1
|
Younes A, Razmjooyhassankhani N, Waglay A, Mdimagh A, Karboune S. Pilot plant extraction of oligo/polysaccharides from cocoa bean shells and their incorporation into chocolate based formulations. Food Chem 2024; 437:137893. [PMID: 37918159 DOI: 10.1016/j.foodchem.2023.137893] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/26/2023] [Revised: 10/24/2023] [Accepted: 10/26/2023] [Indexed: 11/04/2023]
Abstract
A pilot plant extraction of cocoa bean shell CBS oligo- (hCHO) and polysaccharide (CHO) extracts using alkali isolation (0.5 M KOH) and a commercially available multi-enzymatic product, Depol™ 670L, was performed. Comparisons between laboratory and pilot scale recovery yields and saccharide profiles were assessed. Enrichment of chocolate-based formulations, composed of cocoa and carob, using CBS CHO extracts was investigated for its use as a functional food ingredient without compromising flavor. Sensory panels were held to assess formulations before and after enrichment by rating the intensity and likeness using a 9-point hedonic scale, where attributes consist of chocolate flavor, sweetness, sourness, bitterness, and melting in mouth. Overall, enriched formulations accounted for higher overall acceptance and purchase intent consumer ratings. Factor and boxplot analyses were applied to gain insight and perspective regarding the correlation and interaction between the intensity and likeness attributes, perceived complexity of the sensory attributes, as well as the mean quantitative descriptive responses specified by consumers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Amalie Younes
- Department of Food Science and Agricultural Chemistry, Macdonald Campus, McGill University, Ste Anne de Bellevue, QC H9X3V9, Canada
| | - Nastaran Razmjooyhassankhani
- Department of Food Science and Agricultural Chemistry, Macdonald Campus, McGill University, Ste Anne de Bellevue, QC H9X3V9, Canada
| | - Amanda Waglay
- Department of Food Science and Agricultural Chemistry, Macdonald Campus, McGill University, Ste Anne de Bellevue, QC H9X3V9, Canada
| | - Asma Mdimagh
- Department of Food Science and Agricultural Chemistry, Macdonald Campus, McGill University, Ste Anne de Bellevue, QC H9X3V9, Canada
| | - Salwa Karboune
- Department of Food Science and Agricultural Chemistry, Macdonald Campus, McGill University, Ste Anne de Bellevue, QC H9X3V9, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Olfactory attributes and colors associated with naturalness. Food Qual Prefer 2022. [DOI: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2021.104495] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
|
3
|
Coureaud G, Thomas-Danguin T, Sandoz JC, Wilson DA. Biological constraints on configural odour mixture perception. J Exp Biol 2022; 225:274695. [PMID: 35285471 PMCID: PMC8996812 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.242274] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Animals, including humans, detect odours and use this information to behave efficiently in the environment. Frequently, odours consist of complex mixtures of odorants rather than single odorants, and mixtures are often perceived as configural wholes, i.e. as odour objects (e.g. food, partners). The biological rules governing this 'configural perception' (as opposed to the elemental perception of mixtures through their components) remain weakly understood. Here, we first review examples of configural mixture processing in diverse species involving species-specific biological signals. Then, we present the original hypothesis that at least certain mixtures can be processed configurally across species. Indeed, experiments conducted in human adults, newborn rabbits and, more recently, in rodents and honeybees show that these species process some mixtures in a remarkably similar fashion. Strikingly, a mixture AB (A, ethyl isobutyrate; B, ethyl maltol) induces configural processing in humans, who perceive a mixture odour quality (pineapple) distinct from the component qualities (A, strawberry; B, caramel). The same mixture is weakly configurally processed in rabbit neonates, which perceive a particular odour for the mixture in addition to the component odours. Mice and honeybees also perceive the AB mixture configurally, as they respond differently to the mixture compared with its components. Based on these results and others, including neurophysiological approaches, we propose that certain mixtures are convergently perceived across various species of vertebrates/invertebrates, possibly as a result of a similar anatomical organization of their olfactory systems and the common necessity to simplify the environment's chemical complexity in order to display adaptive behaviours.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gérard Coureaud
- Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon, Team Sensory Neuroethology (ENES), CNRS/INSERM/UCBL1/UJM, 69500 Lyon, France
| | - Thierry Thomas-Danguin
- Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation, Team Flavor, Food Oral Processing and Perception, INRAE, CNRS, Institut Agro Dijon, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, 21000 Dijon, France
| | - Jean-Christophe Sandoz
- Evolution, Genomes, Behavior and Ecology, CNRS, Université Paris-Saclay, IRD, 91190 Gif-sur-Yvette, France
| | - Donald A Wilson
- Department of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, New York University Langone School of Medicine and Nathan S. Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, New York, NY 10016, USA
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Patterson NM, Zhong Y, James BJ, Gant N, Hautus MJ. Effect of basic structural variation, aimed at increasing perceivable textures in model foods, on the perception of textural complexity. Food Qual Prefer 2021. [DOI: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2021.104196] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/15/2022]
|
5
|
Duchamp-Viret P, Boyer J, La Villa F, Coureaud G. Brief olfactory learning drives perceptive sensitivity in newborn rabbits: New insights in peripheral processing of odor mixtures and induction. Physiol Behav 2021; 229:113217. [PMID: 33098882 DOI: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.113217] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/29/2020] [Revised: 09/29/2020] [Accepted: 10/20/2020] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Perception of the wide, complex and moving odor world requires that the olfactory system engages processing mechanisms ensuring detection, discrimination and environment adaptation, as early as the peripheral stages. Odor items are mainly elicited by odorant mixtures which give rise to either elemental or configural perceptions. Here, we first explored the contribution of the peripheral olfactory system to configural and elemental perception through odorant interactions at the olfactory receptor (OR) level. This was done in newborn rabbits, which offer the opportunity to pair peripheral electrophysiology and well characterized behavioral responses to two binary mixtures, AB and A'B', which differ in their component ratio (A: ethyl isobutyrate, B: ethyl maltol), and that rabbit pups respectively perceived configurally and elementally. Second, we studied the influence on peripheral reactivity of the brief but powerful learning of one mixture component (odorant B), conditioned by association with the mammary pheromone (MP), which allowed us to assess the possible implication of the phenomenon called induction in neonatal odor learning. Induction is a plasticity mechanism expected to alter both the peripheral electrophysiological responses to, and perceptual detection threshold of, the conditioned stimulus. The results reveal that perceptual modes are partly rooted in differential peripheral processes, the AB configurally perceived mixture mirroring odorant antagonist interactions at OR level to a lesser extent than the A'B' elementally perceived mixture. Further, the results highlight that a single and brief MP-induced odor learning episode is sufficient to alter peripheral responses to the conditioned stimulus and mixtures including it, and shifts the conditioned stimulus detection threshold towards lower concentrations. Thus, MP-induced odor learning relies on induction phenomenon in newborn rabbits.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Patricia Duchamp-Viret
- Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, CNRS UMR 5292 - INSERM U 1028 - Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Centre Hospitalier Le Vinatier - Bâtiment 462 - Neurocampus, 95 Boulevard Pinel, 69675 Bron Cedex, FRANCE.
| | - Jiasmine Boyer
- Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, CNRS UMR 5292 - INSERM U 1028 - Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Centre Hospitalier Le Vinatier - Bâtiment 462 - Neurocampus, 95 Boulevard Pinel, 69675 Bron Cedex, FRANCE
| | - Florian La Villa
- Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, CNRS UMR 5292 - INSERM U 1028 - Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Centre Hospitalier Le Vinatier - Bâtiment 462 - Neurocampus, 95 Boulevard Pinel, 69675 Bron Cedex, FRANCE
| | - Gérard Coureaud
- Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, CNRS UMR 5292 - INSERM U 1028 - Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Centre Hospitalier Le Vinatier - Bâtiment 462 - Neurocampus, 95 Boulevard Pinel, 69675 Bron Cedex, FRANCE.
