151
|
Servián-Franco F, Moreno-Domínguez S, del Paso GAR. Body dissatisfaction and mirror exposure: evidence for a dissociation between self-report and physiological responses in highly body-dissatisfied women. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0122737. [PMID: 25830861 PMCID: PMC4382336 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0122737] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/23/2014] [Accepted: 02/12/2015] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Weight and shape concerns are widespread in the general population. Mirror exposure has been used to reduce body dissatisfaction but little is known about the mechanisms which underlie this therapeutic technique. The present study examined emotional, cognitive, and psychophysiological responses, in women with high and low levels of body dissatisfaction, exposed to their own bodies in a mirror. Method Forty-two university-attending women (21 high body-dissatisfied (HBD) and 21 low body-dissatisfied (LBD)), were confronted with their own body during four 5-min trials in which participants were instructed to focus their attention on different parts of their body under standardized conditions. Emotional and cognitive measures were taken after each exposure trial. Heart rate (HR) and skin conductance (SC) were recorded continuously. Results HBD women experienced more negative emotions and cognitions following body exposure compared to LBD women but, conversely, showed a reduced physiological reaction in terms of HR and SC. In both groups greater physiological responses were observed looking at the thighs, buttocks, and abdomen. Extent of negative emotions and cognitions were positively associated with HR and/or SC in LBD women but no associations were observed in HBD women. Conclusion The dissociation between self-report and psychophysiological measures in HBD women supports the existence of a passive-behavioral inhibited coping style in HBD women and suggests deficiencies in the generation of physiological correlates of emotion related to body dissatisfaction.
Collapse
|
152
|
|
153
|
Madeo D, Castellani E, Mocenni C, Santarcangelo EL. Pain perception and EEG dynamics: does hypnotizability account for the efficacy of the suggestions of analgesia? Physiol Behav 2015; 145:57-63. [PMID: 25837836 DOI: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.03.040] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/08/2014] [Revised: 03/26/2015] [Accepted: 03/28/2015] [Indexed: 12/19/2022]
Abstract
We report novel findings concerning the role of hypnotizability, suggestions of analgesia and the activity of the Behavioral Inhibition/Activation System (BIS/BAS) in the modulation of the subjective experience of pain and of the associated EEG dynamics. The EEG of high (highs) and low hypnotizable participants (lows) who completed the BIS/BAS questionnaire was recorded during basal conditions, tonic nociceptive stimulation without (PAIN) and with suggestions for analgesia (AN). Participants scored the perceived pain intensity at the end of PAIN and AN. The EEG midline dynamics was characterized by indices indicating the signal predictability (Determinism) and complexity (Entropy) obtained through the Recurrence Quantification Analysis. The reduced pain intensity reported by highs during AN was partially accounted for by the activity of the Behavioral Activation System. The decreased midline cortical Determinism observed during nociceptive stimulation in both groups independently of suggestions remained significantly reduced only in lows after controlling for the activity of the Behavioral Activation System. Finally, controlling for the activity of the Behavioral Inhibition System abolished stimulation, suggestions and hypnotizability-related differences. Results indicate that the BIS/BAS activity may be more important than hypnotizability itself in pain modulation and in the associated EEG dynamics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dario Madeo
- Department of Information Engineering and Mathematical Sciences, Italy; Complex Systems Community, University of Siena, Siena, Italy
| | - Eleonora Castellani
- Department of Translational Research and New Technologies in Medicine and Surgery, University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy
| | - Chiara Mocenni
- Department of Information Engineering and Mathematical Sciences, Italy; Complex Systems Community, University of Siena, Siena, Italy
| | - Enrica Laura Santarcangelo
- Department of Translational Research and New Technologies in Medicine and Surgery, University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy.
| |
Collapse
|
154
|
Davies CD, Niles AN, Pittig A, Arch JJ, Craske MG. Physiological and behavioral indices of emotion dysregulation as predictors of outcome from cognitive behavioral therapy and acceptance and commitment therapy for anxiety. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2015; 46:35-43. [PMID: 25199454 DOI: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.08.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/16/2013] [Revised: 06/27/2014] [Accepted: 08/14/2014] [Indexed: 01/17/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES Identifying for whom and under what conditions a treatment is most effective is an essential step toward personalized medicine. The current study examined pre-treatment physiological and behavioral variables as predictors and moderators of outcome in a randomized clinical trial comparing cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) and acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) for anxiety disorders. METHODS Sixty individuals with a DSM-IV defined principal anxiety disorder completed 12 sessions of either CBT or ACT. Baseline physiological and behavioral variables were measured prior to entering treatment. Self-reported anxiety symptoms were assessed at pre-treatment, post-treatment, and 6- and 12-month follow-up from baseline. RESULTS Higher pre-treatment heart rate variability was associated with worse outcome across ACT and CBT. ACT outperformed CBT for individuals with high behavioral avoidance. Subjective anxiety levels during laboratory tasks did not predict or moderate treatment outcome. LIMITATIONS Due to small sample sizes of each disorder, disorder-specific predictors were not tested. Future research should examine these predictors in larger samples and across other outcome variables. CONCLUSIONS Lower heart rate variability was identified as a prognostic indicator of overall outcome, whereas high behavioral avoidance was identified as a prescriptive indicator of superior outcome from ACT versus CBT. Investigation of pre-treatment physiological and behavioral variables as predictors and moderators of outcome may help guide future treatment-matching efforts.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carolyn D Davies
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA), Los Angeles, CA, USA.
