51
|
Behavioral and brain dynamics of executive control in relation to children's fluid intelligence. INTELLIGENCE 2021. [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2020.101513] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
|
52
|
Brevers D, Cheron G, Dahman T, Petieau M, Palmero-Soler E, Foucart J, Verbanck P, Cebolla AM. Spatiotemporal brain signal associated with high and low levels of proactive motor response inhibition. Brain Res 2020; 1747:147064. [PMID: 32818530 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2020.147064] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/30/2020] [Revised: 07/02/2020] [Accepted: 08/14/2020] [Indexed: 01/24/2023]
Abstract
Proactive motor response inhibition is used to strategically restrain actions in preparation for stopping. In this study, we first examined the event related potential (ERP) elicited by low and high level of proactive response inhibition, as assessed by the stop-signal task. Corroborating previous studies, we found an increased amplitude of the contingent negative variation (CNV) in the high level of proactive inhibition. As the main goal of the present study, swLORETA was used to determine the neural generators characterising CNV differences between low and high levels of proactive inhibition. Results showed that the higher level of proactive inhibition involved numerous generators, including within the middle and medial frontal gyrus. Importantly, we observed that the lower level of proactive inhibition also involved a specific neural generator, within the frontopolar cortex. Altogether, present findings identified the specific brain sources of ERP signals involved in the later phase of motor preparation under low or high levels of proactive motor response inhibition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D Brevers
- Addictive and Compulsive Behaviours Lab, Health and Behaviour Institute, University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg; Research in Psychology Applied to Motor Learning, Faculty of Motor Sciences, Erasme Campus, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium; Laboratory of Psychological Medicine and Addictology, Faculty of Medicine, Brugmann-campus, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium; Laboratory of Neurophysiology and Movement Biomechanics, Faculty of Motor Sciences, Erasme Campus, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium.
| | - G Cheron
- Laboratory of Neurophysiology and Movement Biomechanics, Faculty of Motor Sciences, Erasme Campus, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium
| | - T Dahman
- Research in Psychology Applied to Motor Learning, Faculty of Motor Sciences, Erasme Campus, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium
| | - M Petieau
- Laboratory of Neurophysiology and Movement Biomechanics, Faculty of Motor Sciences, Erasme Campus, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium
| | - E Palmero-Soler
- Laboratory of Neurophysiology and Movement Biomechanics, Faculty of Motor Sciences, Erasme Campus, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium
| | - J Foucart
- Research in Psychology Applied to Motor Learning, Faculty of Motor Sciences, Erasme Campus, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium; Haute Ecole Libre de Bruxelles (H.E.L.B.) Ilya Prigogine, Physiotherapy Section, Erasme Campus, Brussels, Belgium
| | - P Verbanck
- Research in Psychology Applied to Motor Learning, Faculty of Motor Sciences, Erasme Campus, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium; Laboratory of Psychological Medicine and Addictology, Faculty of Medicine, Brugmann-campus, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium
| | - A M Cebolla
- Laboratory of Neurophysiology and Movement Biomechanics, Faculty of Motor Sciences, Erasme Campus, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
53
|
Ludyga S, Möhring W, Budde H, Hirt N, Pühse U, Gerber M. Neurocognitive processes mediate the relation between children's motor skills, cardiorespiratory fitness and response inhibition: Evidence from source imaging. Psychophysiology 2020; 58:e13716. [PMID: 33128487 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.13716] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/18/2020] [Revised: 08/26/2020] [Accepted: 10/02/2020] [Indexed: 01/20/2023]
Abstract
Accumulating evidence suggests an association between outcomes of sports participation, such as motor skills and cardiorespiratory fitness, and aspects of inhibitory control in children. However, it remains unclear if motor skills and cardiorespiratory fitness are related to different source activity patterns and if neurophysiological indices of response inhibition mediate the relation of these constructs with behavioral performance. We examined the relative contributions of motor skills and cardiorespiratory fitness to response inhibition and a potential mediation by the neurocognitive processes indexed by the N200 and P300 components of event-related potentials. About 92 children aged 9-13 years completed the Movement ABC-2, the PWC170 and a Go/NoGo task. We employed electroencephalography (EEG) to record the N200 and P300 components elicited by the task, which are considered to reflect conflict monitoring and the allocation of attentional resources toward task-relevant stimuli, respectively. Path-anlayses revealed a moderate association between motor skills and behavioral performance on the Go/NoGo task. This association was fully mediated by the P300 amplitude in the NoGo condition. In contrast, cardiorespiratory fitness was not related to behavioral performance, but accounted for variance in N200. Source analyses supported an association between cardiorespiratory fitness and N200 source activity in prefrontal and primary motor cortex, whereas motor skills were related to P300 source activity in the posterior cingulate cortex. Our findings provide novel insights into the neural mechanisms underlying the relation between motor skills and response inhibition. Moreover, we found that the neural generators of the P300 and N200 varied as a function of children's cardiorespiratory fitness and motor skills.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sebastian Ludyga
- Department of Sport, Exercise and Health, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
| | - Wenke Möhring
- Department of Psychology, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
| | - Henning Budde
- Faculty of Human Sciences, Medical School Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany
| | - Nick Hirt
- Department of Sport, Exercise and Health, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
| | - Uwe Pühse
- Department of Sport, Exercise and Health, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
| | - Markus Gerber
- Department of Sport, Exercise and Health, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
54
|
Kałamała P, Ociepka M, Chuderski A. ERP evidence for rapid within-trial adaptation of cognitive control during conflict resolution. Cortex 2020; 131:151-163. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.07.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/07/2020] [Revised: 06/01/2020] [Accepted: 07/26/2020] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
|
55
|
Effect of Age in Auditory Go/No-Go Tasks: A Magnetoencephalographic Study. Brain Sci 2020; 10:brainsci10100667. [PMID: 32992713 PMCID: PMC7599487 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci10100667] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/26/2020] [Revised: 09/22/2020] [Accepted: 09/24/2020] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Response inhibition is frequently examined using visual go/no-go tasks. Recently, the auditory go/no-go paradigm has been also applied to several clinical and aging populations. However, age-related changes in the neural underpinnings of auditory go/no-go tasks are yet to be elucidated. We used magnetoencephalography combined with distributed source imaging methods to examine age-associated changes in neural responses to auditory no-go stimuli. Additionally, we compared the performance of high- and low-performing older adults to explore differences in cortical activation. Behavioral performance in terms of response inhibition was similar in younger and older adult groups. Relative to the younger adults, the older adults exhibited reduced cortical activation in the superior and middle temporal gyrus. However, we did not find any significant differences in cortical activation between the high- and low-performing older adults. Our results therefore support the hypothesis that inhibition is reduced during aging. The variation in cognitive performance among older adults confirms the need for further study on the underlying mechanisms of inhibition.
Collapse
|
56
|
De Blasio FM, Barry RJ. Prestimulus alpha and beta contributions to equiprobable Go/NoGo processing in healthy ageing. Int J Psychophysiol 2020; 155:32-40. [DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.05.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/28/2020] [Revised: 05/22/2020] [Accepted: 05/26/2020] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
57
|
Skippen P, Fulham WR, Michie PT, Matzke D, Heathcote A, Karayanidis F. Reconsidering electrophysiological markers of response inhibition in light of trigger failures in the stop‐signal task. Psychophysiology 2020; 57:e13619. [DOI: 10.1111/psyp.13619] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/24/2019] [Revised: 04/27/2020] [Accepted: 05/10/2020] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- P. Skippen
- Functional Neuroimaging Laboratory School of Psychology University of Newcastle Newcastle NSW Australia
- Priority Research Centre for Brain and Mental Health University of Newcastle Newcastle NSW Australia
| | - W. R. Fulham
- Functional Neuroimaging Laboratory School of Psychology University of Newcastle Newcastle NSW Australia
- Priority Research Centre for Brain and Mental Health University of Newcastle Newcastle NSW Australia
| | - P. T. Michie
- Functional Neuroimaging Laboratory School of Psychology University of Newcastle Newcastle NSW Australia
- Priority Research Centre for Brain and Mental Health University of Newcastle Newcastle NSW Australia
| | - D. Matzke
- Psychological Methods Department of Psychology University of Amsterdam Amsterdam The Netherlands
| | - A. Heathcote
- School of Psychology University of Tasmania Hobart TAS Australia
| | - F. Karayanidis
- Functional Neuroimaging Laboratory School of Psychology University of Newcastle Newcastle NSW Australia
- Priority Research Centre for Brain and Mental Health University of Newcastle Newcastle NSW Australia
- Priority Research Centre for Stroke and Brain Injury University of Newcastle Newcastle NSW Australia
| |
Collapse
|
58
|
Flanker Task-Elicited Event-Related Potential Sources Reflect Human Recombinant Erythropoietin Differential Effects on Parkinson's Patients. PARKINSON'S DISEASE 2020; 2020:8625794. [PMID: 32566122 PMCID: PMC7261337 DOI: 10.1155/2020/8625794] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/05/2019] [Revised: 04/21/2020] [Accepted: 05/07/2020] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
We used EEG source analysis to identify which cortical areas were involved in the automatic and controlled processes of inhibitory control on a flanker task and compared the potential efficacy of recombinant-human erythropoietin (rHuEPO) on the performance of Parkinson's Disease patients. The samples were 18 medicated PD patients (nine of them received rHuEPO in addition to their usual anti-PD medication through random allocation and the other nine patients were on their regular anti-PD medication only) and 9 age and education-matched healthy controls (HCs) who completed the flanker task with simultaneous EEG recordings. N1 and N2 event-related potential (ERP) components were identified and a low resolution tomography (LORETA) inverse solution was employed to localize the neural generators. Reaction times and errors were increased for the incongruent flankers for PD patients compared to controls. EEG source analysis identified an effect of rHuEPO on the lingual gyri for the early N1 component. N2-related sources in middle cingulate and precuneus were associated with the inhibition of automatic responses evoked by incongruent stimuli differentiated PD and HCs. From our results rHuEPO seems to mediate an effect on N1 sources in lingual gyri but not on behavioural performance. N2-related sources in middle cingulate and precuneus were evoked by incongruent stimuli differentiated PD and HCs.
