1
|
Medrano MC, Darlot F, Cador M, Caille S. Poor inhibitory control predicts sex-specific vulnerability to nicotine rewarding properties in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2023; 240:1973-1986. [PMID: 37439799 DOI: 10.1007/s00213-023-06418-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/12/2023] [Accepted: 07/02/2023] [Indexed: 07/14/2023]
Abstract
RATIONALE The risk of becoming addicted to tobacco varies greatly from individual to individual, raising the possibility of behavioural biomarkers capable of predicting sensitivity to nicotine reward, a crucial step in the development of nicotine addiction. Amongst all of nicotine's pharmacological properties, one of central importance is the enhancement of cognitive performances, which depend on the balance between attentional processes and inhibitory control. However, whether the cognitive enhancement effects of nicotine are predictive of sensitivity to its rewarding properties is still unknown. OBJECTIVE Using male and female mice, we investigated whether the effects of nicotine on cognitive performances are predictive of sensitivity to the rewarding properties of nicotine and, if so, whether this relationship is sex dependent. METHODS Naïve male and female mice were first assessed for their performances in both baseline conditions and following nicotine injection (0.15 and 0.30 mg/kg) in a cued-Fixed Consecutive Number task (FCNcue) measuring both optimal (attention) and premature (inhibitory control) responding. Next, all mice underwent nicotine-induced conditioned place preference (CPP) in order to evaluate inter-individual differences in response to nicotine reward (0.30 mg/kg). RESULTS Results showed that males and females benefited from the effect of nicotine as a cognitive enhancer in the FCNcue task. However, only those males displaying poor inhibitory control, namely high-impulsive animals, subsequently displayed sensitivity to nicotine reward. In females, sensitivity to nicotine reward was independent of FCNcue performances, in both basal and nicotine conditions. CONCLUSION Thus, our study suggests that poor inhibitory control and its modulation by nicotine may be a behavioural biomarker for sensitivity to nicotine reward and consequent vulnerability to nicotine addiction in males but not females.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Florence Darlot
- Univ. Bordeaux, CNRS, INCIA, UMR 5287, F-33000, Bordeaux, France
| | - Martine Cador
- Univ. Bordeaux, CNRS, INCIA, UMR 5287, F-33000, Bordeaux, France
| | - Stephanie Caille
- Univ. Bordeaux, CNRS, INCIA, UMR 5287, F-33000, Bordeaux, France.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Gupta TA, Sanabria F. Motivated to time: Effects of reinforcer devaluation and opportunity cost on interval timing. Learn Behav 2023; 51:308-320. [PMID: 36781823 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-023-00572-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 01/22/2023] [Indexed: 02/15/2023]
Abstract
Prior research suggests that interval timing performance is sensitive to reinforcer devaluation effects and to the rate of competing sources of reinforcement. The present study sought to replicate and account for these findings in rats. A self-paced concurrent fixed-interval (FI) random-ratio (RR) schedule of reinforcement was implemented in which the FI requirement varied across training conditions (12, 24, 48 s). The RR requirement-which imposed an opportunity cost to responding on the FI component-was adjusted so that it took about twice the FI requirement, on average, to complete it. Probe reinforcer devaluation (prefeeding) sessions were conducted at the end of each condition. To assess the effect of contextual reinforcement on timing performance, the RR requirement was removed before the end of the experiment. Consistent with prior findings, performance on the FI component tracked schedule requirement and displayed scalar invariance; the removal of the RR component yielded more premature FI responses. For some rats, prefeeding reduced the number of trials initiated without affecting timing performance; for other rats, prefeeding delayed responding on the FI component but had a weaker effect on trial initiation. These results support the notion that timing and motivational processes are separable, suggesting novel explanations for ostensible motivational effects on timing performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tanya A Gupta
- Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, USA.
- Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
3
|
Li Y, Wang MY, Xu M, Xie WT, Zhang YM, Yang XY, Wang ZX, Song R, Yang L, Ma JP, Zhang J, Han CX, Wang CZ, Liu WY, Gan WH, Su R, Ma HL, Li H. High-Altitude Exposure and Time Interval Perception of Chinese Migrants in Tibet. Brain Sci 2022; 12:585. [PMID: 35624972 PMCID: PMC9139562 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci12050585] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/14/2022] [Revised: 04/26/2022] [Accepted: 04/27/2022] [Indexed: 02/06/2023] Open
Abstract
High-altitude exposure can negatively impact one’s ability to accurately perceive time. This study focuses on Chinese migrants who have traveled to the Tibetan plateau and explores the effects of high-altitude exposure on their time interval judgment abilities based on three separate studies. In Study 1, it was found that exposure to high altitudes negatively impacted the time interval judgment functions of the migrants compared with a low-altitude control group; they exhibited a prolonged response time (540 ms: p = 0.006, 95% CI (−1.70 −0.32)) and reduced accuracy (1080 ms: p = 0.032, 95% CI (0.06 1.26)) in certain behavioral tasks. In Study 2, the results showed that high-altitude exposure and sleepiness had an interactive effect on time interval judgment (1080 ms) (p < 0.05, 95% CI (−0.83 −0.40)). To further verify our interaction hypothesis, in Study 3, we investigated the time interval judgment of interactions between acute high-altitude exposure and sleepiness level. The results revealed that the adaptation effect disappeared and sleepiness significantly exacerbated the negative effects of high-altitude exposure on time interval judgment (p < 0.001, 95% CI (−0.85 −0.34)). This study is the first to examine the effects of high-altitude exposure on time interval judgment processing functions and the effects of sleep-related factors on individual time interval judgment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yuan Li
- Plateau Brain Science Research Center, Tibet University/South China Normal University, Lhasa 850012, China; (Y.L.); (M.-Y.W.); (M.X.); (W.-T.X.); (Y.-M.Z.); (X.-Y.Y.); (Z.-X.W.); (L.Y.); (J.-P.M.); (J.Z.); (C.-X.H.); (C.-Z.W.); (W.-Y.L.); (W.-H.G.); (R.S.); (H.-L.M.)
| | - Mei-Yi Wang
- Plateau Brain Science Research Center, Tibet University/South China Normal University, Lhasa 850012, China; (Y.L.); (M.-Y.W.); (M.X.); (W.-T.X.); (Y.-M.Z.); (X.-Y.Y.); (Z.-X.W.); (L.Y.); (J.-P.M.); (J.Z.); (C.-X.H.); (C.-Z.W.); (W.-Y.L.); (W.-H.G.); (R.S.); (H.-L.M.)
| | - Meng Xu
- Plateau Brain Science Research Center, Tibet University/South China Normal University, Lhasa 850012, China; (Y.L.); (M.-Y.W.); (M.X.); (W.-T.X.); (Y.-M.Z.); (X.-Y.Y.); (Z.-X.W.); (L.Y.); (J.-P.M.); (J.Z.); (C.-X.H.); (C.-Z.W.); (W.-Y.L.); (W.-H.G.); (R.S.); (H.-L.M.)
| | - Wen-Ting Xie
- Plateau Brain Science Research Center, Tibet University/South China Normal University, Lhasa 850012, China; (Y.L.); (M.-Y.W.); (M.X.); (W.-T.X.); (Y.-M.Z.); (X.-Y.Y.); (Z.-X.W.); (L.Y.); (J.-P.M.); (J.Z.); (C.-X.H.); (C.-Z.W.); (W.-Y.L.); (W.-H.G.); (R.S.); (H.-L.M.)
| | - Yu-Ming Zhang
- Plateau Brain Science Research Center, Tibet University/South China Normal University, Lhasa 850012, China; (Y.L.); (M.-Y.W.); (M.X.); (W.-T.X.); (Y.-M.Z.); (X.-Y.Y.); (Z.-X.W.); (L.Y.); (J.-P.M.); (J.Z.); (C.-X.H.); (C.-Z.W.); (W.-Y.L.); (W.-H.G.); (R.S.); (H.-L.M.)
| | - Xi-Yue Yang
- Plateau Brain Science Research Center, Tibet University/South China Normal University, Lhasa 850012, China; (Y.L.); (M.-Y.W.); (M.X.); (W.-T.X.); (Y.-M.Z.); (X.-Y.Y.); (Z.-X.W.); (L.Y.); (J.-P.M.); (J.Z.); (C.-X.H.); (C.-Z.W.); (W.-Y.L.); (W.-H.G.); (R.S.); (H.-L.M.)
| | - Zhi-Xin Wang
- Plateau Brain Science Research Center, Tibet University/South China Normal University, Lhasa 850012, China; (Y.L.); (M.-Y.W.); (M.X.); (W.-T.X.); (Y.-M.Z.); (X.-Y.Y.); (Z.-X.W.); (L.Y.); (J.-P.M.); (J.Z.); (C.-X.H.); (C.-Z.W.); (W.-Y.L.); (W.-H.G.); (R.S.); (H.-L.M.)