| | - Andrea N Niles
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA), Los Angeles, CA, USA
| | - Andre Pittig
- Department of Psychology, University of Mannheim, Mannheim, Germany
| | - Joanna J Arch
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado, Boulder, Boulder, CO, USA
| | - Michelle G Craske
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA), Los Angeles, CA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
155
|
Attachment and physiological reactivity to infant crying in young adulthood: Dissociation between experiential and physiological arousal in insecure adoptees. Physiol Behav 2015; 139:549-56. [DOI: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.11.055] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/20/2014] [Revised: 11/15/2014] [Accepted: 11/17/2014] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
|
156
|
Sanders W, Abaied J. Motivational systems and autonomic functioning: Overlapping and differential contributions to anhedonic depression and anxious arousal. MOTIVATION AND EMOTION 2015. [DOI: 10.1007/s11031-015-9470-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/07/2023]
|
157
|
Neuhaus E, Bernier RA, Beauchaine TP. Electrodermal Response to Reward and Non-Reward Among Children With Autism. Autism Res 2015; 8:357-70. [DOI: 10.1002/aur.1451] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/18/2014] [Revised: 10/16/2014] [Accepted: 11/25/2014] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Emily Neuhaus
- Seattle Children's Research Institute, Center for Child Health, Behavior, and Development; M/S CW8-6. PO Box 5371 Seattle Washington 98121
| | - Raphael A. Bernier
- Dept of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences; University of Washington; Box 357920 Seattle Washington 98195
| | - Theodore P. Beauchaine
- Department of Psychology; The Ohio State University; 1835 Neil Avenue Columbus Ohio 43210
| |
Collapse
|
158
|
Venables NC, Hall JR, Yancey JR, Patrick CJ. Factors of psychopathy and electrocortical response to emotional pictures: Further evidence for a two-process theory. JOURNAL OF ABNORMAL PSYCHOLOGY 2015; 124:319-28. [PMID: 25603361 DOI: 10.1037/abn0000032] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/04/2023]
Abstract
The Two-Process theory of psychopathy posits that distinct etiological mechanisms contribute to the condition: (a) a weakness in defensive (fear) reactivity related to affective-interpersonal features, and (b) impaired cognitive-executive functioning, marked by reductions in brain responses such as P3, related to impulsive-antisocial features. The current study examined relations between psychopathy factors and electrocortical response to emotional and neutral pictures in male offenders (N = 139) assessed using the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R). Impulsive-antisocial features of the PCL-R (Factor 2) were associated with reduced amplitude of earlier P3 brain response to pictures regardless of valence, whereas the affective-interpersonal dimension (Factor 1) was associated specifically with reductions in late positive potential response to aversive pictures. Findings provide further support for the Two-Process theory and add to a growing body of evidence linking the impulsive-antisocial facet of psychopathy to the broader construct of externalizing proneness. Findings are discussed in terms of current initiatives directed at incorporating neuroscientific concepts into psychopathology classification. (PsycINFO Database Record
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Jason R Hall
- Department of Mental Health Law and Policy, University of South Florida
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
159
|
Raby KL, Roisman GI, Simpson JA, Collins WA, Steele RD. Greater maternal insensitivity in childhood predicts greater electrodermal reactivity during conflict discussions with romantic partners in adulthood. Psychol Sci 2015; 26:348-53. [PMID: 25576344 DOI: 10.1177/0956797614563340] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
In this study, we drew on prospective, longitudinal data to investigate the long-term predictive significance of the quality of early parent-child relationship experiences for adults' sympathetic nervous system (SNS) activity during conflict discussions with their romantic partners. Maternal sensitivity was repeatedly assessed across childhood via direct observations of mother-child interactions. When the children in the study became adults (34-37 years old), electrodermal activity-an index of SNS arousal and a psychophysiological marker of behavioral inhibition-was recorded for 37 participants while at rest and while they attempted to resolve conflicts in their romantic relationships. Individuals who had experienced less sensitive maternal caregiving during childhood had greater increases in electrodermal activity during conflict discussions with their adult partners, relative to resting conditions. This longitudinal association was not accounted for by observed or self-reported romantic-relationship quality, gender, ethnicity, or early socioeconomic factors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- K Lee Raby
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, University of Delaware
| | | | | | - W A Collins
- Institute of Child Development, University of Minnesota
| | - Ryan D Steele
- Institute of Child Development, University of Minnesota
| |
Collapse
|
160
|
Kujawa A, Proudfit GH, Kessel EM, Dyson M, Olino T, Klein DN. Neural reactivity to monetary rewards and losses in childhood: longitudinal and concurrent associations with observed and self-reported positive emotionality. Biol Psychol 2015; 104:41-7. [PMID: 25433097 PMCID: PMC4300239 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.11.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/19/2013] [Revised: 08/06/2014] [Accepted: 11/18/2014] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
Reward reactivity and positive emotion are key components of a theoretical, early-emerging approach motivational system, yet few studies have examined associations between positive emotion and neural reactivity to reward across development. In this multi-method prospective study, we examined the association of laboratory observations of positive emotionality (PE) at age 3 and self-reported positive affect (PA) at age 9 with an event-related potential component sensitive to the relative response to winning vs. losing money, the feedback negativity (ΔFN), at age 9 (N=381). Males had a larger ΔFN than females, and both greater observed PE at age 3 and self-reported PA at age 9 significantly, but modestly, predicted an enhanced ΔFN at age 9. Negative emotionality and behavioral inhibition did not predict ΔFN. Results contribute to understanding the neural correlates of PE and suggest that the FN and PE may be related to the same biobehavioral approach system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Autumn Kujawa
- Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY 11794-2500, USA.
| | - Greg Hajcak Proudfit
- Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY 11794-2500, USA
| | - Ellen M Kessel
- Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY 11794-2500, USA
| | - Margaret Dyson
- Child and Adolescent Services Research Center, Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, 3020 Children's Way, San Diego, CA 92123, USA
| | - Thomas Olino
- Department of Psychology, Temple University, 1701 North 13th St, Weiss Hall, Philadelphia, PA 19122, USA
| | - Daniel N Klein
- Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY 11794-2500, USA
| |
Collapse
|
161
|
Zhao K, Smillie LD. The Role of Interpersonal Traits in Social Decision Making. PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW 2014; 19:277-302. [DOI: 10.1177/1088868314553709] [Citation(s) in RCA: 138] [Impact Index Per Article: 13.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Economic games are well-established experimental paradigms for modeling social decision making. A large body of literature has pointed to the heterogeneity of behavior within many of these games, which might be partly explained by broad interpersonal trait dispositions. Using the Big Five and HEXACO (Honesty-Humility, Emotionality, eXtraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Openness to Experience) personality frameworks, we review the role of personality in two main classes of economic games: social dilemmas and bargaining games. This reveals an emerging role for Big Five agreeableness in promoting cooperative, egalitarian, and altruistic behaviors across several games, consistent with its core characteristic of maintaining harmonious interpersonal relations. The role for extraversion is less clear, which may reflect the divergent effects of its underlying agentic and affiliative motivational components. In addition, HEXACO honesty-humility and agreeableness may capture distinct aspects of prosocial behavior outside the bounds of the Five-Factor Model. Important considerations and directions for future studies are discussed within the emerging personality–economics interface.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kun Zhao
- University of Melbourne, Australia
| | | |
Collapse
|
162
|
Lemelin C, Lussier Y, Sabourin S, Brassard A, Naud C. Risky sexual behaviours: The role of substance use, psychopathic traits, and attachment insecurity among adolescents and young adults in Quebec. THE CANADIAN JOURNAL OF HUMAN SEXUALITY 2014. [DOI: 10.3138/cjhs.2625] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
The purpose of this study was to examine how substance use, psychopathic traits, and attachment representations explain sexual risk-taking in adolescence. Variables used to define risky sexual behaviours were age at first sexual intercourse and number of lifetime sexual partners. A sample of 1,553 adolescents and young adults completed a battery of questionnaires including measures of substance use, psychopathic traits, attachment representations, and sexual behaviours. Structural equation modelling demonstrated that, in the present sample, risky sexual behaviours were best modelled through both direct effects of avoidant attachment representations and indirect effects of psychopathic traits and abandonment anxiety through increases in substance use. This model was gender-invariant and may represent a contemporary trend toward homogenization of sexual practices among young people in Quebec.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carmen Lemelin
- Department of Psychology, Université du Québec à Trois-Rivières, Trois-Rivières, QC
| | - Yvan Lussier
- Department of Psychology, Université du Québec à Trois-Rivières, Trois-Rivières, QC
| | | | - Audrey Brassard
- Department of Psychology, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC
| | - Christopher Naud
- Department of Psychology, Université du Québec à Trois-Rivières, Trois-Rivières, QC
| |
Collapse
|
163
|
Balconi M, Finocchiaro R, Canavesio Y, Messina R. Reward bias and lateralization in gambling behavior: behavioral activation system and alpha band analysis. Psychiatry Res 2014; 219:570-6. [PMID: 25017618 DOI: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.06.020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/01/2013] [Revised: 06/04/2014] [Accepted: 06/14/2014] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
The present research explored the main factors that can influence subjects' choices in the case of decisions. In order to elucidate the individual differences that influence the decisional processes, making their strategies more or less advantageous, we tested the effect of a reward sensitivity in the behavioral activation system (BAS-Reward) constructed on the ability to distinguish between high- and low-risk decisions. Secondly, the lateralization effect, related to increased activation of the left (BAS-related) hemisphere, was explored. Thirty-one subjects were tested using the Iowa Gambling Task, and the BAS-Reward measure was applied to distinguish between high-BAS and low-BAS groups. Behavioral responses (gain/loss options) and alpha-band modulation were considered. It was found that high-BAS group increased their tendency to opt in favor of the immediate reward (loss strategy) rather than the long-term option (win strategy). Secondly, high-BAS subjects showed an increased left-hemisphere activation in response to losing (with immediate reward) choices in comparison with low-BAS subjects. A "reward bias" effect was supposed to explain both the bad strategy and the unbalanced hemispheric activation for high-BAS and more risk-taking subjects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michela Balconi
- Research Unit in Affective and Social Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Largo Gemelli, 1, 20123 Milan, Italy; Department of Psychology, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Milan, Italy.
