed subjects with a right sighting-eye. Our findings brings support to the hypothesis that the origin of the rightward hemispheric dominance for spatial attention may have a manipulo-spatial origin neither perceptual nor motor per se but rather reflecting a mechanism by which a spatial context is mapped onto the perceptual and motor activities, including the exploration of the spatial environment with eyes and hands. Within this context, strongly left-handers with a right sighting-eye may benefit from the advantage of having the same right hemispheric control of their dominant hand and visuospatial attention processing. We suggest that this phenomenon explains why left-handed right sighting-eye athletes can outperform their competitors in sporting duels and that the prehistoric and historical constancy of the left-handers ratio over the general population may relate in part on the hemispheric specialization of spatial attention.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laurent Petit
- Université BordeauxGINUMR5296BordeauxFrance
- CNRSGINUMR5296BordeauxFrance
- CEAGINUMR5296BordeauxFrance
| | - Laure Zago
- Université BordeauxGINUMR5296BordeauxFrance
- CNRSGINUMR5296BordeauxFrance
- CEAGINUMR5296BordeauxFrance
| | - Emmanuel Mellet
- Université BordeauxGINUMR5296BordeauxFrance
- CNRSGINUMR5296BordeauxFrance
- CEAGINUMR5296BordeauxFrance
| | - Gaël Jobard
- Université BordeauxGINUMR5296BordeauxFrance
- CNRSGINUMR5296BordeauxFrance
- CEAGINUMR5296BordeauxFrance
| | - Fabrice Crivello
- Université BordeauxGINUMR5296BordeauxFrance
- CNRSGINUMR5296BordeauxFrance
- CEAGINUMR5296BordeauxFrance
| | - Marc Joliot
- Université BordeauxGINUMR5296BordeauxFrance
- CNRSGINUMR5296BordeauxFrance
- CEAGINUMR5296BordeauxFrance
| | - Bernard Mazoyer
- Université BordeauxGINUMR5296BordeauxFrance
- CNRSGINUMR5296BordeauxFrance
- CEAGINUMR5296BordeauxFrance
| | - Nathalie Tzourio‐Mazoyer
- Université BordeauxGINUMR5296BordeauxFrance
- CNRSGINUMR5296BordeauxFrance
- CEAGINUMR5296BordeauxFrance
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Abstract
Abstract
In boxing, athletes choose between two strategies: the orthodox stance characteristic of right handed competitors, or the southpaw stance characteristic of left-handers. Despite a conviction popular among the practitioners of this sport that fighting against a southpaw opponent constitutes a handicap, the effectiveness of the type of stance has so far not been examined. We extracted the statistics of the top twenty active male professionals boxing in each of the seventeen weight divisions. Out of the 340 boxers who composed our group, 75% used the orthodox stance and 25% were southpaw. Generally, we found that boxing stance had no effect on the percentage of 340 top professional boxers’ victories. However, both the southpaw and the orthodox athletes had a higher percentage of victories against orthodox boxers than against southpaws.
Collapse
|
21
|
Loffing F, Hagemann N. Pushing through evolution? Incidence and fight records of left-oriented fighters in professional boxing history. Laterality 2014; 20:270-86. [DOI: 10.1080/1357650x.2014.961471] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
|
22
|
Baker J, Kungl AM, Pabst J, Strauß B, Büsch D, Schorer J. Your fate is in your hands? Handedness, digit ratio (2D:4D), and selection to a national talent development system. Laterality 2013; 18:710-8. [DOI: 10.1080/1357650x.2012.755992] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
|
23
|
Pollet TV, Stulp G, Groothuis TG. Born to win? Testing the fighting hypothesis in realistic fights: left-handedness in the Ultimate Fighting Championship. Anim Behav 2013. [DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.07.026] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
24
|
Groothuis TGG, McManus IC, Schaafsma SM, Geuze RH. The fighting hypothesis in combat: how well does the fighting hypothesis explain human left-handed minorities? Ann N Y Acad Sci 2013; 1288:100-9. [PMID: 23742682 DOI: 10.1111/nyas.12164] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
The strong population bias in hand preference in favor of right-handedness seems to be a typical human trait. An elegant evolutionary hypothesis explaining this trait is the so-called fighting hypothesis that postulates that left-handedness is under frequency-dependent selection. The fighting hypothesis assumes that left-handers, being in the minority because of health issues, are still maintained in the population since they would have a greater chance of winning in fights than right-handers due to a surprise effect. This review critically evaluates the assumptions and evidence for this hypothesis and concludes that some evidence, although consistent with the fighting hypothesis, does not directly support it and may also be interpreted differently. Other supportive data are ambiguous or open for both statistical and theoretical criticism. We conclude that, presently, evidence for the fighting hypothesis is not particularly strong, but that there is little evidence to reject it either. The hypothesis thus remains an intuitively plausible explanation for the persistent left-hand preference in the population. We suggest alternative explanations and several ways forward for obtaining more crucial data for testing this frequently cited hypothesis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ton G G Groothuis
