451
|
Mercado R, Constantoyannis C, Mandat T, Kumar A, Schulzer M, Stoessl AJ, Honey CR. Expectation and the placebo effect in Parkinson's disease patients with subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation. Mov Disord 2006; 21:1457-61. [PMID: 16721750 DOI: 10.1002/mds.20935] [Citation(s) in RCA: 86] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
To determine whether the degree to which a patient with Parkinson's disease expects therapeutic benefit from subthalamic nucleus-deep brain stimulation (STN-DBS) influences the magnitude of his or her improved motor response, 10 patients with idiopathic Parkinson's and bilateral STN-DBS were tested after a 12-hour period off medication and stimulation. Four consecutive UPDRS III scores were performed in the following conditions: (a) stimulation OFF, patient aware; (b) stimulation OFF, patient blind; (c) stimulation ON, patient aware; and (d) stimulation ON, patient blind. Statistical significance (P = 0.0001) was observed when comparing main effect ON versus OFF (mean ON: 32.55; mean OFF: 49.15). When the stimulation was OFF, patients aware of this condition had higher UPDRS motor scores than when they were blinded (mean: 50.7 vs. 47.6). With the stimulation ON, UPDRS motor scores were lower when the patients were aware of the stimulation compared with when they were blinded (mean: 30.6 vs. 34.5). The interaction between these levels was significant (P = 0.049). This variation was important for bradykinesia and was not significant for tremor and rigidity. The authors conclude that the information about the condition of the stimulation enhanced the final clinical effect in opposite directions. The results presented support the role of expectation and placebo effects in STN-DBS in Parkinson's disease patients.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rodrigo Mercado
- Surgical Centre for Movement Disorders, University of British Columbia, British Columbia, Canada
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
452
|
Bingel U, Lorenz J, Schoell E, Weiller C, Büchel C. Mechanisms of placebo analgesia: rACC recruitment of a subcortical antinociceptive network. Pain 2005; 120:8-15. [PMID: 16364549 DOI: 10.1016/j.pain.2005.08.027] [Citation(s) in RCA: 441] [Impact Index Per Article: 23.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/31/2005] [Revised: 07/19/2005] [Accepted: 08/18/2005] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
Placebo analgesia is one of the most striking examples of the cognitive modulation of pain perception and the underlying mechanisms are finally beginning to be understood. According to pharmacological studies, the endogenous opioid system is essential for placebo analgesia. Recent functional imaging data provides evidence that the rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC) represents a crucial cortical area for this type of endogenous pain control. We therefore hypothesized that placebo analgesia recruits other brain areas outside the rACC and that interactions of the rACC with these brain areas mediate opioid-dependent endogenous antinociception as part of a top-down mechanism. Nineteen healthy subjects received and rated painful laser stimuli to the dorsum of both hands, one of them treated with a fake analgesic cream (placebo). Painful stimulation was preceded by an auditory cue, indicating the side of the next laser stimulation. BOLD-responses to the painful laser-stimulation during the placebo and no-placebo condition were assessed using event-related fMRI. After having confirmed placebo related activity in the rACC, a connectivity analysis identified placebo dependent contributions of rACC activity with bilateral amygdalae and the periaqueductal gray (PAG). This finding supports the view that placebo analgesia depends on the enhanced functional connectivity of the rACC with subcortical brain structures that are crucial for conditioned learning and descending inhibition of nociception.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- U Bingel
- NeuroImage Nord, Institute for Systems Neuroscience, University Medical Center Hamburg Eppendorf, Germany NeuroImage Nord, Department of Neurology, University Medical Center Hamburg Eppendorf, Germany Department of Physiology, University Medical Center Hamburg Eppendorf, Germany Department of Neurology, University of Freiburg, Germany
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
453
|
To what should we attribute the effects of OMT? INT J OSTEOPATH MED 2005. [DOI: 10.1016/j.ijosm.2005.09.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
454
|
Finniss DG, Benedetti F. The Neural Matrix of Pain Processing and Placebo Analgesia: Implications for Clinical Practice. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2005. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1743-5013.2005.00023.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
455
|
Lanotte M, Lopiano L, Torre E, Bergamasco B, Colloca L, Benedetti F. Expectation enhances autonomic responses to stimulation of the human subthalamic limbic region. Brain Behav Immun 2005; 19:500-9. [PMID: 16055306 DOI: 10.1016/j.bbi.2005.06.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/14/2005] [Revised: 06/08/2005] [Accepted: 06/13/2005] [Indexed: 12/26/2022] Open
Abstract
Recent studies show that the placebo component of a treatment can be investigated by administering therapies either overtly or covertly, without the administration of any placebo. Here, we analyze the effects of open (i.e., expected) versus hidden (i.e., unexpected) stimulations of the human subthalamic region on autonomic responses in Parkinson patients. To do this, we mapped the whole subthalamic region, from the dorsal to the ventral part, and recorded both heart rate and sympathetic responses by using spectral analysis of heart rate variability. We found that open stimulations were more effective than hidden ones only in the ventral subthalamic region, whereas no difference between the two conditions was found in the dorsal aspect. By analyzing the stimulus-response curves in the dorsal, middle, and ventral subthalamic regions, we found that the autonomic response threshold was higher in the hidden than open condition for both heart rate and sympathetic responses only in the ventral part. As this ventralmost portion of the subthalamic region is involved in associative-limbic functions, these data suggest that expectation enhances autonomic responses only if these are elicited in the limbic system. These results extend previous findings on the open-hidden paradigm in deep brain stimulation [Benedetti, F., Colloca, L., Lanotte, M., Bergamasco, B., Torre, E., Lopiano, L., 2004a. Autonomic and emotional responses to open and hidden stimulations of the human subthalamic region. Brain Res. Bull. 63, 203-211.], and indicate that expectation plays a major role in the therapeutic outcome. In light of the interactions between the sympathetic adrenergic system and the immune system, the open-hidden difference in autonomic responses might be relevant to the understanding of how expectations might affect the immune system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michele Lanotte
