1
|
Semeraro C, Musso P, Cassibba R, Annese S, Scurani A, Lucangeli D, Taurino A, Coppola G. Relation between fluid intelligence and mathematics and reading comprehension achievements: The moderating role of student teacher relationships and school bonding. PLoS One 2023; 18:e0290677. [PMID: 37768944 PMCID: PMC10538704 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0290677] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/14/2023] [Accepted: 08/13/2023] [Indexed: 09/30/2023] Open
Abstract
Several studies have shown the relevance among students of the quality of their interpersonal relationships for their academic achievement. Nevertheless, most studies available have explored the relation between the cognitive functioning and academic achievement without taking into account the quality of the relationships experienced in the school environment. Furthermore, the studies that have begun to consider the joint role of these factors in the prediction of academic achievement are scant. Therefore, it appears of relevance to deepen the relation between cognitive functioning and quality of school relationships in order to support students' academic achievement and the potential of youth. In this paper, we examined the moderating role of the quality of student-teacher relationships and school bonding (STR-SB) in the associations of fluid intelligence (Gf) with academic achievement among adolescents (N = 219). A multiple-group structural equation modelling analysis revealed that STR-SB quality moderated unexpectedly only the link between Gf and mathematics. The findings support the idea that the quality of student-teacher relationships may be a relevant dimension to be considered to clarify the association between cognitive functioning and academic achievement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cristina Semeraro
- Department of Education, Psychology, Communication, University of Bari Aldo Moro, Bari, Italy
| | - Pasquale Musso
- Department of Education, Psychology, Communication, University of Bari Aldo Moro, Bari, Italy
| | - Rosalinda Cassibba
- Department of Education, Psychology, Communication, University of Bari Aldo Moro, Bari, Italy
| | - Susanna Annese
- Department of Education, Psychology, Communication, University of Bari Aldo Moro, Bari, Italy
| | | | - Daniela Lucangeli
- Department of Developmental Psychology and Socialization, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
| | - Alessandro Taurino
- Department of Education, Psychology, Communication, University of Bari Aldo Moro, Bari, Italy
| | - Gabrielle Coppola
- Department of Education, Psychology, Communication, University of Bari Aldo Moro, Bari, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Lisá E, Sokolová L, Jablonická P, Kardelisová L. Motivation to succeed is not enough: motivated students need to know how to plan/organize their steps on their way to success. Front Psychol 2023; 14:1119409. [PMID: 37384170 PMCID: PMC10293740 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1119409] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/09/2022] [Accepted: 05/25/2023] [Indexed: 06/30/2023] Open
Abstract
The study is based on dispositional (career motivation) and social-cognitive (generalized self-efficacy) theories of personality, further on the expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation and future time perspective theory (task value, time, and study environment). The study aimed to explain the mechanism of the prediction relationship between motivation and students' performance. It was assumed that skills of planning and organizing (operationalized as generalized self-efficacy and learning strategies) mediate the prediction of motivation (career motivation and task value) on students' success (operationalized as academic achievement and employability). In two studies (N = 313, N = 219), the hypotheses of the mediation models were supported by structural equation modeling. Generally, the skills of organizing/planning fully mediated the students' performance, measured as academic achievement and employability (number of employers). The results show the importance of combining dispositional motivation characteristics with dynamic planning skills on the way to students' success. Traditional psychological predictors of performance, like general mental ability and conscientiousness, were not controlled. Higher education institutions could support motivated students on their way to success by teaching them how to plan and organize specific steps on their way to success.
Collapse
|
3
|
Covey TJ, Shucard JL, Wang X, Gregory MA, Shucard DW. Cognitive skill learning in multiple sclerosis: A meaningful component of the neuropsychological profile. Brain Cogn 2023; 166:105959. [PMID: 36842286 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2023.105959] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/15/2022] [Revised: 01/31/2023] [Accepted: 02/02/2023] [Indexed: 02/27/2023]
Abstract
Cognitive skill learning (CSL) refers to the capacity to improve performance on specific cognitive operations through repeated practice. We hypothesized that high CSL aptitude may promote accumulation of cognitive reserve, and resiliency to cognitive decline, in people with Multiple Sclerosis (MS). Using an adaptive working memory training paradigm, we obtained CSL aptitude indices (amount of improvement on the training task over time) in MS patients for a single session of practice (25-30 min), and longer-term practice (twenty sessions). Neuropsychological performance was assessed with the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT), Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT), and the Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices (RAPM). CSL aptitude measures were positively correlated with neuropsychological performance, and had high diagnostic accuracy for classifying cognitive impairment in MS, defined as 1.5 SD below the demographics-corrected normative mean of the SDMT. Positive relationships between CSL aptitude measures and neuropsychological performance tended to be more pronounced for individuals with high estimated cognitive reserve, suggesting that high CSL aptitude is a a factor that promotes the protective effects of cognitive reserve. Furthermore, regression analyses indicated that CSL aptitude is separable from baseline cognitive capacity. The findings suggest that CSL aptitude impacts the neuropsychological profile in MS, and may be a factor underlying variance in cognitive resiliency.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas J Covey
- Division of Cognitive and Behavioral Neurosciences, Department of Neurology, Jacobs School of Medicine and Biomedical Sciences, University at Buffalo, United States; Neuroscience Program, Jacobs School of Medicine and Biomedical Sciences, University at Buffalo, United States.
