51
|
Munneke J, Heslenfeld DJ, Usrey WM, Theeuwes J, Mangun GR. Preparatory effects of distractor suppression: evidence from visual cortex. PLoS One 2011; 6:e27700. [PMID: 22164213 PMCID: PMC3229494 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027700] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/07/2011] [Accepted: 10/23/2011] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Spatial selective attention is the mechanism that facilitates the selection of relevant information over irrelevant information in the visual field. The current study investigated whether foreknowledge of the presence or absence of distractors surrounding an impending target stimulus results in preparatory changes in visual cortex. We cued the location of the target and the presence or absence of distractors surrounding the target while changes in blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) signals were measured. In line with prior work, we found that top-down spatial attention resulted in an increased contralateral BOLD response, evoked by the cue throughout early visual cortex (areas V1, V2 and V3). In addition, cues indicating distractor presence evoked a substantial increase in the magnitude of the BOLD signal in visual area V3, but not in V2 or V1. This study shows that prior knowledge concerning the presence of a distractor results in enhanced attentional modulation of visual cortex, in visual areas where neuronal receptive fields are large enough to encompass both targets and distractors. We interpret these findings as evidence that top-down attentional control processes include active preparatory suppression mechanisms for irrelevant, distracting information in the visual scene.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jaap Munneke
- Department of Cognitive Psychology, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
52
|
Strasburger H, Rentschler I, Jüttner M. Peripheral vision and pattern recognition: a review. J Vis 2011; 11:13. [PMID: 22207654 PMCID: PMC11073400 DOI: 10.1167/11.5.13] [Citation(s) in RCA: 330] [Impact Index Per Article: 25.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/18/2011] [Accepted: 09/06/2011] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
We summarize the various strands of research on peripheral vision and relate them to theories of form perception. After a historical overview, we describe quantifications of the cortical magnification hypothesis, including an extension of Schwartz's cortical mapping function. The merits of this concept are considered across a wide range of psychophysical tasks, followed by a discussion of its limitations and the need for non-spatial scaling. We also review the eccentricity dependence of other low-level functions including reaction time, temporal resolution, and spatial summation, as well as perimetric methods. A central topic is then the recognition of characters in peripheral vision, both at low and high levels of contrast, and the impact of surrounding contours known as crowding. We demonstrate how Bouma's law, specifying the critical distance for the onset of crowding, can be stated in terms of the retinocortical mapping. The recognition of more complex stimuli, like textures, faces, and scenes, reveals a substantial impact of mid-level vision and cognitive factors. We further consider eccentricity-dependent limitations of learning, both at the level of perceptual learning and pattern category learning. Generic limitations of extrafoveal vision are observed for the latter in categorization tasks involving multiple stimulus classes. Finally, models of peripheral form vision are discussed. We report that peripheral vision is limited with regard to pattern categorization by a distinctly lower representational complexity and processing speed. Taken together, the limitations of cognitive processing in peripheral vision appear to be as significant as those imposed on low-level functions and by way of crowding.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hans Strasburger
- Institut für Medizinische Psychologie, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, München, Germany
| | - Ingo Rentschler
- Institut für Medizinische Psychologie, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, München, Germany
| | - Martin Jüttner
- Department of Psychology, School of Life & Health Sciences, Aston University, Birmingham, UK
| |
Collapse
|
53
|
Utochkin IS. Hide-and-seek around the centre of interest: The dead zone of attention revealed by change blindness. VISUAL COGNITION 2011. [DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2011.613421] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/17/2022]
|
54
|
Abstract
Visual cognition, high-level vision, mid-level vision and top-down processing all refer to decision-based scene analyses that combine prior knowledge with retinal input to generate representations. The label "visual cognition" is little used at present, but research and experiments on mid- and high-level, inference-based vision have flourished, becoming in the 21st century a significant, if often understated part, of current vision research. How does visual cognition work? What are its moving parts? This paper reviews the origins and architecture of visual cognition and briefly describes some work in the areas of routines, attention, surfaces, objects, and events (motion, causality, and agency). Most vision scientists avoid being too explicit when presenting concepts about visual cognition, having learned that explicit models invite easy criticism. What we see in the literature is ample evidence for visual cognition, but few or only cautious attempts to detail how it might work. This is the great unfinished business of vision research: at some point we will be done with characterizing how the visual system measures the world and we will have to return to the question of how vision constructs models of objects, surfaces, scenes, and events.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Patrick Cavanagh
- Centre Attention & Vision, LPP CNRS UMR 8158, Université Paris Descartes, Paris, France.