Collapse
|
59
|
Barry RJ, Fogarty JS, De Blasio FM. Caffeine as a Tool to Explore Active Cognitive Processing Stages in Two-Choice Tasks. J Caffeine Adenosine Res 2020; 10:71-83. [PMID: 32566904 PMCID: PMC7301320 DOI: 10.1089/caff.2019.0021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Background: We used caffeine as a tool to explore the active cognitive-processing stages in a simple Go/NoGo task, in terms of the event-related potential (ERP) components elicited by the Go and NoGo stimuli. Methods: Two hundred and fifty milligrams of caffeine was administered to adult participants (N = 24) in a randomized double-blind placebo-controlled repeated-measures crossover study. Two blocks of an equiprobable auditory Go/NoGo task were completed, each with a random mix of 75 tones at 1000 Hz and 75 at 1500 Hz, all 60 dB sound pressure level (SPL). Results: Major ERP effects of caffeine were apparent in enhancements of the Go N1-1, P3b, and Slow Wave (SW), and the NoGo Processing Negativity, SW, and NoGo Late Positivity. Conclusions: Novel differential findings indicate the potential of our caffeine as a tool approach to elucidate the functional nature of ERP markers of active cognitive processing in a range of developmental and clinical populations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robert J Barry
- Brain & Behaviour Research Institute, University of Wollongong, Wollongong, Australia.,School of Psychology, University of Wollongong, Wollongong, Australia
| | - Jack S Fogarty
- Brain & Behaviour Research Institute, University of Wollongong, Wollongong, Australia.,School of Psychology, University of Wollongong, Wollongong, Australia
| | - Frances M De Blasio
- Brain & Behaviour Research Institute, University of Wollongong, Wollongong, Australia.,School of Psychology, University of Wollongong, Wollongong, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
60
|
Nguyen AT, Albrecht MA, Lipp OV, Marinovic W. Motor output matters: Evidence of a continuous relationship between Stop/No-go P300 amplitude and peak force on failed inhibitions at the trial-level. Psychophysiology 2020; 57:e13558. [PMID: 32129505 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.13558] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/13/2019] [Revised: 01/09/2020] [Accepted: 02/12/2020] [Indexed: 01/02/2023]
Abstract
Motor actions can be suppressed with varying degrees of success, but this variability is not often captured as responses are typically represented as binary (response vs. no-response). Although the Stop/No-go P300 has been implicated as an index of inhibitory-control, it is unclear how the range of motor outputs relates to the P300. We examined the nature of this association in two experiments using an Anticipatory Timing and a Go/No-go Task, while measuring peak force, movement onset time, and P300. In both experiments, our results showed that trial-by-trial P300 amplitude on Failed Inhibitions were continuously related to peak force, where higher force (reflecting a greater degree of error) was associated with smaller P300 amplitude. Compared to Successful Inhibitions, P300 amplitude and onset latency on Failed Inhibitions were significantly reduced and delayed. Although the binary categorization of inhibition-success (Successful vs. Failed) accounts for significant variance in the P300, it misses a reliable linear relationship that can be captured by continuous measures of motor output. Overall, the results provide evidence that P300 may reflect the continuously varying engagement of inhibitory-control. We present an activation model to visualize the P300-force association and to illustrate how motor output might be modeled in the context of inhibitory-control. Our results highlight the relevance of P300 amplitude and the importance of studying the spectrum of motor output and the need for future models to account for motor output.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- An T Nguyen
- School of Psychology, Curtin University, Perth, WA, Australia
| | | | - Ottmar V Lipp
- School of Psychology, Curtin University, Perth, WA, Australia
| | | |
Collapse
|
61
|
Tan HK, Goh SKY, Tsotsi S, Bruntraeger M, Chen HY, Broekman B, Tan KH, Chong YS, Meaney MJ, Qiu A, Rifkin-Graboi A. Maternal antenatal anxiety and electrophysiological functioning amongst a sub-set of preschoolers participating in the GUSTO cohort. BMC Psychiatry 2020; 20:62. [PMID: 32050929 PMCID: PMC7017524 DOI: 10.1186/s12888-020-2454-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/07/2018] [Accepted: 01/22/2020] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Antenatal maternal anxiety is a risk for offspring psychological and cognitive difficulties. The preschool years represent an important time for brain development, and so may be a window for intervention. However, electrophysiological investigations of maternal anxiety and preschoolers' brain functioning are lacking. We ask whether anxiety symptoms predict neurophysiology, and consider timing specificity (26-weeks antenatal or 24-months postnatal), form of insult (anxiety symptoms, per se, or also depression symptoms), and offspring gender. METHODS The sample consisted of a subset of 71 mothers and their 3 year old children taking part in the prospective birth cohort, GUSTO. Mothers provided antenatal (26 weeks) and postnatal (2 years) anxiety and depressive symptomatology data, respectively via the "State Trait Anxiety Questionnaire" and the "Edinburgh Postpartum Depression Scale." Offspring provided electrophysiological data, obtained while they indicated the emotional expression of actors whose facial expressions remained consistent throughout a pre-switch block, but were reversed at "post-switch." RESULTS Three electrophysiological components linked to different information processing stages were identified. The two earliest occurring components (i.e., the N1 and P2) differed across blocks. During post-switch, both were significantly predicted by maternal anxiety, after controlling for pre-switch neurophysiology. Similar results were observed with depression. Antenatal mental health remained a significant predictor after controlling for postnatal mental health. CONCLUSION In combination with past work, these findings suggest the importance of reducing symptoms in women prior to and during pregnancy, and offering support to offspring early in development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hong Kuang Tan
- grid.452264.30000 0004 0530 269XIntegrative Neurosciences, Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences (SICS), Agency for Science and Technology (A*STAR), Brenner Centre for Molecular Medicine, 30 Medical Drive, Singapore, 117609 Singapore ,grid.428397.30000 0004 0385 0924Duke-National University of Singapore, 8 College Road, Singapore, 169857 Singapore
| | - Shaun K. Y. Goh
- grid.4280.e0000 0001 2180 6431Department of Biomedical Engineering, National University Singapore, 4 Engineering Drive 3, Singapore, 117583 Singapore ,grid.59025.3b0000 0001 2224 0361Present Address: Centre for Research in Child Development, National Institute of Education, 1 Nanyang Walk, Singapore, S637616 Singapore
| | - Stella Tsotsi
- grid.452264.30000 0004 0530 269XIntegrative Neurosciences, Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences (SICS), Agency for Science and Technology (A*STAR), Brenner Centre for Molecular Medicine, 30 Medical Drive, Singapore, 117609 Singapore ,grid.59025.3b0000 0001 2224 0361Present Address: Centre for Research in Child Development, National Institute of Education, 1 Nanyang Walk, Singapore, S637616 Singapore
| | - Michaela Bruntraeger
- grid.452264.30000 0004 0530 269XIntegrative Neurosciences, Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences (SICS), Agency for Science and Technology (A*STAR), Brenner Centre for Molecular Medicine, 30 Medical Drive, Singapore, 117609 Singapore ,grid.10306.340000 0004 0606 5382Wellcome Trust Sanger Institute, Wellcome Genome Campus, Hinxton, Cambridge, CB10 1SA UK
| | - Helen Yu Chen
- grid.428397.30000 0004 0385 0924Duke-National University of Singapore, 8 College Road, Singapore, 169857 Singapore ,Department of Psychological Medicine, KK Women and Children’s Hospital, 100 Bukit Timah Road, Singapore, 229899 Singapore
| | - Birit Broekman
- grid.452264.30000 0004 0530 269XIntegrative Neurosciences, Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences (SICS), Agency for Science and Technology (A*STAR), Brenner Centre for Molecular Medicine, 30 Medical Drive, Singapore, 117609 Singapore ,Department of Psychiatry, OLVG and Amsterdam UMC, Amsterdam, Netherlands
| | - Kok Hian Tan
- Division of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, KK Women and Children’s Hospital, 100 Bukit Timah Road, Singapore, 229899 Singapore
| | - Yap Seng Chong
- grid.452264.30000 0004 0530 269XIntegrative Neurosciences, Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences (SICS), Agency for Science and Technology (A*STAR), Brenner Centre for Molecular Medicine, 30 Medical Drive, Singapore, 117609 Singapore ,grid.412106.00000 0004 0621 9599Department of Gynaecology and Obstetrics, National University Hospital Singapore, 1E, Kent Ridge Road, Singapore, 119228 Singapore
| | - Michael J. Meaney
- grid.452264.30000 0004 0530 269XIntegrative Neurosciences, Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences (SICS), Agency for Science and Technology (A*STAR), Brenner Centre for Molecular Medicine, 30 Medical Drive, Singapore, 117609 Singapore ,grid.14709.3b0000 0004 1936 8649McGill University, 6875 Boulevard Lasalle, Montréal, QC H4H 1R3 Canada ,Ludmer Centre for Neuroinformatics and Mental Health, 6875 Boulevard Lasalle, Montréal, QC H4H 1R3 Canada
| | - Anqi Qiu
- grid.452264.30000 0004 0530 269XIntegrative Neurosciences, Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences (SICS), Agency for Science and Technology (A*STAR), Brenner Centre for Molecular Medicine, 30 Medical Drive, Singapore, 117609 Singapore ,grid.4280.e0000 0001 2180 6431Department of Biomedical Engineering, National University Singapore, 4 Engineering Drive 3, Singapore, 117583 Singapore
| | - Anne Rifkin-Graboi
- Integrative Neurosciences, Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences (SICS), Agency for Science and Technology (A*STAR), Brenner Centre for Molecular Medicine, 30 Medical Drive, Singapore, 117609, Singapore. .,Present Address: Centre for Research in Child Development, National Institute of Education, 1 Nanyang Walk, Singapore, S637616, Singapore.
