1
|
Li N, Yang J, Long C, Lei X. Test-Retest Reliability of EEG Aperiodic Components in Resting and Mental Task States. Brain Topogr 2024; 37:961-971. [PMID: 39017780 DOI: 10.1007/s10548-024-01067-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/07/2023] [Accepted: 07/03/2024] [Indexed: 07/18/2024]
Abstract
Aperiodic activity is derived from the electroencephalography (EEG) power spectrum and reflects changes in the slope and shifts of the broadband spectrum. Studies have shown inconsistent test-retest reliability of the aperiodic components. This study systematically measured how the test-retest reliability of the aperiodic components was affected by data duration (1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 min), states (resting with eyes closed, resting with eyes open, performing mental arithmetic, recalling the events of the day, and mentally singing songs), and methods (the Fitting Oscillations and One-Over-F (FOOOF) and Linear Mixed-Effects Regression (LMER)) at both short (90-min) and long (one-month) intervals. The results showed that aperiodic components had fair, good, or excellent test-retest reliability (ranging from 0.53 to 0.91) at both short and long intervals. It is recommended that better reliability of the aperiodic components be obtained using data durations longer than 3 min, the resting state with eyes closed, the mental arithmetic task state, and the LMER method.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Na Li
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality of the Ministry of Education, Southwest University, Chongqing, 400715, China
| | - Jingqi Yang
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality of the Ministry of Education, Southwest University, Chongqing, 400715, China
| | - Changquan Long
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality of the Ministry of Education, Southwest University, Chongqing, 400715, China.
| | - Xu Lei
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality of the Ministry of Education, Southwest University, Chongqing, 400715, China
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Sparks SD, Kritikos A. The ownership memory self-reference effect shifts recognition criterion but not recognition sensitivity. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2024; 88:1952-1968. [PMID: 38904705 PMCID: PMC11450121 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-024-01994-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/30/2023] [Accepted: 06/13/2024] [Indexed: 06/22/2024]
Abstract
Information referenced to the self is retrieved more accurately than information referenced to others, known as the memory self-reference effect. It is unclear, however, whether social context (identity of the other) or task factors alter decision-making processes. In a virtual object allocation task, female participants sorted objects into their own or another's (stranger or mother) basket based on a colour cue. Subsequently, they performed a recognition memory task in which they first indicated whether each object was old or new, and then whether it had been allocated to themselves or to the other. We obtained owner-specific hit rates and false-alarm rates and applied signal detection theory to derive separate recognition sensitivity (d') and recognition criterion parameters (c) for self- and other-owned objects. While there was no clear evidence of a recognition self-reference effect, or a change in sensitivity, participants adopted a more conservative recognition criterion for self- compared with other-owned objects, and particularly when the other-referent was the participant's mother compared with the stranger. Moreover, when discriminating whether the originally presented objects were self- or other-owned, participants were biased toward ascribing ownership to the 'other'. We speculate that these findings reflect ownership-based changes in decisional processing during the recognition memory self-reference paradigm.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S D Sparks
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Brisbane, 4072, Australia
| | - A Kritikos
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Brisbane, 4072, Australia.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Ahmed Z, Cunningham SJ, Rhodes S, Gow A, Macmillan K, Hutchison J, Ross J. The self-reference effect in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. BRITISH JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2024; 42:348-358. [PMID: 38660978 DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12489] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/17/2023] [Accepted: 04/12/2024] [Indexed: 04/26/2024]
Abstract
The self-memory system depends on the prioritization and capture of self-relevant information, so may be disrupted by difficulties in attending to, encoding and retrieving self-relevant information. The current study compares memory for self-referenced and other-referenced items in children with ADHD and typically developing comparison groups matched for verbal and chronological age. Children aged 5-14 (N = 90) were presented with everyday objects alongside an own-face image (self-reference trials) or an unknown child's image (other-referenced trials). They were asked whether the child shown would like the object, before completing a surprise source memory test. In a second task, children performed, and watched another person perform, a series of actions before their memory for the actions was tested. A significant self-reference effect (SRE) was found in the typically developing children (i.e. both verbal and chronological age-matched comparison groups) for the first task, with significantly better memory for self-referenced than other-referenced objects. However, children with ADHD showed no SRE, suggesting a compromised ability to bind information with the cognitive self-concept. In the second task, all groups showed superior memory for actions carried out by the self, suggesting a preserved enactment effect in ADHD. Implications and applications for the self-memory system in ADHD are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | | | - Kirsty Macmillan
- Abertay University, Dundee, UK
- Heriot-Watt University, Edinburgh, UK
| | - Jacqui Hutchison
- Abertay University, Dundee, UK
- University of Aberdeen, Aberdeen, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
4
|
Wong C, Navangul AS, Philipps SC, Kim K. The role of attention in the emergence of the evaluative and incidental self-reference effects. Memory 2024; 32:935-946. [PMID: 38963906 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2371571] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/24/2024] [Accepted: 06/18/2024] [Indexed: 07/06/2024]
Abstract
The self-reference effect (SRE) is a memory advantage produced by encoding information in a self-relevant manner. The "evaluative" SRE arises when people engage in explicit self-evaluation/reflection to process to-be-remembered items, while the "incidental" SRE occurs when self-referential information (e.g., one's own name) is co-presented with to-be-remembered items but is irrelevant to a given task. Using a divided-attention paradigm, the present study examined potential differences in the attentional requirements of the evaluative and incidental SREs. During encoding, personality-trait words were presented simultaneously with the participant's own or a celebrity's name. The participants' task was either to evaluate whether each word described themselves/the celebrity (evaluative encoding) or to indicate the location of each word (incidental encoding), in the presence or absence of a secondary task. A subsequent recognition test with a remember/know procedure showed better overall recognition and enhanced episodic recollection for words presented with one's own name vs. another name, with this SRE being larger in the evaluative than incidental encoding condition. Critically, divided attention at encoding attenuated the magnitudes of both evaluative and incidental SREs to a comparable degree in overall recognition and episodic recollection. These findings suggest that both the evaluative and incidental SREs are resource-demanding, effortful mnemonic benefits.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christy Wong
- Department of Psychology, Wesleyan University, Middletown, CT, USA
| | - Anaya S Navangul
- Department of Psychology, Wesleyan University, Middletown, CT, USA
| | | | - Kyungmi Kim
- Department of Psychology, Wesleyan University, Middletown, CT, USA
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Humphreys MS, Hockley WE, Chalmers KA. Recognition memory: The probe, the returned signal, and the decision. Psychon Bull Rev 2024; 31:568-598. [PMID: 37803230 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-021-01955-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 05/12/2021] [Indexed: 10/08/2023]
Abstract
In an attempt to better understand recognition memory we look at how three approaches (dual processing, signal detection, and global matching) have addressed the probe, the returned signal and the decision in four recognition paradigms. These are single-item recognition (including the remember/know paradigm), recognition in relational context, associative recognition, and source monitoring. The contrast, with regards to the double-miss rate (the probability of recognizing neither item in intact and rearranged pairs) and the effect of the oldness of the other member of the test pair, between identifying the old words in test pairs (the relational context paradigm) and first identifying the intact test pairs and then identifying the old words (adding associative recognition to the relational context paradigm) suggests that the retrieval of associative information in the relational context paradigm is unintentional, unlike the retrieval of associative information in associative recognition. It also seems possible that the information that is spontaneously retrieved in single-item recognition, possibly including the remember/know paradigm, is also unintentional, unlike the retrieval of information in source monitoring. Probable differences between intentional and unintentional retrieval, together with the pattern of effects with regards to the double-miss rate and the effect of the other member of the test pair, are used to evaluate the three approaches. Our conclusion is that all three approaches have something valid to say about recognition, but none is equally applicable across all four paradigms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael S Humphreys
- Department of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
| | - William E Hockley
- Department of Psychology, Wilfrid Laurier University, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada.