| |
Collapse
|
55
|
Abstract
In the present review, we address the relationship between attention and visual stability. Even though with each eye, head and body movement the retinal image changes dramatically, we perceive the world as stable and are able to perform visually guided actions. However, visual stability is not as complete as introspection would lead us to believe. We attend to only a few items at a time and stability is maintained only for those items. There appear to be two distinct mechanisms underlying visual stability. The first is a passive mechanism: the visual system assumes the world to be stable, unless there is a clear discrepancy between the pre- and post-saccadic image of the region surrounding the saccade target. This is related to the pre-saccadic shift of attention, which allows for an accurate preview of the saccade target. The second is an active mechanism: information about attended objects is remapped within retinotopic maps to compensate for eye movements. The locus of attention itself, which is also characterized by localized retinotopic activity, is remapped as well. We conclude that visual attention is crucial in our perception of a stable world.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sebastiaan Mathôt
- Department of Cognitive Psychology, Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
| | | |
Collapse
|
56
|
Modelling Visual Search with the Selective Attention for Identification Model (VS-SAIM): A Novel Explanation for Visual Search Asymmetries. Cognit Comput 2010; 3:185-205. [PMID: 21475687 PMCID: PMC3059816 DOI: 10.1007/s12559-010-9076-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/25/2010] [Accepted: 10/06/2010] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
In earlier work, we developed the Selective Attention for Identification Model (SAIM [16]). SAIM models the human ability to perform translation-invariant object identification in multiple object scenes. SAIM suggests that central for this ability is an interaction between parallel competitive processes in a selection stage and a object identification stage. In this paper, we applied the model to visual search experiments involving simple lines and letters. We presented successful simulation results for asymmetric and symmetric searches and for the influence of background line orientations. Search asymmetry refers to changes in search performance when the roles of target item and non-target item (distractor) are swapped. In line with other models of visual search, the results suggest that a large part of the empirical evidence can be explained by competitive processes in the brain, which are modulated by the similarity between target and distractor. The simulations also suggest that another important factor is the feature properties of distractors. Finally, the simulations indicate that search asymmetries can be the outcome of interactions between top-down (knowledge about search items) and bottom-up (feature of search items) processing. This interaction in VS-SAIM is dominated by a novel mechanism, the knowledge-based on-centre-off-surround receptive field. This receptive field is reminiscent of the classical receptive fields but the exact shape is modulated by both, top-down and bottom-up processes. The paper discusses supporting evidence for the existence of this novel concept.
Collapse
|
57
|
Theeuwes J. Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2010; 135:77-99. [PMID: 20507828 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.02.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 740] [Impact Index Per Article: 52.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/14/2009] [Revised: 02/13/2010] [Accepted: 02/16/2010] [Indexed: 10/19/2022] Open
Abstract
The present paper argues for the notion that when attention is spread across the visual field in the first sweep of information through the brain visual selection is completely stimulus-driven. Only later in time, through recurrent feedback processing, volitional control based on expectancy and goal set will bias visual selection in a top-down manner. Here we review behavioral evidence as well as evidence from ERP, fMRI, TMS and single cell recording consistent with stimulus-driven selection. Alternative viewpoints that assume a large role for top-down processing are discussed. It is argued that in most cases evidence supporting top-down control on visual selection in fact demonstrates top-down control on processes occurring later in time, following initial selection. We conclude that top-down knowledge regarding non-spatial features of the objects cannot alter the initial selection priority. Only by adjusting the size of the attentional window, the initial sweep of information through the brain may be altered in a top-down way.
Collapse
|
58
|
Grossberg S, Vladusich T. How do children learn to follow gaze, share joint attention, imitate their teachers, and use tools during social interactions? Neural Netw 2010; 23:940-65. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neunet.2010.07.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/25/2010] [Accepted: 07/29/2010] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
|
59
|
From reorienting of attention to biased competition: Evidence from hemifield effects. Atten Percept Psychophys 2010; 72:651-7. [DOI: 10.3758/app.72.3.651] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
60
|
Attentional inhibition mediates inattentional blindness. Conscious Cogn 2010; 19:636-43. [PMID: 20227894 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.02.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/12/2009] [Revised: 02/12/2010] [Accepted: 02/18/2010] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
Salient stimuli presented at unattended locations are not always perceived, a phenomenon termed inattentional blindness. We hypothesized that inattentional blindness may be mediated by attentional inhibition. It has been shown that attentional inhibition effects are maximal near an attended location. If our hypothesis is correct, inattentional blindness effects should similarly be maximal near an attended location. During central fixation, participants viewed rapidly presented colored digits at a peripheral location. An unexpected black circle (the critical stimulus) was concurrently presented. Participants were instructed to maintain central fixation and name each color/digit, requiring focused attention to that location. For each participant, the critical stimulus was presented either near to or far from the attended location (at the same eccentricity). In support of our hypothesis, inattentional blindness effects were maximal near the attended location, but only at intermediate task accuracy.
Collapse
|
61
|
Hopf JM, Boehler CN, Schoenfeld MA, Heinze HJ, Tsotsos JK. The spatial profile of the focus of attention in visual search: insights from MEG recordings. Vision Res 2010; 50:1312-20. [PMID: 20117126 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.01.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/17/2009] [Revised: 01/12/2010] [Accepted: 01/13/2010] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
The spatial focus of attention has been suggested to resemble a spotlight, a zoom-lens, a simple gradient, or even a more complex center-surround profile. Here we review evidence from neuromagnetic recordings indicating that the spatial profile is not fixed but depends on the particular perceptual demands of the attention task. We show that visual search requiring spatial scrutiny for target discrimination produces a zone of neural attenuation in the target's immediate surround, whereas search permitting target discrimination without spatial scrutiny is associated with a simple gradient. We provide new evidence indicating that increasing the demands on target discrimination without changing the spatial scale of discrimination does not influence surround attenuation, and that surround attenuation is also not influenced by the type of features involved in forward processing, that is whether the target location is defined by color or luminance contrast in visual search. An assessment of the time-course of attentional selection reveals that, when present, surround attenuation onsets with a substantial delay relative to the initial feed-forward sweep of processing in the visual system. The reported observations together suggest that the more complex center-surround profile arises as a consequence of top-down attentional selection in the visual system. The reviewed neuromagnetic evidence is discussed with respect to key notions of the Selective Tuning model of visual attention for which strong support is provided.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jens-Max Hopf
- Department of Neurology, Otto-von-Guericke University and Leibniz Institute for Neurobiology, Magdeburg 39120, Germany.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
62
|
Kleinschmidt A, Müller NG. The blind, the lame, and the poor signals of brain function--a comment on Sirotin and Das (2009). Neuroimage 2010; 50:622-5. [PMID: 20044008 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.075] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/23/2009] [Revised: 12/16/2009] [Accepted: 12/16/2009] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Last year, a study appeared that questioned the generally held assumption of a generic coupling between electrical and hemodynamic signs of neural activity (Sirotin and Das, 2009). Although the findings of that study can barely surprise the specialists in the field, it has caused a considerable confusion in the nonspecialist community due to the unwarranted claim of having discovered a "hitherto unknown signal." According to this claim, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) would pick up not only signals that reflect electrical brain activity but also purely hemodynamic signals that are not linked to neural activity. Here, we show that that study's failure to obtain significant electrophysiological responses to task structure is easily understood on the basis of findings reported for related functional paradigms. Ironically and counter its intention, the study by Sirotin and Das reminds us of the exquisite sensitivity of spatially pooled hemodynamic signals and the limitations of recording only very local samples of electrical activity by microelectrodes. We suggest that this sensitivity of hemodynamic signals should be converted into spatial resolution. In other words, hemodynamic signals should be used to create maps. Further, we suggest that electrical recordings should be obtained at systematically varying functional positions across these maps. And we speculate that under such appropriate experimental and analytical circumstances correspondence between the two modalities would be retrieved-at the expense of a novel signal lost in oblivion.