| |
Collapse
|
62
|
Wodniecka Z, Szewczyk J, Kałamała P, Mandera P, Durlik J. When a second language hits a native language. What ERPs (do and do not) tell us about language retrieval difficulty in bilingual language production. Neuropsychologia 2020; 141:107390. [PMID: 32057934 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107390] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/29/2018] [Revised: 09/01/2019] [Accepted: 02/09/2020] [Indexed: 02/05/2023]
Abstract
The accumulating evidence suggests that prior usage of a second language (L2) leads to processing costs on the subsequent production of a native language (L1). However, it is unclear what mechanism underlies this effect. It has been proposed that the L1 cost reflects inhibition of L1 representation acting during L1 production; however, previous studies exploring this issue were inconclusive. It is also unsettled whether the mechanism operates on the whole-language level or is restricted to translation equivalents in the two languages. We report a study that allowed us to address both issues behaviorally with the use of ERPs while focusing on the consequences of using L2 on the production of L1. In our experiment, native speakers of Polish (L1) and learners of English (L2) named a set of pictures in L1 following a set of pictures in either L1 or L2. Half of the pictures were repeated from the preceding block and half were new; this enabled dissociation of the effects on the level of the whole language from those specific to individual lexical items. Our results are consistent with the notion that language after-effects operate at a whole-language level. Behaviorally, we observed a clear processing cost on the whole-language level and a small facilitation on the item-specific level. The whole-language effect was accompanied by an enhanced, fronto-centrally distributed negativity in the 250-350 ms time-window which we identified as the N300 (in contrast to previous research, which probably misidentified the effect as the N2), a component that presumably reflects retrieval difficulty of relevant language representations during picture naming. As such, unlike previous studies that reported N2 for naming pictures in L1 after L2 use, we propose that the reported ERPs (N300) indicate that prior usage of L2 hampers lexical access to names in L1. Based on the literature, the after-effects could be caused by L1 inhibition and/or L2 interference, but the ERPs so far have not been informative about the causal mechanism.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zofia Wodniecka
- Psychology of Language and Bilingualism Lab, Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Krakow, Poland.
| | - Jakub Szewczyk
- Psychology of Language and Bilingualism Lab, Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Krakow, Poland.
| | - Patrycja Kałamała
- Psychology of Language and Bilingualism Lab, Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Krakow, Poland
| | - Paweł Mandera
- Psychology of Language and Bilingualism Lab, Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Krakow, Poland
| | - Joanna Durlik
- Psychology of Language and Bilingualism Lab, Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Krakow, Poland
| |
Collapse
|
63
|
Mückschel M, Ziemssen T, Beste C. Properties of lower level processing modulate the actions of the norepinephrine system during response inhibition. Biol Psychol 2020; 152:107862. [PMID: 32032625 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2020.107862] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/03/2019] [Revised: 12/29/2019] [Accepted: 02/03/2020] [Indexed: 02/06/2023]
Abstract
We ask whether actions of the norepinephrine (NE) system during response inhibition are affected by properties of lower level sensory stimulus processing. We used a somato-sensory Go/Nogo task and combined ERP recordings with pupil diameter recordings as an index of NE system activity. The Go/Nogo task was designed to achieve processing of tactile stimuli predominantly over primary somatosensory (SI) and secondary somatosensory (SII) areas. The data show that response inhibition was better when stimuli were processed via SII, compared to SI areas. This was reflected by variations in the Nogo-N2/P3 associated with anterior cingulate structures. Correlations with the pupil diameter data, indicting modulations of the NE system during inhibitory control processes, were only evident when SI sensory areas were involved. These dissociable modulatory effects were associated with activations in the superior frontal gyrus. Actions of the NE system during response inhibition are modulated by properties of lower level processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Moritz Mückschel
- Cognitive Neurophysiology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, TU Dresden, Germany; MS Centre Dresden, Centre of Clinical Neuroscience, Department of Neurology, Faculty of Medicine, TU Dresden, Germany
| | - Tjalf Ziemssen
- MS Centre Dresden, Centre of Clinical Neuroscience, Department of Neurology, Faculty of Medicine, TU Dresden, Germany
| | - Christian Beste
- Cognitive Neurophysiology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, TU Dresden, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
64
|
Massa E, Köpke B, El Yagoubi R. Age-related effect on language control and executive control in bilingual and monolingual speakers: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Neuropsychologia 2020; 138:107336. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107336] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/08/2019] [Revised: 01/01/2020] [Accepted: 01/06/2020] [Indexed: 02/01/2023]
|
65
|
Effects of negative mood state on event-related potentials of restrained eating subgroups during an inhibitory control task. Behav Brain Res 2020; 377:112249. [DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112249] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/20/2019] [Revised: 09/04/2019] [Accepted: 09/17/2019] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
|
66
|
Neurocognitive Correlates of Food-Related Response Inhibition in Overweight/Obese Adults. Brain Topogr 2019; 33:101-111. [DOI: 10.1007/s10548-019-00730-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/24/2019] [Accepted: 09/12/2019] [Indexed: 01/13/2023]
|
67
|
Ludyga S, Mücke M, Colledge FMA, Pühse U, Gerber M. A Combined EEG-fNIRS Study Investigating Mechanisms Underlying the Association between Aerobic Fitness and Inhibitory Control in Young Adults. Neuroscience 2019; 419:23-33. [PMID: 31487542 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2019.08.045] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/08/2019] [Revised: 08/13/2019] [Accepted: 08/26/2019] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
The current evidence suggests that aerobic fitness is associated with inhibitory control of executive functioning in children and older adults. However, the relative contributions of different neurophysiological mechanisms to this relation remain unclear and have not yet been examined in young adults. The present study aimed to compare inhibitory control between high and low-fit young adult men, and to investigate a possible mediation of fitness effects by conflict monitoring (N450 component of event-related potentials) and lateralized oxygenation difference (LOD) in the DLPFC. For the present cross-sectional study, participants with different physical activity levels were recruited and divided into low-fit and high-fit participants based on relative power on the PWC170. A Stroop Color-Word task was administered and combined EEG-fNIRS was simultaneously utilized to assess the N450 and LOD, because these parameters are linked with behavioral performance. The results of the statistical analysis showed that high-fitcompared to low-fit participants showed less Stroop interference and lower negativity of the N450, whereas no difference was found for LOD. Path-analyses further revealed that the relation between aerobic fitness levels and Stroop interference was indirect and mediated by N450. In contrast, LOD was inversely correlated with Stroop interference, but did not explain the relation of aerobic fitness with behavioral performance. The present findings indicate that greater inhibitory control in high- compared to low-fit young men can be explained by more effective conflict monitoring. Moreover, young adults with left-lateralizedDLPFC oxygenation also show higher inhibitory control, but this oxygenation pattern is not influenced by aerobic fitness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S Ludyga
- University of Basel, Department of Sport, Exercise and Health, Basel, Switzerland.
| | - M Mücke
- University of Basel, Department of Sport, Exercise and Health, Basel, Switzerland
| | - F M A Colledge
- University of Basel, Department of Sport, Exercise and Health, Basel, Switzerland
| | - U Pühse
- University of Basel, Department of Sport, Exercise and Health, Basel, Switzerland
| | - M Gerber
- University of Basel, Department of Sport, Exercise and Health, Basel, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
68
|
Abstract
The anatomical structure of the human brain varies widely, as does individual cognitive behavior. It is important and interesting to study the relationship between brain structure and cognitive behavior. There has however been little previous work on the relationship between inhibitory control and brain structure. The goal of this study was to elucidate possible cortical markers related to inhibitory control using structural magnetic resonance imaging (sMRI) data. In this study, we analyzed sMRI data and inhibitory control behavior measurement values from 361 healthy adults from the Human Connectome Project (HCP). The data of all participants were divided into two datasets. In the first dataset, we first constructed individual brain morphometric similarity networks by calculating the inter-regional statistical similarity relationship of nine cortical characteristic measures (such as volume) for each brain area obtained from sMRI data. Areas that covary in their morphology are termed 'connected'. After that, we used a brain connectome-based predictive model (CPM) to search for 'connected' brain areas that were significantly related to inhibitory control. This is a purely data-driven method with built-in cross-validation. Two different 'connected' patterns were observed for high and low inhibitory control networks. The high inhibitory control network comprised 25 'connections' (edges between nodes), mostly involving nodes in the prefrontal and especially orbitofrontal cortex and inferior frontal gyrus. In the low inhibitory control network, nodes were scattered between parietal, occipital and limbic areas. Furthermore, these 'connections' were verified as reliable and generalizable in a cross-validation dataset. Two regions of interest, the right ventromedial prefrontal cortex including a part of medial area 10 (R.OFCmed) and left middle temporal gyrus (L.MTG) were crucial nodes in the two networks, respectively, which suggests that these two regions may be fundamentally involved in inhibitory control. Our findings potentially help to understand the relationship between areas with a correlated cortical structure and inhibitory control, and further help to reveal the brain systems related to inhibition and its disorders.