- Behavioural Biology, Centre for Behaviour and Neuroscience, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
25
|
Abstract
Negative frequency-dependent effects rather than innate predispositions may provide left-handers with an advantage in one-on-one fighting situations. Support mainly comes from cross-sectional studies which found significantly enhanced left-hander frequencies among elite athletes exclusively in interactive sports such as baseball, cricket, fencing and tennis. Since professional athletes' training regimes continuously improve, however, an important unsolved question is whether the left-handers' advantage in individual sports like tennis persists over time. To this end, we longitudinally tracked left-hander frequencies in year-end world rankings (men: 1973-2011, ladies: 1975-2011) and at Grand Slam tournaments (1968-2011) in male and female tennis professionals. Here we show that the positive impact of left-handed performance on high achievement in elite tennis was moderate and decreased in male professionals over time and was almost absent in female professionals. For both sexes, left-hander frequencies among year-end top 10 players linearly decreased over the period considered. Moreover, left-handedness was, however, no longer seems associated with higher probability of attaining high year-end world ranking position in male professionals. In contrast, cross-sectional data on left-hander frequencies in male and female amateur players suggest that a left-handers' advantage may still occur on lower performance levels. Collectively, our data is in accordance with the frequency-dependent hypothesis since reduced experience with left-handers in tennis is likely to be compensated by players' professionalism.
Collapse
|
26
|
Faurie C, Llaurens V, Alvergne A, Goldberg M, Zins M, Raymond M. Left-Handedness and Male-Male Competition: Insights from Fighting and Hormonal Data. EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY 2011. [DOI: 10.1177/147470491100900307] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Male-male competition can shape some behavioral or morphological traits of males. Here we investigate if this competition could play a role in the persistence of the polymorphism of handedness in human populations. A negative frequency-dependent selection mechanism has been hypothesized, based on the fact that left-handed men may benefit from a “surprise” advantage during fighting interactions because they are rare in human populations. This advantage may thereby enhance the probability of survival of left-handed men and/or their reproductive success through an increase in social status. In this study, we first explored the association between hand preference and lifetime fighting behavior in a population of 1,161 French men. No effect of hand preference on the probability of fighting was detected, suggesting that the innate propensity to fight does not differ between left- and right-handers. However, among men who had been involved in at least one fight during their lifetime, left-handers reported significantly more fights than right-handers. To explore the biological basis of this behavior, we also investigated the testosterone concentration in saliva samples from 64 French university students. Consistent with frequencies of fights, we found a significantly higher average testosterone concentration in left-handers than in right-handers. We suggest that these behavioral and hormonal differences may be acquired throughout life due to previous experiences in a social context and may favor the persistence of left-handers in humans.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Charlotte Faurie
- CNRS, Institute of Evolutionary Sciences, Université de Montpellier 2, Montpellier cedex 5, France
| | - Violaine Llaurens
- CNRS, Institute of Evolutionary Sciences, Université de Montpellier 2, Montpellier cedex 5, France
| | - Alexandra Alvergne
- Human Evolutionary Ecology Group, Department of Anthropology, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Marcel Goldberg
- INSERM Unit 687 –IFR 69, Hôpital National de Saint-Maurice, Saint-Maurice cedex, France
| | - Marie Zins
- INSERM Unit 687 –IFR 69, Hôpital National de Saint-Maurice, Saint-Maurice cedex, France
| | - Michel Raymond
- CNRS, Institute of Evolutionary Sciences, Université de Montpellier 2, Montpellier cedex 5, France
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Faurie C, Bonenfant S, Goldberg M, Hercberg S, Zins M, Raymond M. Socio-economic status and handedness in two large cohorts of French adults. Br J Psychol 2010; 99:533-54. [DOI: 10.1348/000712608x291563] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
|
28
|