| | - Kerry A Chalmers
- Department of Psychology, The University of Newcastle, Callaghan, NSW, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Bögge L, Colás-Blanco I, Piolino P. Respiratory sinus arrhythmia during biofeedback is linked to persistent improvements in attention, short-term memory, and positive self-referential episodic memory. Front Neurosci 2022; 16:791498. [PMID: 36177356 PMCID: PMC9514056 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2022.791498] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/08/2021] [Accepted: 07/28/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Heart rate variability (HRV) biofeedback, an intervention based on the voluntary self-regulation of autonomic parameters, has been shown to affect prefrontal brain functioning and improve executive functions. The interest in using HRV biofeedback as cognitive training is typically ascribed to parasympathetic activation and optimized physiological functioning deriving from increased cardiac vagal control. However, the persistence of cognitive effects is poorly studied and their association with biofeedback-evoked autonomic changes has not yet been explored. In addition, no study has so far investigated the influence of HRV biofeedback in adults on long-term episodic memory, which is particularly concerned with self-referential encoding processing. Methods In the present study, a novel training system was developed integrating HRV and respiratory biofeedback into an immersive virtual reality environment to enhance training efficacy. Twenty-two young healthy adults were subjected to a blinded randomized placebo-controlled experiment, including six self-regulation training sessions, to evaluate the effect of biofeedback on autonomic and cognitive changes. Cardiac vagal control was assessed before, during, and 5 min after each training session. Executive functions, episodic memory, and the self-referential encoding effect were evaluated 1 week before and after the training program using a set of validated tasks. Results Linear mixed-effects models showed that HRV biofeedback greatly stimulated respiratory sinus arrhythmia during and after training. Moreover, it improved the attentional capabilities required for the identification and discrimination of stimuli ( η p 2 = 0.17), auditory short-term memory ( η p 2 = 0.23), and self-referential episodic memory recollection of positive stimuli ( η p 2 = 0.23). Episodic memory outcomes indicated that HRV biofeedback reinforced positive self-reference encoding processing. Cognitive changes were strongly dependent on the level of respiratory sinus arrhythmia evoked during self-regulation training. Conclusion The present study provides evidence that biofeedback moderates respiration-related cardiac vagal control, which in turn mediates improvements in several cognitive processes crucial for everyday functioning including episodic memory, that are maintained beyond the training period. The results highlight the interest in HRV biofeedback as an innovative research tool and medication-free therapeutic approach to affect autonomic and neurocognitive functioning. Finally, a neurocognitive model of biofeedback-supported autonomic self-regulation as a scaffolding for episodic memory is proposed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lukas Bögge
- Laboratoire Mémoire, Cerveau et Cognition, Université Paris Cité, Paris, France
| | - Itsaso Colás-Blanco
- Laboratoire Mémoire, Cerveau et Cognition, Université Paris Cité, Paris, France
| | - Pascale Piolino
- Laboratoire Mémoire, Cerveau et Cognition, Université Paris Cité, Paris, France
- Institut Universitaire de France (IUF), Paris, France
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Blondé P, Girardeau JC, Sperduti M, Piolino P. A wandering mind is a forgetful mind: A systematic review on the influence of mind wandering on episodic memory encoding. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2021; 132:774-792. [PMID: 34906400 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.11.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/06/2021] [Revised: 11/03/2021] [Accepted: 11/12/2021] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
In the last two decades, mind wandering has received increased interest in the field of cognitive neuroscience. Despite the strong links between attention and memory, its effect on episodic memory encoding has only been recently investigated. To date, there is no systematic synthesis on this link. Following the PRISMA guidelines, a systematic review of the literature on mind wandering and episodic memory was conducted. Five online bibliographic databases (PsycNET, Pubmed, ScienceDirect, Web of Science and Taylor & Francis) were searched. Twenty-four studies were eligible for the current review and were compared based on their methodologies and results. Overall, stimulus-independent mind wandering appeared to be a reliable negative factor influencing the encoding of both words and audio-visual stimuli. However, a few studies pointed out a potential positive effect of stimulus-dependent mind wandering on episodic memory encoding. Theoretical explanations of these results, the limits of existing investigations and avenues for potential future research are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Philippe Blondé
- Laboratoire Mémoire, Cerveau et Cognition (UR 7536), Institut de Psychologie, Université de Paris, Paris, France.
| | - Jean-Charles Girardeau
- Laboratoire Mémoire, Cerveau et Cognition (UR 7536), Institut de Psychologie, Université de Paris, Paris, France.
| | - Marco Sperduti
- Laboratoire Mémoire, Cerveau et Cognition (UR 7536), Institut de Psychologie, Université de Paris, Paris, France.