Collapse
|
69
|
Cheng CH, Tsai HY, Cheng HN. The effect of age on N2 and P3 components: A meta-analysis of Go/Nogo tasks. Brain Cogn 2019; 135:103574. [PMID: 31200173 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2019.05.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/19/2018] [Revised: 05/24/2019] [Accepted: 05/27/2019] [Indexed: 01/27/2023]
Abstract
Suppressing the neural activities to non-target stimuli becomes problematic with advancing age. Go/Nogo tasks, in which subjects are instructed to respond to a certain type of stimuli (Go) and withhold responses to other types of predefined stimuli (Nogo), have been extensively employed to study the age-related alterations of cognitive inhibition. However, it remains inconclusive whether the N2 and P3 electrophysiological responses to successful inhibition to Nogo stimuli are affected by aging processes. Thus, we performed a meta-analysis of Go/Nogo studies to investigate the age effect on Nogo-N2 and Nogo-P3 activities as well as behavioral performance of commission errors. The potential moderators regarding different probabilities of Nogo trials and levels of task difficulty on the effect sizes were also assessed. There were no significant age-related differences in commission errors. However, compared to the younger group, the elderly demonstrated reduced Nogo-N2 amplitudes, particularly in the condition where Nogo probability was less than 50%. Furthermore, age-related reduction of Nogo-P3 amplitudes and prolongation of Nogo-P3 latencies were observed in the condition where Nogo probability was less than 50%. In conclusion, our data suggest that despite similar behavioral performance in the younger and older adults, neural processing of response inhibition becomes inefficient with advancing age.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chia-Hsiung Cheng
- Department of Occupational Therapy and Graduate Institute of Behavioral Sciences, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan; Laboratory of Brain Imaging and Neural Dynamics (BIND Lab), Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan; Healthy Aging Research Center, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan; Department of Psychiatry, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou, Taiwan.
| | - Huei-Yu Tsai
- Department of Occupational Therapy and Graduate Institute of Behavioral Sciences, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan; Laboratory of Brain Imaging and Neural Dynamics (BIND Lab), Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan
| | - Hsin-Ni Cheng
- Department of Occupational Therapy and Graduate Institute of Behavioral Sciences, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan; Laboratory of Brain Imaging and Neural Dynamics (BIND Lab), Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan
| |
Collapse
|
70
|
Raz S, Koren A, Levin C. Attention, response inhibition and brain event-related potential alterations in adults with beta-thalassaemia major. Br J Haematol 2019; 186:580-591. [PMID: 31111483 DOI: 10.1111/bjh.15957] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/27/2019] [Accepted: 03/26/2019] [Indexed: 01/19/2023]
Abstract
We investigated neural correlates of cognitive function in adults with beta thalassaemia major (β-TM) compared to healthy controls using scalp-recorded event-related potentials (ERPs). Event-related potential studies in the field of β-TM are scarce and mostly limited to children. A stop-signal task was used to evaluate indices of attention and response inhibition function, considered to be the hallmark of executive control. Correlations between task performance, ERPs and haemoglobin were also examined. Results showed impaired cognitive performance in β-TM patients, as indicated by longer response times than controls. Haemoglobin was negatively correlated with response times to Go stimuli. Electrophysiological results indicated significant β-TM-related alterations in neuronal activity, reflected in greater peak amplitudes of several task-related ERP components. A possible interpretation of these ERP results is that β-TM patients need to recruit additional brain resources when dealing with cognitive challenge. Significant correlations were found between levels of haemoglobin and amplitude of all ERP components; the lower the haemoglobin, the more pronounced the ERPs amplitude. The present study represents a novel investigation of cognitive function and related brain dynamics in β-TM in adult. Integrating neuropsychological assessment and interventions into traditional disease management, may be imperative in achieving a better quality of life for these patients.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sivan Raz
- Department of Behavioral Sciences, The Centre for Psychobiological Research, The Max Stern Yezreel Valley College, Yezreel Valley.,Department of Psychology, Tel-Hai College, Upper Galilee
| | - Ariel Koren
- Paediatric Haematology Unit, Emek Medical Centre, Afula.,The Ruth and Bruce Rappaport Faculty of Medicine, Technion - Israel Institute of Technology, Haifa, Israel
| | - Carina Levin
- Paediatric Haematology Unit, Emek Medical Centre, Afula.,The Ruth and Bruce Rappaport Faculty of Medicine, Technion - Israel Institute of Technology, Haifa, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
71
|
Debruille JB, Touzel M, Segal J, Snidal C, Renoult L. A Central Component of the N1 Event-Related Brain Potential Could Index the Early and Automatic Inhibition of the Actions Systematically Activated by Objects. Front Behav Neurosci 2019; 13:95. [PMID: 31139060 PMCID: PMC6517799 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00095] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/11/2018] [Accepted: 04/17/2019] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Stimuli of the environment, like objects, systematically activate the actions they are associated to. These activations occur extremely fast. Nevertheless, behavioral data reveal that, in most cases, these activations are then automatically inhibited, around 100 ms after the occurrence of the stimulus. We thus tested whether this early inhibition could be indexed by a central component of the N1 event-related brain potential (ERP). To achieve that goal, we looked at whether this ERP component is larger in tasks that could increase the inhibition and in trials where reaction times (RTs) happen to be long. The illumination of a real space bar of a keyboard out of the dark was used as a stimulus. To maximize the modulation of the inhibition, the task participants had to perform was manipulated across blocks. A look-only task and a count task were used to increase inhibition and an immediate press task was used to decrease it. ERPs of the two block-conditions where presses had to be prevented and where the largest central N1s were predicted were compared to those elicited in the press task, differentiating the ERPs to the third of the trials where presses were the slowest from the ERPs to the third of the trials with the fastest presses. Despite larger negativities due to lateralized readiness potentials (LRPs) and despite greater attention likely in immediate press-trials, central N1s were found to be minimal for the fastest presses, intermediate for the slowest ones and maximal for the two no-press conditions. These results thus provide a strong support for the idea that the central N1 indexes an early and short lasting automatic inhibition of the actions systematically activated by objects. They also confirm that the strength of this automatic inhibition spontaneously fluctuates across trials and tasks. On the other hand, just before N1s, parietal P1s were found larger for fastest presses. They might thus index the initial activation of these actions. Finally, consistent with the idea that N300s index late inhibition processes, that occur preferentially when the task requires them, these ERPs were quasi absent for fast presses trials and much larger in the three other conditions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J. Bruno Debruille
- Department of Neuroscience, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
- Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
| | - Molly Touzel
- Department of Neuroscience, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
| | - Julia Segal
- Department of Neuroscience, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
| | - Christine Snidal
- Department of Neuroscience, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
| | - Louis Renoult
- School of Psychology, University of East Anglia, Norwich, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
72
|
Iceta S, Benoit J, Cristini P, Lambert-Porcheron S, Segrestin B, Laville M, Poulet E, Disse E. Attentional bias and response inhibition in severe obesity with food disinhibition: a study of P300 and N200 event-related potential. Int J Obes (Lond) 2019; 44:204-212. [PMID: 30967609 DOI: 10.1038/s41366-019-0360-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/22/2018] [Revised: 02/05/2019] [Accepted: 02/24/2019] [Indexed: 12/29/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND/OBJECTIVE In obesity there is growing evidence for common mechanism between food intake regulation and substance use disorders, especially more attentional bias and less cognitive control. In the present study we investigated whether severely obese subjects with or without disordered eating exhibit electroencephalographic (EEG) event-related potential (ERP) modifications as observed in substance abusers. SUBJECTS/METHODS A total of 90 women were included; 30 in the normal-weight (NW) group (18.5 < BMI < 24.5 kg/m2; no food disinhibition or restriction on the Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire) and 60 participants with BMI ≥ 35 kg/m2 were separated into two groups (n = 30): without food disinhibition (disinhibition score ≤8; ObFD- group) and with food disinhibition (score >8; ObFD+). Clinical and metabolic parameters as well as compartmental aspects (Eating Disorders Inventory-2, EDI-2) were assessed. Participants underwent an ERP recording with an auditory oddball paradigm. RESULTS The mean ± SD P300 amplitudes in Pz were significantly (p < 0.05) lower in ObFD- (12.4 ± 4.6) and ObFD+ (12.5 ± 4.4) groups than in the NW group (15.8 ± 5.9). The mean ± SD N200 amplitude in Cz was significantly lower in the ObFD- group (-2.0 ± 5.4) than in the NW group (-5.2 ± 4.2 vs; p = 0.035). N200 Cz amplitude was correlated with EDI-2 Binge eating risk score (ρ = 0.331; p = 0.01), EDI-2 Body Dissatisfaction score (ρ = 0.351; p = 0.007), and Drive for Thinness score (ρ = 0.26; p = 0.05). CONCLUSIONS The present study provides evidence for reduction of P300 and N200 amplitude in obese women and that N200 amplitude may be related to more disordered eating and eating disorder risk. This leads to consider attentional bias and response inhibition as core mechanisms in obesity and as possible targets for new therapeutic strategy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sylvain Iceta
- Centre Référent pour l'Anorexie et les Troubles du Comportement Alimentaire (CREATYON), Hospices Civils de Lyon, Lyon, France. .,INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon (CRNL), Equipe PSYR2 Centre Hospitalier Le Vinatier, Lyon, France. .,Centre Intégré de l'Obésité Rhône-Alpes; Fédération Hospitalo-Universitaire DO-iT, Service Endocrinologie-Diabète-Nutrition, Université de Lyon, Groupement Hospitalier Sud, Hospices Civils de Lyon, Lyon, France.