| | - Roberta Finocchiaro
- Research Unit in Affective and Social Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Largo Gemelli, 1, 20123 Milan, Italy; Department of Psychology, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Milan, Italy
| | - Ylenia Canavesio
- Department of Psychology, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Milan, Italy
| | - Rossella Messina
- Department of Psychology, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Milan, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
164
|
Reward Sensitivity, Decisional Bias, and Metacognitive Deficits in Cocaine Drug Addiction. J Addict Med 2014; 8:399-406. [PMID: 25303980 DOI: 10.1097/adm.0000000000000065] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
165
|
Li Y, Qiao L, Sun J, Wei D, Li W, Qiu J, Zhang Q, Shi H. Gender-specific neuroanatomical basis of behavioral inhibition/approach systems (BIS/BAS) in a large sample of young adults: A voxel-based morphometric investigation. Behav Brain Res 2014; 274:400-8. [DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.08.041] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/15/2014] [Revised: 08/14/2014] [Accepted: 08/20/2014] [Indexed: 12/21/2022]
|
166
|
Thacker JS, Middleton LE, McIlroy WE, Staines WR. The influence of an acute bout of aerobic exercise on cortical contributions to motor preparation and execution. Physiol Rep 2014; 2:2/10/e12178. [PMID: 25355852 PMCID: PMC4254103 DOI: 10.14814/phy2.12178] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/28/2023] Open
Abstract
Increasing evidence supports the use of physical activity for modifying brain activity and overall neurological health. Specifically, aerobic exercise appears to have a positive effect on cognitive function, which some have suggested to be a result of increasing levels of arousal. However, the role of aerobic exercise on movement-related cortical activity is less clear. We tested the hypothesis that (1) an acute bout of exercise modulates excitability within motor areas and (2) transient effects would be sustained as long as sympathetic drive remained elevated (indicated by heart rate). In experiment 1, participants performed unimanual self-paced wrist extension movements before and after a 20-min, moderate intensity aerobic exercise intervention on a recumbent cycle ergometer. After the cessation of exercise, Bereitschaftspotentials (BP), representative cortical markers for motor preparation, were recorded immediately postexercise (Post) and following a return to baseline heart rate (Post[Rest]). Electroencephalography (EEG) was used to measure the BP time-locked to onset of muscle activity and separated into three main components: early, late and reafferent potentials. In experiment 2, two additional time points postexercise were added to the original protocol following the Post[Rest] condition. Early BP but not late BP was influenced by aerobic exercise, evidenced by an earlier onset, indicative of a regionally selective effect across BP generators. Moreover, this effect was sustained for up to an hour following exercise cessation and this effect was following a return to baseline heart rate. These data demonstrate that acute aerobic exercise may alter and possibly enhance the cortical substrates required for the preparation of movement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jonathan S Thacker
- Department of Kinesiology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
| | - Laura E Middleton
- Department of Kinesiology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
| | - William E McIlroy
- Department of Kinesiology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
| | - W Richard Staines
- Department of Kinesiology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
167
|
Throwing more light on the dark side of psychopathy: An extension of previous findings for the revised Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory. PERSONALITY AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2014. [DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.04.024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
168
|
Balconi M, Cobelli C. Motivational mechanisms (BAS) and prefrontal cortical activation contribute to recognition memory for emotional words. rTMS effect on performance and EEG (alpha band) measures. BRAIN AND LANGUAGE 2014; 137:77-85. [PMID: 25190327 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.08.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/03/2014] [Revised: 08/02/2014] [Accepted: 08/11/2014] [Indexed: 05/26/2023]
Abstract
The present research addressed the question of where memories for emotional words could be represented in the brain. A second main question was related to the effect of personality traits, in terms of the Behavior Activation System (BAS), in emotional word recognition. We tested the role of the left DLPFC (LDLPFC) by performing a memory task in which old (previously encoded targets) and new (previously not encoded distractors) positive or negative emotional words had to be recognized. High-BAS and low-BAS subjects were compared when a repetitive TMS (rTMS) was applied on the LDLPFC. We found significant differences between high-BAS vs. low-BAS subjects, with better performance for high-BAS in response to positive words. In parallel, an increased left cortical activity (alpha desynchronization) was observed for high-BAS in the case of positive words. Thus, we can conclude that the left approach-related hemisphere, underlying BAS, may support faster recognition of positive words.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michela Balconi
- Research Unit in Affective and Social Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, Catholic University of Sacred Heart, Milan, Italy; Laboratory of Cognitive Psychology, Department of Psychology, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Milan, Italy.
| | - Chiara Cobelli
- Laboratory of Cognitive Psychology, Department of Psychology, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Milan, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
169
|
Westbury C. You can't drink a word: lexical and individual emotionality affect subjective familiarity judgments. JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLINGUISTIC RESEARCH 2014; 43:631-649. [PMID: 24061785 DOI: 10.1007/s10936-013-9266-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/02/2023]
Abstract
For almost 30 years, subjective familiarity has been used in psycholinguistics as an explanatory variable, allegedly able to explain many phenomena that have no other obvious explanation (Gernsbacher in J Exp Psychol General 113:256-281, 1984). In this paper, the hypothesis tested is that the subjective familiarity of words is reflecting personal familiarity with or importance of the referents of words. Using an empirically-grounded model of affective force derived from Wundt (Grundriss der Psychologie [Outlines of Psychology]. Engelmann, Leibzig, 1896) and based in a co-occurrence model of semantics (which involves no human judgment), it is shown that affective force can account for the same variance in a large set of human subjective familiarity judgments as other human subjective familiarity judgments, can predict whether people will rate new words of the same objective frequency as more or less familiar, can predict lexical access as well as human subjective familiarity judgments do, and has a predicted relationship to age of acquisition norms. Individuals who have highly affective reactivity [as measured by Carver and White's (J Pers Soc Psychol 67(2):319-333, 1994) Behavioral Inhibition Scale and Behavioral Activation Scales] rate words as significantly more familiar than individuals who have low affective reactivity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chris Westbury
- Department of Psychology, University of Alberta, P220 Biological Sciences Building, Edmonton, AB, T6G 2E9, Canada,
| |
Collapse
|
170
|
Patrick CJ, Drislane LE. Triarchic Model of Psychopathy: Origins, Operationalizations, and Observed Linkages with Personality and General Psychopathology. J Pers 2014; 83:627-43. [PMID: 25109906 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12119] [Citation(s) in RCA: 169] [Impact Index Per Article: 16.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/26/2022]
Abstract
The triarchic model (Patrick, Fowles, & Krueger, 2009) was formulated to reconcile contrasting conceptions of psychopathy reflected in historic writings and contemporary assessment instruments, and to address persisting unresolved issues in the field. The model conceives of psychopathy as encompassing three distinct but interrelated phenotypic dispositions--disinhibition, boldness, and meanness--with biological referents. These dispositional constructs can be viewed as building blocks for alternative conceptions of psychopathy, and various existing psychopathy measures are presumed to index these constructs to differing degrees. This article summarizes the bases of the triarchic model in the conceptual and empirical literatures on psychopathy, and it describes linkages between the constructs of the model and established structural frameworks for personality and psychological disorders. Alternative methods for indexing the constructs of the model are described, and evidence regarding their interrelations and criterion-related validity is reviewed. Promising aspects of the model for ongoing research on psychopathy are discussed, along with current gaps in knowledge/methods and recommended avenues for future research.