| | - Pascale Piolino
- Laboratoire Mémoire, Cerveau et Cognition (UR 7536), Institut de Psychologie, Université de Paris, Paris, France; Institut Universitaire de France, France.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Li Q, Liang T, Zhou E, Pan X, Zhang X. The group-reference effect and soldiers' recognition memory. Memory 2021; 29:146-152. [PMID: 33573460 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2020.1866014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
Previous research has consistently found that the self-reference effect (SRE) is equal to, or stronger than, the group-reference effect (GRE) for memory performance. The military strongly emphasises group identity; this study investigated whether the GRE was stronger than the SRE for soldiers. Soldiers were recruited to participate in Experiments 1 and Experiment 2. Experiment 1 revealed that recognition was better under the group-reference condition than the self-reference condition. Experiment 2 was identical to Experiment 1, with the exception that the recognition test required participants to use "remember" / "know" / "guess" judgments. The results were consistent with those of Experiment 1, that is, the GRE contributed to better recognition than the SRE, but the difference was statistically significant only for "know" responses. Using a less cohesive group (university students) as participants, Experiment 3 found that the GRE was not superior to the SRE for memory recognition, which indicated that the results of Experiments 1 and 2 were exclusive to soldiers. The findings suggest that soldiers' sense of self might be unique, and that an ingroup sense of self might be dominant for soldiers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Qiong Li
- School of Humanities and Foreign Languages, Xi'an University of Posts and Telecommunications, Xi'an, People's Republic of China
| | - Ting Liang
- School of Psychology, Northwest Normal University, LanZhou, People's Republic of China
| | - Eqiao Zhou
- School of Psychology, Northwest Normal University, LanZhou, People's Republic of China
| | - Xinxing Pan
- School of Economics and Management, Xi'an University of Posts and Telecommunications, Xi'an, People's Republic of China
| | - Xiaobin Zhang
- School of Psychology, Shaanxi Normal University, Xian, People's Republic of China
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Carson N, Rosenbaum RS, Moscovitch M, Murphy KJ. Self-referential processing improves memory for narrative information in healthy aging and amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment. Neuropsychologia 2019; 134:107179. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107179] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/14/2019] [Revised: 08/16/2019] [Accepted: 08/28/2019] [Indexed: 12/15/2022]
|
10
|
Mondragón JD, Maurits NM, De Deyn PP. Functional Neural Correlates of Anosognosia in Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer's Disease: a Systematic Review. Neuropsychol Rev 2019; 29:139-165. [PMID: 31161466 PMCID: PMC6560017 DOI: 10.1007/s11065-019-09410-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/15/2018] [Accepted: 05/08/2019] [Indexed: 12/11/2022]
Abstract
Functional neuroimaging techniques (i.e. single photon emission computed tomography, positron emission tomography, and functional magnetic resonance imaging) have been used to assess the neural correlates of anosognosia in mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and Alzheimer's disease (AD). A systematic review of this literature was performed, following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta Analyses statement, on PubMed, EMBASE, and PsycINFO databases. Twenty-five articles met all inclusion criteria. Specifically, four brain connectivity and 21 brain perfusion, metabolism, and activation articles. Anosognosia is associated in MCI with frontal lobe and cortical midline regional dysfunction (reduced perfusion and activation), and with reduced parietotemporal metabolism. Reduced within and between network connectivity is observed in the default mode network regions of AD patients with anosognosia compared to AD patients without anosognosia and controls. During initial stages of cognitive decline in anosognosia, reduced indirect neural activity (i.e. perfusion, metabolism, and activation) is associated with the cortical midline regions, followed by the parietotemporal structures in later stages and culminating in frontotemporal dysfunction. Although the current evidence suggests differences in activation between AD or MCI patients with anosognosia and healthy controls, more evidence is needed exploring the differences between MCI and AD patients with and without anosognosia using resting state and task related paradigms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jaime D Mondragón
- Department of Neurology, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, PO Box 30001, 9700 RB, Groningen, the Netherlands.
- Alzheimer Research Center, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands.
| | - Natasha M Maurits
- Department of Neurology, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, PO Box 30001, 9700 RB, Groningen, the Netherlands
| | - Peter P De Deyn
- Department of Neurology, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, PO Box 30001, 9700 RB, Groningen, the Netherlands
- Alzheimer Research Center, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
- Institute Born-Bunge, Laboratory of Neurochemistry and Behavior, University of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Kalenzaga S, Clarys D, Piolino P. Effortful memory processes under stereotype threat and self-concept in aging. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2018; 72:1876-1887. [PMID: 30501470 DOI: 10.1177/1747021818819787] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
The present study aimed at exploring the effect of stereotype threat on the controlled part of memory in older adults using a deep level of processing, namely, self-reference encoding. To meet this objective, 25 younger adults and 25 older adults performed a Remember/Know recognition task following self-reference versus other-reference encoding of adjective traits, under stereotype threat or not. The results indicated that under stereotype threat, older adults' production of Remember responses was specifically impaired following self-reference encoding. Moreover, whereas executive functioning and group identification did not moderate stereotype threat effect, measure of self-worth did. These findings suggest that stereotype threat in older adults may be a self-concept threat and that moderators of stereotype threat found in other groups (i.e., group identification and executive functioning) may not be generalised to this group. Rather, as stereotype threat in aging may represent a threat to the self, self-worth might have a central role, whereby individuals with high self-worth remain self-confident even under stereotype threat and are thus able to down-regulate their negative affects to face such a threat.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sandrine Kalenzaga
- 1 UMR-CNRS 7295 Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition et l'Apprentissage, Université de Poitiers, Poitiers, France
| | - David Clarys
- 1 UMR-CNRS 7295 Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition et l'Apprentissage, Université de Poitiers, Poitiers, France
| | - Pascale Piolino
- 2 Laboratoire Mémoire et Cognition, Institut de Psychologie, Université Paris Descartes-Sorbonne Paris Cité, Boulogne-Billancourt, France.,3 INSERM UMR S894, Centre de Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Université Paris Descartes, Paris, France.,4 Institut Universitaire de France (IUF), Paris, France
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Self-Reference Effect and Self-Reference Recollection Effect for Trait Adjectives in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2018; 24:821-832. [PMID: 30064540 DOI: 10.1017/s1355617718000395] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES The self-reference effect (SRE), enhanced memory for self-related information, has been studied in healthy young and older adults but has had little investigation in people with age-related memory disorders, such as amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI). Self-referential encoding may help to improve episodic memory in aMCI. Additionally, self-referential processing has been shown to benefit recollection, the vivid re-experiencing of past events, a phenomenon that has been termed the self-reference recollection effect (SRRE; Conway & Dewhurst, 1995). Furthermore, it remains unclear whether the valence of stimuli influences the appearance of the SRE and SRRE. METHODS The current study investigated the SRE and SRRE for trait adjective words in 20 individuals with aMCI and 30 healthy older adult controls. Ninety trait adjective words were allocated to self-reference, semantic, or structural encoding conditions; memory was later tested using a recognition test. RESULTS While healthy older adults showed a SRE, individuals with aMCI did not benefit from self-referential encoding over and above that of semantic encoding (an effect of "deep encoding"). A similar pattern was apparent for the SRRE; healthy controls showed enhanced recollection for words encoded in the self-reference condition, while the aMCI group did not show specific benefit to recollection for self-referenced over semantically encoded items. No effects of valence were found. CONCLUSIONS These results indicate that while memory for trait adjective words can be improved in aMCI with deep encoding strategies (whether self-reference or semantic), self-referencing does not provide an additional benefit. (JINS, 2018, 24, 821-832).