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Gazit I, Goldblatt A, Grinstein D, Terkel J. Dogs can detect the individual odors in a mixture of explosives. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2021. [DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2020.105212] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
7
|
Sinding C, Hummel T, Béno N, Prescott J, Bensafi M, Coureaud G, Thomas-Danguin T. Configural memory of a blending aromatic mixture reflected in activation of the left orbital part of the inferior frontal gyrus. Behav Brain Res 2021; 402:113088. [PMID: 33358920 DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.113088] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/24/2020] [Revised: 12/13/2020] [Accepted: 12/16/2020] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
Blending aromatic mixtures components naturally fuse to form a unique odor - a configuration- qualitatively different from each component's odor. Repeated exposure to the components either in the mixture or separately, favors respectively, configural and elemental processings. The neural bases of such processes are still unknown. We examined the brain correlates of the experienced-induced configural processing of a well-known model of binary blending odor mixture, the aromatic pineapple blending (AB, ethyl maltol + ethyl isobutyrate). Before fMRI recording, half of the participants were repeatedly exposed to the mixture (AB, group Gmix), with the other half exposed to its separate components (A and B; Gcomp). During the fMRI recording, all participants were stimulated with the mixture (AB) and the components (A and B). Finally, participants rated the number of odors perceived for each stimulus. Gmix perceived the AB mixture as less complex than did Gcomp. While Gcomp perceived the mixture as more complex than its components, Gmix did not. These results show the presence of experience-induced configural or elemental processing of the AB mixture in each group. Contrasting the brain activity of Gcomp and Gmix, when stimulated with AB, revealed higher activation in the left orbital part of the inferior frontal gyrus. This result sheds light on this area's function, commonly found activated in olfactory studies, and closely connected with the lateral orbitofrontal cortex. We discuss the role of this area as a mediator of configural percepts between temporal and orbitofrontal areas involved in configural memory processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- C Sinding
- Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation, INRAE, CNRS, AgroSup Dijon, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, F-21000, Dijon, France.
| | - T Hummel
- Smell & Taste Clinic, Department of ORL, University of Dresden Medical School, Dresden, Germany
| | - N Béno
- Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation, INRAE, CNRS, AgroSup Dijon, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, F-21000, Dijon, France
| | - J Prescott
- University of Newcastle, School of Psychology, Australia; Università degli Studi di Firenze, Italy
| | - M Bensafi
- Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon, CNRS UMR 5292, INSERM U1028, Université Claude Bernard, Lyon 1, France
| | - G Coureaud
- Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon, CNRS UMR 5292, INSERM U1028, Université Claude Bernard, Lyon 1, France
| | - T Thomas-Danguin
- Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation, INRAE, CNRS, AgroSup Dijon, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, F-21000, Dijon, France
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Luckett CR, Pellegrino R, Heatherly M, Alfaro Martinez K, Dein M, Munafo PJ. Discrimination of Complex Odor Mixtures: A Study Using Wine Aroma Models. Chem Senses 2020; 46:6043126. [PMID: 33347541 DOI: 10.1093/chemse/bjaa079] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Abstract
There are key unanswered questions when it comes to multicomponent odor discrimination. This study was designed to assess discrimination of odorant mixtures that elicit a singular percept. We collected data to address the following two questions: (1) What odor features do humans notice when attempting to discriminate between subtly different odor mixtures? (2) Are odor mixtures easier to discriminate when an odorant is added, compared with when a component is removed? Using modern aroma chemistry techniques, an odor mixture resembling a generic white wine was constructed. This wine odor mixture was modified using a series of three esters which are commonly found in white wines that vary in chain length and branching. Participants performed a sequence of discrimination tasks for the addition/subtraction of modifiers to the base wine at different concentrations. Only one of the esters (ethyl propanoate) led to a discriminable odor mixture. As concentration of the modifying odorant was increased, discrimination of odor mixtures was first reported because of changes in odor mixture familiarity and then intensity. We found similar sensitivity to changes in odor mixtures regardless whether the modifying compound was added or subtracted, suggesting that perceptual stability of odor mixtures is equally dependent on both imputing missing information (pattern completion) and disregarding extraneous information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Curtis R Luckett
- Department of Food Science, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN, USA
| | - Robert Pellegrino
- Department of Food Science, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN, USA
| | | | - Katherine Alfaro Martinez
- Department of Food Science, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN, USA.,Department of Food Science, Zamorano University, Francisco Morazán, Tegucigalpa, Honduras, C.A
| | - Melissa Dein
- Department of Food Science, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN, USA
| | - P John Munafo
- Department of Food Science, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN, USA
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Schifferstein HN, Kudrowitz BM, Breuer C. Food Perception and Aesthetics - Linking Sensory Science to Culinary Practice. JOURNAL OF CULINARY SCIENCE & TECHNOLOGY 2020. [DOI: 10.1080/15428052.2020.1824833] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/13/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | - Barry M. Kudrowitz
- Department of Design, Housing, and Apparel, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
| | - Carola Breuer
- Independent Food & Design Professional, Munich, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Walker SC, Williams K, Moore DJ. Superior Identification of Component Odors in a Mixture Is Linked to Autistic Traits in Children and Adults. Chem Senses 2020; 45:391-399. [PMID: 32249289 DOI: 10.1093/chemse/bjaa026] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/26/2022] Open
Abstract
Most familiar odors are complex mixtures of volatile molecules, which the olfactory system automatically synthesizes into a perceptual whole. However, odors are rarely encountered in isolation; thus, the brain must also separate distinct odor objects from complex and variable backgrounds. In vision, autistic traits are associated with superior performance in tasks that require focus on the local features of a perceptual scene. The aim of the present study was to determine whether the same advantage was observed in the analysis of olfactory scenes. To do this, we compared the ability of 1) 40 young adults (aged 16-35) with high (n = 20) and low levels of autistic traits and 2) 20 children (aged 7-11), with (n = 10) and without an autism spectrum disorder diagnosis, to identify individual odor objects presented within odor mixtures. First, we used a 4-alternative forced choice task to confirm that both adults and children were able to reliably identify 8 blended fragrances, representing food-related odors, when presented individually. We then used the same forced choice format to test participants' ability to identify the odors when they were combined in either binary or ternary mixtures. Adults with high levels of autistic traits showed superior performance on binary but not ternary mixture trials, whereas children with an autism spectrum disorder diagnosis outperformed age-matched neurotypical peers, irrespective of mixture complexity. These findings indicate that the local processing advantages associated with high levels of autistic traits in visual tasks are also apparent in a task requiring analytical processing of odor mixtures.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Susannah C Walker