Collapse
|
171
|
Anderson NE, Kiehl KA. Psychopathy: developmental perspectives and their implications for treatment. Restor Neurol Neurosci 2014; 32:103-17. [PMID: 23542910 DOI: 10.3233/rnn-139001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
Abstract
Psychopathy is a mental disorder marked by deficient emotional responses, lack of empathy, and poor behavioral controls, commonly resulting in persistent antisocial deviance and criminal behavior. Accumulating research suggests that psychopathy follows a developmental trajectory with strong genetic influences, and which precipitates deleterious effects on widespread functional networks, particularly within paralimbic regions of the brain. While traditional therapeutic interventions commonly administered in prisons and forensic institutions have been notoriously ineffective at combating these outcomes, alternative strategies informed by an understanding of these specific neuropsychological obstacles to healthy development, and which target younger individuals with nascent symptoms of psychopathy are more promising. Here we review recent neurobehavioral and neuroimaging literature that informs our understanding of the brain systems compromised in psychopathy, and apply these data to a broader understanding of its developmental course, ultimately promoting more proactive intervention strategies profiting from adaptive neuroplasticity in youth.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nathaniel E Anderson
- Mind Research Network, Lovelace Biomedical and Environmental Research Institute, Albuquerque, NM, USA University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA
| | - Kent A Kiehl
- Mind Research Network, Lovelace Biomedical and Environmental Research Institute, Albuquerque, NM, USA University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA
| |
Collapse
|
172
|
Mothers’ electrophysiological, subjective, and observed emotional responding to infant crying: The role of secure base script knowledge. Dev Psychopathol 2014; 27:1237-50. [PMID: 25196113 PMCID: PMC10395036 DOI: 10.1017/s0954579414000881] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
AbstractThis study examined the extent to which secure base script knowledge—reflected in the ability to generate narratives in which attachment-relevant events are encountered, a clear need for assistance is communicated, competent help is provided and accepted, and the problem is resolved—is associated with mothers’ electrophysiological, subjective, and observed emotional responses to an infant distress vocalization. While listening to an infant crying, mothers (N= 108,Mage = 34 years) lower on secure base script knowledge exhibited smaller shifts in relative left (vs. right) frontal EEG activation from rest, reported smaller reductions in feelings of positive emotion from rest, and expressed greater levels of tension. Findings indicate that lower levels of secure base script knowledge are associated with an organization of emotional responding indicative of a less flexible and more emotionally restricted response to infant distress. Discussion focuses on the contribution of mothers’ attachment representations to their ability to effectively manage emotional responding to infant distress in a manner expected to support sensitive caregiving.
Collapse
|
173
|
Patrick CJ. For distinguished contributions to psychophysiology: Don C. Fowles. Psychophysiology 2014; 51:715-7. [DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12219] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
174
|
Balconi M, Finocchiaro R, Canavesio Y. Reward-system effect (BAS rating), left hemispheric "unbalance" (alpha band oscillations) and decisional impairments in drug addiction. Addict Behav 2014; 39:1026-32. [PMID: 24629323 DOI: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.02.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/25/2013] [Revised: 02/12/2014] [Accepted: 02/13/2014] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
The current research explored the impact of cortical frontal asymmetry (left-lateralization effect) and Behavioral Activation System (BAS) on Substance Use Disorder (SUD) in decisional processes using the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT). Forty SUD participants and forty-two controls (CG) were tested using the IGT. Behavioral responses (gain/loss options), BIS/BAS scores and lateralized alpha band modulation (LTA) were considered. The SUD group increased the tendency to opt in favor of the immediate reward (loss strategy) more than the long-term option (win strategy) compared to the CG. Secondly, higher reward-subscale scores were observed in SUD. Thirdly, SUD showed an increase in left-hemisphere activation in response to losing (with immediate reward) choices in comparison with the CG. An imbalanced left hemispheric effect related to higher BAS trait could explain this "reward bias," because these components were found to explain (through the regression analysis) the main behavioral deficits.
Collapse
|
175
|
The relationship between reward and punishment sensitivity and antisocial behavior in male adolescents. PERSONALITY AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2014. [DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.054] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
|
176
|
Maples JL, Miller JD, Fortune E, MacKillop J, Campbell WK, Lynam DR, Lance CE, Goodie AS. An examination of the correlates of fearless dominance and self-centered impulsivity among high-frequency gamblers. J Pers Disord 2014; 28:379-93. [PMID: 24344842 DOI: 10.1521/pedi_2013_27_125] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
The Psychopathic Personality Inventory (PPI) is a commonly used assessment of psychopathy. Questions have been raised, however, regarding the validity of its Fearless Dominance (FD) factor. In the current study, the correlations manifested by FD and Self-centered Impulsivity (ScI) in relation to external criteria were examined in a sample of gamblers. Two key hypotheses were also tested: (a) does FD perform differently when paired with high scores on ScI, and (b) does FD serve as an index of narcissism. As expected, FD and ScI manifested a divergent pattern of correlations such that only ScI was associated with psychopathology or impairment. FD's relations with the external criteria were not generally moderated by scores on ScI. FD was significantly correlated with narcissism, but the two differed such that only narcissism was associated with any degree of maladaptivity. It remains unclear whether FD should be considered a core component of psychopathy.
Collapse
|
177
|
Ottaviani C, Borlimi R, Brighetti G, Caselli G, Favaretto E, Giardini I, Marzocchi C, Nucifora V, Rebecchi D, Ruggiero GM, Sassaroli S. Worry as an adaptive avoidance strategy in healthy controls but not in pathological worriers. Int J Psychophysiol 2014; 93:349-55. [PMID: 24873888 DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.05.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/29/2013] [Revised: 05/16/2014] [Accepted: 05/20/2014] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
The cognitive avoidance model of worry assumes that worry has the adaptive function to keep under control the physiological arousal associated with anxiety. This study aimed to test this model by the use of a fear induction paradigm in both pathological and healthy individuals. Thirty-one pathological worriers and 36 healthy controls accepted to be exposed to a fear induction paradigm (white noise) during three experimental conditions: worry, distraction, and reappraisal. Skin conductance (SCR) and heart rate variability (HRV) were measured as indices of sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous system functioning. Worriers showed increased sympathetic and decreased parasympathetic activation during the worry condition compared to non-worriers. There were no differences between groups for the distraction and reappraisal conditions. SCRs to the white noises during worry were higher in worriers versus controls throughout the entire worry period. Intolerance of uncertainty - but not metacognitive beliefs about worry - was a significant moderator of the relationship between worry and LF/HF-HRV in pathological worriers. Results support the cognitive avoidance model in healthy controls, suggesting that worry is no longer a functional attitude when it becomes the default/automatic and pathological response.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Rosita Borlimi
- Department of Psychology, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
| | | | - Gabriele Caselli
- Studi Cognitivi, Post Graduate Cognitive Psychotherapy School, Milano, Modena and San Benedetto del Tronto, Italy
| | - Ettore Favaretto
- Studi Cognitivi, Post Graduate Cognitive Psychotherapy School, Milano, Modena and San Benedetto del Tronto, Italy; Azienda Sanitaria di Bolzano, Italy
| | - Irene Giardini
- Studi Cognitivi, Post Graduate Cognitive Psychotherapy School, Milano, Modena and San Benedetto del Tronto, Italy
| | - Camilla Marzocchi
- Studi Cognitivi, Post Graduate Cognitive Psychotherapy School, Milano, Modena and San Benedetto del Tronto, Italy
| | | | - Daniela Rebecchi
- Studi Cognitivi, Post Graduate Cognitive Psychotherapy School, Milano, Modena and San Benedetto del Tronto, Italy
| | - Giovanni M Ruggiero
- Psicoterapia Cognitiva e Ricerca, Post Graduate Cognitive Psychotherapy School, Milano and Bolzano, Italy.