Collapse
|
63
|
Caparos S, Linnell KJ. The interacting effect of load and space on visual selective attention. VISUAL COGNITION 2009. [DOI: 10.1080/13506280902924083] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
|
64
|
Grossberg S. Cortical and subcortical predictive dynamics and learning during perception, cognition, emotion and action. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2009; 364:1223-34. [PMID: 19528003 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0307] [Citation(s) in RCA: 53] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
An intimate link exists between the predictive and learning processes in the brain. Perceptual/cognitive and spatial/motor processes use complementary predictive mechanisms to learn, recognize, attend and plan about objects in the world, determine their current value, and act upon them. Recent neural models clarify these mechanisms and how they interact in cortical and subcortical brain regions. The present paper reviews and synthesizes data and models of these processes, and outlines a unified theory of predictive brain processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stephen Grossberg
- Department of Cognitive and Neural Systems, Center for Adaptive Systems, Center of Excellence for Learning in Education, Science and Technology, Boston University, Boston, MA 02215, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
65
|
Hilimire MR, Mounts JRW, Parks NA, Corballis PM. Competitive interaction degrades target selection: An ERP study. Psychophysiology 2009; 46:1080-9. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00846.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
|
66
|
Mazza V, Turatto M, Caramazza A. Attention selection, distractor suppression and N2pc. Cortex 2009; 45:879-90. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.10.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 131] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/10/2008] [Revised: 10/27/2008] [Accepted: 10/31/2008] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
67
|
Limited influence of perceptual organization on the precision of attentional control. Atten Percept Psychophys 2009; 71:971-83. [PMID: 19429973 DOI: 10.3758/app.71.4.971] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The role of perceptual organization in the precision of attentional control was assessed in three experiments. Observers viewed circular arrays of disks that varied in density. One disk was cued, directing attention to that disk. A series of tones then indicated shifts of attention to the next disk that was of the same color (Experiments 1 and 2) or on the same depth plane (Experiment 3). In the homogeneous condition, all of the disks were the same color (Experiments 1 and 2) or on the same depth plane (Experiment 3). In the heterogeneous condition, the disks alternated in color (Experiments 1 and 2) or stereoscopically defined depth (Experiment 3). If the observers were able to limit attention to disks within a group, the effective density of the displays in the heterogeneous conditions should have been one half that in the homogeneous conditions. There was little evidence that the observers could do this, indicating a limited role of perceptual organization in the precision of attentional control.
Collapse
|
68
|
Goolsby BA, Shapiro KL, Silvert L, Kiss M, Fragopanagos N, Taylor JG, Eimer M, Nobre AC, Raymond JE. Feature-based inhibition underlies the affective consequences of attention. VISUAL COGNITION 2009. [DOI: 10.1080/13506280801904095] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | - Monika Kiss
- c Birkbeck College , University of London , London, UK
| | | | | | - Martin Eimer
- c Birkbeck College , University of London , London, UK
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
69
|
Bilateral field advantage in visual crowding. Vision Res 2009; 49:1638-46. [PMID: 19362572 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.03.026] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/21/2008] [Revised: 03/19/2009] [Accepted: 03/31/2009] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Thirty randomly oriented T's were presented in a circle around fixation at an eccentricity of 11 degrees such that each T was crowded by its neighbors. Two locations within the same hemifield (unilateral condition) or one location in each hemifield (bilateral condition) were precued for subsequent probing. Observers were then asked to report the orientation of a target T at one of these locations. A bilateral field advantage was found: target identification was better when the two precued targets were in different hemifields than when they were within the same hemifield. This bilateral advantage was absent when only targets were presented, without any distracters. Further controls showed that this advantage could not be attributed to differences between horizontal and vertical target alignments or to visual field anisotropies. A similar bilateral advantage has been reported for multiple object tracking (Alvarez, G. A., & Cavanagh, P. (2005). Independent resources for attentional tracking in the left and right visual fields. Psychological Science 16(8), 637-643) and other attentional tasks. Our results suggest that crowding also demonstrates separate attentional resources in the left and right hemifields. There was a cost to attending to two targets presented unilaterally over attending to a single target. However, this cost was reduced when the two crowded targets were in separate hemifields.