- Research Centre for Brain and Behaviour, School of Natural Sciences and Psychology, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, UK
| | | | - David J Moore
- Research Centre for Brain and Behaviour, School of Natural Sciences and Psychology, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, UK
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Palczak J, Giboreau A, Rogeaux M, Delarue J. How do pastry and culinary chefs design sensory complexity? Int J Gastron Food Sci 2020. [DOI: 10.1016/j.ijgfs.2019.100182] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
12
|
Albayay J, Castiello U, Parma V. Task-irrelevant odours affect both response inhibition and response readiness in fast-paced Go/No-Go task: the case of valence. Sci Rep 2019; 9:19329. [PMID: 31852962 PMCID: PMC6920346 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-55977-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/26/2019] [Accepted: 12/04/2019] [Indexed: 01/06/2023] Open
Abstract
Whether emotional stimuli influence both response readiness and inhibition is highly controversial. Visual emotional stimuli appear to interfere with both under certain conditions (e.g., task relevance). Whether the effect is generalisable to salient yet task-irrelevant stimuli, such as odours, remains elusive. We tested the effect of orthonasally-presented pleasant (orange) and unpleasant odours (trimethyloxazole and hexenol) and clean air as a control on response inhibition. In emotional Go/No-Go paradigms, we manipulated the intertrial interval and ratios of Go/No-Go trials to account for motor (Experiment 1, N = 31) and cognitive (Experiment 2, N = 29) response inhibition processes. In Experiment 1, participants had greater difficulty in withholding and produced more accurate and faster Go responses under the pleasant vs. the control condition. Faster Go responses were also evident in the unpleasant vs. the control condition. In Experiment 2, neither pleasant nor unpleasant odours modulated action withholding, but both elicited more accurate and faster Go responses as compared to the control condition. Pleasant odours significantly impair action withholding (as compared to the control condition), indicating that more inhibitory resources are required to elicit successful inhibition in the presence of positive emotional information. This modulation was revealed for the motor aspect of response inhibition (fast-paced design with lower Go/No-Go trial ratio) rather than for attentional interference processes. Response readiness is critically impacted by the emotional nature of the odour (but not by its valence). Our findings highlight that the valence of task-irrelevant odour stimuli is a factor significantly influencing response inhibition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Javier Albayay
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Via Venezia 8, 35131, Padova, Italy.
| | - Umberto Castiello
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Via Venezia 8, 35131, Padova, Italy
| | - Valentina Parma
- Department of Psychology, Temple University, 1701 N 13th St, 19122, Philadelphia, PA, United States of America.,Neuroscience Area, International School for Advanced Studies, Via Bonomea 265, 34151, Trieste, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Poupon D, Fernandez P, Archambault Boisvert S, Migneault-Bouchard C, Frasnelli J. Can the Identification of Odorants Within a Mixture Be Trained? Chem Senses 2019; 43:721-726. [PMID: 30260369 DOI: 10.1093/chemse/bjy060] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/06/2023] Open
Abstract
Identifying odors within mixtures is a difficult task: humans are able to recognize only up to 4 odors within a mixture. We wanted to test the effects of olfactory training on this ability. We used 7 odorants to create 35 olfactory stimuli of 1, 2, 3, 4, or 5 odorants. The task consisted of identifying the odorants present within the mixture. We trained novices on this task for 5 days: they came to the laboratory to perform the task once a day before coming back for the final testing. Then, we compared them to sommeliers, thus olfaction experts, and untrained novices. Results showed that sommeliers outperformed the other groups with mixtures of up to 4 odorants but not with mixtures of 5 odorants. The short olfactory training allowed trained participants to perform as well as sommeliers when it came to identifying single odorants but was not enough to improve their performance when stimuli were mixtures of 2 or more odorants. This study supports the idea that the number of odors we can recognize within a mixture is limited but suggests training can improve the performance: a short olfactory training is enough to enhance the ability to identify single odorants, whereas expertise refines identification ability of mixtures of up to 4 odorants.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daphnée Poupon
- Department of Anatomy, Université du Québec à Trois-Rivières, Trois-Rivières (Québec), Canada
| | - Pauline Fernandez
- Institut du Tourisme et d'Hôtellerie du Québec, Montréal (Québec), Canada
| | | | | | - Johannes Frasnelli
- Department of Anatomy, Université du Québec à Trois-Rivières, Trois-Rivières (Québec), Canada.,Research Centre, Sacré Coeur Hospital, Montréal (Québec), Canada
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Johnson AJ, Hjelmeland AK, Heymann H, Ebeler SE. GC-Recomposition-Olfactometry (GC-R) and multivariate study of three terpenoid compounds in the aroma profile of Angostura bitters. Sci Rep 2019; 9:7633. [PMID: 31113980 PMCID: PMC6529406 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-44064-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/10/2019] [Accepted: 05/02/2019] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Foods and beverage aroma results from multicomponent mixtures of volatile compounds present in the food that interact with olfactory receptors and produce a perceptual response in the brain. However, the perceptual interactions that occur when complex odor mixtures are combined are not well understood. Here we used Gas chromatography-Recomposition-Olfactometry (GC-R) to better understand the role that individual compounds have on the perceived sensory aroma of bitters. Bitters are the concentrated alcoholic extract of flavorful plant materials with a wide range of complex sensory and chemical aroma profiles that have not been extensively studied. Previously, we demonstrated that Angostura bitters are characterized by complex aroma attributes described as cola, ginger, orange peel, and black pepper and that the volatile composition of Angostura bitters is predominantly composed of terpenoids. Using GC-R to create in-instrument mixtures of the Angostura headspace extracts, the sensory attributes of Angostura extracts with linalool, α-terpinyl-acetate and caryophyllene omitted were evaluated. The omission experiments demonstrated direct and indirect effects of the individual compounds on the aroma attributes of Angostura bitters, through masking, additive, and synergistic interactions. Caryophyllene in particular, which was present in the headspace extracts at concentration only slightly above sensory threshold levels, had a large and unexpected impact on the sensory properties of the mixtures and may be most responsible for the aromas associated with the whole sample. The GC-R and statistical approaches used here provided valuable tools to reveal relationships among individual compounds and aroma attributes of foods that have not been currently theorized using existing analytical approaches.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Arielle J Johnson
- Department of Viticulture and Enology, University of California, Davis, CA, USA
| | - Anna K Hjelmeland
- Department of Viticulture and Enology, University of California, Davis, CA, USA
| | - Hildegarde Heymann
- Department of Viticulture and Enology, University of California, Davis, CA, USA
| | - Susan E Ebeler
- Department of Viticulture and Enology, University of California, Davis, CA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Morquecho-Campos P, Larsson M, Boesveldt S, Olofsson JK. Achieving Olfactory Expertise: Training for Transfer in Odor Identification. Chem Senses 2019; 44:197-203. [DOI: 10.1093/chemse/bjz007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Paulina Morquecho-Campos
- Division of Human Nutrition and Health, Wageningen University and Research, Wageningen, The Netherlands
| | - Maria Larsson
- Gösta Ekman Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden
| | - Sanne Boesveldt
- Division of Human Nutrition and Health, Wageningen University and Research, Wageningen, The Netherlands
| | - Jonas K Olofsson