| | - Sandra Sassaroli
- Studi Cognitivi, Post Graduate Cognitive Psychotherapy School, Milano, Modena and San Benedetto del Tronto, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
178
|
Darragh M, Booth RJ, Consedine NS. Investigating the 'placebo personality' outside the pain paradigm. J Psychosom Res 2014; 76:414-21. [PMID: 24745784 DOI: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2014.02.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/09/2013] [Revised: 02/21/2014] [Accepted: 02/27/2014] [Indexed: 12/25/2022]
Abstract
AIM To identify personality traits related to placebo responding outside the context of pain. METHODS Sixty three healthy volunteers completed the study. Personality traits were measured online one week prior to a laboratory session in which two psychosocial stress tests were administered. Prior to the second test, the placebo group received an intranasal spray of 'serotonin' (placebo) with the suggestion that it would enhance recovery. Subjective stress, heart rate and heart rate variability were measured. Self reported and physiological responses to the placebo suggestion were assessed against personality variables. RESULTS Placebo effects were demonstrated in both self reported and physiological stress metrics. Lower optimism and less empathic concern predicted greater perceived benefits from the placebo treatment; and lower drive, fun, and sensation seeking were related to a greater physiological response to the manipulation. Multivariate analyses revealed lower optimism and behavioural drive to be predictive of responding to the placebo manipulation. CONCLUSION Findings are in contrast with prior work in pain paradigms which found higher levels of the same traits to be related to greater placebo analgesic responses. A cluster of traits characterised by behavioural drive, extraversion, optimism and novelty or fun seeking appears to be germane to placebo responsiveness, but contextual stimuli may generate different patterns of responding. A new conceptualisation of placebo responsiveness may be useful. Rather than a 'placebo personality' it may be that responsiveness is better typified by a two faceted transactional model, in which different personality facets respond to different contextual contingencies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Margot Darragh
- Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Auckland, New Zealand.
| | - Roger J Booth
- Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Auckland, New Zealand
| | - Nathan S Consedine
- Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Auckland, New Zealand
| |
Collapse
|
179
|
Tomé-Pires C, Miró J. Electrodermal responses and memory recall in migraineurs and headache-free controls. Eur J Pain 2014; 18:1298-306. [DOI: 10.1002/j.1532-2149.2014.490.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 02/16/2014] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- C. Tomé-Pires
- Unit for the Study and Treatment of Pain - ALGOS, Research Center for Behavior Assessment (CRAMC); Department of Psychology and Institut d'Investigació Sanitària Pere Virgili; Universitat Rovira i Virgili; Catalonia Spain
| | - J. Miró
- Unit for the Study and Treatment of Pain - ALGOS, Research Center for Behavior Assessment (CRAMC); Department of Psychology and Institut d'Investigació Sanitària Pere Virgili; Universitat Rovira i Virgili; Catalonia Spain
| |
Collapse
|
180
|
Gardiner E, Jackson CJ, Loxton NJ. Left Hemispheric Lateral Preference and High Neuroticism Predict Disinhibition in Two Go/No-Go Experiments. J Pers 2014; 83:84-96. [DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12084] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
181
|
Schreuder E, Hoeksma MR, Smeets MAM, Semin GR. The effects of odor and body posture on perceived duration. Front Neurorobot 2014; 8:6. [PMID: 24567718 PMCID: PMC3915145 DOI: 10.3389/fnbot.2014.00006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/13/2013] [Accepted: 01/19/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
This study reports an examination of the internal clock model, according to which subjective time duration is influenced by attention and arousal state. In a time production task, we examine the hypothesis that an arousing odor and an upright body posture affect perceived duration. The experimental task was performed while participants were exposed to an odor and either sitting upright (arousing condition) or lying down in a relaxing chair (relaxing condition). They were allocated to one of three experimental odor conditions: rosemary (arousing condition), peppermint (relaxing condition), and no odor (control condition). The predicted effects of the odors were not borne out by the results. Self-reported arousal (SRA) and pleasure (PL) states were measured before, during (after each body posture condition) and postexperimentally. Heart rate (HR) and skin conductance were measured before and during the experiment. As expected, odor had an effect on perceived duration. When participants were exposed to rosemary odor, they produced significantly shorter time intervals than in the no odor condition. This effect, however, could not be explained by increased arousal. There was no effect of body posture on perceived duration, even though body posture did induce arousal. The results do not support the proposed arousal mechanism of the internal clock model.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eliane Schreuder
- Behavioural and Societal Sciences, Toegepast Natuurwetenschappelijk Onderzoek Soesterberg, Netherlands
| | - Marco R Hoeksma
- Sensation Perception and Behaviour SSG, Unilever R&D Vlaardingen, Netherlands
| | - Monique A M Smeets
- Sensation Perception and Behaviour SSG, Unilever R&D Vlaardingen, Netherlands ; Department of Psychology, Faculty of Social and Behavioral Sciences, University of Utrecht Utrecht, Netherlands
| | - Gün R Semin
- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Social and Behavioral Sciences, University of Utrecht Utrecht, Netherlands ; Department of Psychology, Koç University Istanbul, Turkey ; Department of Psychology, Instituto Superior de Psicologia Aplicada Lisbon, Portugal
| |
Collapse
|
182
|
Demianczyk AC, Jenkins AL, Henson JM, Conner BT. Psychometric Evaluation and Revision of Carver and White's BIS/BAS Scales in a Diverse Sample of Young Adults. J Pers Assess 2014; 96:485-94. [DOI: 10.1080/00223891.2013.870570] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
183
|
Petit G, Kornreich C, Verbanck P, Cimochowska A, Campanella S. Why is adolescence a key period of alcohol initiation and who is prone to develop long-term problem use?: A review of current available data. SOCIOAFFECTIVE NEUROSCIENCE & PSYCHOLOGY 2013; 3:21890. [PMID: 24693359 PMCID: PMC3960066 DOI: 10.3402/snp.v3i0.21890] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/02/2013] [Revised: 08/02/2013] [Accepted: 09/02/2013] [Indexed: 11/14/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Early adolescence is a key developmental period for the initiation of alcohol use, and consumption among adolescents is characterized by drinking in high quantities. At the same time, adolescence is characterized by rapid biological transformations including dramatic changes in the brain, particularly in the prefrontal cortex and the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system. METHODS This article begins with an overview of the unique neural and behavioural characteristics of adolescent development that predispose these individuals to seek rewards and take risks such as initiation of drinking and high levels of alcohol intake. The authors then outline important factors associated with an increased risk for developing alcohol problems in later adolescence and young adulthood. Thereafter they address causality and the complex interplay of risk factors that lead to the development of alcohol use problems in late adolescence and young adults. CONCLUSIONS A few recommendations for the prevention of underage drinking are presented.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | | | - Salvatore Campanella
- Laboratory of Psychological Medicine and Addiction, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
184
|
Navarick DJ. Moral Ambivalence: Modeling and Measuring Bivariate Evaluative Processes in Moral Judgment. REVIEW OF GENERAL PSYCHOLOGY 2013. [DOI: 10.1037/a0034527] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
185
|
Santarcangelo EL, Varanini M, Paoletti G, Castellani E, Palombo C, Carli G. Pain-inducing imagery as a function of hypnotisability and of the activity of Gray's Behavioral Inhibition/Activation Systems. Neurosci Lett 2013; 557 Pt B:184-7. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.06.049] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/03/2013] [Revised: 06/22/2013] [Accepted: 06/24/2013] [Indexed: 02/07/2023]
|
186
|
The role of motivational factors in the relationship between hypnotizability and pain-inducing imagery. Neurosci Lett 2013; 557 Pt B:181-3. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.09.018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/02/2013] [Accepted: 09/06/2013] [Indexed: 01/17/2023]