| | - Julien Benoit
- Centre de Recherche en Nutrition Humaine Rhône-Alpes, Univ-Lyon, CarMeN Laboratory, Université Claude Bernard Lyon1, Hospices Civils de Lyon, CENS, FCRIN/FORCE Network, Pierre Benite, France
| | - Philippe Cristini
- Centre de Recherche en Nutrition Humaine Rhône-Alpes, Univ-Lyon, CarMeN Laboratory, Université Claude Bernard Lyon1, Hospices Civils de Lyon, CENS, FCRIN/FORCE Network, Pierre Benite, France
| | - Stéphanie Lambert-Porcheron
- Centre de Recherche en Nutrition Humaine Rhône-Alpes, Univ-Lyon, CarMeN Laboratory, Université Claude Bernard Lyon1, Hospices Civils de Lyon, CENS, FCRIN/FORCE Network, Pierre Benite, France
| | - Bérénice Segrestin
- Centre Référent pour l'Anorexie et les Troubles du Comportement Alimentaire (CREATYON), Hospices Civils de Lyon, Lyon, France.,Centre de Recherche en Nutrition Humaine Rhône-Alpes, Univ-Lyon, CarMeN Laboratory, Université Claude Bernard Lyon1, Hospices Civils de Lyon, CENS, FCRIN/FORCE Network, Pierre Benite, France
| | - Martine Laville
- Centre Intégré de l'Obésité Rhône-Alpes; Fédération Hospitalo-Universitaire DO-iT, Service Endocrinologie-Diabète-Nutrition, Université de Lyon, Groupement Hospitalier Sud, Hospices Civils de Lyon, Lyon, France.,Centre de Recherche en Nutrition Humaine Rhône-Alpes, Univ-Lyon, CarMeN Laboratory, Université Claude Bernard Lyon1, Hospices Civils de Lyon, CENS, FCRIN/FORCE Network, Pierre Benite, France
| | - Emmanuel Poulet
- INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon (CRNL), Equipe PSYR2 Centre Hospitalier Le Vinatier, Lyon, France
| | - Emmanuel Disse
- Centre Intégré de l'Obésité Rhône-Alpes; Fédération Hospitalo-Universitaire DO-iT, Service Endocrinologie-Diabète-Nutrition, Université de Lyon, Groupement Hospitalier Sud, Hospices Civils de Lyon, Lyon, France.,Centre de Recherche en Nutrition Humaine Rhône-Alpes, Univ-Lyon, CarMeN Laboratory, Université Claude Bernard Lyon1, Hospices Civils de Lyon, CENS, FCRIN/FORCE Network, Pierre Benite, France
| |
Collapse
|
73
|
Flanker paradigm contains conflict and distraction factors with distinct neural mechanisms: an ERP analysis in a 2-1 mapping task. Cogn Neurodyn 2019; 13:341-356. [PMID: 31354880 DOI: 10.1007/s11571-019-09529-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/01/2018] [Revised: 02/25/2019] [Accepted: 03/18/2019] [Indexed: 01/13/2023] Open
Abstract
Behavioral studies using the flanker 2-1 mapping task suggest that both stimulus and response conflicts contribute to flanker conflict effect. However, both are intertwined with distraction effect. Their underlying neural mechanisms remain unclear. We applied a perceptual flanker 2-1 mapping task to 24 healthy young adults, while the event-related potentials were recorded. The task included stimulus-incongruent (SI), response-incongruent (RI), congruent (CO) and neutral (NE) stimuli. Our reaction time data demonstrated conflict effect, distraction effect and their interaction. Furthermore, the conflict factor successively enhanced the frontal P2 (160-240 ms), the posterior N2pc (200-240 ms), the fronto-central and the right frontal N2b (240-420 ms), and the posterior N2c (320-420 ms). Only the frontal P2 was larger for RI than SI. The distraction factor increased the right N2pc and reduced the left parietal P3b (460-480 ms). Overall, our findings suggested that the flanker conflict involved an early attentional processing of task-relevant and distractive information, and a later processing of conflict evaluation and response inhibition.
Collapse
|
74
|
Hartmann L, Wachtl L, de Lucia M, Spierer L. Practice-induced functional plasticity in inhibitory control interacts with aging. Brain Cogn 2019; 132:22-32. [PMID: 30802731 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2019.02.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/14/2018] [Revised: 02/18/2019] [Accepted: 02/18/2019] [Indexed: 12/25/2022]
Abstract
Inhibitory control deficits represent a key aspect of the cognitive declines associated with aging. Practicing inhibitory control has thus been advanced as a potential approach to compensate for age-induced neurocognitive impairments. Yet, the functional brain changes associated with practicing inhibitory control tasks in older adults and whether they differ from those observed in young populations remains unresolved. We compared electrical neuroimaging analyses of ERPs recorded during a Go/NoGo practice session with a Group (Young; Older adults) by Session (Beginning; End of the practice) design to identify whether the practice of an inhibition task in older adults reinforces already implemented compensatory activity or reduce it by enhancing the functioning of the brain networks primarily involved in the tasks. We observed an equivalent small effect of practice on performance in the two age-groups. The topographic ERP analyses and source estimations revealed qualitatively different effects of the practice over the N2 and P3 ERP components, respectively driven by a decrease in supplementary motor area activity and an increase in left ventrolateral prefrontal activity in the older but not in the young adults with practice. Our results thus indicate that inhibition task practice in older adults increases age-related divergences in the underlying functional processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lea Hartmann
- Neurology Unit, Medicine Section, Faculty of Sciences and Medicine, University of Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland
| | - Laura Wachtl
- Neurology Unit, Medicine Section, Faculty of Sciences and Medicine, University of Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland
| | - Marzia de Lucia
- Laboratoire de Recherche en Neuroimagerie (LREN), Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Lausanne University Hospital and University of Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland
| | - Lucas Spierer
- Neurology Unit, Medicine Section, Faculty of Sciences and Medicine, University of Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland.
| |
Collapse
|
75
|
Ludyga S, Koutsandréou F, Reuter EM, Voelcker-Rehage C, Budde H. A Randomized Controlled Trial on the Effects of Aerobic and Coordinative Training on Neural Correlates of Inhibitory Control in Children. J Clin Med 2019; 8:E184. [PMID: 30720733 PMCID: PMC6406880 DOI: 10.3390/jcm8020184] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/21/2018] [Revised: 01/22/2019] [Accepted: 01/26/2019] [Indexed: 12/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Whereas aerobic training has found to be beneficial for inhibitory control, less is known on the efficiency of other exercise types in children. The present study compared the effects of aerobic and coordinative training on behavioral and neurophysiological measures of inhibitory control. Forty-five children were randomly assigned (1:1:1 ratio) to groups performing aerobic training, coordinative training or assisted homework sessions over 10 weeks. Before and after intervention, all participants completed a Flanker task. The P300 component of event-related potentials elicited from the task was recorded via electroencephalography. Additionally, aerobic fitness and gross-motor skills were assessed using 20 m Shuttle Run and Heidelberg Gross-Motor Test, respectively. Statistical analyses revealed no time by group interactions for the P300 component (amplitude, latency), p = 0.976, η² = 0.007, and behavioral performance (reaction time, accuracy), p = 0.570, η² = 0.045. In contrast, there was a significant group-difference in pre- to post-test changes in aerobic fitness, p = 0.008, η² = 0.246, with greater improvements following aerobic and coordinative training compared to assisted homework sessions. In conclusion, no differences regarding the efficiency of aerobic and coordinative training for the enhancement of inhibitory control were found as both exercise programs failed to elicit changes in speed and accuracy of stimulus evaluation and the allocation of attentional resources.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sebastian Ludyga
- Department of Sport, Exercise and Health, University of Basel, 4052 Basel, Switzerland.
| | - Flora Koutsandréou
- Faculty of Human Sciences, Medical School Hamburg, 20457 Hamburg, Germany.
| | - Eva-Maria Reuter
- Centre for Sensorimotor Performance, School of Human Movement and Nutrition Sciences, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD 4072, Australia.
| | - Claudia Voelcker-Rehage
- Institute of Human Movement Science and Health, Faculty of Behavioral and Social Sciences, Chemnitz University of Technology, 09111 Chemnitz, Germany.
| | - Henning Budde
- Faculty of Human Sciences, Medical School Hamburg, 20457 Hamburg, Germany.
- Institute of Sport Science and Innovations, Lithuanian Sports University, 44221 Kaunas, Lithuania.
- Physical Activity, Physical Education, Health and Sport Research Centre (PAPESH), Sports Science Department, School of Science and Engineering, Reykjavik University, IS-101 Reykjavik, Iceland.
| |
Collapse
|
76
|
Reuter EM, Vieluf S, Koutsandreou F, Hübner L, Budde H, Godde B, Voelcker-Rehage C. A Non-linear Relationship Between Selective Attention and Associated ERP Markers Across the Lifespan. Front Psychol 2019; 10:30. [PMID: 30745886 PMCID: PMC6360996 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00030] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/02/2018] [Accepted: 01/07/2019] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The ability to selectively attend to task-relevant information increases throughout childhood and decreases in older age. Here, we intended to investigate these opposing developmental trajectories, to assess whether gains and losses early and late in life are associated with similar or different electrophysiological changes, and to get a better understanding about the development in middle-adulthood. We (re-)analyzed behavioral and electrophysiological data of 211 participants, who performed a colored Flanker task while their Electroencephalography (EEG) was recorded. Participants were subdivided into six groups depending on their age, ranging from 8 to 83 years. We analyzed response speed and accuracy as well as the event replated potential (ERP) components P1 and N1, associated with visual processing and attention, N2 as marker of interference suppression and cognitive control, and P3 as a marker of cognitive updating and stimulus categorization. Response speed and accuracy were low early and later in life, with peak performance in young adults. Similarly, ERP latencies of all components and P1 and N1 amplitudes followed a u-shape pattern with shortest latencies and smallest amplitudes occurring in middle-age. N2 amplitudes were larger in children, and for incongruent stimuli in adults middle-aged and older. P3 amplitudes showed a parietal-to-frontal shift with age. Further, group-wise regression analyses suggested that children’s performance depended on cognitive processing speed, while older adults’ performance depended on cognitive resources. Together these results imply that different mechanisms restrict performance early and late in life and suggest a non-linear relationship between electrophysiological markers and performance in the Flanker task across the lifespan.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eva-Maria Reuter
- Centre for Sensorimotor Performance, School of Human Movement and Nutrition Sciences, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
| | - Solveig Vieluf
- Institute of Sports Medicine, Paderborn University, Paderborn, Germany
| | | | - Lena Hübner
- Institute of Human Movement Science and Health, Chemnitz University of Technology, Chemnitz, Germany
| | - Henning Budde
- Faculty of Human Sciences, Medical School Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany.,Physical Activity, Physical Education, Health and Sport Research Centre, Sports Science Department, School of Science and Engineering, Reykjavik University, Reykjavik, Iceland.,Institute of Sport Science and Innovations, Lithuanian Sports University, Kaunas, Lithuania
| | - Ben Godde
- Department of Psychology and Methods, Jacobs University, Bremen, Germany
| | - Claudia Voelcker-Rehage
- Institute of Human Movement Science and Health, Chemnitz University of Technology, Chemnitz, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
77
|
Barry RJ, De Blasio FM, Fogarty JS. Caffeine affects children’s ERPs and performance in an equiprobable go/no‐go task: Testing a processing schema. Psychophysiology 2019; 56:e13330. [DOI: 10.1111/psyp.13330] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/19/2018] [Revised: 11/13/2018] [Accepted: 12/12/2018] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Robert J. Barry
- Brain & Behaviour Research Institute and School of Psychology University of Wollongong Wollongong New South Wales Australia
| | - Frances M. De Blasio
- Brain & Behaviour Research Institute and School of Psychology University of Wollongong Wollongong New South Wales Australia
| | - Jack S. Fogarty
- Brain & Behaviour Research Institute and School of Psychology University of Wollongong Wollongong New South Wales Australia
| |
Collapse
|
78
|