Llaurens V, Raymond M, Faurie C. Why are some people left-handed? An evolutionary perspective. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2009; 364:881-94. [PMID: 19064347 PMCID: PMC2666081 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0235] [Citation(s) in RCA: 104] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Since prehistoric times, left-handed individuals have been ubiquitous in human populations, exhibiting geographical frequency variations. Evolutionary explanations have been proposed for the persistence of the handedness polymorphism. Left-handedness could be favoured by negative frequency-dependent selection. Data have suggested that left-handedness, as the rare hand preference, could represent an important strategic advantage in fighting interactions. However, the fact that left-handedness occurs at a low frequency indicates that some evolutionary costs could be associated with left-handedness. Overall, the evolutionary dynamics of this polymorphism are not fully understood. Here, we review the abundant literature available regarding the possible mechanisms and consequences of left-handedness. We point out that hand preference is heritable, and report how hand preference is influenced by genetic, hormonal, developmental and cultural factors. We review the available information on potential fitness costs and benefits acting as selective forces on the proportion of left-handers. Thus, evolutionary perspectives on the persistence of this polymorphism in humans are gathered for the first time, highlighting the necessity for an assessment of fitness differences between right- and left-handers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- V Llaurens
- Institut des Sciences de l'Evolution de Montpellier (UMR CNRS 5554), Université de Montpellier, Montpellier Cedex 5, France.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
29
|
Abstract
Biological asymmetries are important elements of the structure and function of many living organisms. Using the Plio-Pleistocene fossil record of crab predation on morphologically similar pairs of right- and left-handed snail species, we show here for the first time, contrary to traditional wisdom, that rare left-handed coiling promotes survival from attacks by right-handed crabs. This frequency-dependent result influences the balance of selection processes that maintain left-handedness at the species level and parallels some social interactions in human cultures, such as sports that involve dual contests between opponents of opposite handedness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gregory P Dietl
- Department of Geology and Geophysics, Yale University, New Haven, CT 06520, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
30
|
Hábitos quirúrgicos de los cirujanos zurdos. ANGIOLOGIA 2006. [DOI: 10.1016/s0003-3170(06)74973-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
|
31
|
Billiard S, Faurie C, Raymond M. Maintenance of handedness polymorphism in humans: a frequency-dependent selection model. J Theor Biol 2005; 235:85-93. [PMID: 15833315 DOI: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2004.12.021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/21/2004] [Revised: 12/17/2004] [Accepted: 12/17/2004] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
Frequency-dependent selection is an important process in the maintenance of genetic variation in fitness. In humans, it has been proposed that the polymorphism of handedness is maintained by negative frequency-dependent selection, through a strategic advantage of left-handers in fighting interactions. Using simple mathematical models, we explore: (1) whether it is possible to predict the range of left-handedness frequencies observed in human populations by the frequency and the violence of fighting interactions; (2) the consequences of the sex differences in the probability of transmission of hand preference to offspring. We show that a wide range of values of the frequency of left-handers can be obtained with realistic changes of the parameters values. Our models reinforce the idea that negative frequency-dependence may have played a role in maintaining left-handedness in human populations, and provide further support for the importance of fighting interactions in the evolution of hand preference. Moreover, they suggest an explanation for the occurrence of left-handedness among women in this context, namely an indirect selective advantage through their male offspring.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sylvain Billiard
- Institute of Evolutionary Sciences, University of Montpellier, France
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
32
|
Abstract
Humans exhibit hand preference for most manual activities in which they are specialized. Right- and left-handers have coexisted at least since the Upper Palaeolithic, and left-handers are in the minority in all human populations. The persistence of the polymorphism of handedness is a puzzle because this trait is substantially heritable and several fitness costs are associated with left-handedness. Some countervailing benefit is required to maintain the polymorphism. Left-handers may have a frequency-dependent advantage in fights--the advantage being greater when their frequency is lower. Sports data from Western societies are consistent with this prediction. Here, we show that the frequency of left-handers is strongly and positively correlated with the rate of homicides across traditional societies. It ranges from 3% in the most pacifistic societies, to 27% in the most violent and warlike. This finding is consistent with a frequency-dependent selection mechanism maintaining left-handedness in these societies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Charlotte Faurie
- Genetics of Adaptation, Institute of Evolutionary Sciences (UMR CNRS 5554), University Montpellier II, CC 065, 34095 Montpellier 5, France.