Collapse
|
70
|
Hickey C, Di Lollo V, McDonald JJ. Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search. J Cogn Neurosci 2009; 21:760-75. [PMID: 18564048 DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21039] [Citation(s) in RCA: 413] [Impact Index Per Article: 27.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Abstract
Attentional selection of a target presented among distractors can be indexed with an event-related potential (ERP) component known as the N2pc. Theoretical interpretation of the N2pc has suggested that it reflects a fundamental mechanism of attention that shelters the cortical representation of targets by suppressing neural activity stemming from distractors. Results from fields other than human electrophysiology, however, suggest that attention does not act solely through distractor suppression; rather, it modulates the processing of both target and distractors. We conducted four ERP experiments designed to investigate whether the N2pc reflects multiple attentional mechanisms. Our goal was to reconcile ostensibly conflicting outcomes obtained in electrophysiological studies of attention with those obtained using other methodologies. Participants viewed visual search arrays containing one target and one distractor. In Experiments 1 through 3, the distractor was isoluminant with the background, and therefore, did not elicit early lateralized ERP activity. This work revealed a novel contralateral ERP component that appears to reflect direct suppression of the cortical representation of the distractor. We accordingly name this component the distractor positivity (PD). In Experiment 4, an ERP component associated with target processing was additionally isolated. We refer to this component as the target negativity (NT). We believe that the N2pc reflects the summation of the PD and NT, and that these discrete components may have been confounded in earlier electrophysiological studies. Overall, this study demonstrates that attention acts on both target and distractor representations, and that this can be indexed in the visual ERP.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Clayton Hickey
- 1Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, The Netherlands
- 2Simon Fraser University, Vancouver, Canada
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
71
|
Raymond J. Interactions of attention, emotion and motivation. PROGRESS IN BRAIN RESEARCH 2009; 176:293-308. [PMID: 19733764 DOI: 10.1016/s0079-6123(09)17617-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 43] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/13/2022]
Abstract
Although successful visually guided action begins with sensory processes and ends with motor control, the intervening processes related to the appropriate selection of information for processing are especially critical because of the brain's limited capacity to handle information. Three important mechanisms--attention, emotion and motivation--contribute to the prioritization and selection of information. In this chapter, the interplay between these systems is discussed with emphasis placed on interactions between attention (or immediate task relevance of stimuli) and emotion (or affective evaluation of stimuli), and between attention and motivation (or the predicted value of stimuli). Although numerous studies have shown that emotional stimuli modulate mechanisms of selective attention in humans, little work has been directed at exploring whether such interactions can be reciprocal, that is, whether attention can influence emotional response. Recent work on this question (showing that distracting information is typically devalued upon later encounters) is reviewed in the first half of the chapter. In the second half, some recent experiments exploring how prior value-prediction learning (i.e., learning to associate potential outcomes, good or bad, with specific stimuli) plays a role in visual selection and conscious perception. The results indicate that some aspects of motivation act on selection independently of traditionally defined attention and other aspects interact with it.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jane Raymond
- School of Psychology, Bangor University, Bangor, Gwynedd, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
72
|
|
73
|
Munneke J, Van der Stigchel S, Theeuwes J. Cueing the location of a distractor: an inhibitory mechanism of spatial attention? Acta Psychol (Amst) 2008; 129:101-7. [PMID: 18589391 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.05.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 57] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/27/2007] [Revised: 03/18/2008] [Accepted: 05/04/2008] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Presenting an irrelevant distractor increases reaction times to a target. The current study shows that cueing the location of an upcoming 'distractor' can help to reduce the effects the distractor has on target processing. It is hypothesized that this reduction is due to the active inhibition of the cued location. In two experiments in which the location of the distractor was cued in advance, a reduced effect of the distractor on target-processing was observed. Analyses indicated that this effect was most likely caused by inhibition of the distractor location. The present findings suggest that inhibition plays an important role in visual-spatial selection processes and that this inhibitory mechanism can be controlled in a top-down fashion.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jaap Munneke
- Department of Cognitive Psychology, Vrije Universiteit, Van der Boechorststraat 1, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
74
|
Boehler CN, Tsotsos JK, Schoenfeld MA, Heinze HJ, Hopf JM. The center-surround profile of the focus of attention arises from recurrent processing in visual cortex. Cereb Cortex 2008; 19:982-91. [PMID: 18755778 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhn139] [Citation(s) in RCA: 57] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
We recently demonstrated with magnetoencephalographic recordings in human observers that the focus of attention in visual search has a spatial profile consisting of a center enhancement surrounded by a narrow zone of sensory attenuation. Here, we report new data from 2 experiments providing insights into the cortical processes that cause the surround attenuation. We show that surround suppression appears in search tasks that require spatial scrutiny, that is the precise binding of search-relevant features at the target's location but not in tasks that permit target discrimination without precise localization. Furthermore, we demonstrate that surround attenuation is linked with a stronger recurrent activity modulation in early visual cortex. Finally, we show that surround suppression appears with a delay (more than 175 ms) that is beyond the time course of the initial feedforward sweep of processing in the visual system. These observations together indicate that the suppressive surround is associated with recurrent processing and binding in the visual cortex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- C N Boehler
- Leibniz-Institute for Neurobiology, Magdeburg, Germany
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
75
|
McCarley JS, Mounts JR. On the relationship between flanker interference and localized attentional interference. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2008; 128:102-9. [PMID: 18155179 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2007.10.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/21/2007] [Revised: 10/15/2007] [Accepted: 10/18/2007] [Indexed: 10/22/2022] Open
Abstract
The flanker interference (FI) effect suggests that visual attention operates like a mental spotlight, enhancing all stimuli within a selected region. In contrast, other data suggest difficulty dividing attention between objects near one another in the visual field, an effect termed localized attentional interference (LAI). The present experiment examined the relationship between these phenomena. Observers made speeded identity judgments of a colored target letter embedded among gray fillers. A response-compatible or -incompatible flanker of a non-target color appeared at varying distances from the target. Data gave evidence of LAI and spatially-graded FI, with mean RTs and flanker effects both decreasing with target-flanker separation. Both effects were reduced when target location was pre-cued and when the target was of higher salience than the flanker. Results suggest that the distribution of spatial attention modulates the strength of objects competing for selection, with this competition underlying both the FI and LAI effects.