Karthaus M, Wascher E, Getzmann S. Effects of Visual and Acoustic Distraction on Driving Behavior and EEG in Young and Older Car Drivers: A Driving Simulation Study. Front Aging Neurosci 2018; 10:420. [PMID: 30618726 PMCID: PMC6305392 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00420] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/05/2018] [Accepted: 12/04/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Driving safety depends on the drivers' attentional focus on the driving task. Especially in complex situations, distraction due to secondary stimuli can impair driving performance. The inhibition of distractors or inadequate prepotent responses to irrelevant stimuli requires cognitive control, which is assumed to be reduced with increasing age. The present EEG study investigated the effects of secondary acoustic and visual stimuli on driving performance of younger and older car drivers in a driving simulator task. The participants had to respond to brake lights of a preceding car under different distraction conditions and with varying task difficulties. Overall, the anticipation of high demanding tasks affected braking response behavior in young and especially in older adults, who showed reduced cognitive control to task-relevant braking stimuli, as reflected by a smaller P3b. In a more easy (perception only) task, simultaneously presented acoustic stimuli accelerated braking response times (RTs) in young and older adults, which was associated with a pronounced P2. In contrast, secondary visual stimuli increased braking RTs in older adults, associated with a reduced P3b. In a more difficult (discrimination) task, braking response behavior was impaired by the presence of secondary acoustic and visual stimuli in young and older drivers. Braking RT increased (and the P3b decreased), especially when the responses to the secondary stimuli had to be suppressed. This negative effect was more pronounced with visual secondary stimuli, and especially so in the older group. In sum, the results suggest an impaired resistance to distractor interference and a reduced inhibition of prepotent responses in older drivers. This was most pronounced when the processing of task-relevant and irrelevant stimuli engage the same mental resources, for example, by sharing the same stimulus modality.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Melanie Karthaus
- IfADo – Leibniz Research Centre for Working Environment and Human Factors, Dortmund, Germany
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
79
|
Lyu Z, Zheng P, Lu S, Qin M. Impaired Conflict Monitoring to Food Cues in Women Who Binge Eat. Front Psychol 2018; 9:2585. [PMID: 30618999 PMCID: PMC6304389 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02585] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/28/2018] [Accepted: 12/03/2018] [Indexed: 12/11/2022] Open
Abstract
Previous research demonstrated the associations between cognitive biases toward food cues and binge eating (BE) behavior. To determine the characteristics of conflict monitoring to food cues in women who binge eat and non-eating disordered controls, a flanker task featured high-caloric food and low-caloric food images was used to examine conflict monitoring with measures of accuracy and reaction time. Women who binge eat displayed longer reaction times (RTs) to incongruent trials (i.e., flanked by pictures from the different category) than to congruent trials (i.e., flanked by pictures from the same category), while controls showed no such difference. This finding demonstrated women who binge eat displayed a general flanker effect toward food-related stimuli compared to controls. Faster reaction times in response to high-caloric food images disturbed by low-caloric food images predicted lower self-reported motor impulsiveness in the women who binge eat, but not in controls. These data suggest a relative conflict monitoring deficit in women with BE pathology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zhenyong Lyu
- School of Education Science, Xinyang Normal University, Xinyang, China
| | - Panpan Zheng
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Southwest University, Chongqing, China
| | - Songkai Lu
- School of Education Science, Xinyang Normal University, Xinyang, China
| | - Mingzhi Qin
- School of Education Science, Xinyang Normal University, Xinyang, China
| |
Collapse
|
80
|
Lin MY, Tseng YJ, Cheng CH. Age Effects on Spatiotemporal Dynamics of Response Inhibition: An MEG Study. Front Aging Neurosci 2018; 10:386. [PMID: 30515093 PMCID: PMC6255792 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00386] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/11/2018] [Accepted: 11/02/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Inhibition, the ability to suppress irrelevant information, thoughts or movements, is crucial for humans to perform context-appropriate behaviors. It was suggested that declined cognitive performance in older adults might be attributed to inhibitory deficiencies. Although previous studies have shown an age-associated reduction in inhibitory ability, the understanding regarding its cortical spatiotemporal maps remained limited. Thus, we used a whole-head magnetoencephalography (MEG) to elucidate the age effects on response inhibition, and to explore the brain activation differences in high- and low-performing seniors. We recruited 22 younger and 22 older adults to participate in the visual Go/No-go task. Both behavioral performance and neuromagnetic responses to No-go stimuli were analyzed. The behavioral results showed that the older adults made more false alarm (FA) errors than the younger adults did. The MEG results showed that the seniors exhibited declined cortical activities in middle temporal gyrus (MTG) and delayed activation in MTG, prefrontal cortex (PFC) and pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA). Furthermore, among the older adults, more recruitment of the left PFC was found in the high-performers than in the lower-performers. In conclusion, age-related deficiencies in response inhibition were observed in both behavioral performance and neurophysiological measurement. Our results also suggested that frontal recruitment plays a compensatory role in successful inhibition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mei-Yin Lin
- Department of Occupational Therapy and Graduate Institute of Behavioral Sciences, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan.,Laboratory of Brain Imaging and Neural Dynamics (BIND Lab), Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan.,Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Taichung Hospital, Ministry of Health and Welfare, Taichung, Taiwan
| | - Yi-Jhan Tseng
- Department of Medical Research, Hsinchu MacKay Memorial Hospital, Hsinchu, Taiwan
| | - Chia-Hsiung Cheng
- Department of Occupational Therapy and Graduate Institute of Behavioral Sciences, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan.,Laboratory of Brain Imaging and Neural Dynamics (BIND Lab), Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan.,Healthy Aging Research Center, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan.,Department of Psychiatry, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou, Taiwan
| |
Collapse
|
81
|
Zhao X, Liu X, Maes JHR. Male Smokers’ Behavioral and Brain Responses to Deviant Cigarette-Related Stimuli in a Two-Choice Oddball Paradigm. J PSYCHOPHYSIOL 2018. [DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000195] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/28/2022]
Abstract
Abstract. Experimental studies on smoking and response-inhibition capacity have revealed inconsistent findings, which might be due to differences in sensitivity of the behavioral paradigms used. Here we aimed to replicate the impaired response inhibition in male smokers that was found in a previous study using a two-choice oddball task. This task enables the use of response times as index of inhibition capacity and equalizes the response requirement for the different trial types. In addition, we measured event-related brain potentials to explore the nature of the cognitive processes underlying the behavioral difference. Smokers (n = 19) and non-smokers (n = 19) were asked to make a different response to frequent standard stimuli (cigarette-unrelated pictures) than to infrequent deviant stimuli (cigarette-related pictures). Compared to non-smokers, smokers took a longer time to respond to deviant but not standard stimuli. In addition, smokers, but not non-smokers, displayed a smaller N2 amplitude to deviant than standard stimuli, and only the non-smokers showed larger P3 amplitudes to deviant compared to standard stimuli. Moreover, the response time (RT) measure was differentially correlated with N2 and P3 amplitudes in smokers and non-smokers. The joint results support the notion of deviant cognitive processes in smokers compared to non-smokers that are either directly or indirectly related to response-inhibition capacity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xin Zhao
- Behavior Rehabilitation Training Research Institution, School of Psychology, Northwest Normal University, Lanzhou, PR China
| | - Xiaoting Liu
- Behavior Rehabilitation Training Research Institution, School of Psychology, Northwest Normal University, Lanzhou, PR China
| | - Joseph H. R. Maes
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Centre for Cognition, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
82
|
Gallant SN, Pun C, Yang L. Age differences in the neural correlates underlying control of emotional memory: An event-related potential study. Brain Res 2018; 1697:83-92. [DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2018.06.019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/12/2018] [Revised: 06/07/2018] [Accepted: 06/15/2018] [Indexed: 10/14/2022]
|
83
|
Gajewski PD, Ferdinand NK, Kray J, Falkenstein M. Understanding sources of adult age differences in task switching: Evidence from behavioral and ERP studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2018; 92:255-275. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.05.029] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/20/2017] [Revised: 05/18/2018] [Accepted: 05/28/2018] [Indexed: 01/14/2023]
|
84
|
Moderate-intensity exercise boosts the N2 neural inhibition marker: A randomized and counterbalanced ERP study with precisely controlled exercise intensity. Biol Psychol 2018; 135:170-179. [DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.04.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/08/2017] [Revised: 04/06/2018] [Accepted: 04/09/2018] [Indexed: 12/19/2022]
|
85
|
Ross RS, Smolen A, Curran T, Nyhus E. MAO-A Phenotype Effects Response Sensitivity and the Parietal Old/New Effect during Recognition Memory. Front Hum Neurosci 2018; 12:53. [PMID: 29487517 PMCID: PMC5816743 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00053] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/26/2017] [Accepted: 01/31/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
A critical problem for developing personalized treatment plans for cognitive disruptions is the lack of understanding how individual differences influence cognition. Recognition memory is one cognitive ability that varies from person to person and that variation may be related to different genetic phenotypes. One gene that may impact recognition memory is the monoamine oxidase A gene (MAO-A), which influences the transcription rate of MAO-A. Examination of how MAO-A phenotypes impact behavioral and event-related potentials (ERPs) correlates of recognition memory may help explain individual differences in recognition memory performance. Therefore, the current study uses electroencephalography (EEG) in combination with genetic phenotyping of the MAO-A gene to determine how well-characterized ERP components of recognition memory, the early frontal old/new effect, left parietal old/new effect, late frontal old/new effect, and the late posterior negativity (LPN) are impacted by MAO-A phenotype during item and source memory. Our results show that individuals with the MAO-A phenotype leading to increased transcription have lower response sensitivity during both item and source memory. Additionally, during item memory the left parietal old/new effect is not present due to increased ERP amplitude for correct rejections. The results suggest that MAO-A phenotype changes EEG correlates of recognition memory and influences how well individuals differentiate between old and new items.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robert S Ross
- Neuroscience and Behavior Program, Department of Psychology, University of New Hampshire, Durham, NH, United States
| | - Andrew Smolen
- Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO, United States
| | - Tim Curran
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO, United States
| | - Erika Nyhus
- Department of Psychology and Program in Neuroscience, Bowdoin College, Brunswick, ME, United States
| |
Collapse
|
86
|
Zhao G, Chen F, Zhang Q, Shen M, Gao Z. Feature-based information filtering in visual working memory is impaired in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia 2018; 111:317-323. [PMID: 29427571 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.02.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/11/2017] [Revised: 02/05/2018] [Accepted: 02/05/2018] [Indexed: 12/22/2022]
Abstract
Increasing attention has been given to working memory (WM) impairment in Parkinson's disease (PD) patients. Previous studies revealed that the space-orientated feature-based filtering (target and distractors in distinct locations) was impaired in PD patients. However, the object-orientated feature-based filtering (target and distractor information pertaining to one object) ability in PD patients remains unclear. In this study, we examined the object-orientated feature-based filtering ability of 14 PD patients and 14 healthy controls in a change detection task under EEG monitoring. Participants were asked to remember the colors of two different objects while ignoring their shapes. Critically, the irrelevant feature could be changed in the probe. A failure in complete feature-based filtering would lead to an "irrelevant-change distracting effect," where the change of the irrelevant feature would impair the performance of the target feature, and lead to an enhanced anterior N2. We found that the distracting effect was larger in PD patients than in the control group in terms of d'; however, the N2 amplitude evoked by the irrelevant change was smaller in PD patients than in the control group. These results suggested that the object-orientated feature-based filtering ability was impaired in PD, which might derive from the deficit of their executive control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Guohua Zhao
- Department of Neurology, Second Affiliated Hospital, College of Medicine, Zhejiang University, China
| | - Feiyan Chen
- Bio-X Laboratory, Department of Physics, Zhejiang University, China
| | - Qiong Zhang
- Department of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, Zhejiang University, China.