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Turin Medical School, Turin, Italy
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
456
|
Tracey I. The Neural Matrix of Pain Processing and Placebo Analgesia: Evidence from Functional Imaging. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2005. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1743-5013.2005.00022.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/03/2023]
|
457
|
Abstract
A common feature of research investigating the placebo effect is deception of research participants about the nature of the research. Miller and colleagues examine the ethical issues surrounding such deception.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Franklin G Miller
- Department of Clinical Bioethics, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
458
|
Abstract
Considerable progress has been made in our understanding of the neurobiological mechanisms of the placebo effect, and most of our knowledge originates from the field of pain and analgesia. Today, the placebo effect represents a promising model that could allow us to shed new light on mind-body interactions. The mental events induced by placebo administration can activate mechanisms that are similar to those activated by drugs, which indicates a similarity between psychosocial and pharmacodynamic effects. These new neurobiological advances are already changing our conception of how clinical trials and medical practice must be viewed and conducted.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Luana Colloca
- Department of Neuroscience, Clinical and Applied Physiology Program, University of Turin Medical School, Corso Raffaello 30, 10125 Turin, Italy
| | | |
Collapse
|
459
|
Svedman P, Ingvar M, Gordh T. "Anxiebo", placebo, and postoperative pain. BMC Anesthesiol 2005; 5:9. [PMID: 15982414 PMCID: PMC1187870 DOI: 10.1186/1471-2253-5-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/24/2004] [Accepted: 06/27/2005] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Surgical treatment and its consequences expose patients to stress, and here we investigated the importance of the psychological component of postoperative pain based on reports in the clinical literature. Discussion Postoperative pain remains a significant clinical problem. Increased pain intensity with increased demand for opioid medication, and/or a relative unresponsiveness to pain treatment was reported both when the analgesia was administered by means of conventional nurse injection regimes and patient-controlled analgesia (PCA). Both the quality of the analgesia, and the sensitivity of postoperative models for assessing analgesic efficacy could be significantly influenced. The findings could be explained by increased penetration of an algesic anxiety-related nocebo influence (which we chose to call "anxiebo") relative to its analgesic placebo counterpart. To counteract this influence, the importance of psychological effects must be acknowledged, and doctors and attending nurses should focus on maintaining trustful therapist-patient relationships throughout the treatment period. The physical mechanism of anxiebo should be further explored, and those at risk for anxiebo better characterized. In addition, future systemic analgesic therapies should be directed towards being prophylactic and continuous to eliminate surgical pain as it appears in order to prevent the anxiebo effect. Addressing anxiebo is the key to developing reproducible models for measuring pain in the postoperative setting, and to improving the accuracy of measurements of the minimum effective analgesic concentration. Summary Anxiebo and placebo act as counterparts postoperatively. The anxiebo state may impair clinical analgesia and reduce the sensitivity of analgesic trials. Ways to minimize anxiebo are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paul Svedman
- Department of Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery, Malmö University Hospital, Malmö, Lund University, Sweden
| | - Martin Ingvar
- Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Cognitive Neurophysiology Research Group, Karolinska Institute, Stockholm, Sweden
| | - Torsten Gordh
- Department of Anaesthesiology and Intensive Care and Multidisciplinary Pain Center, Uppsala University Hospital, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
460
|
Enck P, Klosterhalfen S. The placebo response in functional bowel disorders: perspectives and putative mechanisms. Neurogastroenterol Motil 2005; 17:325-31. [PMID: 15916619 DOI: 10.1111/j.1365-2982.2005.00676.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 75] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/15/2022]
Abstract
The nature and determinants of the placebo response are widely unknown, as are the underlying psychological and biological mechanisms. High placebo response rates in functional bowel disorders (functional dyspepsia, irritable bowel syndrome) are similar to those in non-intestinal diseases (depression, pain, Parkinson's disease) and not too dissimilar to other organic gastrointestinal diseases (duodenal ulcer, inflammatory bowel diseases). Methodological reasons (regression to the mean, shift in signal detection through manipulation of expectations) and psycho-biological mechanisms (Pavlovian conditioning of biological processes) are proposed to explain a large component of the response variance in clinical trials. Psychobiological mechanisms of the placebo response in functional and organic diseases can also be identified in brain function studies (such as imaging).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- P Enck
- Department of Internal Medicine VI/Psychosomatics, University Hospitals Tuebingen, Tuebingen, Germany.