Collapse
|
13
|
Gutchess A, Kensinger EA. Shared Mechanisms May Support Mnemonic Benefits from Self-Referencing and Emotion. Trends Cogn Sci 2018; 22:712-724. [PMID: 29886010 PMCID: PMC6652178 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.05.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/26/2017] [Revised: 05/10/2018] [Accepted: 05/11/2018] [Indexed: 12/17/2022]
Abstract
The literatures on episodic memory for self-referential and emotional information have proceeded relatively independently, and most studies examining the effects of age on these memory processes have been interpreted within domain-specific frameworks. However, there is increasing evidence for shared mechanisms that contribute to episodic memory benefits in these two domains. We review this evidence and propose a model that incorporates overlapping as well as domain-specific contributions to episodic memory encoding of self-referential and emotional material. We discuss the implications for understanding the relatively intact memory of older adults for these classes of stimuli, and conclude with suggestions for future research to test key tenets and extensions of this shared-process model.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Angela Gutchess
- Aging, Culture, and Cognition Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, Waltham, MA, USA; Equal contributions.
| | - Elizabeth A Kensinger
- Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Boston College, Chestnut Hill, MA, USA; Equal contributions
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Axelsson EL, Dawson RL, Yim SY, Quddus T. Mine, Mine, Mine: Self-Reference and Children's Retention of Novel Words. Front Psychol 2018; 9:958. [PMID: 29946288 PMCID: PMC6005863 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00958] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/01/2018] [Accepted: 05/24/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Adults demonstrate enhanced memory for words encoded as belonging to themselves compared to those belonging to another. Known as the self-reference effect, there is evidence for the effect in children as young as three. Toddlers are efficient in linking novel words to novel objects, but have difficulties retaining multiple word-object associations. The aim here was to investigate the self-reference ownership paradigm on 3-year-old children's retention of novel words. Following exposure to each of four novel word-object pairings, children were told that objects either belonged to them or another character. Children demonstrated significantly higher immediate retention of self-referenced compared to other-referenced items. Retention was also tested 4 h later and the following morning. Retention for self- and other-referenced words was significantly higher than chance at both delayed time points, but the difference between the self- and other-referenced words was no longer significant. The findings suggest that when it comes to toddlers' retention of multiple novel words there is an initial memory enhancing effect for self- compared to other-referenced items, but the difference diminishes over time. Children's looking times during the self-reference presentations were positively associated with retention of self-referenced words 4 h later. Looking times during the other-reference presentations were positively associated with proportional looking at other-referenced items during immediate retention testing. The findings have implications for children's memory for novel words and future studies could test children's explicit memories for the ownership manipulation itself and whether the effect is superior to other forms of memory supports such as ostensive naming.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emma L. Axelsson
- Research School of Psychology, ANU College of Health & Medicine, Australian National University, Canberra, ACT, Australia
- Department of Psychology, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden
| | - Rachelle L. Dawson
- Research School of Psychology, ANU College of Health & Medicine, Australian National University, Canberra, ACT, Australia
| | - Sharon Y. Yim
- Research School of Psychology, ANU College of Health & Medicine, Australian National University, Canberra, ACT, Australia
| | - Tashfia Quddus
- Research School of Psychology, ANU College of Health & Medicine, Australian National University, Canberra, ACT, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Gaubert M, Villain N, Landeau B, Mézenge F, Egret S, Perrotin A, Belliard S, de La Sayette V, Eustache F, Desgranges B, Chételat G, Rauchs G. Neural Correlates of Self-Reference Effect in Early Alzheimer’s Disease. J Alzheimers Dis 2017; 56:717-731. [DOI: 10.3233/jad-160561] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Malo Gaubert
- U1077, INSERM, GIP Cyceron, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, University of Caen Normandy, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, Ecole Pratique des Hautes Etudes, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, Caen University Hospital, Caen, France
| | - Nicolas Villain
- U1077, INSERM, GIP Cyceron, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, University of Caen Normandy, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, Ecole Pratique des Hautes Etudes, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, Caen University Hospital, Caen, France
| | - Brigitte Landeau
- U1077, INSERM, GIP Cyceron, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, University of Caen Normandy, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, Ecole Pratique des Hautes Etudes, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, Caen University Hospital, Caen, France
| | - Florence Mézenge
- U1077, INSERM, GIP Cyceron, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, University of Caen Normandy, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, Ecole Pratique des Hautes Etudes, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, Caen University Hospital, Caen, France
| | - Stéphanie Egret
- U1077, INSERM, GIP Cyceron, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, University of Caen Normandy, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, Ecole Pratique des Hautes Etudes, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, Caen University Hospital, Caen, France
| | - Audrey Perrotin
- U1077, INSERM, GIP Cyceron, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, University of Caen Normandy, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, Ecole Pratique des Hautes Etudes, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, Caen University Hospital, Caen, France
| | - Serge Belliard
- U1077, INSERM, GIP Cyceron, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, University of Caen Normandy, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, Ecole Pratique des Hautes Etudes, Caen, France
- Neurology Department, Pontchaillou University Hospital, Rennes, France
| | - Vincent de La Sayette
- U1077, INSERM, GIP Cyceron, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, University of Caen Normandy, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, Ecole Pratique des Hautes Etudes, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, Caen University Hospital, Caen, France
| | - Francis Eustache