| | - Mowei Shen
- National Key Lab of Human Factors, Department of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, Zhejiang University, China; Department of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, Zhejiang University, China.
| | - Zaifeng Gao
- National Key Lab of Human Factors, Department of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, Zhejiang University, China; Department of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, Zhejiang University, China
| |
Collapse
|
87
|
Fogarty JS, Barry RJ, De Blasio FM, Steiner GZ. ERP components and behavior in the auditory equiprobable go/no-go task: Inhibition in young adults. Psychophysiology 2018; 55:e13065. [DOI: 10.1111/psyp.13065] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/22/2017] [Revised: 12/11/2017] [Accepted: 12/11/2017] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Jack S. Fogarty
- Brain & Behaviour Research Institute, and School of Psychology; University of Wollongong; Wollongong New South Wales Australia
| | - Robert J. Barry
- Brain & Behaviour Research Institute, and School of Psychology; University of Wollongong; Wollongong New South Wales Australia
| | - Frances M. De Blasio
- Brain & Behaviour Research Institute, and School of Psychology; University of Wollongong; Wollongong New South Wales Australia
| | - Genevieve Z. Steiner
- Brain & Behaviour Research Institute, and School of Psychology; University of Wollongong; Wollongong New South Wales Australia
- NICM, and Translational Health Research Institute (THRI), Western Sydney University; Campbelltown New South Wales Australia
| |
Collapse
|
88
|
Zhao Q, Li H, Hu B, Wu H, Liu Q. Abstinent Heroin Addicts Tend to Take Risks: ERP and Source Localization. Front Neurosci 2017; 11:681. [PMID: 29270107 PMCID: PMC5723666 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00681] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/12/2017] [Accepted: 11/21/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Abnormal decision making is a behavioral characteristic of drug addiction. Indeed, drug addicts prefer immediate rewards at the expense of future interests. Assessing the neurocognitive basis of decision-making related to drug dependence, combining event-related potential (ERP) analysis and source localization techniques, may provide new insights into understanding decision-making deficits in drug addicts and further guide withdrawal treatment. In this study, EEG was performed in 20 abstinent heroin addicts (AHAs) and 20 age-, education- and gender-matched healthy controls (HCs) while they participated in a simple two-choice gambling task (99 vs. 9). Our behavioral results showed that AHAs tend to select higher-risk choices compared with HCs (i.e., more "99" choices than "9"). ERP results showed that right hemisphere preponderance of stimulus-preceding negativity was disrupted in AHAs, but not in HCs. Feedback-related negativity of difference wave was higher in AHAs than HCs, with the P300 amplitude associated with risk magnitude and valence. Using source localization that allows identification of abnormal brain activity in consequential cognitive stages, including the reward expectation and outcome evaluation stages, we found abnormalities in both behavioral and neural responses on gambling in AHAs. Taken together, our findings suggest AHAs have risk-prone tendency and dysfunction in adaptive decision making, since they continue to choose risky options even after accruing considerable negative scores, and fail to shift to a safer strategy to avoid risk. Such abnormal decision-making bias to risk and immediate reward seeking may be accompanied by abnormal reward expectation and evaluation in AHAs, which explains their high risk-seeking and impulsivity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Qinglin Zhao
- Ubiquitous Awareness and Intelligent Solutions Lab, Lanzhou University, Lanzhou, China
| | - Hongqian Li
- Ubiquitous Awareness and Intelligent Solutions Lab, Lanzhou University, Lanzhou, China
| | - Bin Hu
- Ubiquitous Awareness and Intelligent Solutions Lab, Lanzhou University, Lanzhou, China
| | - Haiyan Wu
- Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China.,Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
| | - Quanying Liu
- Movement Control & Neuroplasticity Research Group, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium.,Neural Control of Movement Laboratory, ETH Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
89
|
Kałamała P, Szewczyk J, Senderecka M, Wodniecka Z. Flanker task with equiprobable congruent and incongruent conditions does not elicit the conflict N2. Psychophysiology 2017; 55. [DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12980] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/02/2016] [Revised: 06/13/2017] [Accepted: 07/16/2017] [Indexed: 01/10/2023]
Affiliation(s)
| | - Jakub Szewczyk
- Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University; Kraków Poland
| | | | - Zofia Wodniecka
- Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University; Kraków Poland
| |
Collapse
|
90
|
Ellis AJ, Kinzel C, Salgari GC, Loo SK. Frontal alpha asymmetry predicts inhibitory processing in youth with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychologia 2017; 102:45-51. [PMID: 28587767 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.06.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/19/2017] [Revised: 05/30/2017] [Accepted: 06/02/2017] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Atypical asymmetry in brain activity has been implicated in the behavioral and attentional dysregulation observed in ADHD. Specifically, asymmetry in neural activity in the right versus left frontal regions has been linked to ADHD, as well as to symptoms often associated with ADHD such as heightened approach behaviors, impulsivity and difficulties with inhibition. Clarifying the role of frontal asymmetry in ADHD-like traits, such as disinhibition, may provide information on the neurophysiological processes underlying these behaviors. METHOD ADHD youth (ADHD: n = 25) and healthy, typically developing controls (TD: n = 25) underwent an electroencephalography (EEG) recording while completing a go/no-go task-a commonly used test measuring behavioral inhibition. In addition, advanced signal processing for source localization estimated the location of signal generators underlying frontal alpha asymmetry (FA) during correct and incorrect trials. RESULTS This is the first study in ADHD to demonstrate that the dorsal-lateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) may be responsible for generating frontal alpha. During failed inhibition trials, ADHD youth displayed greater FA than TD youth. In addition, within the ADHD group, frontal asymmetry during later processing stages (i.e., 400-800ms after stimulus) predicted a higher number of commission errors throughout the task. CONCLUSIONS These results suggest that frontal alpha asymmetry may be a specific biomarker of cognitive disinhibition among youth with ADHD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alissa J Ellis
- Semel Institute of Neuroscience and Human Behavior, Department of Psychiatry, University of California, Los Angeles, USA.
