1
|
Sağel-Çetiner E, Yılmaz Irmak T, Açık Yavuz B. To tell the truth or not: What effortful control, false belief, and sympathy tell us about preschoolers' instrumental lies. J Exp Child Psychol 2024; 240:105839. [PMID: 38184957 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2023.105839] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/06/2023] [Revised: 09/10/2023] [Accepted: 12/13/2023] [Indexed: 01/09/2024]
Abstract
This study aimed to examine the predictors of instrumental lies in preschool children, specifically focusing on false belief, effortful control, and sympathy. Instrumental lies are intentional falsehoods used to achieve personal goals such as avoiding punishment and obtaining an undeserved reward. A total of 192 preschool children (age range = 32-73 month-olds), along with their mothers and fathers, participated in the study. The Temptation Resistance Paradigm, an experimental task, was employed to elicit instrumental lies from the children. The children also completed first-order false belief measures, and their parents filled out questionnaires assessing their children's effortful control and sympathy skills. Results revealed a positive association between children's effortful control and their decisions to tell instrumental lies. However, no significant relationship was found between false belief and instrumental lying. Age moderated the link between sympathy and the decision to tell instrumental lies, with sympathy being negatively associated with lie-telling behavior among older children but showing no effect among younger children. The study variables did not predict the maintenance of instrumental lies. Overall, this study provides valuable insights into the role of effortful control and sympathy as underlying temperamental and emotional processes influencing children's decisions to engage in instrumental lie-telling.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ece Sağel-Çetiner
- Department of Psychology, Aydın Adnan Menderes University, 09010 Efeler/Aydın, Turkey.
| | | | - Begüm Açık Yavuz
- Department of Psychology, Aydın Adnan Menderes University, 09010 Efeler/Aydın, Turkey
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Dys SP, Jambon M, Buono S, Malti T. Attentional Control Moderates the Relation between Sympathy and Ethical Guilt. J Genet Psychol 2023; 184:198-211. [PMID: 36803666 DOI: 10.1080/00221325.2023.2177522] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/22/2023]
Abstract
In response to ethical transgressions, some children respond with ethical guilt (e.g., remorse), while others do not. The affective and cognitive precursors of ethical guilt have been widely studied on their own, however, few studies have looked at the interaction of affective (e.g., sympathy) and cognitive (e.g., attention) precursors on ethical guilt. This study examined the effects of children's sympathy, attentional control, and their interaction on 4 and 6-year-old children's ethical guilt. A sample of 118 children (50% girls, 4-year-olds: Mage = 4.58, SD = .24, n = 57; 6-year-old: Mage = 6.52, SD = .33, n = 61) completed an attentional control task and provided self-reports of dispositional sympathy and ethical guilt in response to hypothetical ethical violations. Sympathy and attentional control were not directly associated with ethical guilt. Attentional control, however, moderated the relation between sympathy and ethical guilt, such that sympathy was more strongly related to ethical guilt at increasing levels of attentional control. This interaction did not differ between 4- and 6-year-olds or boys and girls. These findings illustrate an interaction between emotion and cognitive processes and suggest that promoting children's ethical development may require a focus on both attentional control and sympathy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sebastian P Dys
- Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, ON, Canada
| | - Marc Jambon
- Department of Psychology, Wilfrid Laurier University, Waterloo, ON, Canada
| | - Stephanie Buono
- Department of Applied Psychology, Ontario Institute for Studies in Education, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Tina Malti
- Centre for Child Development, Mental Health, and Policy, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Li B, Blijd-Hoogewys E, Stockmann L, Vergari I, Rieffe C. Toward feeling, understanding, and caring: The development of empathy in young autistic children. AUTISM : THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF RESEARCH AND PRACTICE 2022:13623613221117955. [PMID: 35999700 DOI: 10.1177/13623613221117955] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
LAY ABSTRACT Empathy is a highly valued human capacity. Yet, autistic people are often portrayed as lacking in empathy. Recent research, which views empathy as a complex construct emerging from multiple interrelated emotional and cognitive processes, argues that, although many autistic people do have difficulty understanding others' emotions, and this may hinder them from responding to others in a prosocial manner, they are not indifferent to other people's feelings. Hoping to contribute to a better understanding of the unique challenges that autistic children face in their empathy development, we followed the development of four empathy abilities: emotion contagion, attention to others, emotion acknowledgment, and prosocial actions, in 1- to 6-year-old autistic children, in comparison with non-autistic children. Once a year, for 4 consecutive years, children's empathy abilities were evaluated by experimenters who acted out emotional episodes to provoke empathy in children, and by parents who filled out empathy questionnaires. We found that autistic children experienced indeed more difficulty attending to others, acknowledging others' emotions, and initiating prosocial actions toward others. However, according to parents, they did not differ from their non-autistic peers in feeling along with others' negative emotions. This indicates that it might not be the case that autistic children did not want to act empathetically toward others. Rather, they might not know how to do so. Notably, despite these difficulties, when looking at children's developmental trajectories, autistic children showed similar improvements over time as non-autistic children. This provides evidence that autistic children have the potential to learn and to improve their empathy skills.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Boya Li