- Gösta Ekman Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Sensory complexity and its influence on hedonic responses: A systematic review of applications in food and beverages. Food Qual Prefer 2019. [DOI: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2018.06.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
17
|
Nieminen V, Karjalainen M, Salminen K, Rantala J, Kontunen A, Isokoski P, Müller P, Kallio P, Surakka V, Lekkala J. A compact olfactometer for IMS measurements and testing human perception. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2018. [DOI: 10.1007/s12127-018-0235-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/12/2023]
|
18
|
Rochelle MM, Prévost GJ, Acree TE. Computing Odor Images. JOURNAL OF AGRICULTURAL AND FOOD CHEMISTRY 2018; 66:2219-2225. [PMID: 28285523 DOI: 10.1021/acs.jafc.6b05573] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/06/2023]
Abstract
This perspective examines psychophysical methods that may reveal the algorithms that encode odor images by integrating current data from sensory measurement into a computational model of odor perception. There is evidence that algorithms used by the nervous system to process odor sensations require input from only a few odorants, between three and eight. Furthermore, the number of recognizable odors in foods that contribute anything to the aroma of all foods is approximately 250. This may imply that it is the ratio of a small number of key odorants (KOs) that create a multitude of food odors. Studies with large mixtures of odorants (formulated to be of equal potency) show that a subject's ability to detect individual odorants in these mixtures was vanishingly small. These large mixtures had weak and nondescript but similar odor character. If only a few stimulants are used to represent complex images, it is direct evidence of the simplicity and therefore the tractability of the computational process.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Madeleine M Rochelle
- Food Science Department , Cornell University , 411 Tower Road , Ithaca , New York 14853 , United States
| | - Géraldine Julie Prévost
- Food Science Department , Cornell University , 411 Tower Road , Ithaca , New York 14853 , United States
| | - Terry E Acree
- Food Science Department, 347 , Cornell University , 411 Tower Road Ithaca , New York 14853 , United States
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Romagny S, Coureaud G, Thomas-Danguin T. Key odorants or key associations? Insights into elemental and configural odour processing. FLAVOUR FRAG J 2017. [DOI: 10.1002/ffj.3429] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Sébastien Romagny
- Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation, INRA, CNRS, AgroSup Dijon; Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté; F-21000 Dijon France
| | - Gérard Coureaud
- Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation, INRA, CNRS, AgroSup Dijon; Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté; F-21000 Dijon France
| | - Thierry Thomas-Danguin
- Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation, INRA, CNRS, AgroSup Dijon; Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté; F-21000 Dijon France
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Abstract
Natural olfactory stimuli are volatile-chemical mixtures in which relative perceptual saliencies determine which odor-components are identified. Odor identification also depends on rapid selective adaptation, as shown for 4 odor stimuli in an earlier experimental simulation of natural conditions. Adapt-test pairs of mixtures of water-soluble, distinct odor stimuli with chemical features in common were studied. Identification decreased for adapted components but increased for unadapted mixture-suppressed components, showing compound identities were retained, not degraded to individual molecular features. Four additional odor stimuli, 1 with 2 perceptible odor notes, and an added "water-adapted" control tested whether this finding would generalize to other 4-compound sets. Selective adaptation of mixtures of the compounds (odors): 3 mM benzaldehyde (cherry), 5 mM maltol (caramel), 1 mM guaiacol (smoke), and 4 mM methyl anthranilate (grape-smoke) again reciprocally unmasked odors of mixture-suppressed components in 2-, 3-, and 4-component mixtures with 2 exceptions. The cherry note of "benzaldehyde" (itself) and the shared note of "methyl anthranilate and guaiacol" (together) were more readily identified. The pervasive mixture-component dominance and dynamic perceptual salience may be mediated through peripheral adaptation and central mutual inhibition of neural responses. Originating in individual olfactory receptor variants, it limits odor identification and provides analytic properties for momentary recognition of a few remaining mixture-components.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marion E Frank
- Oral Health & Diagnostic Sciences, School of Dental Medicine, UConn Health, MC 1715, 263 Farmington Avenue, Farmington, CT 06030, USA
| | - Dane B Fletcher
- Oral Health & Diagnostic Sciences, School of Dental Medicine, UConn Health, MC 1715, 263 Farmington Avenue, Farmington, CT 06030, USA
| | - Thomas P Hettinger
- Oral Health & Diagnostic Sciences, School of Dental Medicine, UConn Health, MC 1715, 263 Farmington Avenue, Farmington, CT 06030, USA
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
The smell of terroir! Olfactory discrimination between wines of different grape variety and different terroir. Food Qual Prefer 2017. [DOI: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.12.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
22
|
Abstract
Object recognition is a crucial component of both visual and auditory perception. It is also critical for olfaction. Most odours are composed of 10s or 100s of volatile components, yet they are perceived as unitary perceptual events against a continually shifting olfactory background (ie figure—ground segregation). We argue here that this occurs by rapid central adaptation to background odours combined with a pattern-matching system to recognise discrete sets of spatial and temporal olfactory features—an odour object. We present supporting neuropsychological, learning, and developmental evidence and then describe the neural circuitry which underpins this. The vagaries of an object-recognition approach are then discussed, with emphasis on the putative importance of memory, multimodal representations, and top—down processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Donald A Wilson
- Department of Zoology, University of Oklahoma, Norman, OK 73019, USA
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
|
24
|
Experience shapes our odor perception but depends on the initial perceptual processing of the stimulus. Atten Percept Psychophys 2015; 77:1794-806. [PMID: 25832188 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-015-0883-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The questions of whether configural and elemental perceptions are competitive or exclusive perceptual processes and whether they rely on independent or dependent mechanisms are poorly understood. To examine these questions, we modified perceptual experience through preexposure to mixed or single odors and measured the resulting variation in the levels of configural and elemental perception of target odor mixtures. We used target mixtures that were spontaneously processed in a configural or an elemental manner. The AB binary mixture spontaneously involved the configural perception of a pineapple odor, whereas component A smelled like strawberry and component B smelled like caramel. The CD mixture produced the elemental perceptions of banana (C) and smoky (D) odors. Perceptual experience was manipulated through repeated exposure to either a mixture (AB or CD) or the components (A and B or C and D). The odor typicality rating data recorded after exposure revealed different influences of experience on odor mixtures and single-component perception, depending both on the type of exposure (components or mixture) and the mixture's initial perceptual property (configural or elemental). Although preexposure to A and B decreased the pineapple typicality of the configural AB mixture, preexposure to AB did not modify its odor quality. In contrast, preexposure to the CD elemental mixture induced a quality transfer between the components. These results emphasize the relative plasticity of odor mixture perception, which is prone to experience-induced modulations but depends on the stimulus's initial perceptual properties, suggesting that configural and elemental forms of odor mixture perception rely on rather independent processes.