- U1077, INSERM, GIP Cyceron, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, University of Caen Normandy, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, Ecole Pratique des Hautes Etudes, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, Caen University Hospital, Caen, France
| | - Béatrice Desgranges
- U1077, INSERM, GIP Cyceron, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, University of Caen Normandy, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, Ecole Pratique des Hautes Etudes, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, Caen University Hospital, Caen, France
| | - Gaël Chételat
- U1077, INSERM, GIP Cyceron, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, University of Caen Normandy, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, Ecole Pratique des Hautes Etudes, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, Caen University Hospital, Caen, France
| | - Géraldine Rauchs
- U1077, INSERM, GIP Cyceron, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, University of Caen Normandy, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, Ecole Pratique des Hautes Etudes, Caen, France
- UMR-S1077, Caen University Hospital, Caen, France
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Leblond M, Laisney M, Lamidey V, Egret S, de La Sayette V, Chételat G, Piolino P, Rauchs G, Desgranges B, Eustache F. Self-reference effect on memory in healthy aging, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease: Influence of identity valence. Cortex 2016; 74:177-90. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.10.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/19/2015] [Revised: 07/10/2015] [Accepted: 10/27/2015] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
|
17
|
Carson N, Murphy KJ, Moscovitch M, Rosenbaum RS. Older adults show a self-reference effect for narrative information. Memory 2015; 24:1157-72. [DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2015.1080277] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
18
|
|
19
|
Bugaïska A, Ferreri L, Bouquet CA, Kalenzaga S, Clarys D. Self as a moderator of age-related deficit on Recollection. ANNEE PSYCHOLOGIQUE 2015. [DOI: 10.3917/anpsy.151.0077] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022]
|
20
|
Kalenzaga S, Sperduti M, Anssens A, Martinelli P, Devauchelle AD, Gallarda T, Delhommeau M, Lion S, Amado I, Krebs MO, Oppenheim C, Piolino P. Episodic memory and self-reference via semantic autobiographical memory: insights from an fMRI study in younger and older adults. Front Behav Neurosci 2015; 8:449. [PMID: 25628546 PMCID: PMC4292587 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00449] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/17/2014] [Accepted: 12/15/2014] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Self-referential processing relies mainly on the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) and enhances memory encoding (i.e., Self-Reference Effect, SRE) as it improves the accuracy and richness of remembering in both young and older adults. However, studies on age-related changes in the neural correlates of the SRE on the subjective (i.e., autonoetic consciousness) and the objective (i.e., source memory) qualitative features of episodic memory are lacking. In the present fMRI study, we compared the effects of a self-related (semantic autobiographical memory task) and a non self-related (general semantic memory task) encoding condition on subsequent episodic memory retrieval. We investigated encoding-related activity during each condition in two groups of 19 younger and 16 older adults. Behaviorally, the SRE improved subjective memory performance in both groups but objective memory only in young adults. At the neural level, a direct comparison between self-related and non self-related conditions revealed that SRE mainly activated the cortical midline system, especially the MPFC, in both groups. Additionally, in older adults and regardless of the condition, greater activity was found in a fronto-parietal network. Overall, correlations were noted between source memory performance and activity in the MPFC (irrespective of age) and visual areas (mediated by age). Thus, the present findings expand evidence of the role of the MPFC in self-referential processing in the context of source memory benefit in both young and older adults using incidental encoding via semantic autobiographical memory. However, our finding suggests that its role is less effective in aging.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sandrine Kalenzaga
- Memory and Cognition Laboratory, Institut de Psychologie, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité Boulogne-Billancourt, France ; Center of Psychiatry and Neurosciences, INSERM UMR S894, Université Paris Descartes Paris, France ; Research Center in Cognition and Learning, UMR-CNRS 7295, Université de Poitiers Poitiers, France
| | - Marco Sperduti
- Memory and Cognition Laboratory, Institut de Psychologie, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité Boulogne-Billancourt, France ; Center of Psychiatry and Neurosciences, INSERM UMR S894, Université Paris Descartes Paris, France
| | - Adèle Anssens
- Memory and Cognition Laboratory, Institut de Psychologie, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité Boulogne-Billancourt, France ; Center of Psychiatry and Neurosciences, INSERM UMR S894, Université Paris Descartes Paris, France
| | - Penelope Martinelli
- Memory and Cognition Laboratory, Institut de Psychologie, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité Boulogne-Billancourt, France ; Center of Psychiatry and Neurosciences, INSERM UMR S894, Université Paris Descartes Paris, France
| | - Anne-Dominique Devauchelle
- Department of Radiology, Centre Hospitalier Sainte-Anne, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité Paris, France
| | - Thierry Gallarda
- Center of Psychiatry and Neurosciences, INSERM UMR S894, Université Paris Descartes Paris, France ; Laboratory of Physiopathology of Psychiatric Diseases, Centre Hospitalier Sainte Anne Paris, France
| | - Marion Delhommeau
- Memory and Cognition Laboratory, Institut de Psychologie, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité Boulogne-Billancourt, France ; Center of Psychiatry and Neurosciences, INSERM UMR S894, Université Paris Descartes Paris, France
| | - Stéphanie Lion
- Department of Radiology, Centre Hospitalier Sainte-Anne, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité Paris, France
| | - Isabelle Amado
- Center of Psychiatry and Neurosciences, INSERM UMR S894, Université Paris Descartes Paris, France ; Laboratory of Physiopathology of Psychiatric Diseases, Centre Hospitalier Sainte Anne Paris, France
| | - Marie-Odile Krebs
- Center of Psychiatry and Neurosciences, INSERM UMR S894, Université Paris Descartes Paris, France ; Laboratory of Physiopathology of Psychiatric Diseases, Centre Hospitalier Sainte Anne Paris, France
| | - Catherine Oppenheim
- Center of Psychiatry and Neurosciences, INSERM UMR S894, Université Paris Descartes Paris, France ; Department of Radiology, Centre Hospitalier Sainte-Anne, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité Paris, France
| | - Pascale Piolino
- Memory and Cognition Laboratory, Institut de Psychologie, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité Boulogne-Billancourt, France ; Center of Psychiatry and Neurosciences, INSERM UMR S894, Université Paris Descartes Paris, France ; Institut Universitaire de France France