| | - Rui Song
- Center on Aging Psychology, CAS Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Beijing 100101, China;
| | - Liu Yang
- Plateau Brain Science Research Center, Tibet University/South China Normal University, Lhasa 850012, China; (Y.L.); (M.-Y.W.); (M.X.); (W.-T.X.); (Y.-M.Z.); (X.-Y.Y.); (Z.-X.W.); (L.Y.); (J.-P.M.); (J.Z.); (C.-X.H.); (C.-Z.W.); (W.-Y.L.); (W.-H.G.); (R.S.); (H.-L.M.)
| | - Jin-Ping Ma
- Plateau Brain Science Research Center, Tibet University/South China Normal University, Lhasa 850012, China; (Y.L.); (M.-Y.W.); (M.X.); (W.-T.X.); (Y.-M.Z.); (X.-Y.Y.); (Z.-X.W.); (L.Y.); (J.-P.M.); (J.Z.); (C.-X.H.); (C.-Z.W.); (W.-Y.L.); (W.-H.G.); (R.S.); (H.-L.M.)
| | - Jia Zhang
- Plateau Brain Science Research Center, Tibet University/South China Normal University, Lhasa 850012, China; (Y.L.); (M.-Y.W.); (M.X.); (W.-T.X.); (Y.-M.Z.); (X.-Y.Y.); (Z.-X.W.); (L.Y.); (J.-P.M.); (J.Z.); (C.-X.H.); (C.-Z.W.); (W.-Y.L.); (W.-H.G.); (R.S.); (H.-L.M.)
| | - Chen-Xiao Han
- Plateau Brain Science Research Center, Tibet University/South China Normal University, Lhasa 850012, China; (Y.L.); (M.-Y.W.); (M.X.); (W.-T.X.); (Y.-M.Z.); (X.-Y.Y.); (Z.-X.W.); (L.Y.); (J.-P.M.); (J.Z.); (C.-X.H.); (C.-Z.W.); (W.-Y.L.); (W.-H.G.); (R.S.); (H.-L.M.)
| | - Cheng-Zhi Wang
- Plateau Brain Science Research Center, Tibet University/South China Normal University, Lhasa 850012, China; (Y.L.); (M.-Y.W.); (M.X.); (W.-T.X.); (Y.-M.Z.); (X.-Y.Y.); (Z.-X.W.); (L.Y.); (J.-P.M.); (J.Z.); (C.-X.H.); (C.-Z.W.); (W.-Y.L.); (W.-H.G.); (R.S.); (H.-L.M.)
| | - Wan-Ying Liu
- Plateau Brain Science Research Center, Tibet University/South China Normal University, Lhasa 850012, China; (Y.L.); (M.-Y.W.); (M.X.); (W.-T.X.); (Y.-M.Z.); (X.-Y.Y.); (Z.-X.W.); (L.Y.); (J.-P.M.); (J.Z.); (C.-X.H.); (C.-Z.W.); (W.-Y.L.); (W.-H.G.); (R.S.); (H.-L.M.)
| | - Wan-Hong Gan
- Plateau Brain Science Research Center, Tibet University/South China Normal University, Lhasa 850012, China; (Y.L.); (M.-Y.W.); (M.X.); (W.-T.X.); (Y.-M.Z.); (X.-Y.Y.); (Z.-X.W.); (L.Y.); (J.-P.M.); (J.Z.); (C.-X.H.); (C.-Z.W.); (W.-Y.L.); (W.-H.G.); (R.S.); (H.-L.M.)
| | - Rui Su
- Plateau Brain Science Research Center, Tibet University/South China Normal University, Lhasa 850012, China; (Y.L.); (M.-Y.W.); (M.X.); (W.-T.X.); (Y.-M.Z.); (X.-Y.Y.); (Z.-X.W.); (L.Y.); (J.-P.M.); (J.Z.); (C.-X.H.); (C.-Z.W.); (W.-Y.L.); (W.-H.G.); (R.S.); (H.-L.M.)
- Institute of Oxygen Supply, Tibet University, Lhasa 850012, China
| | - Hai-Lin Ma
- Plateau Brain Science Research Center, Tibet University/South China Normal University, Lhasa 850012, China; (Y.L.); (M.-Y.W.); (M.X.); (W.-T.X.); (Y.-M.Z.); (X.-Y.Y.); (Z.-X.W.); (L.Y.); (J.-P.M.); (J.Z.); (C.-X.H.); (C.-Z.W.); (W.-Y.L.); (W.-H.G.); (R.S.); (H.-L.M.)
