• Reference Citation Analysis
  • v
  • v
  • Find an Article
Find an Article PDF (4602056)   Today's Articles (6066)   Subscriber (49366)
For: Oaksford M, Chater N. Optimal data selection: revision, review, and reevaluation. Psychon Bull Rev 2003;10:289-318. [PMID: 12921410 DOI: 10.3758/bf03196492] [Citation(s) in RCA: 66] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [What about the content of this article? (0)] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Number Cited by Other Article(s)
1
Lefebvre G, Summerfield C, Bogacz R. A Normative Account of Confirmation Bias During Reinforcement Learning. Neural Comput 2021;34:307-337. [PMID: 34758486 DOI: 10.1162/neco_a_01455] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/16/2021] [Accepted: 07/26/2021] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
2
Summerfield C, Parpart P. Normative Principles for Decision-Making in Natural Environments. Annu Rev Psychol 2021;73:53-77. [PMID: 34555286 DOI: 10.1146/annurev-psych-020821-104057] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
3
Nakamura K. Information seeking criteria: artificial intelligence, economics, psychology, and neuroscience. Rev Neurosci 2021;33:31-41. [PMID: 33855841 DOI: 10.1515/revneuro-2020-0137] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/26/2020] [Accepted: 03/19/2021] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
4
Shreeves M, Gugerty L, Moore D. Individual differences in strategy use and performance during fault diagnosis. COGNITIVE RESEARCH-PRINCIPLES AND IMPLICATIONS 2020;5:49. [PMID: 33095326 PMCID: PMC7584695 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-020-00250-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/21/2020] [Accepted: 09/22/2020] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
5
Liefgreen A, Pilditch T, Lagnado D. Strategies for selecting and evaluating information. Cogn Psychol 2020;123:101332. [PMID: 32977167 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2020.101332] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/10/2019] [Revised: 07/08/2020] [Accepted: 07/16/2020] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
6
Smith R, Schwartenbeck P, Parr T, Friston KJ. An Active Inference Approach to Modeling Structure Learning: Concept Learning as an Example Case. Front Comput Neurosci 2020;14:41. [PMID: 32508611 PMCID: PMC7250191 DOI: 10.3389/fncom.2020.00041] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/21/2019] [Accepted: 04/17/2020] [Indexed: 11/13/2022]  Open
7
Donner-Banzhoff N. Solving the Diagnostic Challenge: A Patient-Centered Approach. Ann Fam Med 2018;16:353-358. [PMID: 29987086 PMCID: PMC6037523 DOI: 10.1370/afm.2264] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/06/2017] [Revised: 03/15/2018] [Accepted: 04/12/2018] [Indexed: 12/27/2022]  Open
8
Crupi V, Nelson JD, Meder B, Cevolani G, Tentori K. Generalized Information Theory Meets Human Cognition: Introducing a Unified Framework to Model Uncertainty and Information Search. Cogn Sci 2018;42:1410-1456. [PMID: 29911318 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12613] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/27/2017] [Revised: 03/05/2018] [Accepted: 03/06/2018] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
9
Weidenfeld A, Oberauer K, Hörnig R. Causal and noncausal conditionals: An integrated model of interpretation and reasoning. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2018;58:1479-513. [PMID: 16365951 DOI: 10.1080/02724980443000719] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
10
Friston KJ, Lin M, Frith CD, Pezzulo G, Hobson JA, Ondobaka S. Active Inference, Curiosity and Insight. Neural Comput 2017;29:2633-2683. [PMID: 28777724 DOI: 10.1162/neco_a_00999] [Citation(s) in RCA: 152] [Impact Index Per Article: 21.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
11
Hattori I, Hattori M, Over DE, Takahashi T, Baratgin J. Dual frames for causal induction: the normative and the heuristic. THINKING & REASONING 2017. [DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2017.1316314] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
12
von Sydow M. Towards a pattern-based logic of probability judgements and logical inclusion “fallacies”. THINKING & REASONING 2016. [DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2016.1140678] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
13
Aczel B, Palfi B, Kekecs Z. What is stupid? INTELLIGENCE 2015. [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2015.08.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
14
Howes A, Duggan GB, Kalidindi K, Tseng YC, Lewis RL. Predicting Short-Term Remembering as Boundedly Optimal Strategy Choice. Cogn Sci 2015;40:1192-223. [PMID: 26294328 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12271] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/24/2012] [Revised: 03/20/2015] [Accepted: 04/13/2015] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
15