|
187
|
Zemp M, Bodenmann G, Mark Cummings E. The role of skin conductance level reactivity in the impact of children's exposure to interparental conflict on their attention performance. J Exp Child Psychol 2013; 118:1-12. [PMID: 24184777 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.09.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/09/2013] [Revised: 08/22/2013] [Accepted: 09/18/2013] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Previous research suggests that undermining of attention performance might be one decisive underlying mechanism in the link between marital conflict and children's academic maladjustment, but little is known about specific risk patterns in this regard. This study examines, in an experimental approach, the role of children's history of interparental discord and skin conductance level reactivity (SCLR) as moderators in the link between analogue marital conflict exposure and children's attention. The attention performance of 57 children, aged 11 to 13 years, was assessed prior to and immediately after a 1-min video exposure to either (a) a couple conflict or (b) a neutral condition. SCLR was measured continuously throughout the stimulus presentation. Results indicated that children's family background of interparental conflict and their physiological reactivity moderated the influence of the experimental stimulus on children's short-term attention performance. Lower SCLR served as a protective factor in children from high-conflict homes exposed to the couple conflict. The current study advances the body of knowledge in this field by identifying risk patterns for the development of attention problems in children in relation to marital conflict exposure.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Martina Zemp
- Department of Psychology, University of Zurich, 8050 Zurich, Switzerland.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
188
|
Daughters SB, Gorka SM, Matusiewicz A, Anderson K. Gender specific effect of psychological stress and cortisol reactivity on adolescent risk taking. JOURNAL OF ABNORMAL CHILD PSYCHOLOGY 2013; 41:749-58. [PMID: 23338478 DOI: 10.1007/s10802-013-9713-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
The purpose of this study was to evaluate how psychological stress, gender and cortisol response to stress relate to risk behavior among 132 14-18 year old adolescents. Participants completed a laboratory based risk task prior to and immediately after a computerized psychological stress task, and salivary cortisol was collected from pre-stress to 60 min following initial stress exposure. Results indicate that adolescent boys (n = 59) and girls (n = 73) demonstrate different patterns of risk taking (RT) in response to stress, such that boys evidenced an increase in RT following stress exposure, whereas girls evidenced a decrease in RT. In addition, a gender by cortisol interaction demonstrated that for boys, both a smaller total cortisol output (AUCg) and peak cortisol response to stress (PC) was associated with greater stress-induced RT. Both cortisol measures were unrelated to stress-induced RT among girls. Taken together, data suggest that among boys, a blunted cortisol response to stress underlies an increase in risk taking in the context of psychological stress. Further research with an additional behavioral stress task is needed prior to drawing conclusions regarding the relation between female gender, cortisol response to stress, and risk taking in the context of psychological stress.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stacey B Daughters
- Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
189
|
Yaseen ZS, Chartrand H, Mojtabai R, Bolton J, Galynker II. Fear of dying in panic attacks predicts suicide attempt in comorbid depressive illness: prospective evidence from the National Epidemiological Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Depress Anxiety 2013; 30:930-9. [PMID: 23281011 DOI: 10.1002/da.22039] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/11/2012] [Revised: 11/07/2012] [Accepted: 11/14/2012] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
CONTEXT As a group, comorbid anxiety disorders among depressed persons have consistently been found to increase risk of suicide attempt (SA). Growing evidence supports the link between panic attacks (PAs) and suicidality, but prospective evidence is limited and the nature of the linkage remains unclear. The positive-feedback model of suicide suggests that PAs eliciting catastrophic cognitions may drive SA. OBJECTIVE To prospectively examine the relationship between PAs, panic symptoms and suicidality in individuals meeting DSM-IV criteria for past-year major depressive episodes in a large epidemiological study. METHODS In data on 2,864 participants of the National Epidemiological Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions (NESARC) wave 1 and 2 surveys with depressive disorders, the associations of PAs and panic symptoms with subsequent suicidal ideation (SI) and SAs were assessed using logistic regression. RESULTS Past-year PAs in wave 1 significantly increased odds for subsequent SI and attempt in the 3-year follow-up interval; however, in multivariate analyses, PAs were not a significant predictor, but PAs featuring fear of dying were. Further, among subjects with PAs, fear of dying during a PA increased the odds of subsequent SA sevenfold, even after controlling for comorbid disorders, demographic factors, and other PA symptoms. CONCLUSION PAs characterized by prominent morbid catastrophic cognitions may mediate the transition to SIs and SAs in subjects with depressive episodes. Presence of these symptoms in clinical settings may serve as a warning sign for future suicidality.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zimri S Yaseen
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Beth Israel Medical Center, First Avenue at Sixteenth Street, New York
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
190
|
Ginty AT. Blunted responses to stress and reward: Reflections on biological disengagement? Int J Psychophysiol 2013; 90:90-4. [DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.06.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/25/2013] [Revised: 06/07/2013] [Accepted: 06/10/2013] [Indexed: 01/08/2023]
|
191
|
De Pascalis V, Cozzuto G, Caprara GV, Alessandri G. Relations among EEG-alpha asymmetry, BIS/BAS, and dispositional optimism. Biol Psychol 2013; 94:198-209. [PMID: 23735707 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.05.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 72] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/27/2012] [Revised: 05/02/2013] [Accepted: 05/27/2013] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
192
|
van der Zee K, van Oudenhoven JP. Culture Shock or Challenge? The Role of Personality as a Determinant of Intercultural Competence. JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY 2013. [DOI: 10.1177/0022022113493138] [Citation(s) in RCA: 48] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
This paper provides a theoretical basis for the empirical link between traits and intercultural success indicators relying on the A (Affect) B (Behavior) C (Cognition)-model of culture shock. With respect to affect, we argue that intercultural traits can be differentiated according to whether they predispose individuals to be (in-)sensitive to either threat or challenge. Whereas stress-related traits (emotional stability, flexibility) are linked to a lower tendency to perceive an intercultural situation as threatening, social-perceptual traits (social initiative, open-mindedness) may predispose individuals to perceive its challenging aspects and respond with positive affect. As a behavioral consequence, stress-buffering traits may protect against culture shock, whereas social-perceptual traits may facilitate cultural learning. Finally, the ABC-model defines cognitions in terms of associated cultural identity patterns. Whereas stress-related traits may help individuals refrain from sticking to one’s own culture, social-perceptual traits reinforce identification with new culture. Implications for training and development are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Karen van der Zee
- University of Twente, Enschede, The Netherlands
- University of Groningen, The Netherlands
| | | |
Collapse
|
193
|
|
194
|
Schmidt-Daffy M, Brandenburg S, Beliavski A. Velocity, safety, or both? How do balance and strength of goal conflicts affect drivers' behaviour, feelings and physiological responses? ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2013; 55:90-100. [PMID: 23523895 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2013.02.030] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/28/2012] [Revised: 01/29/2013] [Accepted: 02/18/2013] [Indexed: 06/02/2023]
Abstract