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Boduroglu A, Pehlivanoglu D, Tekcan AI, Kapucu A. Effects of self-referencing on feeling-of-knowing accuracy and recollective experience. Memory 2014; 23:736-47. [PMID: 24936954 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2014.925927] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
The current research investigated the impact of self-referencing (SR) on feeling-of-knowing (FOK) judgements to improve our understanding of the mechanisms underlying these metamemory judgements and specifically test the relationship between recollective experiences and FOK accuracy within the accessibility framework FOK judgements are thought to be by-products of the retrieval process and are therefore closely related to memory performance. Because relating information to one's self is one of the factors enhancing memory performance, we investigated the effect of self-related encoding on FOK accuracy and recollective experience. We compared performance on this condition to a separate deep processing condition in which participants reported the frequency of occurrence of pairs of words. Participants encoded pairs of words incidentally, and following a delay interval, they attempted at retrieving each target prompted by its cue. Then, they were re-presented with all cues and asked to provide FOK ratings regarding their likelihood of recognising the targets amongst distractors. Finally, they were given a surprise recognition task in which following each response they identified whether the response was remembered, known or just guessed. Our results showed that only SR at encoding resulted in better memory, higher FOK accuracy and increased recollective experience.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aysecan Boduroglu
- a Department of Psychology , Bogazici University , Bebek , Istanbul , Turkey
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
22
|
Bugaiska A, Mermillod M, Bonin P. Does the thought of death contribute to the memory benefit of encoding with a survival scenario? Memory 2014; 23:213-32. [PMID: 24502242 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2014.881881] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
Four studies tested whether the thought of death contributes to the survival processing advantage found in memory tests (i.e., the survival effect). In the first study, we replicated the "Dying To Remember" (DTR) effect identified by Burns and colleagues whereby activation of death thoughts led to better retention than an aversive control situation. In Study 2, we compared an ancestral survival scenario, a modern survival scenario and a "life-after-death" scenario. The modern survival scenario and the dying scenario led to higher levels of recall than the ancestral scenario. In Study 3, we used a more salient death-thought scenario in which people imagine themselves on death row. Results showed that the "death-row" scenario yielded a level of recall similar to that of the ancestral survival condition. We also collected ratings of death-related thoughts (Studies 3 and 4) and of survival-related and planning thoughts (Study 4). The ratings indicated that death-related thoughts were induced more by the dying scenarios than by the survival scenarios, whereas the reverse was observed for both survival-related and planning thoughts. The findings are discussed in the light of two contrasting views of the influence of mortality salience in the survival effect.
Collapse
|
23
|
Cunningham SJ, Brebner JL, Quinn F, Turk DJ. The self-reference effect on memory in early childhood. Child Dev 2013; 85:808-23. [PMID: 23888928 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12144] [Citation(s) in RCA: 67] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
The self-reference effect in memory is the advantage for information encoded about self, relative to other people. The early development of this effect was explored here using a concrete encoding paradigm. Trials comprised presentation of a self- or other-image paired with a concrete object. In Study 1, 4- to 6-year-old children (N = 53) were asked in each trial whether the child pictured would like the object. Recognition memory showed an advantage for self-paired objects. Study 2 (N = 55) replicated this finding in source memory. In Study 3 (N = 56), participants simply indicated object location. Again, recognition and source memory showed an advantage for self-paired items. These findings are discussed with reference to mechanisms that ensure information of potential self-relevance is reliably encoded.
Collapse
|
24
|
Genon S, Bahri MA, Collette F, Angel L, d'Argembeau A, Clarys D, Kalenzaga S, Salmon E, Bastin C. Cognitive and neuroimaging evidence of impaired interaction between self and memory in Alzheimer's disease. Cortex 2013; 51:11-24. [PMID: 23993283 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.06.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/06/2013] [Revised: 06/13/2013] [Accepted: 06/20/2013] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
In human cognition, self and memory processes strongly interact, as evidenced by the memory advantage for self-referential materials [Self-Reference Effect (SRE) and Self-Reference Recollection Effect (SRRE)]. The current study examined this interaction at the behavioural level and its neural correlates in patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD). Healthy older controls (HC) and AD patients performed trait-adjectives judgements either for self-relevance or for other-relevance (encoding phase). In a first experiment, the encoding and subsequent yes-no recognition phases were administrated in a Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) scanner. Brain activation as measured by functional MRI (fMRI) was examined during self-relevance judgements and anatomical images were used to search for correlation between the memory advantage for self-related items and grey matter density (GMD). In a second experiment, participants described the retrieval experience that had driven their recognition decisions (familiarity vs recollective experience). The behavioural results revealed that the SRE and SRRE were impaired in AD patients compared to HC participants. Furthermore, verbal reports revealed that the retrieval of self-related information was preferentially associated with the retrieval of contextual details, such as source memory in the HC participants, but less so in the AD patients. Our imaging findings revealed that both groups activated the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) at encoding during self-relevance judgements. However, the variable and limited memory advantage for self-related information was associated with GMD in the lateral prefrontal cortex in the AD patients, a region supporting high-order processes linking self and memory. These findings suggest that even if AD patients engage MPFC during self-referential judgements, the retrieval of self-related memories is qualitatively and quantitatively impaired in relation with altered high-order processes in the lateral PFC.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah Genon
- Cyclotron Research Centre, University of Liège, Belgium.