Collapse
|
76
|
Palmer J, Moore CM. Using a filtering task to measure the spatial extent of selective attention. Vision Res 2008; 49:1045-64. [PMID: 18405935 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.02.022] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/18/2007] [Revised: 01/19/2008] [Accepted: 02/18/2008] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
The spatial extent of attention was investigated by measuring sensitivity to stimuli at to-be-ignored locations. Observers detected a stimulus at a cued location (target), while ignoring otherwise identical stimuli at nearby locations (foils). Only an attentional cue distinguished target from foil. Several experiments varied the contrast and separation of targets and foils. Two theories of selection were compared: contrast gain and a version of attention switching called an all-or-none mixture model. Results included large effects of separation, rejection of the contrast gain model, and the measurement of the size and profile of the spatial extent of attention.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- John Palmer
- Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Seattle, 98195-1525, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
77
|
Müller NG, Ebeling D. Attention-modulated activity in visual cortex—More than a simple ‘spotlight’. Neuroimage 2008; 40:818-827. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.11.060] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/29/2007] [Revised: 11/24/2007] [Accepted: 11/28/2007] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
|
78
|
La Cara GE, Ursino M. A model of contour extraction including multiple scales, flexible inhibition and attention. Neural Netw 2008; 21:759-73. [PMID: 18406105 DOI: 10.1016/j.neunet.2007.11.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/19/2006] [Accepted: 11/07/2007] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
A mathematical model of contextual integration and contour extraction in the primary visual cortex developed in a recent work [Ursino, M., & La Cara, G. E. (2004). A model of contextual interactions and contour detection in primary visual cortex. Neural Networks, 17, 719-735] has been significantly improved to include two fundamental additional aspects, i.e., multi-scale decomposition and attention. The model incorporates two independent paths for visual processing corresponding to two different scales. Attention from higher hierarchical levels works by modifying different properties of the network: by selecting the portion of the image to be scrutinized and the appropriate scale, by modulating the threshold of a gating mechanism, and by modifying the width and/or strength of lateral inhibition. Through computer simulations of real complex and noisy black-and-white images, we demonstrate that appropriate selection of the above factors allows accurate analysis of image contours at different levels, from global perception of the overall objects without details, down to a fine examination of minute particulars (such as the lips in a face or the fingers of a hand). Attentive reconfiguration of lateral inhibition plays a key role in the analysis of images at different detail levels.
Collapse
|
79
|
Wei P, Lü J, Müller HJ, Zhou X. Searching for two feature singletons in the visual scene: the localized attentional interference effect. Exp Brain Res 2007; 185:175-88. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-007-1141-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/28/2007] [Accepted: 09/14/2007] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
|
80
|
McCarley JS, Mounts JRW, Kramer AF. Spatially mediated capacity limits in attentive visual perception. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2007; 126:98-119. [PMID: 17196153 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.11.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/24/2006] [Revised: 11/11/2006] [Accepted: 11/16/2006] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Modern theories conceptualize visual selective attention as a competition between objects for the control of cortical receptive fields (RFs). Implicit in this framework is the suggestion that spatially proximal objects, which draw from overlapping pools of RFs, should be more difficult to represent in parallel and with excess capacity than spatially separated objects. The present experiments tested this prediction using analysis of response time distributions in a redundant-targets letter identification task. Data revealed that excess-capacity parallel processing is possible when redundant targets are widely separated within the visual field, but that capacity is near fixed when targets are adjacent. Even at the largest separations tested, however, processing capacity remained strongly limited.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jason S McCarley
- Institute of Aviation, Human Factors Division, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Savoy, IL 61874, United States.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
81
|
Moore CM, Lanagan-Leitzel LK, Chen P, Halterman R, Fine EM. Nonspatial attributes of stimuli can influence spatial limitations of attentional control. PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS 2007; 69:363-71. [PMID: 17672424 PMCID: PMC6490965 DOI: 10.3758/bf03193757] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
A wide variety of psychophysical and neurophysiological research suggests that when stimuli are very close together, they cannot be attended separately. As a consequence, they cannot be represented as individual items with specific feature information associated with them. Here we report evidence that the spatial control of attention can be modulated by nonspatial features of the stimuli (such as color and luminance). Observers shifted attention from item to item within highly dense arrays of stimuli. Performance was extremely poor when all of the items in the array were an identical gray. In contrast, performance improved when items differed in color. This finding indicates that nonspatial features, such as color, can facilitate spatial selection and suggests moreover that features can be reliably associated with particular items even when the items are densely clustered.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cathleen M Moore
- Department of Psychology, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pennsylvania 16802, USA.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
82
|
Effects of spatial separation between stimuli in whole report from brief visual displays. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2007; 69:1040-50. [DOI: 10.3758/bf03193942] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
83
|
Mounts JRW, McCarley JS, Terech AM. Attentional templates regulate competitive interactions among attended visual objects. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2007; 69:209-17. [PMID: 17557591 DOI: 10.3758/bf03193743] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
In two experiments, we examined the mechanisms responsible for creating a zone of interference surrounding an attended visual object (see, e.g., Mounts & Gavett, 2004). In Experiment 1, the similarity between attended stimuli and noise items was manipulated in order to contrast an account based on competitive interactions between attended items with an account based on inefficient filtering of unattended stimuli. Consistent with the competitive interaction account, the data revealed that similarity between attended items increased the strength of localized interference, whereas similarity of noise items to the attended stimuli did not. Experiment 2 showed that the interference observed between attended items was determined by their match to attentional templates.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jeffrey R W Mounts