- Institute of Oxygen Supply, Tibet University, Lhasa 850012, China
| | - Hao Li
- Plateau Brain Science Research Center, Tibet University/South China Normal University, Lhasa 850012, China; (Y.L.); (M.-Y.W.); (M.X.); (W.-T.X.); (Y.-M.Z.); (X.-Y.Y.); (Z.-X.W.); (L.Y.); (J.-P.M.); (J.Z.); (C.-X.H.); (C.-Z.W.); (W.-Y.L.); (W.-H.G.); (R.S.); (H.-L.M.)
- Institute of Oxygen Supply, Tibet University, Lhasa 850012, China
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
McNealy KR, Ramsay ME, Barrett ST, Bevins RA. Reward-enhancing effects of d-amphetamine and its interactions with nicotine were greater in female rats and persisted across schedules of reinforcement. Behav Pharmacol 2021; 32:435-447. [PMID: 34010168 PMCID: PMC8266728 DOI: 10.1097/fbp.0000000000000637] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/08/2023]
Abstract
Nicotine enhances the value of environmental stimuli and rewards, and reward enhancement can maintain nicotine consumption. Stimulants such as d-amphetamine are misused more by women and are commonly co-used with nicotine. d-Amphetamine potentiates nicotine's effects in human and animal research. To date, there are no published studies examining this interaction in a reward-enhancement task. The current study sought to investigate the reward-enhancing effects of nicotine alongside and coadministered with d-amphetamine. Further, we evaluated the persistence of reward enhancement across ratio and temporal schedules of reinforcement. We used 10 male and 10 female Sprague-Dawley rats. Enhancement was assessed within subjects by examining active lever pressing for a visual stimulus reinforcer on variable ratio 3, variable interval 30 s and variable time 30 s - variable ratio 3 schedules. Before 1-h sessions, rats received one injection of saline, 0.1 or 0.3 mg/kg d-amphetamine and one of saline or 0.4 mg/kg nicotine, making six possible drug combinations (saline + saline, saline + nicotine, 0.1 d-amphetamine + aline, 0.1 d-amphetamine + nicotine, 0.3 d-amphetamine + saline and 0.3 d-amphetamine + nicotine) experienced in a randomized order by each rat. When d-amphetamine was coadministered with nicotine, we found an interaction effect on reward enhancement that persisted across schedules of reinforcement. Males and females exhibited reward enhancement by 0.3 d-amphetamine, while only females showed reward enhancement by 0.1 d-amphetamine. Further, females responded more for the visual stimulus than males in all d-amphetamine conditions. Future studies should assess how reward enhancement is involved in high nicotine-amphetamine comorbidity rates and enhanced amphetamine misuse in women.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kathleen R McNealy
- Department of Psychology, University of Nebraska-Lincoln, Lincoln, Nebraska, USA
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Response-inhibition capacity in spontaneously hypertensive and Wistar rats: acquisition of fixed minimum interval performance and responsiveness to D-amphetamine. Behav Pharmacol 2019; 29:668-675. [PMID: 29877871 DOI: 10.1097/fbp.0000000000000411] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/03/2023]
Abstract
Reduced response-inhibition capacity is a defining feature of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. The fixed minimum interval (FMI) schedule has been systematically validated to assess such capacity in rats. On each FMI trial, the first lever press initiates an inter-response time (IRT); a potentially consummatory response terminates the IRT; only IRTs longer than a target interval result in access to food. Despite task validity, steady-state FMI performance in the most common animal model of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, the spontaneously hypertensive rat (SHR), is similar to normotensive control performance, even though SHR performs at lower levels, especially during acquisition, in similar response-withholding tasks. To determine whether such limitations of the model are specific to stable-state performance, this experiment compared FMI 6-s performance in SHR and Wistar rats during acquisition and in steady state, and assessed the effect of acute D-amphetamine (AMP) administration (0.1, 0.5, and 1.0 mg/kg) on steady-state performance. Median latencies to first lever press were consistently shorter in SHR than in Wistar rats; IRTs were shorter for SHR than for Wistar rats during acquisition, but substantially less so during asymptotic performance. AMP dose-dependently reduced latencies, shortened IRTs, and, at the highest dose, increased the proportion of IRTs under schedule control. These results suggest that, relative to Wistar rats, SHR have a reduced capacity to learn to withhold a reinforced response; once the FMI is acquired, high doses of D-AMP disrupt withholding performance in both strains, but they also enhance the responsiveness of both strains to reinforcement contingencies.