Oaksford M. Imaging deductive reasoning and the new paradigm. Front Hum Neurosci 2015;9:101. [PMID: 25774130 PMCID: PMC4343022 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00101] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/08/2014] [Accepted: 02/10/2015] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]  Open
16
Act first, think later: the presence and absence of inferential planning in problem solving. Mem Cognit 2014;41:1096-108. [PMID: 23589154 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-013-0318-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
17
Brase GL. The nature of thinking, shallow and deep. Front Psychol 2014;5:435. [PMID: 24860542 PMCID: PMC4030158 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00435] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/30/2013] [Accepted: 04/24/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022]  Open
18
Crupi V, Girotto V. From is to ought, and back: how normative concerns foster progress in reasoning research. Front Psychol 2014;5:219. [PMID: 24659981 PMCID: PMC3952137 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00219] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/29/2013] [Accepted: 02/25/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022]  Open
19
Children’s sequential information search is sensitive to environmental probabilities. Cognition 2014;130:74-80. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.09.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/27/2013] [Revised: 09/24/2013] [Accepted: 09/25/2013] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
20
Oaksford M, Chater N. Dynamic inference and everyday conditional reasoning in the new paradigm. THINKING & REASONING 2013. [DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2013.808163] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
21
Donner-Banzhoff N, Hertwig R. Inductive foraging: Improving the diagnostic yield of primary care consultations. Eur J Gen Pract 2013;20:69-73. [DOI: 10.3109/13814788.2013.805197] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022]  Open
22
Cherubini P, Rusconi P, Russo S, Crippa F. Missing the dog that failed to bark in the nighttime: on the overestimation of occurrences over non-occurrences in hypothesis testing. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2012;77:348-70. [PMID: 22415224 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-012-0430-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/29/2011] [Accepted: 02/26/2012] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
23
Wakebe T, Sato T, Watamura E, Takano Y. Risk aversion in information seeking. JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2012. [DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2011.596825] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/17/2022]
24
Dual processes, probabilities, and cognitive architecture. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2012. [DOI: 10.1007/s11299-011-0096-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/14/2022]
25
Fiedler K. Meta-Cognitive Myopia and the Dilemmas of Inductive-Statistical Inference. PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 2012. [DOI: 10.1016/b978-0-12-394293-7.00001-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/15/2022]
26
The “is-ought fallacy” fallacy. Behav Brain Sci 2011. [DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x11000665] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
27
Bayesian inference for the information gain model. Behav Res Methods 2011;43:297-309. [PMID: 21302022 PMCID: PMC3098374 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-010-0057-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/04/2022]
28
The imaginary fundamentalists: The unshocking truth about Bayesian cognitive science. Behav Brain Sci 2011. [DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x11000239] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
29
The effects of problem content and scientific background on information search and the assessment and valuation of correlations. Mem Cognit 2011;39:107-16. [PMID: 21264572 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-010-0008-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
30
Broadening the study of inductive reasoning: confirmation judgments with uncertain evidence. Mem Cognit 2011;38:941-50. [PMID: 20921106 DOI: 10.3758/mc.38.7.941] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
31
Information Seeking in a Non-Hypothesis Testing Task. PSYCHOLOGICAL STUDIES 2010. [DOI: 10.1007/s12646-010-0046-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]  Open
32
Reverberi C, Cherubini P, Frackowiak RSJ, Caltagirone C, Paulesu E, Macaluso E. Conditional and syllogistic deductive tasks dissociate functionally during premise integration. Hum Brain Mapp 2010;31:1430-45. [PMID: 20112243 PMCID: PMC6871069 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.20947] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/25/2008] [Revised: 10/01/2009] [Accepted: 10/16/2009] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]  Open