- Leiden University, The Netherlands
| | | | | | | | - Carolien Rieffe
- Leiden University, The Netherlands.,Twente University, The Netherlands.,University College London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Mehrotra M, Dys SP, Song KH, Malti T. Children's Reflection and Sympathy as Predictors of Reparative Behavior. The Journal of Genetic Psychology 2022; 183:222-234. [PMID: 35196963 DOI: 10.1080/00221325.2022.2042179] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
This study investigated the links between reflection, sympathy, and reparative behavior in an ethnically diverse sample of 4-, 6- and 8-year-old children from Canada (N = 752). Primary caregivers responded to questionnaires assessing their children's reflection, sympathy, and reparative behaviors. The links between these variables were examined using structural equation modeling. In accordance with our hypotheses, children's reflection and sympathy were both related to reparation. We did not, however, find any interaction between reflection and sympathy in relation to reparation. These findings suggest that the pathways to reparative behavior through reflection (a cognitive capacity) and sympathy (an affective capacity) are independent. We discuss these findings in relation to the differential roles of cognitive and affective processes in promoting reparative behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mishika Mehrotra
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
| | - Sebastian P Dys
- Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
| | - Keng-Hie Song
- Department of Counseling Psychology, Woosuk University, Jincheon, South Korea
| | - Tina Malti
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.,Centre for Child Development, Mental Health, and Policy, University of Toronto Mississauga, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Harris VW, Anderson J, Visconti B. Social emotional ability development (SEAD): An integrated model of practical emotion-based competencies. MOTIVATION AND EMOTION 2022; 46:226-253. [PMID: 35034996 PMCID: PMC8742702 DOI: 10.1007/s11031-021-09922-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 11/26/2021] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
Social emotional abilities (i.e., specific skills), defined as the set of cognitive abilities, emotion-based knowledge, and behavioral competencies (i.e., skill levels) that facilitate adaptively employing prosocial processes and behaviors (i.e., "actions"), such as emotional regulation and sympathetic and empathetic response behaviors, is contemporarily modeled and measured as emotional intelligence. This conceptualization can be problematic, however, as the two concepts are not the same and traditional methods of measuring emotional intelligence can have limited practical utility. The social emotional ability development (SEAD) theoretical model introduced in this treatise represents a pragmatic and simplified approach to the development of social emotional ability and competency as abstracted from constructs of emotional intelligence, social intelligence, and sociocultural learning theory. Further, the SEAD model reaches beyond the individual as the unit of analysis to explore, conceptualize, differentiate, investigate, and define the hierarchal, bi-directional, and contextual nature of the dimensions of social emotional ability within close relationships. Implications for how the SEAD model can be used by researchers, practitioners, educators, individuals, families, and couples across a broad spectrum of domains and interventions are discussed.
Collapse
|
6
|
Jambon M, Colasante T, Ngo H, Dys S, Malti T. Peer victimization and sympathy development in childhood: The moderating role of emotion regulation. SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT 2020. [DOI: 10.1111/sode.12482] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Marc Jambon
- Department of Psychology Centre for Child Development, Mental Health, and Policy (CCDMP) University of Toronto Mississauga Mississauga ON Canada
| | - Tyler Colasante
- Department of Psychology Centre for Child Development, Mental Health, and Policy (CCDMP) University of Toronto Mississauga Mississauga ON Canada
| | - Hazel Ngo
- Department of Psychology Centre for Child Development, Mental Health, and Policy (CCDMP) University of Toronto Mississauga Mississauga ON Canada
- Department of Applied Psychology & Human Development University of Toronto Mississauga ON Canada
| | - Sebastian Dys
- Department of Psychology Centre for Child Development, Mental Health, and Policy (CCDMP) University of Toronto Mississauga Mississauga ON Canada
| | - Tina Malti
- Department of Psychology Centre for Child Development, Mental Health, and Policy (CCDMP) University of Toronto Mississauga Mississauga ON Canada
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Colasante T, Peplak J, Sette S, Malti T. Understanding the Victimization-Aggression Link in Childhood: The Roles of Sympathy and Resting Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 2019; 50:291-299. [PMID: 30171390 DOI: 10.1007/s10578-018-0841-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
With a sample of 4- and 8-year-olds (N = 131), we tested the extent to which more frequent experiences of victimization were associated with heightened aggression towards others, and how sympathetic concern and resting respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) factored into this relationship. Caregivers reported their children's aggression and sympathy. Children reported their victimization and their resting RSA was calculated from electrocardiogram data in response to a nondescript video. Findings revealed that children who reported more frequent victimization were rated as less sympathetic and, in turn, more aggressive. However, resting RSA moderated this path, such that children with high levels were rated as more versus less sympathetic when they reported less versus more victimization, respectively. Results suggest that considering children's sympathetic tendencies and physiology is important to gain a nuanced understanding of their victimization-related aggression.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tyler Colasante
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Mississauga, Canada.