Collapse
|
6
|
Daniels CW, Overby PF, Sanabria F. Between-session memory degradation accounts for within-session changes in fixed-interval performance. Behav Processes 2018; 153:31-39. [PMID: 29729953 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2018.05.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/12/2018] [Revised: 04/15/2018] [Accepted: 05/02/2018] [Indexed: 01/02/2023]
Abstract
A common assumption in the study of fixed-interval (FI) timing is that FI performance is largely stable within sessions, once it is stable between sessions. Within-session changes in FI performance were examined in published data (Daniels and Sanabria, 2017), wherein some rats were trained on a FI 30-s schedule of food reinforcement (FI30) and others on a FI 90-s schedule (FI90). Following stability, FI90 rats were pre-fed for five sessions. Response rates declined as a function of trial, due more to latency lengthening than to run-rate reduction. Latencies were best described by a dynamic gamma-exponential mixture distribution, in which latency lengthening was driven by the growth of the criterion pulse count for a response and not by a reduction in the speed of an endogenous clock. The speed of the clock was selectively sensitive to the length of the FI; the prevalence and length of exponentially-distributed latencies were selectively sensitive to pre-feeding. These findings reveal (a) that parameters governing FI latencies are selectively sensitive to a range of manipulations, (b) a potential degradation of the criterion pulse count between consecutive sessions, and (c) a subsequent recovery of the criterion pulse count within sessions.
Collapse
|
7
|
Interval timing under a behavioral microscope: Dissociating motivational and timing processes in fixed-interval performance. Learn Behav 2018; 45:29-48. [PMID: 27443193 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-016-0234-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The distribution of latencies and interresponse times (IRTs) of rats was compared between two fixed-interval (FI) schedules of food reinforcement (FI 30 s and FI 90 s), and between two levels of food deprivation. Computational modeling revealed that latencies and IRTs were well described by mixture probability distributions embodying two-state Markov chains. Analysis of these models revealed that only a subset of latencies is sensitive to the periodicity of reinforcement, and prefeeding only reduces the size of this subset. The distribution of IRTs suggests that behavior in FI schedules is organized in bouts that lengthen and ramp up in frequency with proximity to reinforcement. Prefeeding slowed down the lengthening of bouts and increased the time between bouts. When concatenated, latency and IRT models adequately reproduced sigmoidal FI response functions. These findings suggest that behavior in FI schedules fluctuates in and out of schedule control; an account of such fluctuation suggests that timing and motivation are dissociable components of FI performance. These mixture-distribution models also provide novel insights on the motivational, associative, and timing processes expressed in FI performance. These processes may be obscured, however, when performance in timing tasks is analyzed in terms of mean response rates.
Collapse
|
8
|
Overby PF, Daniels CW, Del Franco A, Goenaga J, Powell GL, Gipson CD, Sanabria F. Effects of nicotine self-administration on incentive salience in male Sprague Dawley rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2018; 235:1121-1130. [PMID: 29374305 DOI: 10.1007/s00213-018-4829-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/10/2017] [Accepted: 01/03/2018] [Indexed: 12/26/2022]
Abstract
RATIONALE Prolonged use of nicotine appears to enhance incentive salience, a motivational-cognitive process that transforms an otherwise neutral stimulus into a "wanted" stimulus. It has been suggested that nicotinic enhancement of incentive salience contributes to the potential of relapse in individuals with tobacco addiction. However, there are two main limitations of prior research that caution this claim: (a) the use of passive experimentally delivered nicotine and (b) the use of sign-tracking as an index of incentive salience, without acknowledging the competing nature of goal- and sign-tracking responses. OBJECTIVES To determine whether nicotinic enhancement of incentive salience attributed to non-nicotinic stimuli occurs when rats self-administer nicotine, and whether it is facilitated by a prior history of nicotine self-administration. METHODS Twenty-three male rats were trained daily, for 24 days, on a nicotine self-administration (SA) paradigm in the morning, and on a four-conditioned-stimuli Pavlovian conditioned approach (4-CS PCA) task in the afternoon. Self-administration was followed by extinction and cue reinstatement. A subcutaneous nicotine challenge was performed during the last 7 days of the study. RESULTS Nicotine self-administration selectively enhanced sign-tracking in the 4-CS PCA. Upon extinction, sign-tracking quickly declined to control levels. Experimenter-administered nicotine enhanced sign-tracking similarly regardless of nicotine history. CONCLUSIONS The results suggest that nicotinic enhancement of incentive salience is transient, and a previous history of nicotine use does not cause further sensitization. Taken together, these results suggest that nicotine enhances incentive salience, particularly-and perhaps exclusively-while onboard.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paula F Overby
- Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, P.O. Box 871104, Tempe, AZ, 85287-1104, USA
| | - Carter W Daniels
- Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, P.O. Box 871104, Tempe, AZ, 85287-1104, USA
| | - Armani Del Franco
- School of Life Sciences, Arizona State University, P.O. Box 871104, Tempe, AZ, 85287-1104, USA
| | - Julianna Goenaga
- Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, P.O. Box 871104, Tempe, AZ, 85287-1104, USA
| | - Gregory L Powell
- Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, P.O. Box 871104, Tempe, AZ, 85287-1104, USA
- School of Life Sciences, Arizona State University, P.O. Box 871104, Tempe, AZ, 85287-1104, USA
| | - Cassandra D Gipson
- Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, P.O. Box 871104, Tempe, AZ, 85287-1104, USA
| | - Federico Sanabria
- Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, P.O. Box 871104, Tempe, AZ, 85287-1104, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Romero K, Daniels CW, Gipson CD, Sanabria F. Suppressive and enhancing effects of nicotine on food-seeking behavior. Behav Brain Res 2018; 339:130-139. [PMID: 29175447 PMCID: PMC5736011 DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.11.023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/25/2017] [Revised: 11/10/2017] [Accepted: 11/17/2017] [Indexed: 12/14/2022]
Abstract
The present study examined how systemic low doses of nicotine affect the microstructure of reinforced food-seeking behavior in rats. Rats were first given an acute saline or nicotine treatment (0.1-0.6mg/kg, with an inter-injection interval of at least 48h), and then a chronic saline or nicotine treatment (0.3mg/kg/day for 10 consecutive days). Immediately after each injection, rats were required to press a lever five times to obtain food that was available at unpredictable times (on average every 80s) with constant probability. Acute nicotine dose-dependently suppressed behavior prior to the delivery of the first reinforcer, but enhanced food-reinforced behavior afterwards. These effects were primarily observed in the time it took rats to initiate food-seeking behavior. Enhancing effects were also observed in the microstructure of food-seeking behavior, with lower nicotine doses (0.1-0.3mg/kg) increasing the rate at which response bouts were initiated, and higher doses (0.3-0.6mg/kg) increasing within-bout response rates. A pre-feeding control suggests that changes in appetite alone cannot explain these effects. Over the course of chronic nicotine exposure, tolerance developed to the suppressive, but not to the enhancing effects of nicotine on food-seeking behavior. These results suggest that (a) lower doses of nicotine enhance the reward value of food and/or food-associated stimuli, (b) higher doses of nicotine enhance motoric activity, and (c) ostensive sensitization effects of nicotine on behavior partially reflect a tolerance to its transient suppressive motoric effects.
Collapse
|
10
|
Laude JR, Daniels CW, Wade JC, Zentall TR. I can time with a little help from my friends: effect of social enrichment on timing processes in Pigeons (Columba livia). Anim Cogn 2016; 19:1205-1213. [PMID: 27632157 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-016-1032-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/25/2016] [Revised: 08/05/2016] [Accepted: 08/28/2016] [Indexed: 12/14/2022]
Abstract
There is evidence that impulsive decision-making is associated with errors in timing. However, there has been little attempt to identify the putative mechanism responsible for impulsive animals' timing errors. One means of manipulating impulsivity in non-human animals is providing different levels of access to conspecifics. These preclinical models have revealed that social isolation increases impulsive responding across a wide range of tasks. The goal of the present study was to determine whether social isolation modulates time perception in pigeons by inducing more variability or a bias to underestimate the passage of time in temporal judgments. A temporal bisection task was used to characterize time perception. One group of pigeons performed the bisection following social enrichment, and the remaining half of the pigeons were tested following social isolation. Results revealed pigeons in the social isolation condition categorized a temporal stimulus sample as "long" at shorter durations than pigeons in the social enrichment condition. These data highlight the mechanism(s) thought to underlie timing-based interventions aimed at reducing impulsivity in humans. Future work should consider whether impulsivity is produced by misperceptions of time or a reduced threshold for a response.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jennifer R Laude
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA.,Department of Medicine, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA, USA
| | - Carter W Daniels
- Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, USA
| | - Jordan C Wade
- Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY, 40506-0044, USA
| | - Thomas R Zentall
- Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY, 40506-0044, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
|