| | | |
Collapse
|
461
|
Goebel MU, Hübell D, Kou W, Janssen OE, Katsarava Z, Limmroth V, Schedlowski M. Behavioral conditioning with interferon beta-1a in humans. Physiol Behav 2005; 84:807-14. [PMID: 15885259 DOI: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.03.021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/26/2004] [Revised: 01/22/2005] [Accepted: 03/30/2005] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
Behavioral conditioning is one of the most impressive demonstrations of brain-immune system interaction. Numerous animal studies have demonstrated behavioral conditioned effects on immune functions, however, human studies are rare. We investigated whether it is possible to behaviorally condition the acute response to interferon (IFN)beta-1a. In a double-blind placebo-controlled study, 30 healthy subjects received a single injection of IFN(beta)-1a (6MIU of REBIF, Serono International) (unconditioned stimulus, UCS) together with a novel drink (conditioned stimulus, CS). Blood was drawn at baseline, 4, 8, and 24 h after drug administration. Within the first 8 h peripheral granulocytes significantly increased, while monocytes, lymphocytes, T-, B- and natural killer (NK) cell numbers were significantly reduced. In parallel, body temperature, heart rate, norepinephrine and interleukin (IL)-6 plasma levels were heightened within 8 h after injection. 8 days later, all previously IFN(beta)-treated subjects received a subcutaneous placebo (NaCl) injection, but only 15 subjects were re-exposed to the CS (experimental group), while a control group (N=15) drank water and an additional group of subjects (n=8) remained untreated (untreated group). Blood sampling was performed at baseline and at 4, 8, and 24 h. Re-exposition to the CS did not elicit conditioned responses in the experimental group. Moreover, no differences were observed between groups. These data provide negative findings regarding behavioral conditioning of cytokine effects in humans employing a one-trial learning paradigm.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marion U Goebel
- Department of Medical Psychology, Medical Faculty, University of Duisburg-Essen, Hufelandstr. 55, 45122 Essen, Germany
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
462
|
Hunter AM, Leuchter AF, Morgan ML, Cook IA, Abrams M, Siegman B, DeBrota DJ, Potter WZ. Neurophysiologic correlates of side effects in normal subjects randomized to venlafaxine or placebo. Neuropsychopharmacology 2005; 30:792-9. [PMID: 15637637 DOI: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300652] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
Adverse events reported in the context of medication administration may be due to pharmacodynamic and/or nonpharmacodynamic effects (eg, nocebo phenomena). Neurophysiological substrates of side effects may be examined in placebo-controlled antidepressant treatment trials. We explored the relationship between side effects and regional neurophysiologic changes in normal subjects receiving a 1-week placebo lead-in followed by 4 weeks randomized treatment with placebo (n = 15) or venlafaxine IR (n = 17). Quantitative electroencephalographic (QEEG) cordance measures were obtained before and during treatment, and side effects were assessed weekly using semistructured interviews. Side effect burden, characterized as the mean number of side effects per postrandomization visit, correlated significantly with neurophysiologic changes in the antidepressant group but not the placebo group. Medication group side effects were negatively correlated with changes in prefrontal cordance at end of placebo lead-in (r = -0.67, p < 0.003), at 2 weeks (r = -0.77, p < 0.002), and at 4 weeks (r = -0.77, p < 0.004) post randomization. After controlling for the prefrontal change at the end of placebo lead-in, postrandomization brain changes did not further explain side effect burden. Changes in prefrontal brain function associated with later antidepressant side effects were observed during placebo lead-in-prior to the administration of medication. Prefrontal brain function during brief placebo administration may help explain susceptibility to the development of antidepressant side effects. Results of these exploratory hypothesis-generating analyses should be considered tentative until replicated.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aimee M Hunter
- Behavioral Pharmacology Laboratory, Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, UCLA Neuropsychiatric Institute, David Geffen School of Medicine, Los Angeles, CA 90024-1759, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
463
|
Lidstone SC, de la Fuente-Fernandez R, Stoessl AJ. The placebo response as a reward mechanism. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2005. [DOI: 10.1016/j.spmd.2005.02.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
464
|
|
465
|
Colloca L, Lopiano L, Benedetti F, Lanotte M. The placebo response in conditions other than pain. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2005. [DOI: 10.1016/j.spmd.2005.02.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
|
466
|
|
467
|
|
468
|
Abstract