| | | | | | - Lucie Angel
- University François-Rabelais of Tours, UMR CNRS 7295 CeRCA, Tours, France
| | - Arnaud d'Argembeau
- Cyclotron Research Centre, University of Liège, Belgium; Department of Psychology, University of Liège, Belgium
| | - David Clarys
- University of Poitiers, UMR CNRS 7295 CeRCA, Poitiers, France
| | | | - Eric Salmon
- Cyclotron Research Centre, University of Liège, Belgium; Memory Centre, CHU Liège, Belgium
| | | |
Collapse
|
25
|
Kalenzaga S, Clarys D. Self-referential processing in Alzheimer's disease: two different ways of processing self-knowledge? J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2013; 35:455-71. [PMID: 23631426 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2013.789485] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
Abstract
Two previous studies showed that self-reference encoding had no effect on Alzheimer's disease (AD) patients' recollective experience when it was compared to other-reference encoding, whereas it did have an effect when it was compared to semantic processing, but only for emotional trait adjectives. In the present study, the performance of 22 AD patients was compared with that of 21 normal controls on a task involving recognition of emotional versus neutral adjective traits following self-reference versus other-reference encoding, using the remember/know/guess paradigm. Results showed that although AD patients had a positive explicit view of themselves, their self became salient for negative adjective traits only. We concluded that there might exist two ways of processing self-referential knowledge in human cognition: one explicit and the other more implicit.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sandrine Kalenzaga
- UMR-CNRS 6234 Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition et l'Apprentissage, Université de Poitiers, Poitiers, France.
| | | |
Collapse
|
26
|
Recollection is Fast and Easy. PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 2013. [DOI: 10.1016/b978-0-12-407187-2.00005-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
27
|
|
28
|
Cunningham SJ, Vergunst F, Macrae CN, Turk DJ. Exploring early self-referential memory effects through ownership. BRITISH JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2012; 31:289-301. [PMID: 23901843 DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/28/2010] [Accepted: 10/07/2012] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
The self-reference effect (SRE) is the reliable memory advantage for information encoded about self over material encoded about other people. The developmental pathway of the SRE has proved difficult to chart, because the standard SRE task is unsuitable for young children. The current inquiry was designed to address this issue using an ownership paradigm, as encoding objects in the context of self-ownership have been shown to elicit self-referential memory advantages in adults. Pairs of 4- to 6-year-old children (n = 64) sorted toy pictures into self- and other-owned sets. A surprise recognition memory test revealed a significant advantage for toys owned by self, which decreased with age. Neither verbal ability nor theory of mind attainment predicted the size of the memory advantage for self-owned items. This finding suggests that contrary to some previous reports, memory in early childhood can be shaped by the same self-referential biases that pervade adult cognition.
Collapse
|
29
|
Cunningham SJ, Brady-Van den Bos M, Turk DJ. Exploring the effects of ownership and choice on self-memory biases. Memory 2011; 19:449-61. [DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2011.584388] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/17/2022]
|
30
|
D'Argembeau A, Comblain C, Van der Linden M. Affective valence and the self-reference effect: Influence of retrieval conditions. Br J Psychol 2010; 96:457-66. [PMID: 16248936 DOI: 10.1348/000712605x53218] [Citation(s) in RCA: 64] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022]
Abstract
Positive trait information is typically better recalled than negative trait information when encoded in reference to the self, but not when encoded in reference to someone else or when processed for general meaning. This study examined whether this influence of affective meaning is modulated by retrieval conditions. Participants encoded positive and negative trait adjectives in reference to themselves or to a celebrity. They were then presented with either a free-recall task (Experiment 1) or a recognition memory task (Experiment 2). Positive adjectives were better recalled than negative adjectives, but only when they were encoded in reference to the self. In contrast, encoding condition and valence did not interact in the recognition memory task. Taken together, these findings suggest that the difference in memory between positive and negative self-referent information is due, at least in part, to a control exerted on memory retrieval.
Collapse
|
31
|
van den Bos M, Cunningham SJ, Conway MA, Turk DJ. Mine to remember: The impact of ownership on recollective experience. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2010; 63:1065-71. [DOI: 10.1080/17470211003770938] [Citation(s) in RCA: 57] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
Evaluating information with reference to self is associated with enhanced memory, the “self-reference effect”. The effect is found in recognition accompanied by recollective experience (remembering), but not in recognition based on a feeling of knowing. The current research employed an ownership procedure to investigate whether less evaluative forms of self-referential cognition produce similar enhancement of recollective experience. Participants were asked to sort items into baskets that belonged to themselves or a fictitious other. A subsequent remember–know recognition test showed that items encoded in the context of self-ownership were more likely to be correctly recognized than other-owned items. This ownership effect was found in remember, but not know, responses. This finding suggests that creating a self-referential encoding context leads to elaborative representations in episodic memory, even in the absence of explicit self-evaluation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Martin A. Conway
- Institute of Psychological Sciences, University of Leeds, Leeds, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
32
|
Ewbank MP, Barnard PJ, Croucher CJ, Ramponi C, Calder AJ. The amygdala response to images with impact. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2009; 4:127-33. [PMID: 19151376 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsn048] [Citation(s) in RCA: 69] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Effective photojournalism provokes an emotional reaction and leaves a lasting impression upon the viewer. Striking and memorable images are often said to possess 'impact'. Within cognitive neuroscience memorable emotional images evoke a greater amygdala response. Research to date has focused on arousal as a causative factor, while the contribution of appraisal dimensions relating to salience of an item, goal relevance, or impact are yet to be addressed. We explored how differences in ratings of impact influenced amygdala activity to negative emotional images matched for valence, arousal and other factors. Increased amygdala activation was found to high impact when compared to neutral images, or high impact when compared to low impact images (matched for arousal). Our findings demonstrate that the amygdala response to emotional stimuli is not a function of arousal (or valence) alone and accord more with the proposal that the amygdala responds to the significance or relevance of an event.