| | - Janet L Shucard
- Division of Cognitive and Behavioral Neurosciences, Department of Neurology, Jacobs School of Medicine and Biomedical Sciences, University at Buffalo, United States; Neuroscience Program, Jacobs School of Medicine and Biomedical Sciences, University at Buffalo, United States
| | - Xuedi Wang
- Division of Cognitive and Behavioral Neurosciences, Department of Neurology, Jacobs School of Medicine and Biomedical Sciences, University at Buffalo, United States; Neuroscience Program, Jacobs School of Medicine and Biomedical Sciences, University at Buffalo, United States
| | - Madeline A Gregory
- Division of Cognitive and Behavioral Neurosciences, Department of Neurology, Jacobs School of Medicine and Biomedical Sciences, University at Buffalo, United States; Neuroscience Program, Jacobs School of Medicine and Biomedical Sciences, University at Buffalo, United States
| | - David W Shucard
- Division of Cognitive and Behavioral Neurosciences, Department of Neurology, Jacobs School of Medicine and Biomedical Sciences, University at Buffalo, United States; Neuroscience Program, Jacobs School of Medicine and Biomedical Sciences, University at Buffalo, United States
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Hanik M, Demirtaş MA, Gharsallaoui MA, Rekik I. Predicting cognitive scores with graph neural networks through sample selection learning. Brain Imaging Behav 2022; 16:1123-1138. [PMID: 34757563 PMCID: PMC9107424 DOI: 10.1007/s11682-021-00585-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 10/06/2021] [Indexed: 10/29/2022]
Abstract
Analyzing the relation between intelligence and neural activity is of the utmost importance in understanding the working principles of the human brain in health and disease. In existing literature, functional brain connectomes have been used successfully to predict cognitive measures such as intelligence quotient (IQ) scores in both healthy and disordered cohorts using machine learning models. However, existing methods resort to flattening the brain connectome (i.e., graph) through vectorization which overlooks its topological properties. To address this limitation and inspired from the emerging graph neural networks (GNNs), we design a novel regression GNN model (namely RegGNN) for predicting IQ scores from brain connectivity. On top of that, we introduce a novel, fully modular sample selection method to select the best samples to learn from for our target prediction task. However, since such deep learning architectures are computationally expensive to train, we further propose a learning-based sample selection method that learns how to choose the training samples with the highest expected predictive power on unseen samples. For this, we capitalize on the fact that connectomes (i.e., their adjacency matrices) lie in the symmetric positive definite (SPD) matrix cone. Our results on full-scale and verbal IQ prediction outperforms comparison methods in autism spectrum disorder cohorts and achieves a competitive performance for neurotypical subjects using 3-fold cross-validation. Furthermore, we show that our sample selection approach generalizes to other learning-based methods, which shows its usefulness beyond our GNN architecture.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Mehmet Arif Demirtaş
- BASIRA Lab, Faculty of Computer and Informatics, Istanbul Technical University, Istanbul, Turkey
| | - Mohammed Amine Gharsallaoui
- BASIRA Lab, Faculty of Computer and Informatics, Istanbul Technical University, Istanbul, Turkey
- Ecole Polytechnique de Tunisie (EPT), Tunis, Tunisia
| | - Islem Rekik
- BASIRA Lab, Faculty of Computer and Informatics, Istanbul Technical University, Istanbul, Turkey.