PURPOSE OF REVIEW Classical conditioning is a form of associative learning, based on the pioneering work of I. P. Pavlov: due to its association with an unconditioned stimulus that induces an unconditioned response, an initially neutral stimulus will become a conditioned stimulus evoking a conditioned response in the absence of the unconditioned stimulus. One important area for the application of conditioning principles is the regulation of physiological systems in general, and endocrine responses and its concomitant changes specifically. Conditioned endocrine responses were predominantly addressed in animal studies so far, mainly examining conditioned insulin production (and blood-glucose change) and corticosterone release. RECENT FINDINGS There are very few studies on classical conditioning of endocrine responses in the annual period of this review. The advancement, however, is that some are conducted with humans. Recently, as a new avenue, hormones and neurotransmitters have been examined as mediators of basic conditioning processes. Moreover, there is an increasing interest in gender-specific conditioning responses which are influenced by gonadal hormones. SUMMARY Research on classical conditioning demonstrates that endocrine systems are adaptable to environmental signals. Likewise, the endocrine status of an organism (at least with regard to glucocorticoids and gonadal hormones) was shown to modify classically conditioned responses. Partly due to the high expenditure of conducting conditioning experiments, the quantity of studies is limited, but there is a need to extend this research to humans. In sum, the application of conditioning paradigms constitutes an important research tool for behavioral medicine as well as psychiatry to examine brain-behavior relationships.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ursula Stockhorst
- Institute of Medical Psychology, Heinrich-Heine-University Duesseldorf, Duesseldorf, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
469
|
Finniss DG, Benedetti F. Mechanisms of the placebo response and their impact on clinical trials and clinical practice. Pain 2005; 114:3-6. [PMID: 15733625 DOI: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.12.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 116] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/30/2004] [Accepted: 12/06/2004] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Damien G Finniss
- Pain Management Research Institute, University of Sydney and Royal North Shore Hospital, 2065 Sydney, NSW, Australia.
| | | |
Collapse
|
470
|
Kupers R, Faymonville ME, Laureys S. The cognitive modulation of pain: hypnosis- and placebo-induced analgesia. PROGRESS IN BRAIN RESEARCH 2005; 150:251-69. [PMID: 16186029 DOI: 10.1016/s0079-6123(05)50019-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 64] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/13/2022]
Abstract
Nowadays, there is compelling evidence that there is a poor relationship between the incoming sensory input and the resulting pain sensation. Signals coming from the peripheral nervous system undergo a complex modulation by cognitive, affective, and motivational processes when they enter the central nervous system. Placebo- and hypnosis-induced analgesia form two extreme examples of how cognitive processes may influence the pain sensation. With the advent of modern brain imaging techniques, researchers have started to disentangle the brain mechanisms involved in these forms of cognitive modulation of pain. These studies have shown that the prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices form important structures in a descending pathway that modulates incoming sensory input, likely via activation of the endogenous pain modulatory structures in the midbrain periaqueductal gray. Although little is known about the receptor systems involved in hypnosis-induced analgesia, studies of the placebo response suggest that the opiodergic and dopaminergic systems play an important role in the mediation of the placebo response.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ron Kupers
- Center for Functionally Integrative Neuroscience (CFIN), Aarhus University and Aarhus University Hospital, Aarhus, Denmark
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
471
|
Colloca L, Lopiano L, Lanotte M, Benedetti F. Overt versus covert treatment for pain, anxiety, and Parkinson's disease. Lancet Neurol 2004; 3:679-84. [PMID: 15488461 DOI: 10.1016/s1474-4422(04)00908-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 325] [Impact Index Per Article: 16.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/16/2022]
Abstract
The recent introduction of covert administration of treatment to biomedical research has produced some interesting results, with many clinical and ethical implications. Concealed treatment has been used in people with nervous system conditions including pain, anxiety, and Parkinson's disease. The main finding is that when the patient is completely unaware that a treatment is being given, the treatment is less effective than when it is given overtly in accordance with routine medical practice. The difference between open and hidden administrations is thought to represent the placebo component of the treatment, even though no placebo has been given. The decreased effectiveness of hidden treatments indicates that knowledge about a treatment affects outcome and highlights the importance of the patient-provider interaction. In addition, by use of covert administration, the efficacy of some treatments can be assessed without the use of a placebo and associated ethical issues.