Collapse
|
25
|
Meier-Dinkel L, Gertheiss J, Müller S, Wesoly R, Mörlein D. Evaluating the performance of sensory quality control: the case of boar taint. Meat Sci 2014; 100:73-84. [PMID: 25310880 DOI: 10.1016/j.meatsci.2014.09.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/04/2014] [Revised: 09/16/2014] [Accepted: 09/17/2014] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Detection of malodours referred to as 'boar taint' in entire male pigs is essential for quality control when refraining piglet castration. This study analysed the sensitivity and specificity of sensory evaluation by trained assessors (n=18) compared to chemical analysis of two marker compounds (androstenone, skatole) in backfat (n=794). Taking the measurement uncertainty into consideration, several cut-off thresholds for chemical analysis were exemplarily evaluated. Using the panel average score, sensitivity and specificity of sensory analysis ranged from 61 to 69% and 77 to 85%, respectively. Performance of individual assessors varied highly (sensitivity: 47 to 86%; specificity: 45 to 88%) and correlated to olfactory acuity to the compounds. According to receiver operating characteristic-curves, the average panel performed better than single assessors regardless of the sensory criterion shift. Agreement plots illustrate that high skatole levels are better detected than high androstenone levels (useful for assessor feedback). The agreement between sensory and chemical analyses was moderate. Assessors need to be selected carefully.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lisa Meier-Dinkel
- Department of Animal Sciences, Meat Quality Section, University of Goettingen, D-37075 Goettingen, Germany
| | - Jan Gertheiss
- Department of Animal Sciences, Biometrics and Bioinformatics Section, University of Goettingen, D-37075 Goettingen, Germany
| | - Simone Müller
- Thuringian State Institute for Agriculture, Section Animal Production, Regional Office Bad Salzungen, D-36433 Bad Salzungen, Germany
| | - Raffael Wesoly
- Institute of Animal Husbandry and Animal Breeding, Behavioral Physiology of Farm Animals, University of Hohenheim, Stuttgart, Germany
| | - Daniel Mörlein
- Department of Animal Sciences, Meat Quality Section, University of Goettingen, D-37075 Goettingen, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Newborn rabbit perception of 6-odorant mixtures depends on configural processing and number of familiar elements. PLoS One 2014; 9:e107560. [PMID: 25248149 PMCID: PMC4172776 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0107560] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/28/2014] [Accepted: 08/19/2014] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Perception of odors, i.e. usually of mixtures of odorants, is elemental (the odorants' odor qualities are perceived in the mixture) or configural (the odor quality of the mixture differs from the one of each odorant). In human adults, the Red Cordial (RC) mixture is a configurally-processed, 6-odorant mixture. It evokes a red cordial odor quality while none of the elements carries that odor. Interestingly, in newborn rabbits, the same RC mixture is weak configurally perceived: the newborns behaviorally respond to all the elements after conditioning to the whole mixture, but not to the mixture after conditioning to a single element. Thus, they perceive in the RC mixture both the odor quality of the RC configuration and the quality of each element. Here, we aimed to determine whether this perception is modulated by quantitative (number of elements) and/or qualitative bits of information (nature of elements) previously learned by the animals. Newborns were conditioned to RC sub-mixtures of different complexity and composition before behavioral testing to RC. Pups generalized their sucking-related response to RC after learning at least 4 odorants. In contrast, after conditioning to sub-mixtures of another 6-odorant mixture, the elementally perceived MV mixture, pups responded to MV after learning one or two odorants. The different generalization to RC and MV mixtures after learning some of their elements is discussed according to three hypotheses: i) the configural perception of RC sub-mixtures, ii) the ratio of familiar/unfamiliar individual information elementally and configurally perceived, iii) the perception of RC becoming purely elemental. The results allow the first hypothesis to be dismissed, while further experiments are required to distinguish between the remaining two.
Collapse
|
27
|
Thomas-Danguin T, Sinding C, Romagny S, El Mountassir F, Atanasova B, Le Berre E, Le Bon AM, Coureaud G. The perception of odor objects in everyday life: a review on the processing of odor mixtures. Front Psychol 2014; 5:504. [PMID: 24917831 PMCID: PMC4040494 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00504] [Citation(s) in RCA: 111] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/11/2014] [Accepted: 05/08/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Smelling monomolecular odors hardly ever occurs in everyday life, and the daily functioning of the sense of smell relies primarily on the processing of complex mixtures of volatiles that are present in the environment (e.g., emanating from food or conspecifics). Such processing allows for the instantaneous recognition and categorization of smells and also for the discrimination of odors among others to extract relevant information and to adapt efficiently in different contexts. The neurophysiological mechanisms underpinning this highly efficient analysis of complex mixtures of odorants is beginning to be unraveled and support the idea that olfaction, as vision and audition, relies on odor-objects encoding. This configural processing of odor mixtures, which is empirically subject to important applications in our societies (e.g., the art of perfumers, flavorists, and wine makers), has been scientifically studied only during the last decades. This processing depends on many individual factors, among which are the developmental stage, lifestyle, physiological and mood state, and cognitive skills; this processing also presents striking similarities between species. The present review gathers the recent findings, as observed in animals, healthy subjects, and/or individuals with affective disorders, supporting the perception of complex odor stimuli as odor objects. It also discusses peripheral to central processing, and cognitive and behavioral significance. Finally, this review highlights that the study of odor mixtures is an original window allowing for the investigation of daily olfaction and emphasizes the need for knowledge about the underlying biological processes, which appear to be crucial for our representation and adaptation to the chemical environment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thierry Thomas-Danguin
- Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation, CNRS UMR6265, INRA UMR1324, Université de Bourgogne Dijon, France
| | - Charlotte Sinding
- Smell and Taste Clinic, Department of Otorhinolaryngoly TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany
| | - Sébastien Romagny
- Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation, CNRS UMR6265, INRA UMR1324, Université de Bourgogne Dijon, France
| | - Fouzia El Mountassir
- Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation, CNRS UMR6265, INRA UMR1324, Université de Bourgogne Dijon, France
| | | | | | - Anne-Marie Le Bon
- Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation, CNRS UMR6265, INRA UMR1324, Université de Bourgogne Dijon, France
| | - Gérard Coureaud
- Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation, CNRS UMR6265, INRA UMR1324, Université de Bourgogne Dijon, France
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Giacalone D, Duerlund M, Bøegh-Petersen J, Bredie WL, Frøst MB. Stimulus collative properties and consumers’ flavor preferences☆. Appetite 2014; 77:20-30. [DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.02.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 51] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/18/2013] [Revised: 01/19/2014] [Accepted: 02/10/2014] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
|
29
|
Lazarowski L, Dorman DC. Explosives detection by military working dogs: Olfactory generalization from components to mixtures. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2014. [DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2013.11.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/15/2023]
|
30
|