| | - Chantelle Kinzel
- Semel Institute of Neuroscience and Human Behavior, Department of Psychiatry, University of California, Los Angeles, USA
| | - Giulia C Salgari
- Semel Institute of Neuroscience and Human Behavior, Department of Psychiatry, University of California, Los Angeles, USA
| | - Sandra K Loo
- Semel Institute of Neuroscience and Human Behavior, Department of Psychiatry, University of California, Los Angeles, USA
| |
Collapse
|
91
|
Liebrand M, Pein I, Tzvi E, Krämer UM. Temporal Dynamics of Proactive and Reactive Motor Inhibition. Front Hum Neurosci 2017; 11:204. [PMID: 28496405 PMCID: PMC5406465 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00204] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/28/2016] [Accepted: 04/07/2017] [Indexed: 01/14/2023] Open
Abstract
Proactive motor inhibition refers to endogenous preparatory mechanisms facilitating action inhibition, whereas reactive motor inhibition is considered to be a sudden stopping process triggered by external signals. Previous studies were inconclusive about the temporal dynamics of involved neurocognitive processes during proactive and reactive motor control. Using electroencephalography (EEG), we investigated the time-course of proactive and reactive inhibition, measuring event-related oscillations and event-related potentials (ERPs). Participants performed in a cued go/nogo paradigm with cues indicating whether the motor response might or might not have to be inhibited. Based on the dual mechanisms of control (DMC) framework by Braver, we investigated the role of attentional effects, motor preparation in the sensorimotor cortex and prefrontal cognitive control mechanisms, separating effects before and after target onset. In the cue-target interval, proactive motor inhibition was associated with increased attention, reflected in reduced visual alpha power and an increased contingent negative variation (CNV). At the same time, motor inhibition was modulated by reduced sensorimotor beta power. After target onset, proactive inhibition resulted in an increased N1, indicating allocation of attention towards relevant stimuli, increased prefrontal beta power and a modulation of sensorimotor mu activity. As in previous studies, reactive stopping of motor actions was associated with increased prefrontal beta power and increased sensorimotor beta activity. The results stress the relevance of attentional mechanisms for proactive inhibition and speak for different neurocognitive mechanisms being involved in the early preparation for and in later implementation of motor inhibition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matthias Liebrand
- Department of Neurology, University of LübeckLübeck, Germany.,Graduate School for Computing in Medicine and Life Sciences, University of LübeckLübeck, Germany
| | - Inga Pein
- Department of Neurology, University of LübeckLübeck, Germany
| | - Elinor Tzvi
- Department of Neurology, University of LübeckLübeck, Germany
| | - Ulrike M Krämer
- Department of Neurology, University of LübeckLübeck, Germany.,Institute of Psychology II, University of LübeckLübeck, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
92
|
Reuter EM, Voelcker-Rehage C, Vieluf S, Parianen Lesemann F, Godde B. The P3 Parietal-To-Frontal Shift Relates to Age-Related Slowing in a Selective Attention Task. J PSYCHOPHYSIOL 2017. [DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000167] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/11/2023]
Abstract
Abstract. Older adults recruit relatively more frontal as compared to parietal resources in a variety of cognitive and perceptual tasks. It is not yet clear whether this parietal-to-frontal shift is a compensatory mechanism, or simply reflects a reduction in processing efficiency. In this study we aimed to investigate how the parietal-to-frontal shift with aging relates to selective attention. Fourteen young and 26 older healthy adults performed a color Flanker task under three conditions (incongruent, congruent, neutral) and event-related potentials (ERPs) were measured. The P3 was analyzed for the electrode positions Pz, Cz, and Fz as an indicator of the parietal-to-frontal shift. Further, behavioral performance and other ERP components (P1 and N1 at electrodes O1 and O2; N2 at electrodes Fz and Cz) were investigated. First young and older adults were compared. Older adults had longer response times, reduced accuracy, longer P3 latencies, and a more frontal distribution of P3 than young adults. These results confirm the parietal-to-frontal shift in the P3 with age for the selective attention task. Second, based on the differences between frontal and parietal P3 activity the group of older adults was subdivided into those showing a rather equal distribution of the P3 and older participants showing a strong frontal focus of the P3. Older adults with a more frontally distributed P3 had longer response times than participants with a more equally distributed P3. These results suggest that the frontally distributed P3 observed in older adults has no compensatory function in selective attention but rather indicates less efficient processing and slowing with age.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eva-Maria Reuter
- Jacobs Center on Lifelong Learning and Institutional Development, Jacobs University Bremen, Germany
- Centre for Sensorimotor Performance, School of Human Movement and Nutrition Sciences, The University of Queensland, St. Lucia, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
| | - Claudia Voelcker-Rehage
- Jacobs Center on Lifelong Learning and Institutional Development, Jacobs University Bremen, Germany
- Institute of Human Movement Science and Health, Technical University Chemnitz, Germany
| | - Solveig Vieluf
- Jacobs Center on Lifelong Learning and Institutional Development, Jacobs University Bremen, Germany
- CNRS, Institut des Sciences du Mouvement UMR 7287, Aix-Marseille Université, Marseille cedex, France
| | - Franca Parianen Lesemann
- Jacobs Center on Lifelong Learning and Institutional Development, Jacobs University Bremen, Germany
- Department of Social Neuroscience, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Ben Godde
- Jacobs Center on Lifelong Learning and Institutional Development, Jacobs University Bremen, Germany
- Department of Psychology & Methods, Focus Area Diversity, Jacobs University Bremen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
93
|
Knežević M, Marinković K. Neurodynamic correlates of response inhibition from emerging to mid adulthood. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2017; 43:106-118. [PMID: 29081593 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.03.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/10/2023]
Abstract
Response inhibition, a key executive function, continues to develop in early adulthood in parallel with maturational processes of the underlying prefrontal regions known to support it. The current study examined behavioral and neurophysiological correlates of response inhibition during a visual Go/No-Go task in a large sample (N = 120) comprised of participants in their Early 20s (ages 19-21), Mid 20s (ages 23-27), and Early 30s (ages 28-42). The two younger groups had lower accuracy, shorter reaction times, and made more premature responses compared to Early 30s. These impulsive behavioral tendencies were reflected in a delayed N2 latency and reduced P2 and P3 amplitudes for Early 20s compared to Early 30s and were associated with personality traits such as impulsivity in an age-dependent manner. The results suggest that response inhibition may not develop fully before the approximate age of 25, as the refinement of the primarily prefrontal cognitive control network follows a protracted developmental trajectory throughout young adulthood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Martina Knežević
- Psychology Department, Croatian Studies, University of Zagreb, Croatia.,Psychology Department, San Diego State University, San Diego, USA
| | - Ksenija Marinković
- Psychology Department, San Diego State University, San Diego, USA.,Radiology Department, University of California, San Diego, USA
| |
Collapse
|
94
|
Dierolf AM, Fechtner J, Böhnke R, Wolf OT, Naumann E. Influence of acute stress on response inhibition in healthy men: An ERP study. Psychophysiology 2017; 54:684-695. [DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12826] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/23/2016] [Accepted: 12/07/2016] [Indexed: 12/16/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | - Julia Fechtner
- Department of Psychology; University of Trier; Trier Germany
| | - Robina Böhnke
- Department of Psychology; University of Trier; Trier Germany
| | - Oliver T. Wolf
- Department of Cognitive Psychology; Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, Ruhr-University Bochum; Bochum Germany
| | - Ewald Naumann
- Department of Psychology; University of Trier; Trier Germany
| |
Collapse
|
95
|
Inhibiting prepotent responses in the elderly: Distraction and disinhibition. COGNITIVE AFFECTIVE & BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE 2016; 16:124-34. [PMID: 26369924 DOI: 10.3758/s13415-015-0378-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/23/2023]
Abstract
In this study, we aimed to examine whether older adults, relative to younger adults, suffer from generic inhibition, selective inhibition, and/or distraction deficits, as assessed by behavioral and electrophysiological measures in a go/no-go task paradigm that included manipulations of no-go stimulus type (irrelevant vs. conflict) and no-go probability. A total of 96 individuals were recruited; each of three experiments included 32 participants (16 adults above and 16 adults below 60 years of age). The older adults performed more poorly than the younger adults in our behavioral test; however, the event-related potential results showed that irrelevant and conflict no-go stimuli incurred different processes that were differentially impacted by aging, as was reflected in the N2 and P3. That is, the older adults' inhibition deficits might be due to different underlying mechanisms: disproportionate processing of irrelevant no-go stimuli, and disproportionate suppression of conflicting information when executing or withholding a response to conflict no-go stimuli. The present results therefore support the theories of age-related selective inhibition and distraction deficits.
Collapse
|
96
|
Event-related potentials and cognition in Parkinson’s disease: An integrative review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2016; 71:691-714. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 67] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/29/2016] [Revised: 06/30/2016] [Accepted: 08/02/2016] [Indexed: 12/14/2022]
|
97
|
Chuderski A, Senderecka M, Kałamała P, Kroczek B, Ociepka M. ERP correlates of the conflict level in the multi-response Stroop task. Brain Res 2016; 1650:93-102. [DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.08.041] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/07/2016] [Revised: 08/18/2016] [Accepted: 08/29/2016] [Indexed: 01/22/2023]
|
98
|
N2 and P3 modulation during partial inhibition in a modified go/nogo task. Int J Psychophysiol 2016; 107:63-71. [DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.07.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/18/2016] [Revised: 06/14/2016] [Accepted: 07/06/2016] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
|
99
|
Räling R, Schröder A, Wartenburger I. The origins of age of acquisition and typicality effects: Semantic processing in aphasia and the ageing brain. Neuropsychologia 2016; 86:80-92. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.04.019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/22/2015] [Revised: 04/01/2016] [Accepted: 04/18/2016] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
100
|
Hsieh S, Wu M, Tang CH. Adaptive Strategies for the Elderly in Inhibiting Irrelevant and Conflict No-Go Trials while Performing the Go/No-Go Task. Front Aging Neurosci 2016; 7:243. [PMID: 26779012 PMCID: PMC4701916 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00243] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/20/2015] [Accepted: 12/10/2015] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
This study aimed to differentiate whether or not older adults are more prone to distraction or conflict, as induced by irrelevant and conflict no-go stimuli (irNOGO and cfNOGO), respectively. This study also aimed to determine whether or not older adults would devote more effort to withholding a no-go trial in the higher-control demand condition (20% no-go trials' probability) as compared to the lower-control demand condition (50 and 80% no-go trials' probability). A total of 96 individuals were recruited, and each of the three no-go trials' probability conditions included 32 participants (16 younger adults and 16 older adults). Both behavioral and event-related potential (ERP) data were measured. The behavioral results showed that the older adults performed more poorly than the younger adults for the go trials, as reflected by slower reaction times (RTs) and higher numbers of omission errors in the go trials. In contrast, in the no-go trials, the older adults counter-intuitively exhibited similar behavioral performance (i.e., equivalent commission errors) as compared to the younger adults. The ERP data further showed that the older adults (but not the younger adults) exhibited larger P3 peak amplitudes for the irNOGO than cfNOGO trials. Yet, on the other hand, the older adults performed more poorly (i.e., had more commission errors) in the cfNOGO than irNOGO trials. These results seem to suggest that the older adults recruited more control processes in order to conquer the commitment of responses in the no-go trials, especially in the irNOGO trials. This age-related compensatory response of recruiting more control processes was specifically seen in the 20% no-go trial probability condition. This study therefore provides a deeper understanding into how older adults adopt strategies for performing the go/no-go task such as devoting more control processes to inhibiting the irNOGO trials compared to the cfNOGO trials in order to cope with their deficient inhibition ability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shulan Hsieh
- Cognitive Electrophysiology Laboratory: Control, Aging, Sleep, and Emotion (CASE), Department of Psychology, National Cheng Kung UniversityTainan, Taiwan; Institute of Allied Health Sciences, National Cheng Kung UniversityTainan, Taiwan
| | - Mengyao Wu
- Cognitive Electrophysiology Laboratory: Control, Aging, Sleep, and Emotion (CASE), Department of Psychology, National Cheng Kung UniversityTainan, Taiwan; Department of Occupational Therapy, Shu-Zen Junior College of Medicine and ManagementKaohsiung, Taiwan
| | - Chien-Hui Tang
- Cognitive Electrophysiology Laboratory: Control, Aging, Sleep, and Emotion (CASE), Department of Psychology, National Cheng Kung University Tainan, Taiwan
| |
Collapse
|