| | - Joanna Peplak
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Mississauga, Canada
| | - Stefania Sette
- Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
| | - Tina Malti
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Mississauga, Canada.,Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Abramson L, Paz Y, Knafo-Noam A. From negative reactivity to empathic responding: Infants high in negative reactivity express more empathy later in development, with the help of regulation. Dev Sci 2018; 22:e12766. [PMID: 30339317 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12766] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/22/2018] [Accepted: 10/10/2018] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
Empathy has great effect on human well-being, promoting healthy relationships and social competence. Although it is increasingly acknowledged that infants show empathy toward others, individual differences in infants' empathy from the first year of life have rarely been investigated longitudinally. Here we examined how negative reactivity and regulation, two temperament traits that predict empathic responses in older children and adults, relate to infants' empathy. Infants were studied at the ages of nine (N = 275) and 18 (N = 301) months (194 infants were studied at both ages). Empathic responses were assessed by infants' observed reactions to an experimenter's simulated distress. Negative reactivity (fear, sadness, and distress to limitations) and regulation (soothability and effortful control) were assessed by parental reports. Negative reactivity was also examined by infants' observed reactions to an adult stranger (fear) and during interaction with their mothers (displays of sadness/distress). When examined cross-sectionally, infants' fear and distress to limitations associated with self-distress in response to others' distress. In contrast, when examined longitudinally, early sadness and distress to limitations, but not fear, associated with later empathic concern and inquisitiveness. Moreover, this longitudinal relation was moderated by infants' soothability and was evident only for children that had high soothability by the later time-point. Our findings suggest that infants who at an earlier age show negative reactivity, react later in development with more empathy if they achieve sufficient regulation abilities. By that, the findings stress the developmental nature of temperament-empathy relations during infancy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Yael Paz
- Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel
| | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Kienbaum J, Zorzi M, Kunina-Habenicht O. The development of interindividual differences in sympathy: The role of child personality and adults’ responsiveness to distress. SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT 2018. [DOI: 10.1111/sode.12343] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Jutta Kienbaum
- Institute of Psychology; Paedagogische Hochschule Karlsruhe; Karlsruhe Germany
| | - Miriam Zorzi
- Institute of Psychology; Paedagogische Hochschule Karlsruhe; Karlsruhe Germany
| | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
The multidimensional nature of early prosocial behavior: a motivational perspective. Curr Opin Psychol 2018; 20:111-116. [DOI: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.09.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 42] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/22/2017] [Revised: 08/28/2017] [Accepted: 09/08/2017] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
|
11
|
Peplak J, Malti T. “That Really Hurt, Charlie!” Investigating the Role of Sympathy and Moral Respect in Children's Aggressive Behavior. The Journal of Genetic Psychology 2016; 178:89-101. [DOI: 10.1080/00221325.2016.1245178] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Joanna Peplak
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Mississauga, Canada
| | - Tina Malti
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Mississauga, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Paulus M. Friendship trumps neediness: The impact of social relations and others’ wealth on preschool children’s sharing. J Exp Child Psychol 2016; 146:106-20. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.02.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/15/2015] [Revised: 01/30/2016] [Accepted: 02/01/2016] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
|
13
|
Kenward B, Hellmer K, Winter LS, Eriksson M. Four-year-olds' strategic allocation of resources: attempts to elicit reciprocation correlate negatively with spontaneous helping. Cognition 2014; 136:1-8. [PMID: 25490123 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.11.035] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/03/2013] [Revised: 08/28/2014] [Accepted: 11/17/2014] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
Behaviour benefitting others (prosocial behaviour) can be motivated by self-interested strategic concerns as well as by genuine concern for others. Even in very young children such behaviour can be motivated by concern for others, but whether it can be strategically motivated by self-interest is currently less clear. Here, children had to distribute resources in a game in which a rich but not a poor recipient could reciprocate. From four years of age participants strategically favoured the rich recipient, but only when recipients had stated an intention to reciprocate. Six- and eight-year-olds distributed more equally. Children allocating strategically to the rich recipient were less likely to help when an adult needed assistance but was not in a position to immediately reciprocate, demonstrating consistent cross-task individual differences in the extent to which social behaviour is self- versus other-oriented even in early childhood. By four years of age children are capable of strategically allocating resources to others as a tool to advance their own self-interest.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ben Kenward
- Department of Psychology, Uppsala University, Box 1225, 751 42 Uppsala, Sweden.
| | - Kahl Hellmer
- Department of Psychology, Uppsala University, Box 1225, 751 42 Uppsala, Sweden
| | | | - Malin Eriksson
- Department of Psychology, Uppsala University, Box 1225, 751 42 Uppsala, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|