Hummel T, Olgun S, Gerber J, Huchel U, Frasnelli J. Brain responses to odor mixtures with sub-threshold components. Front Psychol 2013; 4:786. [PMID: 24167499 PMCID: PMC3807048 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00786] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/03/2013] [Accepted: 10/07/2013] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Although most odorants we encounter in daily life are mixtures of several chemical substances, we still lack significant information on how we perceive and how the brain processes mixtures of odorants. We aimed to investigate the processing of odor mixtures using behavioral measures and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). The odor mixture contained a target odor (ambroxan) in a concentration at which it could be perceived by half of the subjects (sensitive group); the other half could not perceive the odor (insensitive group). In line with previous findings on multi-component odor mixtures, both groups of subjects were not able to distinguish a complex odor mixture containing or not containing the target odor. However, sensitive subjects had stronger activations than insensitive subjects in chemosensory processing areas such as the insula when exposed to the mixture containing the target odor. Furthermore, the sensitive group exhibited larger brain activations when presented with the odor mixture containing the target odor compared to the odor mixture without the target odor; this difference was smaller, though present for the insensitive group. In conclusion, we show that a target odor presented within a mixture of odors can influence brain activations although on a psychophysical level subjects are not able to distinguish the mixture with and without the target. On the practical side these results suggest that the addition of a certain compound to a mixture of odors may not be detected on a cognitive level; however, this additional odor may significantly change the cerebral processing of this mixture. In this context, FMRI offers unique possibilities to look at the subliminal effects of odors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas Hummel
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Technical University of Dresden Medical School Dresden, Germany
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
31
|
Deegan KC, Heikintalo N, Ritvanen T, Putkonen T, Rekonen J, McSweeney PL, Alatossava T, Tuorila H. Effects of low-pressure homogenisation on the sensory and chemical properties of Emmental cheese. INNOV FOOD SCI EMERG 2013. [DOI: 10.1016/j.ifset.2013.04.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
32
|
Sensory evaluation of boar loins: Trained assessors' olfactory acuity affects the perception of boar taint compounds. Meat Sci 2013; 94:19-26. [DOI: 10.1016/j.meatsci.2012.12.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/20/2012] [Revised: 12/03/2012] [Accepted: 12/10/2012] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
|
33
|
Stevenson RJ. Olfactory perception, cognition, and dysfunction in humans. WILEY INTERDISCIPLINARY REVIEWS. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 2013; 4:273-284. [PMID: 26304205 DOI: 10.1002/wcs.1224] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
The main functions of olfaction relate to finding food, avoiding predators and disease, and social communication. Its role in detecting food has resulted in a unique dual mode sensory system. Environmental odorants are 'smelled' via the external nostrils, while volatile chemicals in food-detected by the same receptors-arrive via the nasopharynx, contributing to flavor. This arrangement allows the brain to link the consequences of eating with a food's odor, and then later to use this information in the search for food. Recognizing an odorant-a food, mate, or predator-requires the detection of complex chemical blends against a noisy chemical background. The brain solves this problem in two ways. First, by rapid adaptation to background odorants so that new odorants stand out. Second, by pattern matching the neural representation of an odorant to prior olfactory experiences. This account is consistent with olfactory sensory physiology, anatomy, and psychology. Odor perception, and its products, may be subject to further processing-olfactory cognition. While olfactory cognition has features in common with visual or auditory cognition, several aspects are unique, and even those that are common may be instantiated in different ways. These differences can be productively used to evaluate the generality of models of cognition and consciousness. Finally, the olfactory system can breakdown, and this may be predictive of the onset of neurodegenerative conditions such as Alzheimer's, as well as having prognostic value in other disorders such as schizophrenia. WIREs Cogn Sci 2013, 4:273-284. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1224 For further resources related to this article, please visit the WIREs website.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Richard J Stevenson
- Department of Psychology, Macquarie University, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Sinding C, Thomas-Danguin T, Chambault A, Béno N, Dosne T, Chabanet C, Schaal B, Coureaud G. Rabbit neonates and human adults perceive a blending 6-component odor mixture in a comparable manner. PLoS One 2013; 8:e53534. [PMID: 23341948 PMCID: PMC3547025 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0053534] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/18/2012] [Accepted: 12/03/2012] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Young and adult mammals are constantly exposed to chemically complex stimuli. The olfactory system allows for a dual processing of relevant information from the environment either as single odorants in mixtures (elemental perception) or as mixtures of odorants as a whole (configural perception). However, it seems that human adults have certain limits in elemental perception of odor mixtures, as suggested by their inability to identify each odorant in mixtures of more than 4 components. Here, we explored some of these limits by evaluating the perception of three 6-odorant mixtures in human adults and newborn rabbits. Using free-sorting tasks in humans, we investigated the configural or elemental perception of these mixtures, or of 5-component sub-mixtures, or of the 6-odorant mixtures with modified odorants' proportion. In rabbit pups, the perception of the same mixtures was evaluated by measuring the orocephalic sucking response to the mixtures or their components after conditioning to one of these stimuli. The results revealed that one mixture, previously shown to carry the specific odor of red cordial in humans, was indeed configurally processed in humans and in rabbits while the two other 6-component mixtures were not. Moreover, in both species, such configural perception was specific not only to the 6 odorants included in the mixture but also to their respective proportion. Interestingly, rabbit neonates also responded to each odorant after conditioning to the red cordial mixture, which demonstrates their ability to perceive elements in addition to configuration in this complex mixture. Taken together, the results provide new insights related to the processing of relatively complex odor mixtures in mammals and the inter-species conservation of certain perceptual mechanisms; the results also revealed some differences in the expression of these capacities between species putatively linked to developmental and ecological constraints.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | - Noelle Béno
- Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation, Dijon, France
| | - Thibaut Dosne
- Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation, Dijon, France
| | - Claire Chabanet
- Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation, Dijon, France
| | - Benoist Schaal
- Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation, Dijon, France
| | - Gérard Coureaud
- Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation, Dijon, France
- * E-mail: (TTD); (GC)
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
Lenochová P, Vohnoutová P, Roberts SC, Oberzaucher E, Grammer K, Havlíček J. Psychology of fragrance use: perception of individual odor and perfume blends reveals a mechanism for idiosyncratic effects on fragrance choice. PLoS One 2012; 7:e33810. [PMID: 22470479 PMCID: PMC3314678 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0033810] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/14/2011] [Accepted: 02/16/2012] [Indexed: 01/10/2023] Open