Collapse
|
472
|
Sorge J, Sittl R. Transdermal buprenorphine in the treatment of chronic pain: Resultsof a phase III, multicenter, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Clin Ther 2004; 26:1808-20. [PMID: 15639693 DOI: 10.1016/j.clinthera.2004.11.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 92] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 09/23/2004] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Buprenorphine, a potent opioid analgesic, has been available in parenteral and oral or sublingual(SL) formulations for >25 years. In 2001, the buprenorphine transdermal delivery system (TES) was introduced at 3 release rates (35, 52.5, and 70 microg/h) for the treatment of chronic cancer and noncancer pain. OBJECTIVE This study compared the analgesic efficacy and tolerability of buprenorphine TES at a release rate of 35 microg/h with those of buprenorphine SL and placebo in patients with severe or very severe chronic cancer or noncancer pain. METHODS This multicenter, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel-group trial was 1 of 3 Phase III studies involved in the clinical development of buprenorphine TDS. It comprised a 6-day open-label run-in phase in which patients received buprenorphine SL 0.8 to 1.6 mg/d as needed and a double-blind phase in which patients were randomized to receive 3 sequential patches containing buprenorphine TES 35 microg/h or placebo, each lasting 72 hours. Rescue analgesia consisting of buprenorphine SL 02-mg tablets was available as needed throughout the double-blind phase. The main outcome measures were (1) the number of buprenorphine SL tablets required in addition to buprenorphine TES during the double-blind phase compared with the placebo group and compared with the buprenorphine SL requirement during the run-in phase, and (2) patients' assessments of pain intensity, pain relief, and duration of sleep uninterrupted by pain in the double-blind phase compared with the run-in phase. Adverse events were documented throughout the study. RESULTS One hundred thirty-seven patients were included in the double-blind phase (90 buprenorphine TES, 47 placebo). The buprenorphine TES group included 47 men and 43 women (mean [SD] age, 56.0 [12.1] years), and the placebo group included 23 men and 24 women (mean age, 55.7 [12.9] years). Forty-five patients had cancer-related pain and 92 had noncancer-related pain. The 2 treatment groups were comparable with respect to sex distribution, age, height, and body weight Patients receiving buprenorphine TES significantly reduced their consumption of buprenorphine SL tablets in the double-blind phase compared with patients receiving placebo (reduction of 0.6 [0.4] mg vs 0.4 [0.4] mg; P = 0.03). The relationship between the buprenorphine SL dose in the run-in phase and the number of buprenorphine SL tablets required in the double-blind phase was dose dependent in the active-treatment group only. Patients' assessments of pain intensity and pain relief suggested better analgesia with buprenorphine TES than with placebo, although the differences did not reach statistical significance. The proportion of patients who reported sleeping for >6 hours uninterrupted by pain in the double-blind phase compared with the run-in phase increased by 6.4% in the buprenorphine TDS group (35.6% vs 292%, respectively), compared with a decrease of 5.9% in the placebo group (40.4% vs 463%); no statistical analysis of sleep duration data was performed. Buprenorphine TDS was well tolerated, with adverse events generally similar to those associated with other opioids. The incidence of systemic adverse events in the double-blind phase was similar in the 2 treatment groups (28.9% buprenorphine TDS, 27.6% placebo), with the most common adverse events being nausea, dizziness, and vomiting. After patch removal, skin reactions (mainly mild or moderate pruritus and erythema) were seen in 35.6% of the buprenorphine TDS group and 25.5% of the placebo group. CONCLUSIONS In the population studied, buprenorphine TDS provided adequate pain relief, as well as improvements in pain intensity and duration of pain-free sleep. It may be considered a therapeutic option for the treatment of moderate to severe chronic pain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jürgen Sorge
- Department of Anesthesiology, Surgical Intensive Care and Pain Therapy, Peine District Hospital, Virchowstrasse 8h, 31221 Peine, Germany.