- Department of Psychology, State University of New York, Geneseo, New York 14454, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
84
|
Grossberg S. Towards a unified theory of neocortex: laminar cortical circuits for vision and cognition. PROGRESS IN BRAIN RESEARCH 2007; 165:79-104. [DOI: 10.1016/s0079-6123(06)65006-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 42] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
|
85
|
Grossberg S, Seidman D. Neural dynamics of autistic behaviors: cognitive, emotional, and timing substrates. Psychol Rev 2006; 113:483-525. [PMID: 16802879 DOI: 10.1037/0033-295x.113.3.483] [Citation(s) in RCA: 91] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
What brain mechanisms underlie autism, and how do they give rise to autistic behavioral symptoms? This article describes a neural model, called the Imbalanced Spectrally Timed Adaptive Resonance Theory (iSTART) model, that proposes how cognitive, emotional, timing, and motor processes that involve brain regions such as the prefrontal and temporal cortex, amygdala, hippocampus, and cerebellum may interact to create and perpetuate autistic symptoms. These model processes were originally developed to explain data concerning how the brain controls normal behaviors. The iSTART model shows how autistic behavioral symptoms may arise from prescribed breakdowns in these brain processes, notably a combination of underaroused emotional depression in the amygdala and related affective brain regions, learning of hyperspecific recognition categories in the temporal and prefrontal cortices, and breakdowns of adaptively timed attentional and motor circuits in the hippocampal system and cerebellum. The model clarifies how malfunctions in a subset of these mechanisms can, through a systemwide vicious circle of environmentally mediated feedback, cause and maintain problems with them all.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stephen Grossberg
- Department of Cognitive and Neural Systems, Center for Adaptive Systems and Center of Excellence for Learning in Education, Science, and Technology, Boston University, 677 Beacon Street, Boston, MA 02215, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
86
|
Abstract
The role of salience in localized attentional interference (LAI) was examined. In two experiments, target discrimination performance was measured as a function of the spatial separation between the target and a salient distractor item. In Experiment 1, both the salience of the distractor and that of a target were manipulated. Distractor salience was manipulated via size changes to the distractor, and target salience was manipulated by using unmasked or onset targets. When the target was of low salience, the magnitude of interference from the distractor increased with distractor salience. However, when the target had an abrupt onset, the distractor had no impact on target performance. In Experiment 2, the attentional salience of the distractor was manipulated using a probability manipulation. Displays contained both a target and a color singleton distractor. The color singleton produced LAI when it was predictive of the target location but not when it was unpredictive of the target location. The results of both experiments are consistent with models of competition-based attentional selection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jeffrey R W Mounts
- Department of Psychology, State University of New York, Geneseo, New York 14454, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
87
|
Hopf JM, Boehler CN, Luck SJ, Tsotsos JK, Heinze HJ, Schoenfeld MA. Direct neurophysiological evidence for spatial suppression surrounding the focus of attention in vision. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2006; 103:1053-8. [PMID: 16410356 PMCID: PMC1347985 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0507746103] [Citation(s) in RCA: 160] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
The spatial focus of attention has traditionally been envisioned as a simple spatial gradient of enhanced activity that falls off monotonically with increasing distance. Here, we show with high-density magnetoencephalographic recordings in human observers that the focus of attention is not a simple monotonic gradient but instead contains an excitatory peak surrounded by a narrow inhibitory region. To demonstrate this center-surround profile, we asked subjects to focus attention onto a color pop-out target and then presented probe stimuli at various distances from the target. We observed that the electromagnetic response to the probe was enhanced when the probe was presented at the location of the target, but the probe response was suppressed in a narrow zone surrounding the target and then recovered at more distant locations. Withdrawing attention from the pop-out target by engaging observers in a demanding foveal task eliminated this pattern, confirming a truly attention-driven effect. These results indicate that neural enhancement and suppression coexist in a spatially structured manner that is optimal to attenuate the most deleterious noise during visual object identification.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J-M Hopf
- Department of Neurology II, Otto-von-Guericke-University, D-39120 Magdeburg, Germany.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
88
|
Crawford TJ, Hill S, Higham S. The inhibitory effect of a recent distracter. Vision Res 2005; 45:3365-78. [PMID: 16171841 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.07.024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/31/2004] [Revised: 07/15/2005] [Accepted: 07/26/2005] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
A series of experiments were conducted to examine the inhibitory effect of a visual distracter on saccadic eye movements. Participants were presented with a sequence of two critical displays. In one display a red target was presented together with a green distracter. This was followed by a display with a new red target presented in isolation at one of three locations with respect to the previous display. The lone target was presented either at the location of the recent target, the location of the recent distracter, or a new location. Participants were instructed to fixate the target in both displays and to ignore the green distracter. Experiment 1 revealed a significant increase in saccadic reaction times (SRTs) when the target was presented at the location of the recent distracter. Experiment 2 revealed that SRTs increased only in the conditions where the new target was presented at the location of the recent distracter, irrespective of its colour. Experiment 3 found that the inhibitory effect lasted for at least 2 s. In Experiment 4 the inhibitory effect was abolished when a lone distracter (i.e., anti-target) was presented without a target. Experiments 5 and 6 revealed that inhibition at the location of the recent target ('inhibition-of-return') also emerged with a shorter inter-display interval and when the distracter was removed from the recent display. These results distinguished between inhibition of a recent distracter and 'inhibition-of-return' and are consistent with models of competitive interactions which generate inhibitory effects on the spatial representation of a distracter.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Trevor J Crawford
- Mental Health and Neural Systems Research Unit, Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, UK.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
89
|
Amitay S, Hawkey DJC, Moore DR. Auditory frequency discrimination learning is affected by stimulus variability. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2005; 67:691-8. [PMID: 16134462 DOI: 10.3758/bf03193525] [Citation(s) in RCA: 83] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