Abstract
Cross-culturally, fragrances are used to modulate body odor, but the psychology of fragrance choice has been largely overlooked. The prevalent view is that fragrances mask an individual's body odor and improve its pleasantness. In two experiments, we found positive effects of perfume on body odor perception. Importantly, however, this was modulated by significant interactions with individual odor donors. Fragrances thus appear to interact with body odor, creating an individually-specific odor mixture. In a third experiment, the odor mixture of an individual's body odor and their preferred perfume was perceived as more pleasant than a blend of the same body odor with a randomly-allocated perfume, even when there was no difference in pleasantness between the perfumes. This indicates that fragrance use extends beyond simple masking effects and that people choose perfumes that interact well with their own odor. Our results provide an explanation for the highly individual nature of perfume choice.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pavlína Lenochová
- Department of Anthropology, Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic
| | - Pavla Vohnoutová
- Department of Anthropology, Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic
| | - S. Craig Roberts
- School of Natural Sciences, University of Stirling, Stirling, Scotland, United Kingdom
| | | | - Karl Grammer
- Department of Anthropology, University Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| | - Jan Havlíček
- Department of Anthropology, Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Ferreira V. Revisiting psychophysical work on the quantitative and qualitative odour properties of simple odour mixtures: a flavour chemistry view. Part 2: qualitative aspects. A review. FLAVOUR FRAG J 2011. [DOI: 10.1002/ffj.2091] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Vicente Ferreira
- Laboratory for Aroma Analysis and Enology; Aragón Institute of Engineering Research (I3A); Department of Analytical Chemistry, Faculty of Sciences; University of Zaragoza; 50009; Zaragoza; Spain
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
van Beilen M, Bult H, Renken R, Stieger M, Thumfart S, Cornelissen F, Kooijman V. Effects of visual priming on taste-odor interaction. PLoS One 2011; 6:e23857. [PMID: 21969852 PMCID: PMC3182162 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0023857] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/05/2011] [Accepted: 07/26/2011] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Little is known about the influence of visual characteristics other than colour on flavor perception, and the complex interactions between more than two sensory modalities. This study focused on the effects of recognizability of visual (texture) information on flavor perception of odorized sweet beverages. Participants rated the perceived sweetness of odorized sucrose solutions in the presence or absence of either a congruent or incongruent visual context. Odors were qualitatively reminiscent of sweet foods (strawberry and caramel) or not (savoury). Visual context was either an image of the same sweet foods (figurative context) or a visual texture derived from this product (non-figurative context). Textures were created using a texture synthesis method that preserved perceived food qualities while removing object information. Odor-taste combinations were rated sweeter within a figurative than a non-figurative context. This behaviour was exhibited for all odor-taste combinations, even in trials without images, indicating sustained priming by figurative visual context. A non-figurative context showed a transient sweetening effect. Sweetness was generally enhanced most by the strawberry odor. We conclude that the degree of recognizability of visual information (figurative versus non-figurative), influences flavor perception differently. Our results suggest that this visual context priming is mediated by separate sustained and transient processes that are differently evoked by figurative and non-figurative visual contexts. These components operate independent of the congruency of the image-odor-taste combinations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marije van Beilen
- Laboratory for Experimental Ophthalmology, University Medical Center, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
38
|
Frank ME, Goyert HF, Hettinger TP. Time and intensity factors in identification of components of odor mixtures. Chem Senses 2010; 35:777-87. [PMID: 20720093 DOI: 10.1093/chemse/bjq078] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Identification of odors of compounds introduced into changeable olfactory environments is the essence of olfactory coding, which focuses perception on the latest stimulus with the greatest salience. Effects of stimulus intensity and adapting time on mixture component identification after adapting with one component were each studied in 10 human subjects. Odors of 1 and 5 mM vanillin (vanilla) and phenethyl alcohol (rose) were identified, with adapting time varied by sniffing naturally once or twice, or sniffing 5 times, once every 2 s. Odors of water-adapted single compounds were identified nearly perfectly (94%), self-adapted to 51% but did not cross-adapt (94%), showing the 2 compounds had quickly adapting independent odors. Identifications of the vanilla and rose odors in water-adapted mixtures were reduced to 59% and 79%, respectively. Following single-component adaptation, the average 33% identification of odors of adapted (ambient) mixture components contrasted with the greater average 86% identification of new unadapted (extra) mixture components. Identifications were lower for 1 than 5 mM components when concentrations were not matched, and ambient component identifications were lower after 10-s adaptation than after 1 or 2 sniffs. Rapid selective adaptation and mixture component suppression manipulate effective intensity to promote emergence of characteristic odor qualities in dynamic natural settings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marion E Frank
- Department of Oral Health and Diagnostic Sciences, Division of Periodontology, Center for Chemosensory Sciences, School of Dental Medicine, University of Connecticut Health Center, 263 Farmington Avenue, Farmington, CT 06030-1715, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
39
|
|
40
|
Honeybees learn odour mixtures via a selection of key odorants. PLoS One 2010; 5:e9110. [PMID: 20161714 PMCID: PMC2817008 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009110] [Citation(s) in RCA: 79] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/06/2009] [Accepted: 01/19/2010] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Background The honeybee has to detect, process and learn numerous complex odours from her natural environment on a daily basis. Most of these odours are floral scents, which are mixtures of dozens of different odorants. To date, it is still unclear how the bee brain unravels the complex information contained in scent mixtures. Methodology/Principal Findings This study investigates learning of complex odour mixtures in honeybees using a simple olfactory conditioning procedure, the Proboscis-Extension-Reflex (PER) paradigm. Restrained honeybees were trained to three scent mixtures composed of 14 floral odorants each, and then tested with the individual odorants of each mixture. Bees did not respond to all odorants of a mixture equally: They responded well to a selection of key odorants, which were unique for each of the three scent mixtures. Bees showed less or very little response to the other odorants of the mixtures. The bees' response to mixtures composed of only the key odorants was as good as to the original mixtures of 14 odorants. A mixture composed of the other, non-key-odorants elicited a significantly lower response. Neither an odorant's volatility or molecular structure, nor learning efficiencies for individual odorants affected whether an odorant became a key odorant for a particular mixture. Odorant concentration had a positive effect, with odorants at high concentration likely to become key odorants. Conclusions/Significance Our study suggests that the brain processes complex scent mixtures by predominantly learning information from selected key odorants. Our observations on key odorant learning lend significant support to previous work on olfactory learning and mixture processing in honeybees.