| | | |
Collapse
|
473
|
Romundstad L, Breivik H, Niemi G, Helle A, Stubhaug A. Methylprednisolone intravenously 1 day after surgery has sustained analgesic and opioid-sparing effects. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 2004; 48:1223-31. [PMID: 15504180 DOI: 10.1111/j.1399-6576.2004.00480.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 85] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND In previous studies on glucocorticoids for postoperative pain, the test drug has been given perioperatively, usually before measurement of baseline pain. In order to evaluate the time course and magnitude of the analgesic effect of a glucocorticoid in well-established postoperative pain, we compared methylprednisolone with ketorolac and placebo, after assessment of baseline pain on the first postoperative day. METHODS This was a double-blind, single dose, randomized, parallel comparison of intravenous (i.v.) methylprednisolone 125 mg, ketorolac 30 mg as an active control, and placebo in 75 patients with moderate to severe pain 1 day after orthopaedic surgery. Outcome variables were pain intensity (0-100 VAS), pain relief (0-4 PAR) and rescue opioid consumption. RESULTS Methylprednisolone was not significantly different from ketorolac and gave significantly lower pain intensity from 1 h (0-6 h, P < 0.02), and more pain relief 2-6 h after test drugs (P < 0.05) compared with placebo. After 24 h, pain intensity was lower in both active drug groups compared with placebo (methylprednisolone, P < 0.0001; ketorolac, P < 0.007). Number needed to treat (NNT) calculated from patients having more than at least 50% of maximum obtainable total pain relief during the first 6 h (>50%maxTOTPAR(6 h)) was 3.6 for methylprednisolone and 3.1 for ketorolac. Number needed to treat calculated from the percentage reporting at least 50% pain relief for at least 4 h (>50%PAR(4 h)) was 2.8 for both groups. Opioid consumption was significantly reduced for 72 h after methylprednisolone compared with ketorolac (P < 0.02) and placebo (P < 0.003). CONCLUSION Methylprednisolone 125 mg i.v. 1 day after surgery gave similar early reduction of pain as i.v. ketorolac 30 mg. Less pain than placebo 24 h after methylprednisolone, and lower opioid consumption for 72 h compared with ketorolac and placebo indicate sustained analgesic effects of methylprednisolone.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- L Romundstad
- Department of Anaesthesiology, Rikshospitalet University Hospital, Oslo, Norway.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
474
|
|
475
|
Affiliation(s)
- A Jon Stoessl
- Pacific Parkinson's Research Centre, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada V6T 2B5.
| | | |
Collapse
|
476
|
de la Fuente-Fernández R, Schulzer M, Stoessl AJ. Placebo mechanisms and reward circuitry: clues from Parkinson's disease. Biol Psychiatry 2004; 56:67-71. [PMID: 15231437 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.11.019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 97] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/18/2002] [Revised: 11/13/2003] [Accepted: 11/21/2003] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Recent evidence indicates that the placebo effect in Parkinson's disease is mediated by the release of dopamine in the dorsal striatum. Interestingly, there is also placebo-induced dopamine release in the ventral striatum, which establishes a connection between the placebo effect and reward mechanisms. Specifically, we propose that placebo responses are related to the activation of the reward circuitry. Here, the clinical benefit induced by placebos represents the reward. The magnitude of the placebo effect likely depends on the a priori probability of clinical benefit. This notion has profound implications in the design of clinical trials and placebo investigations.