We explored the effects of training set variability on learning and generalization of pure-tone frequency discrimination (FD) in three groups of untrained, normally hearing adult listeners. Group 1 trained using a fixed standard tone at 1 kHz (fixed), Group 2 on slightly varying (roving) tones around 1 kHz, and Group 3 on widely varying standard frequencies (wide-roving). Initially, two thirds of all listeners had low FD thresholds (good listeners) and one third had intermediate to high thresholds (poor listeners). For good listeners, slight variations in the training set slowed learning but wide variations did not. Transfer to untrained frequencies (up to 4 kHz) and to the fixed condition was equivalent regardless of training condition, but Group 1 listeners did not fully transfer learning to the roving condition. For poor listeners, any variation in the training condition slowed learning and impaired transfer to other frequencies but did not affect transfer to untrained conditions. Thus, the effects of training set on progress and outcome depend on set variability and individual FD ability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sygal Amitay
- MRC Institute of Hearing Research, Nottingham, England.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
90
|
Mounts JRW, Tomaselli RG. Competition for representation is mediated by relative attentional salience. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2005; 118:261-75. [PMID: 15698824 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2004.09.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/15/2003] [Revised: 09/16/2004] [Accepted: 09/20/2004] [Indexed: 10/26/2022] Open
Abstract
The biased competition model of attentional selection proposes that objects compete with one another for neural representation, with the competition rooted in stimulus and attentionally-based salience. Two experiments explore how the salience of a target item relative to flanking items impacts the speed of target identification. The results of two experiments suggest that spatially proximal items compete for shared, spatially dependent processing resources. In both experiments, subjects identified target elements embedded in multi-element displays. In Experiment 1, attentional salience was manipulated by using abrupt onsets (high-salience) and non-onsets (low-salience). Target identifications were slowest when the target was flanked by two high-salience stimuli (abrupt onsets) and fastest when the target was flanked by two low-salience items, (non-onsets). In Experiment 2, the attentional salience of display items was set through a probability manipulation involving the color of the target. The results mirrored those of Experiment 1, consistent with predictions of the biased competition model.
Collapse
|
91
|
Raymond JE, Fenske MJ, Westoby N. Emotional Devaluation of Distracting Patterns and Faces: A Consequence of Attentional Inhibition During Visual Search? ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2005; 31:1404-15. [PMID: 16366798 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.6.1404] [Citation(s) in RCA: 53] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Visual search has been studied extensively, yet little is known about how its constituent processes affect subsequent emotional evaluation of searched-for and searched-through items. In 3 experiments, the authors asked observers to locate a colored pattern or tinted face in an array of other patterns or faces. Shortly thereafter, either the target or a distractor was rated on an emotional scale (patterns, cheerfulness; faces, trustworthiness). In general, distractors were rated more negatively than targets. Moreover, distractors presented near the target during search were rated significantly more negatively than those presented far from the target. Target-distractor proximity affected distractor ratings following both simple-feature and difficult-conjunction search, even when items appeared at different locations during evaluation than during search and when faces previously tinted during search were presented in grayscale at evaluation. An attentional inhibition account is offered to explain these effects of attention on emotional evaluation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jane E Raymond
- School of Psychology, University of Wales, Gwynedd, Wales, UK.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
92
|
Müller NG, Mollenhauer M, Rösler A, Kleinschmidt A. The attentional field has a Mexican hat distribution. Vision Res 2004; 45:1129-37. [PMID: 15707921 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2004.11.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 97] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/18/2004] [Revised: 09/23/2004] [Accepted: 11/04/2004] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
We assessed the interference by distracter letters on target discrimination as a function of the distance between incompatible distracters and target. The slope of the response time-distance function supports a Mexican hat pattern of attentional modulation in the visual field. We relate the results to our recent finding of neural activity suppression in primary visual cortex coding locations in the vicinity of an attended region [Muller, N. G., & Kleinschmidt, A. (2004). The attentional 'spotlight's' penumbra: Center-surround modulation in striate cortex. Neuroreport, 15(6), 977-980]. As behavioral performance parallels activity modulation of primary visual cortex but not other areas we propose that perceptual capacities are determined by attentional response properties of V1.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Notger G Müller
- Cognitive Neurology Unit, Department of Neurology and Brain Imaging Center, Johann Wolfgang Goethe-University, Theodor-Stern-Kai 7, 60590, Frankfurt am Main, Germany.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
93
|
Abstract
Attentional selection of an object in the visual field degrades processing of neighboring stimuli in young adults. A pair of experiments examined the effects of aging on such localized attentional interference. In Experiment 1, younger and older observers made speeded same-different judgments of target shapes that varied in spatial separation. Performance declined for both age groups as the distance between targets decreased, but an Age x Distance interaction indicated that the magnitude of this effect was larger for older adults. Experiment 2 ruled out sensory masking as an explanation for these findings. Results indicate that older observers experience losses in the ability to attend to multiple spatially proximal stimuli within the visual field.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jason S McCarley
- Beckman Institute, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL, US.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
94
|
Mounts JRW, Gavett BE. The role of salience in localized attentional interference. Vision Res 2004; 44:1575-88. [PMID: 15126066 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2004.01.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/20/2003] [Revised: 10/31/2003] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
Observers were cued to attend to two discs from an array and made a discrimination of a target presented within one of the discs. In Experiments 1 and 2, the relative attentional salience of the two attended items was manipulated via the cues (size changes in Experiment 1; size and color changes in Experiment 2). In Experiment 3, the relative salience was manipulated via the luminance contrast of the items themselves. In Experiment 4, relative attentional salience was controlled through a probability manipulation. In all experiments, target performance improved with the relative salience of the target, as well as with increased spatial separation between the two items. This localized interference between cued items varied with visual field. Results are discussed in the context of competition-based models of attentional selection.