Collapse
|
41
|
Le Berre E, Beno N, Ishii A, Chabanet C, Etievant P, Thomas-Danguin T. Just Noticeable Differences in Component Concentrations Modify the Odor Quality of a Blending Mixture. Chem Senses 2008; 33:389-95. [DOI: 10.1093/chemse/bjn006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 66] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
|
42
|
Abstract
Many affective stimuli are hedonically complex mixtures containing both pleasant and unpleasant components. To investigate whether the brain represents the overall affective value of such complex stimuli, or the affective value of the different components simultaneously, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging to measure brain activations to a pleasant odor (jasmine), an unpleasant odor (indole), and a mixture of the two that was pleasant. In brain regions that represented the pleasantness of the odors such as the medial orbitofrontal cortex (as shown by activations that correlated with the pleasantness ratings), the mixture produced activations of similar magnitude to the pleasant jasmine, but very different from the unpleasant indole. These regions thus emphasize the pleasant aspects of the mixture. In contrast, in regions representing the unpleasantness of odors such as the dorsal anterior cingulate and midorbitofrontal cortex the mixture produced activations that were relatively further from the pleasant component jasmine and closer to the indole. These regions thus emphasize the unpleasant aspects of the mixture. Thus mixtures that are found pleasant can have components that are separately pleasant and unpleasant, and the brain can separately and simultaneously represent the positive and negative hedonic value of a complex affective stimulus that contains both pleasant and unpleasant olfactory components. This type of representation may be important for affective decision making in the brain in that separate representations of different affective components of the same sensory stimulus may provide the inputs for making a decision about whether to choose the stimulus or not.
Collapse
|
43
|
Abstract
Many affective stimuli are hedonically complex mixtures containing both pleasant and unpleasant components. To investigate whether the brain represents the overall affective value of such complex stimuli, or the affective value of the different components simultaneously, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging to measure brain activations to a pleasant odor (jasmine), an unpleasant odor (indole), and a mixture of the two that was pleasant. In brain regions that represented the pleasantness of the odors such as the medial orbitofrontal cortex (as shown by activations that correlated with the pleasantness ratings), the mixture produced activations of similar magnitude to the pleasant jasmine, but very different from the unpleasant indole. These regions thus emphasize the pleasant aspects of the mixture. In contrast, in regions representing the unpleasantness of odors such as the dorsal anterior cingulate and midorbitofrontal cortex the mixture produced activations that were relatively further from the pleasant component jasmine and closer to the indole. These regions thus emphasize the unpleasant aspects of the mixture. Thus mixtures that are found pleasant can have components that are separately pleasant and unpleasant, and the brain can separately and simultaneously represent the positive and negative hedonic value of a complex affective stimulus that contains both pleasant and unpleasant olfactory components. This type of representation may be important for affective decision making in the brain in that separate representations of different affective components of the same sensory stimulus may provide the inputs for making a decision about whether to choose the stimulus or not.
Collapse
|
44
|
|
45
|
Goyert HF, Frank ME, Gent JF, Hettinger TP. Characteristic component odors emerge from mixtures after selective adaptation. Brain Res Bull 2007; 72:1-9. [PMID: 17303501 PMCID: PMC1913636 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2006.12.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 43] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/10/2006] [Revised: 12/20/2006] [Accepted: 12/20/2006] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Humans cannot reliably identify the distinctive characteristic odors of components in mixtures containing more than three compounds. In the present study, we demonstrate that selective adaptation can improve component identification. Characteristic component odors, lost in mixtures, were identifiable after presenting other mixture constituents for a few seconds. In mixtures of vanillin, isopropyl alcohol, l-menthol and phenethyl alcohol, this rapid selective adaptation unmasked each component. We suggest that these findings relate directly to how olfactory qualities are coded: olfactory receptors do not act as detectors of isolated molecular features, but likely recognize entire molecules closely associated with perceived olfactory qualities or "notes". Rapid and focused activation of a few distinct receptor types may dominate most odor percepts, emphasizing the importance of many dynamic and specific neural signals. An interaction between two fundamental coding strategies, mixture suppression and selective adaptation, with hundreds of potential olfactory notes, explains humans experiencing the appearance and disappearance of identifiable odors against ambient mixture backgrounds.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Marion E. Frank
- Center for Neurosciences, Department of Oral Health and Diagnostic Sciences, University of Connecticut Health Center, Farmington, CT
- *To whom correspondence should be addressed; e-mail: , telephone: 1-860-679-3354, fax: 1-860-679-4760, Dr. Marion E. Frank, Center for Neurosciences, Oral Health and Diagnostic Sciences, University of Connecticut Health Center, Farmington, CT 06030-1605, USA
| | - Janneane F. Gent
- Center for Perinatal, Pediatric and Environmental Epidemiology, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT USA
| | - Thomas P. Hettinger
- Center for Neurosciences, Department of Oral Health and Diagnostic Sciences, University of Connecticut Health Center, Farmington, CT
| |
Collapse
|
46
|
The mental context for the description of odors: a semantic space. EUROPEAN REVIEW OF APPLIED PSYCHOLOGY-REVUE EUROPEENNE DE PSYCHOLOGIE APPLIQUEE 2006. [DOI: 10.1016/j.erap.2005.09.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
|
47
|
Cashion L, Livermore A, Hummel T. Odour suppression in binary mixtures. Biol Psychol 2006; 73:288-97. [PMID: 16822605 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.05.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/03/2005] [Revised: 05/23/2006] [Accepted: 05/23/2006] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
It has been suggested that odours causing stronger trigeminal activation suppress weaker trigeminal stimuli and that mixed olfactory-trigeminal stimuli suppress odorants that only activate one of these systems. Volunteer normosmic participants (n=20) were exposed to six odorants with varying trigeminal impact to test the hypothesis that more intense "trigeminal" odorants would suppress weaker trigeminal stimuli in binary odour mixtures. It was also hypothesised that stronger trigeminal odorants would dominate six-odour mixtures. The predicted linear pattern of suppression was not seen, with a quadratic model emerging from the data. Stronger trigeminal stimuli failed to dominate six-odour mixtures. Despite the fact that the major hypothesis was not supported, it can be hypothesised from this experiment that the effect of suppression in binary mixtures is reliant upon two major effects: (1) the association formed between odours and the multiple memory systems that they interact with during the encoding and recognition processes, and (2) the balance between activation of the olfactory and trigeminal systems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Larry Cashion
- School of Health Sciences, Charles Darwin University, Darwin, NT 0909, Australia
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
48
|
Park D, Sung H. Malehynobius leechii(amphibia: Hynobiidae) discriminate female reproductive states based on chemical cues. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2006. [DOI: 10.1080/17386357.2006.9647295] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
|
49
|
Cook DJ, Hollowood TA, Linforth RST, Taylor AJ. Correlating instrumental measurements of texture and flavour release with human perception. Int J Food Sci Technol 2005. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1365-2621.2005.00973.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
50
|
Saint-Eve A, Paçi Kora E, Martin N. Impact of the olfactory quality and chemical complexity of the flavouring agent on the texture of low fat stirred yogurts assessed by three different sensory methodologies. Food Qual Prefer 2004. [DOI: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2003.09.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 86] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|