Motivational models of driving behaviour agree that choice of speed is modulated by drivers' goals. Whilst it is accepted that some goals favour fast driving and others favour safe driving, little is known about the interplay of these conflicting goals. In the present study, two aspects of this interplay are investigated: the balance of conflict and the strength of conflict. Thirty-two participants completed several simulated driving runs in which fast driving was rewarded with a monetary gain if the end of the track was reached. However, unpredictably, some runs ended with the appearance of a deer. In these runs, fast driving was punished with a monetary loss. The ratio between the magnitudes of gains and losses varied in order to manipulate the balance of conflict. The absolute magnitudes of both gains and losses altered the strength of conflict. Participants drove slower, reported an increase in anxiety-related feelings, and showed indications of physiological arousal if there was more money at stake. In contrast, only marginal effects of varying the ratio between gains and losses were observed. Results confirm that the strength of a safety-velocity conflict is an important determinant of drivers' behaviour, feelings, and physiological responses. The lack of evidence for the balance of conflict playing a role suggests that in each condition, participants subjectively weighted the loss higher than the gain (loss aversion). It is concluded that the interplay of the subjective values that drivers attribute to objective incentives for fast and safe driving is a promising field for future research. Incorporating this knowledge into motivational theories of driving behaviour might improve their contribution to the design of adequate road safety measures.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Martin Schmidt-Daffy
- Department for Psychology and Ergonomics, Berlin Institute of Technology, Germany.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
195
|
Matejka M, Kazzer P, Seehausen M, Bajbouj M, Klann-Delius G, Menninghaus W, Jacobs AM, Heekeren HR, Prehn K. Talking about Emotion: Prosody and Skin Conductance Indicate Emotion Regulation. Front Psychol 2013; 4:260. [PMID: 23675363 PMCID: PMC3650449 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00260] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/24/2012] [Accepted: 04/19/2013] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Talking about emotion and putting feelings into words has been hypothesized to regulate emotion in psychotherapy as well as in everyday conversation. However, the exact dynamics of how different strategies of verbalization regulate emotion and how these strategies are reflected in characteristics of the voice has received little scientific attention. In the present study, we showed emotional pictures to 30 participants and asked them to verbally admit or deny an emotional experience or a neutral fact concerning the picture in a simulated conversation. We used a 2 × 2 factorial design manipulating the focus (on emotion or facts) as well as the congruency (admitting or denying) of the verbal expression. Analyses of skin conductance response (SCR) and voice during the verbalization conditions revealed a main effect of the factor focus. SCR and pitch of the voice were lower during emotion compared to fact verbalization, indicating lower autonomic arousal. In contradiction to these physiological parameters, participants reported that fact verbalization was more effective in down-regulating their emotion than emotion verbalization. These subjective ratings, however, were in line with voice parameters associated with emotional valence. That is, voice intensity showed that fact verbalization reduced negative valence more than emotion verbalization. In sum, the results of our study provide evidence that emotion verbalization as compared to fact verbalization is an effective emotion regulation strategy. Moreover, based on the results of our study we propose that different verbalization strategies influence valence and arousal aspects of emotion selectively.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Moritz Matejka
- Cluster of Excellence “Languages of Emotion,” Freie UniversitätBerlin, Germany
- Dahlem Institute for Neuroimaging of Emotion, Freie UniversitätBerlin, Germany
- Department of Education and Psychology, Freie UniversitätBerlin, Germany
| | - Philipp Kazzer
- Cluster of Excellence “Languages of Emotion,” Freie UniversitätBerlin, Germany
- Dahlem Institute for Neuroimaging of Emotion, Freie UniversitätBerlin, Germany
- Department of Psychiatry, Campus Benjamin Franklin, CharitéBerlin, Germany
| | - Maria Seehausen
- Cluster of Excellence “Languages of Emotion,” Freie UniversitätBerlin, Germany
- Dahlem Institute for Neuroimaging of Emotion, Freie UniversitätBerlin, Germany
- Department of Psychiatry, Campus Benjamin Franklin, CharitéBerlin, Germany
| | - Malek Bajbouj
- Cluster of Excellence “Languages of Emotion,” Freie UniversitätBerlin, Germany
- Dahlem Institute for Neuroimaging of Emotion, Freie UniversitätBerlin, Germany
- Department of Psychiatry, Campus Benjamin Franklin, CharitéBerlin, Germany
| | - Gisela Klann-Delius
- Cluster of Excellence “Languages of Emotion,” Freie UniversitätBerlin, Germany
- Department of Linguistics, Institut für Deutsche und Niederländische Philologie, Freie Universität BerlinBerlin, Germany
| | - Winfried Menninghaus
- Cluster of Excellence “Languages of Emotion,” Freie UniversitätBerlin, Germany
- Max Planck Institute for Empirical AestheticsFrankfurt am Main, Germany
| | - Arthur M. Jacobs
- Cluster of Excellence “Languages of Emotion,” Freie UniversitätBerlin, Germany
- Dahlem Institute for Neuroimaging of Emotion, Freie UniversitätBerlin, Germany
- Department of Education and Psychology, Freie UniversitätBerlin, Germany
| | - Hauke R. Heekeren
- Cluster of Excellence “Languages of Emotion,” Freie UniversitätBerlin, Germany
- Dahlem Institute for Neuroimaging of Emotion, Freie UniversitätBerlin, Germany
- Department of Education and Psychology, Freie UniversitätBerlin, Germany
| | - Kristin Prehn
- Cluster of Excellence “Languages of Emotion,” Freie UniversitätBerlin, Germany
- Dahlem Institute for Neuroimaging of Emotion, Freie UniversitätBerlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
196
|
He Z, Cassaday HJ, Bonardi C, Bibby PA. Do personality traits predict individual differences in excitatory and inhibitory learning? Front Psychol 2013; 4:245. [PMID: 23658551 PMCID: PMC3647220 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00245] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/07/2013] [Accepted: 04/14/2013] [Indexed: 12/11/2022] Open
Abstract
Conditioned inhibition (CI) is demonstrated in classical conditioning when a stimulus is used to signal the omission of an otherwise expected outcome. This basic learning ability is involved in a wide range of normal behavior – and thus its disruption could produce a correspondingly wide range of behavioral deficits. The present study employed a computer-based task to measure conditioned excitation and inhibition in the same discrimination procedure. CI by summation test was clearly demonstrated. Additionally summary measures of excitatory and inhibitory learning (difference scores) were calculated in order to explore how performance related to individual differences in a large sample of normal participants (n = 176 following exclusion of those not meeting the basic learning criterion). The individual difference measures selected derive from two biologically based personality theories, Gray’s (1982) reinforcement sensitivity theory and Eysenck and Eysenck (1991) psychoticism, extraversion, and neuroticism theory. Following the behavioral tasks, participants completed the behavioral inhibition system/behavioral activation system (BIS/BAS) scales and the Eysenck personality questionnaire revised short scale (EPQ-RS). Analyses of the relationship between scores on each of the scales and summary measures of excitatory and inhibitory learning suggested that those with higher BAS (specifically the drive sub-scale) and higher EPQ-RS neuroticism showed reduced levels of excitatory conditioning. Inhibitory conditioning was similarly attenuated in those with higher EPQ-RS neuroticism, as well as in those with higher BIS scores. Thus the findings are consistent with higher levels of neuroticism being accompanied by generally impaired associative learning, both inhibitory and excitatory. There was also evidence for some dissociation in the effects of behavioral activation and behavioral inhibition on excitatory and inhibitory learning respectively.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zhimin He
- School of Psychology, University of Nottingham Nottingham, UK
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
197
|