Collapse
|
95
|
Theeuwes J, Kramer AF, Kingstone A. Attentional capture modulates perceptual sensitivity. Psychon Bull Rev 2004; 11:551-4. [PMID: 15376809 DOI: 10.3758/bf03196609] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The present study was designed to determine the spatial distribution of attention in displays in which an irrelevant color singleton was present. The results show that the presence of an irrelevant color singleton modulates target detectability (d'). The presence of an irrelevant singleton reduces the gain for input at the target location, particularly when the irrelevant color singleton was close to the target singleton. In line with earlier claims, it is argued that the capture of attention by the irrelevant singleton causes a reduced sensory input at the target location.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jan Theeuwes
- Department of Cognitive Psychology, Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
96
|
Lamy D, Egeth HE. Attentional capture in singleton-detection and feature-search modes. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 2004; 29:1003-20. [PMID: 14585019 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.5.1003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 82] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Six experiments were conducted to determine the circumstances under which an irrelevant singleton captures attention. Subjects searched for a target while ignoring a salient distractor that appeared at different stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs) prior to each search display. Spatial congruency and interference effects were measured. The strategies available to find the target were controlled (only singleton-detection mode, only feature-search mode, or both search strategies available). An irrelevant abrupt onset captured attention in search for a color target, across SOAs, whatever strategies were available. In contrast, in search for a shape target, an irrelevant color singleton captured attention in the singleton-detection condition but delayed response at its location in the feature-search condition, across SOAs. When both strategies were available, capture was short lived (50- to 100-msec SOAs). The theoretical implications of these findings in relation to current views on attentional capture are discussed. ((c) 2003 APA, all rights reserved)
Collapse
|
97
|
Abstract
In a host of studies, the ability of various types of cues to capture attention has been examined. This article reviews a number of these studies by organizing them into a classification scheme based on the relationship between the putative attention-capturing item (the cue) and the item used to assess the distribution of attention (the probe). The second dimension of this taxonomy divides paradigms of attentional capture into those in which capture is indexed by performance benefits and those in which capture is indexed by performance costs. The relative methodological merits and disadvantages of the paradigms that occupy each of the cells of the resulting two-by-two matrix are discussed. A final section offers a new interpretation of the finding that dynamic cues capture attention.
Collapse
|
98
|
Cutzu F, Tsotsos JK. The selective tuning model of attention: psychophysical evidence for a suppressive annulus around an attended item. Vision Res 2003; 43:205-19. [PMID: 12536142 DOI: 10.1016/s0042-6989(02)00491-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 116] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
The selective tuning model [Artif. Intell. 78 (1995) 507] is a neurobiologically plausible neural network model of visual attention. One of its key predictions is that to simultaneously solve the problems of convergence of neural input and selection of attended items, the portions of the visual neural network that process an attended stimulus must be surrounded by inhibition. To test this hypothesis, we mapped the attentional field around an attended location in a matching task where the subject's attention was directed to a cued target while the distance of a probe item to the target was varied systematically. The main result was that accuracy increased with inter-target separation. The observed pattern of variation of accuracy with distance provided strong evidence in favor of the critical prediction of the model that attention is actively inhibited in the immediate vicinity of an attended location.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Florin Cutzu
- Department of Computer Science, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
99
|
Facoetti A, Lorusso ML, Paganoni P, Umiltà C, Mascetti GG. The role of visuospatial attention in developmental dyslexia: evidence from a rehabilitation study. BRAIN RESEARCH. COGNITIVE BRAIN RESEARCH 2003; 15:154-64. [PMID: 12429367 DOI: 10.1016/s0926-6410(02)00148-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 93] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Shifting of visual attention induced by peripheral cues was studied in 24 children with specific reading disorder (SRD) or dyslexia and was compared with that of 19 normal readers by means of a covert orienting paradigm. This paradigm presents participants with valid, neutral and invalid spatial cues preceding the presentation of a target stimulus. As compared to normal readers, in SRD children the inhibition effect (i.e. the difference between neutral and invalid cues) was absent. The 24 SRD children were divided into two groups matched for age, IQ and reading ability to study the efficacy of two different rehabilitation procedures. We assessed the effects on reading accuracy and speed over a 4-month treatment with visual hemisphere specific stimulation (VHSS; J. Learn Disabil. 25 (1992) 102) vs. traditional speech training. The VHSS program trains participants to perform rapid endogenous attentional orienting by presenting briefly flashed words in the peripheral visual field. We found that children treated with VHSS showed significant changes in their attentional inhibition process, as indicated by increased costs for 'reorienting' the attentional focus. As this treatment program also proved to be highly efficient in improving the children's reading abilities, the possible causal relationship between reading and inhibition mechanisms of visuospatial attention was discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrea Facoetti
- Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale, Università degli Studi di Padova, Via Venezia, 8, 35131, Padova, Italy
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
100
|
Abstract
We report the results of two experiments addressing spatiotemporal variations in the "attentional blink" (AB). In the first experiment, six streams of letters were presented simultaneously around a circle on a screen. The identity of the letters changed every 140 ms. The task was to identify two target digits (T1 and T2) that could appear in any of the streams with a variable time lag between the two. The results show that the AB is not constant across space and that following the allocation of attention to a certain location (the location of T1), discrimination can be better at locations quite far away from T1, than at locations closest to T1. Furthermore, performance at the farthest locations seemed to recover sooner from the AB than locations closer to where T1 appeared. Similar results were obtained in a second experiment where observers performed a cued discrimination task. The results accord well with the proposal that there is a region around the attended site (the center of attention) where attentional resolution is particularly poor, worse than at sites further away from the attended one. We propose that this reflects lateral inhibition of neurons responsive to the region around the attended site, with the goal of suppressing potentially distracting or interfering information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Arni Kristjánsson
- Vision Sciences Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|