| | | |
Collapse
|
33
|
Faurie C, Schiefenhövel W, le Bomin S, Billiard S, Raymond M. Variation in the Frequency of Left‐handedness in Traditional Societies. CURRENT ANTHROPOLOGY 2005. [DOI: 10.1086/427101] [Citation(s) in RCA: 75] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/03/2022]
|
34
|
Abstract
The objective of this study was to investigate the relationship between handedness and hobby preference in healthy individuals. For this reason, the Annett handedness questionnaire and a standard questionnaire on preference for hobbies were administered to 879 healthy young men (age, M = 22.3, SD = 4.8 yr.). Analysis showed more cultured individuals were much less likely to be strongly right-handed. Especially, pure right-handedness highly overrepresented among those who mainly preferred doing sports, pure left-handedness among those who preferred reading books, collecting, or going to the cinema/theater, and mixed-handedness among those who preferred arts, like playing music, drawing, or handicraft. The findings support evidence that handedness is associated with hobby preference.
Collapse
|
35
|
Abstract
In the past two decades, laterality in sports has become clearly established. In particular, the concept of motoric dominance in sports has been a fertile area for laterality research. The literature is consistent with the view that in certain sporting skills, sporting competitors with specific motoric-dominance patterns enjoy an advantage over other competitors and are overrepresented in some sports. Two theoretical interpretations have been offered to explain this imbalance of motoric-dominance distribution among sporting individuals: hypotheses of "innate superiority" and "strategic advantage." This paper presents an overview the two hypotheses, along with relevant work so far reported and identifies directions for further empirical research. The theoretical and practical implications of research on laterality in human motor performance are discussed.
Collapse
|
36
|
Brooks R, Bussière LF, Jennions MD, Hunt J. Sinister strategies succeed at the cricket World Cup. Proc Biol Sci 2004; 271 Suppl 3:S64-6. [PMID: 15101421 PMCID: PMC1809987 DOI: 10.1098/rsbl.2003.0100] [Citation(s) in RCA: 48] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Left-handers occur at unexpectedly high frequencies at top levels of many interactive sports. This may occur either because left-handed contestants are innately superior or because they enjoy a negatively frequency-dependent strategic advantage when rare relative to right-handers. We analysed the batting records from the 2003 cricket World Cup and showed that left-handed batsmen were more successful than right-handers, and that the most successful teams had close to 50% left-handed batsmen. We demonstrate that this was because left-handed batsmen have a strategic advantage over bowlers, and that this advantage is greatest over bowlers that are unaccustomed to bowling to left-handers. This provides a clear mechanism for negative frequency-dependent success of left-handed batsmen. Our results may also support a historical role for negative frequency-dependent success in fights and other contests in the maintenance of left-handedness by natural selection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robert Brooks
- School of Biological, Earth and Environmental Sciences, The University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW 2052, Australia.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
37
|
Cameron M, Adams R. Kicking footedness and movement discrimination by elite australian rules footballers. J Sci Med Sport 2003; 6:266-74. [PMID: 14609143 DOI: 10.1016/s1440-2440(03)80020-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Kicking a football accurately with a certain velocity over a desired distance is dependent upon the speed of the kicking foot and the quality of contact between the foot and ball. Control of the swinging lower leg is critical to these factors and the overall performance of the kick, and this was assessed in 20 Australian Football League players. Assessment of movement extent discrimination of the swinging leg was made while standing on the other leg. An automated device accurately set a stop plate to five different positions and each subject was asked to swing the leg to the plate, and make a non-visual judgment of the movement extent. Forty forward and forty backward swings of each leg were assessed, and based on a subject's responses, non-parametric signal detection analysis resulted in a movement extent discrimination score. For left leg performance, coach ratings of overall kicking ability were correlated with movement discrimination ability. Left-foot kickers (0.83) had a higher movement discrimination score than right-foot kickers (0.77), and had a significantly higher score on their dominant side, whereas right-footers had no left-right side difference. Rating of kicking ability by coaches also reflected this difference between the dominant leg performance of left- and right-footers. These data showed left-foot kickers to have higher movement discrimination ability with their dominant leg, and this may be related to their on-field kicking ability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M Cameron
- Sydney Swans Football Club, New South Wales, Australia
| | | |
Collapse
|