Collapse
|
477
|
Lieberman MD, Jarcho JM, Berman S, Naliboff BD, Suyenobu BY, Mandelkern M, Mayer EA. The neural correlates of placebo effects: a disruption account. Neuroimage 2004; 22:447-55. [PMID: 15110038 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.037] [Citation(s) in RCA: 203] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/03/2003] [Revised: 01/15/2004] [Accepted: 01/15/2004] [Indexed: 01/10/2023] Open
Abstract
The neurocognitive pathways by which placebo effects operate are poorly understood. Positron emission tomography (PET) imaging was used to assess the brain response of patients with chronic abdominal pain (irritable bowel syndrome; IBS) to induced intestinal discomfort both before and after a 3-week placebo regimen. A daily symptom diary was used to measure symptom improvement. Increases in right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (RVLPFC) activity from pre- to post-placebo predicted self-reported symptom improvement, and this relationship was mediated by changes in dorsal anterior cingulate (dACC), typically associated with pain unpleasantness. These results are consistent with disruption theory [Lieberman, M.D., 2003. Reflective and reflexive judgment processes: a social cognitive neuroscience approach. In: Forgas, J.P., Williams, K.R., von Hippel, W. (Eds.), Social Judgments: Explicit and Implicit Processes. Cambridge Univ. Press, New York, pp. 44-67], which proposes that activation of prefrontal regions associated with thinking about negative affect can diminish dACC and amygdala reactivity to negative affect stimuli. This is the first study to identify a neural pathway from a region of the brain associated with placebos and affective thought to a region closely linked to the placebo-related outcome of diminished pain unpleasantness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matthew D Lieberman
- Department of Psychology, University of California-Los Angeles, Franz Hall, 405 Hilgard Avenue, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1563, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
478
|
Marchand S. Dr. Marchand's response to Dr. Coffey's letterThalamic stimulation: placebo component in the clinical efficacy. Pain 2004. [DOI: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.01.025] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
479
|
Weiß T. Psychophysiologische Aspekte des Placeboeffekts bei Schmerz. ZEITSCHRIFT FÜR NEUROPSYCHOLOGIE 2004. [DOI: 10.1024/1016-264x.15.2.99] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Zusammenfassung: Placeboeffekte entstehen aus einer komplexen Wechselwirkung innerhalb des neuronalen Netzwerks der Schmerzverarbeitung. Ein Hauptmechanismus für die Entstehung von Placeboeffekten ist Konditionierung, der zweite Hauptmechanismus ist die Erwartung einer erfolgreichen Therapie. Placeboeffekte werden zu einem erheblichen Teil über das endogene Opiatsystem vermittelt. Dies trifft für erwartungsbasierte Placeboeffekte vollständig zu, während opioidvermittelte konditionierte Effekte eine Teilkomponente darstellen. Des weiteren ist mindestens das dopaminerge System von Bedeutung. Für die neurobiologische Vermittlung der erwartungsbasierten Placeboeffekte scheint der rostrale anteriore cinguläre Kortex eine besondere Rolle zu spielen, was auf die enge Verknüpfung mit kognitiv-emotionalen Prozessen hinweist. Aus diesem Wissen heraus lassen sich Schlussfolgerungen für die gezielte Nutzung von Placeboeffekten in der Schmerztherapie, aber auch darüber hinaus ableiten.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas Weiß
- Institut für Psychologie, Biologische und Klinische Psychologie, Friedrich-Schiller-Universität Jena
| |
Collapse
|
480
|
Benedetti F, Colloca L, Torre E, Lanotte M, Melcarne A, Pesare M, Bergamasco B, Lopiano L. Placebo-responsive Parkinson patients show decreased activity in single neurons of subthalamic nucleus. Nat Neurosci 2004; 7:587-8. [PMID: 15146189 DOI: 10.1038/nn1250] [Citation(s) in RCA: 192] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/20/2004] [Accepted: 03/08/2004] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
Placebo administration is known to affect the brain both in pain and in Parkinson disease. Here we show that placebo treatment caused reduced activity in single neurons in the subthalamic nucleus of placebo-responsive Parkinsonian patients. These changes in activity were tightly correlated with clinical improvement; no decrease in activity occurred when the clinical placebo response was absent.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Fabrizio Benedetti
- Department of Neuroscience, Clinical and Applied Physiology Program, University of Turin Medical School, Corso Raffaello 30, 10125 Turin, Italy.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
481
|
de la Fuente-Fernández R. Uncovering the hidden placebo effect in deep-brain stimulation for Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2004; 10:125-7. [PMID: 15036165 DOI: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2003.10.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/05/2003] [Revised: 10/06/2003] [Accepted: 10/06/2003] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE To determine the magnitude of the placebo effect in deep-brain stimulation (DBS) for Parkinson's disease (PD). BACKGROUND The placebo effect in PD is related to the expectation of clinical benefit. Changes in expectation occurring in DBS crossover studies can be used to estimate the associated placebo effect. For example, the response to active-DBS is predicted to be greater in patients who have previously experienced the effect of placebo-DBS than in those without such an experience. METHODS Data were obtained by searches of Medline and PubMed using three keywords: Parkinson's disease, deep-brain stimulation, and crossover study. Between-group comparisons of the effect of active-DBS were performed by t-test and analysis of covariance. RESULTS Data on 126 PD patients were available for analysis. These patients participated in a multicenter, two-period, placebo-controlled crossover study on the efficacy of DBS for PD. As predicted, active-DBS was found to be more effective when preceded by placebo-DBS (i.e. in the high expectation group) than when not (i.e. in the lower expectation group). The estimated magnitude of the placebo effect in DBS was equivalent to 39% of the magnitude of the effect of active-DBS. CONCLUSIONS There is a prominent placebo effect associated with DBS in PD. Crossover studies may be useful for estimating the placebo effect in other medical conditions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Raúl de la Fuente-Fernández
- Division of Neurology, Hospital Arquitecto Marcide, Estrada San Pedro - Catabois s/n, 15405 Ferrol, A Coruña, Spain.