Pujara M, Motzkin JC, Newman JP, Kiehl KA, Koenigs M. Neural correlates of reward and loss sensitivity in psychopathy. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2013; 9:794-801. [PMID: 23552079 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nst054] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Psychopathy is a personality disorder associated with callous and impulsive behavior and criminal recidivism. It has long been theorized that psychopaths have deficits in processing reward and punishment. Here, we use structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging to examine the neural correlates of reward and loss sensitivity in a group of criminal psychopaths. Forty-one adult male prison inmates (n = 18 psychopaths and n = 23 non-psychopaths) completed a functional magnetic resonance imaging task involving the gain or loss of money. Across the entire sample of participants, monetary gains elicited robust activation within the ventral striatum (VS). Although psychopaths and non-psychopaths did not significantly differ with respect to overall levels of VS response to reward vs loss, we observed significantly different correlations between VS responses and psychopathy severity within each group. Volumetric analyses of striatal subregions revealed a similar pattern of correlations, specifically for the right accumbens area within VS. In a separate sample of inmates (n = 93 psychopaths and n = 117 non-psychopaths) who completed a self-report measure of appetitive motivation, we again found that the correlation with psychopathy severity differed between groups. These convergent results offer novel insight into the neural substrates of reward and loss processing in psychopathy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maia Pujara
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53719, USA, Neuroscience Training Program and Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53706, USA, The nonprofit MIND Research Network, an affiliate of Lovelace Biomedical and Environmental Research Institute, Albuquerque, NM 87131, USA, Department of Psychology, and Department of Neuroscience, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131, USADepartment of Psychiatry, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53719, USA, Neuroscience Training Program and Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53706, USA, The nonprofit MIND Research Network, an affiliate of Lovelace Biomedical and Environmental Research Institute, Albuquerque, NM 87131, USA, Department of Psychology, and Department of Neuroscience, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131, USA
| | - Julian C Motzkin
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53719, USA, Neuroscience Training Program and Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53706, USA, The nonprofit MIND Research Network, an affiliate of Lovelace Biomedical and Environmental Research Institute, Albuquerque, NM 87131, USA, Department of Psychology, and Department of Neuroscience, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131, USADepartment of Psychiatry, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53719, USA, Neuroscience Training Program and Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53706, USA, The nonprofit MIND Research Network, an affiliate of Lovelace Biomedical and Environmental Research Institute, Albuquerque, NM 87131, USA, Department of Psychology, and Department of Neuroscience, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131, USA
| | - Joseph P Newman
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53719, USA, Neuroscience Training Program and Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53706, USA, The nonprofit MIND Research Network, an affiliate of Lovelace Biomedical and Environmental Research Institute, Albuquerque, NM 87131, USA, Department of Psychology, and Department of Neuroscience, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131, USA
| | - Kent A Kiehl
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53719, USA, Neuroscience Training Program and Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53706, USA, The nonprofit MIND Research Network, an affiliate of Lovelace Biomedical and Environmental Research Institute, Albuquerque, NM 87131, USA, Department of Psychology, and Department of Neuroscience, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131, USADepartment of Psychiatry, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53719, USA, Neuroscience Training Program and Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53706, USA, The nonprofit MIND Research Network, an affiliate of Lovelace Biomedical and Environmental Research Institute, Albuquerque, NM 87131, USA, Department of Psychology, and Department of Neuroscience, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131, USADepartment of Psychiatry, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53719, USA, Neuroscience Training Program and Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53706, USA, The nonprofit MIND Research Network, an affiliate of Lovelace Biomedical and Environmental Research Institute, Albuquerque, NM 87131, USA, Department of Psychology, and Department of Neuroscience, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131, USA
| | - Michael Koenigs
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53719, USA, Neuroscience Training Program and Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53706, USA, The nonprofit MIND Research Network, an affiliate of Lovelace Biomedical and Environmental Research Institute, Albuquerque, NM 87131, USA, Department of Psychology, and Department of Neuroscience, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131, USA
| |
Collapse
|
198
|
Rhodes JD, Colder CR, Trucco EM, Speidel C, Hawk LW, Lengua LJ, Eiden RD, Wiezcorek W. The interaction between self-regulation and motivation prospectively predicting problem behavior in adolescence. JOURNAL OF CLINICAL CHILD AND ADOLESCENT PSYCHOLOGY : THE OFFICIAL JOURNAL FOR THE SOCIETY OF CLINICAL CHILD AND ADOLESCENT PSYCHOLOGY, AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGICAL ASSOCIATION, DIVISION 53 2013; 42:681-92. [PMID: 23477426 PMCID: PMC3726563 DOI: 10.1080/15374416.2013.773515] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
A large literature suggests associations between self-regulation and motivation and adolescent problem behavior; however, this research has mostly pitted these constructs against one another or tested them in isolation. Following recent neural-systems based theories (e.g., Ernst & Fudge, 2009 ), the present study investigated the interactions between self-regulation and approach and avoidance motivation prospectively predicting delinquency and depressive symptoms in early adolescence. The community sample included 387 adolescents aged 11 to 13 years old (55% female; 17% minority). Laboratory tasks were used to assess self-regulation and approach and avoidance motivation, and adolescent self-reports were used to measure depressive symptoms and delinquency. Analyses suggested that low levels of approach motivation were associated with high levels of depressive symptoms, but only at high levels of self-regulation (p = .01). High levels of approach were associated with high levels of rule breaking, but only at low levels of self-regulation (p < .05). These findings support contemporary neural-based systems theories that posit integration of motivational and self-regulatory individual differences via moderational models to understand adolescent problem behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jessica D. Rhodes
- University at Buffalo – Psychology, 305 Diefendorf Hall, Buffalo, NY 14260
| | - Craig R. Colder
- University at Buffalo – Psychology, 305 Diefendorf Hall, Buffalo, NY 14260
| | | | - Carolyn Speidel
- University at Buffalo – Psychology, 305 Diefendorf Hall, Buffalo, NY 14260
| | - Larry W. Hawk
- University at Buffalo – Psychology, 305 Diefendorf Hall, Buffalo, NY 14260
| | | | - Rina Das Eiden
- University at Buffalo – Psychology, 305 Diefendorf Hall, Buffalo, NY 14260
| | | |
Collapse
|
199
|
김혜미, MunSeon Chang, SO YEUN KIM. The Effects of Behavioral Activation System/ Behavioral Inhibition System (BAS/BIS) on Decision-making in Internet Game Addict. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2013. [DOI: 10.17315/kjhp.2013.18.1.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
|
200
|
Abstract
In accordance with a recently proposed account of amygdala function in psychopathy, it is hypothesized that people with high levels of psychopathic personality traits have a bias in learning style to encode the general valence, and neglect the specific-features, of an outcome. We present a novel learning task designed to operationalize these biases in learning style. The results from pilot samples of healthy adults and children and from a clinical sample of children with conduct problems provide support for the validity of the learning task as a measure of learning style and demonstrate a significant relationship between general-valence style learning and psychopathic personality traits. It is suggested that this relationship may be important for the aetiology of the social-cognitive deficits exhibited by psychopaths. These preliminary results suggest that this measure of learning style has the potential to be utilized as a research tool and may assist with the early identification, and treatment, of children with conduct problems and high levels of callous-unemotional traits.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Caroline Moul
- School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW 2052, Australia.
| | | |
Collapse
|