33
Gozzi M, Cherubini P, Papagno C, Bricolo E. Recruitment of intuitive versus analytic thinking strategies affects the role of working memory in a gambling task. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2010;75:188-201. [PMID: 20697767 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-010-0296-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/05/2010] [Accepted: 06/20/2010] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
34
Cherubini P, Rusconi P, Russo S, Di Bari S, Sacchi S. Preferences for different questions when testing hypotheses in an abstract task: positivity does play a role, asymmetry does not. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2010;134:162-74. [PMID: 20223439 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.01.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/15/2008] [Revised: 01/08/2010] [Accepted: 01/22/2010] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]  Open
35
Jacobs RA, Kruschke JK. Bayesian learning theory applied to human cognition. WILEY INTERDISCIPLINARY REVIEWS. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 2010;2:8-21. [PMID: 26301909 DOI: 10.1002/wcs.80] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
36
Statistical Information and Uncertainty: A Critique of Applications in Experimental Psychology. ENTROPY 2010. [DOI: 10.3390/e12040720] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
37
Hattori M, Oaksford M. Adaptive Non-Interventional Heuristics for Covariation Detection in Causal Induction: Model Comparison and Rational Analysis. Cogn Sci 2010;31:765-814. [DOI: 10.1080/03640210701530755] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
38
Mandel DR, Vartanian O. Weighting of contingency information in causal judgement: evidence of hypothesis dependence and use of a positive-test strategy. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2009;62:2388-408. [PMID: 19391044 DOI: 10.1080/17470210902794148] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
39
Précis of bayesian rationality: The probabilistic approach to human reasoning. Behav Brain Sci 2009;32:69-84; discussion 85-120. [PMID: 19210833 DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x09000284] [Citation(s) in RCA: 164] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
40
How do individuals reason in the Wason card selection task? Behav Brain Sci 2009. [DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x09000508] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
41
McKenzie CRM. Increased sensitivity to differentially diagnostic answers using familiar materials: implications for confirmation bias. Mem Cognit 2006;34:577-88. [PMID: 16933767 DOI: 10.3758/bf03193581] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
42
McKenzie CRM, Mikkelsen LA. A Bayesian view of covariation assessment. Cogn Psychol 2006;54:33-61. [PMID: 16764849 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.04.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 71] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/28/2005] [Accepted: 04/19/2006] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
43
Nelson JD. Finding useful questions: on Bayesian diagnosticity, probability, impact, and information gain. Psychol Rev 2006;112:979-99. [PMID: 16262476 DOI: 10.1037/0033-295x.112.4.979] [Citation(s) in RCA: 104] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
44
Perham N, Oaksford M. Deontic Reasoning With Emotional Content: Evolutionary Psychology or Decision Theory? Cogn Sci 2005;29:681-718. [DOI: 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_35] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/31/2022]
45
McKenzie CRM. Framing effects in inference tasks--and why they are normatively defensible. Mem Cognit 2005;32:874-85. [PMID: 15673176 DOI: 10.3758/bf03196866] [Citation(s) in RCA: 49] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
46
Lucas E, Ball L. Think-aloud protocols and the selection task: Evidence for relevance effects and rationalisation processes. THINKING & REASONING 2005. [DOI: 10.1080/13546780442000114] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
47
Oaksford M, Moussakowski M. Negations and natural sampling in data selection: Ecological versus heuristic explanations of matching bias. Mem Cognit 2004;32:570-81. [PMID: 15478751 DOI: 10.3758/bf03195848] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
48
McKenzie CR. Rational models as theories - not standards - of behavior. Trends Cogn Sci 2003;7:403-406. [PMID: 12963471 DOI: 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00196-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
49
McKenzie CRM, Amin MB. When wrong predictions provide more support than right ones. Psychon Bull Rev 2002;9:821-8. [PMID: 12613689 DOI: 10.3758/bf03196341] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
PrevPage 1 of 1 1Next
© 2004-2024 Baishideng Publishing Group Inc. All rights reserved. 7041 Koll Center Parkway, Suite 160, Pleasanton, CA 94566, USA