| |
Collapse
|
482
|
Abstract
OBJECTIVES The objectives of this medico-legal case report are to consider the current status of the use of placebos in pain medicine from clinical, ethical, and legal perspectives. The focus of the analysis is a particular case in which the deceptive use of placebo pain therapy on an adolescent gave rise to professional grievances filed by the patient's mother against the physician who ordered and several nurses who administered the placebo. The medical board declined to take disciplinary action against the physician, and disciplinary action by the board of registered nursing against the nurses was successfully challenged by two of the charged nurses in an administrative review. While there is a growing literature that challenges the need for or justification of the deceptive use of placebos, the practice continues and, as the case under consideration indicates, retains some influential supporters. DESIGN This is a case report from a community hospital. The patient, referred to here as KC, was an adolescent with migraine headaches. The substitution of a placebo (saline solution) for an opioid analgesic (morphine) was made during KC's treatment. RESULTS The patient's pain subsided sufficiently following the administration of a placebo to permit his discharge from the hospital. The subsequent discovery by the patient's mother of the deceptive use of a placebo prompted her to file charges of professional misconduct against the treating physician and three nurses with their respective professional licensing boards. The medical board declined to take disciplinary action against the physician, and the disciplinary action by the board of registered nursing was successfully challenged in a ruling by an administrative law judge following a hearing in which expert witnesses took conflicting positions on the acceptability of the deceptive use of a placebo. CONCLUSION While there is a developing literature that challenges the ethical legitimacy of the deceptive use of placebos in pain medicine, that literature has yet to be recognized as unqualifiedly setting the standard of care or of professionalism in medicine and nursing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ben A Rich
- University of California, Davis School of Medicine, Sacramento, California 95817, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
483
|
Benedetti F, Colloca L, Lanotte M, Bergamasco B, Torre E, Lopiano L. Autonomic and emotional responses to open and hidden stimulations of the human subthalamic region. Brain Res Bull 2004; 63:203-11. [PMID: 15145139 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2004.01.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 53] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/29/2003] [Revised: 01/10/2004] [Accepted: 01/15/2004] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
We performed a microstimulation study of the subthalamic region of Parkinsonian patients who underwent bilateral electrode implantation in the subthalamic nuclei and whose heart rate and heart rate variability were recorded. The stimulation of the dorsalmost region, which includes the zona incerta and the dorsal pole of the subthalamic nucleus, produced autonomic responses that were constant over time. In fact, hidden stimulations (the patient is not aware of being stimulated) and open stimulations (the patient is aware of being stimulated) always induced the same responses. By contrast, the stimulation of the ventralmost region, which includes the ventral pole of the subthalamic nucleus and the substantia nigra pars reticulata, produced autonomic and emotional responses that were inconstant over time and varied according to the condition. In fact, different responses were elicited with hidden and open stimulations. These data suggest that the dorsal subthalamic nucleus and/or the zona incerta are involved in autonomic control, whereas the ventral subthalamic nucleus and/or the substantia nigra reticulata are involved in associative/limbic-related autonomic activity. The difference between the open and hidden stimulations in the ventral subthalamic region can explain previous studies in which open and hidden stimulations produced different therapeutic outcomes.
Collapse
|
484
|
Abstract
Administration of drugs is often followed by beneficial (placebo effects) and harmful (nocebo effects) effects that are not always related to their mechanism of action. Nocebo effects are rather unknown even when may be the source of many adverse reactions which could be erroneously attributed to drug therapy. Some mechanisms have been postulated which might be associated with the development of nocebo effects. Expectancy, learning and classical conditioning are probably important in the psychological domain. The neuropharmacological substrate is much less known yet an opioid peptide-cholecystokinin interaction has been suggested. At the clinical setting, a nocebo effect should be suspected in those patients who present common unspecific symptoms after drug administration and have a tendency to somatize. An early detection of these patients may contribute to the prevention of the nocebo effect.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jordi Ferreres
- Unitat de Farmacologia. Institut Municipal d'Investigació Mèdica (IMIM). Barcelona. Universitat Autónoma de Barcelona (UAB). Barcelona. Spain
| | | | | |
Collapse
|