Collapse
|
33
|
Manzi A, Nigro G. Long-term memory for performed and observed actions: Retrieval awareness and source monitoring. Memory 2008; 16:595-603. [PMID: 18569687 DOI: 10.1080/09658210802070749] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
|
34
|
Gagnepain P, Lebreton K, Desgranges B, Eustache F. Perceptual priming enhances the creation of new episodic memories. Conscious Cogn 2008; 17:276-87. [PMID: 17468012 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2007.03.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/06/2006] [Revised: 03/15/2007] [Accepted: 03/17/2007] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
In recent years, most studies of human memory systems have placed the emphasis on differences rather than on similarities. The present study sought to assess the impact of perceptual priming on the creation of new episodic memories. It was composed of three distinct experimental phases: (1) an initial study phase, during which the number of repetitions of target words was manipulated; (2) a perceptual priming test phase, involving both target and new control words, which constituted the incidental encoding phase of (3) a subsequent Remember/Know/Guess recognition task. Results showed that the greater the perceptual priming at encoding, the more the episodic memory performance was enhanced, whereas no such relation was found for know judgments or feeling of familiarity. Furthermore, the words that were associated with the creation of new episodic memories had been perceptually primed to a greater extent during the incidental encoding phase than the words that were subsequently judged to be known or forgotten. These results suggest that the perceptual contents of episodic memories are constituted by the very perceptual representations that generate priming effects. Potential mechanisms linking perceptual priming to the creation of episodic memories are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- P Gagnepain
- Inserm, EPHE, Université de Caen/Basse-Normandie, Unité E0218, GIP Cyceron, CHU Côte de Nacre, 14033 Caen Cedex, France
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
35
|
Coluccia E, Bianco C, Brandimonte MA. Dissociating veridicality, consistency, and confidence in autobiographical and event memories for the Columbia shuttle disaster. Memory 2006; 14:452-70. [PMID: 16766448 DOI: 10.1080/09658210500477709] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
A total of 449 students were tested for their event memories and autobiographical memories of hearing about the Columbia shuttle disaster of 1st February 2003. Four different groups were tested 2, 18, 27, or 51 days after the event. All participants were then re-tested after 5 months (second session) and again after 1 year (third session) from the first interview. Dissociations between consistency and confidence and between event and autobiographical memories were found. Consistency and confidence in event memories, but not in autobiographical memories, were affected by time. In contrast, repeated testing selectively enhanced autobiographical memories, in accordance with the narrative and rehearsal hypothesis of Neisser and Harsh (1992). For event memories, veridicality was inversely correlated to consistency, which in turn was inversely correlated to confidence, and mainly based on omissions. As regards veridicality, the analyses showed an increase of false memories at long time delays. Results are discussed with reference to recent studies contrasting autobiographical and event memories.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emanuele Coluccia
- Laboratorio di Psicologia Sperimentale, Università degli Studi Suor Orsola Benincasa (USOB), Corso Vittorio Emanuele, Naples, Italy.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
36
|
Gardiner JM, Gregg VH, Karayianni I. Recognition memory and awareness: occurrence of perceptual effects in remembering or in knowing depends on conscious resources at encoding, but not at retrieval. Mem Cognit 2006; 34:227-39. [PMID: 16752587 DOI: 10.3758/bf03193401] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
We report four experiments in which a remember-know paradigm was combined with a response deadline procedure in order to assess memory awareness in fast, as compared with slow,recognition judgments. In the experiments, we also investigated the perceptual effects of study-test congruence, either for picture size or for speaker's voice, following either full or divided attention at study. These perceptual effects occurred in remembering with full attention and in knowing with divided attention, but they were uninfluenced by recognition speed, indicating that their occurrence in remembering or knowing depends more on conscious resources at encoding than on those at retrieval. The results have implications for theoretical accounts of remembering and knowing that assume that remembering is more consciously controlled and effortful, whereas knowing is more automatic and faster.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- John M Gardiner
- Psychology Department, School of Life Sciences, University of Sussex, Brighton, England.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
37
|
Abstract
We describe an experiment that investigated levels-of-processing effects in recognition memory for famous faces. The degree of conscious control over the recognition decisions was manipulated by using a response deadline procedure, and memory awareness associated with those decisions was assessed using a standard remember-know paradigm. Levels-of-processing effects were found at both short and long response deadlines, and at both deadlines those effects occurred only in remembering. Moreover, knowing, as well as remembering, increased with the greater opportunity for conscious control over recognition decisions at the long deadline. These results have implications for dual-process theories that distinguish between a slower, more controlled recollection process, reflected in remembering, and a faster, more automatic familiarity process, reflected in knowing.
Collapse
|
38
|
Abstract
Abstract. Four experiments were conducted investigating the effect of size congruency on facial recognition memory, measured by remember, know and guess responses. Different study times were employed, that is extremely short (300 and 700 ms), short (1,000 ms), and long times (5,000 ms). With the short study time (1,000 ms) size congruency occurred in knowing. With the long study time the effect of size congruency occurred in remembering. These results support the distinctiveness/fluency account of remembering and knowing as well as the memory systems account, since the size congruency effect that occurred in knowing under conditions that facilitated perceptual fluency also occurred independently in remembering under conditions that facilitated elaborative encoding. They do not support the idea that remember and know responses reflect differences in trace strength.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chrisanthi Nega
- Psychology Department, The American College of Greece, Aghia Paraskevi, Greece.
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
Abstract
This article critically examines the view that the signal detection theory (SDT) interpretation of the remember-know (RK) paradigm has been ruled out by the evidence. The author evaluates 5 empirical arguments against a database of 72 studies reporting RK data under 400 different conditions. These arguments concern (a). the functional independence of remember and know rates, (b). the invariance of estimates of sensitivity, (c). the relationship between remember rates and overall hit and false alarm rates, (d). the relationship between RK responses and confidence judgments, and (e). dissociations between remember and overall hit rates. Each of these arguments is shown to be flawed, and despite being open to refutation, the SDT interpretation is consistent with existing data from both the RK and remember-know-guess paradigms and offers a basis for further theoretical development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- John C Dunn
- School of Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, University of Western Australia, Nedlands, WAU, Australia.
| |
Collapse
|
40
|
Karayianni I, Gardiner JM. Transferring voice effects in recognition memory from remembering to knowing. Mem Cognit 2003; 31:1052-9. [PMID: 14704020 DOI: 10.3758/bf03196126] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
In five experiments, we investigated the effects of voice congruency (same vs. different voices at study and at test) on remembering and knowing in recognition memory. With low- and medium-frequency three- or four-syllable words, a voice congruency effect occurred only in remembering. With nonwords, voice congruency effects occurred both in remembering and in knowing. With nonwords and divided attention at study, the voice congruency effect transferred almost completely from remembering to knowing. By showing a transfer of effects from remembering to knowing as encoding became more impoverished, these findings support a distinctiveness/fluency account of remembering and knowing as well as the theory that remembering and knowing indicate retrieval of events from episodic and semantic memory systems, respectively.
Collapse
|
41
|
Algarabel S, Gotor A, Pitarque A. Remember, know, confidence and the mirror effect: Changes as a function of discriminability conditions. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2003. [DOI: 10.1080/09541440340000033] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
|
42
|
Hirshman E, Lanning K, Master S, Henzler A. Signal-detection models as tools for interpreting judgements of recollections. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2002. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.758] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
|