- Computing, School of Science and Engineering, University of Dundee, Dundee, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Cognition and the development of temperament from late childhood to early adolescence. JOURNAL OF RESEARCH IN PERSONALITY 2022; 95. [PMID: 35027777 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2021.104163] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
|
6
|
Arce T, McMullen K. The Corsi Block-Tapping Test: Evaluating methodological practices with an eye towards modern digital frameworks. COMPUTERS IN HUMAN BEHAVIOR REPORTS 2021. [DOI: 10.1016/j.chbr.2021.100099] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
|
7
|
Nasvytienė D, Lazdauskas T. Temperament and Academic Achievement in Children: A Meta-Analysis. Eur J Investig Health Psychol Educ 2021; 11:736-757. [PMID: 34563066 PMCID: PMC8314362 DOI: 10.3390/ejihpe11030053] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/11/2021] [Revised: 07/07/2021] [Accepted: 07/08/2021] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
This study aimed to systematize the diverse and rather controversial findings of empirical research on the relationship between the temperament and academic achievement of school children, as well as to determine the average effect size between these variables. We included 57 original studies of published and unpublished research conducted in 12 countries between 1985 and 2019, with cumulative sample size of 79,913 (varying from 6333 to 14,126 for links between particular temperament dimensions and specific domains of achievement). A random-effects and mixed-effects model was fitted to the data for the central tendency of the temperament-achievement relation and for analyzing moderators, respectively. The high heterogeneity of studies was tackled by selected specific moderators, namely, education level, transition status, family's socio-economic level, and sources of report on achievement and temperament. The main findings of this meta-analysis affirmed the positive association of effortful control (EC) and inverse relationship of negative affectivity (NA) with a child's academic performance, together with no apparent trend of surgency (SU) in this relationship; additionally, the sources of report significantly moderated the link between temperament and academic achievement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dalia Nasvytienė
- Institute of Psychology, Faculty of Philosophy, Vilnius University, 3 Universiteto Str., LT-01513 Vilnius, Lithuania;
| | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
Examining the relationships among adolescent health behaviours, prefrontal function, and academic achievement using fNIRS. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2021; 50:100983. [PMID: 34265630 PMCID: PMC8280512 DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2021.100983] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/14/2020] [Revised: 05/17/2021] [Accepted: 06/25/2021] [Indexed: 12/05/2022] Open
Abstract
Prior studies document effects of lifestyle behaviors on the brain and academics. Among adolescents we found that activity and eating both predict task performance. Activity also predicts functional activation in the right lateral dlPFC, but not grades. Substance use predicted worse grades but not brain-related mediators.
Several adolescent health behaviours have been hypothesized to improve academic performance via their beneficial impact on cognitive control and functional aspects of the prefrontal cortex (PFC). The primary objective of this study is to examine the association between lifestyle behaviours and academic performance in a sample of adolescents, and to examine the extent to which activity within the PFC and behavioural indices of inhibition may mediate this relationship. Sixty-seven adolescents underwent two study sessions five days apart. Sleep and physical activity were measured using wrist-mounted accelerometry; eating habits, substance use and academic achievement were measured by self-report. Prefrontal function was quantified by Multi-Source Interference Task (MSIT) performance, and task-related activity via functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). Higher levels of physical activity predicted higher MSIT accuracy scores (β = .321, ρ = 0.019) as well as greater activation within the right dlPFC (b = .008, SE = .004, ρ = .0322). Frequency of fast-food consumption and substance use were negatively associated with MSIT accuracy scores (β = −0.307, ρ = .023) and Math grades (b = −3.702, SE = 1.563, ρ = .022), respectively. Overall, the results of this study highlight the importance of lifestyle behaviours as predictors of prefrontal function and academic achievement in youth.
Collapse
|
9
|
Gao Q, Feng Y, Chen W, Ping X. Does perceived chess skills mediate the relationship between fluid intelligence and academic performance? JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY IN AFRICA 2021. [DOI: 10.1080/14330237.2020.1871220] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Qiyang Gao
- Center for Brain, Mind and Education, Shaoxing University, Shaoxing, People’s Republic of China
- School of Teacher Education, Shaoxing University, Shaoxing, People’s Republic of China
| | - Yayi Feng
- School of Teacher Education, Shaoxing University, Shaoxing, People’s Republic of China
| | - Wei Chen
- Center for Brain, Mind and Education, Shaoxing University, Shaoxing, People’s Republic of China
- School of Teacher Education, Shaoxing University, Shaoxing, People’s Republic of China
| | - Xianjie Ping
- Center for Brain, Mind and Education, Shaoxing University, Shaoxing, People’s Republic of China
- School of Teacher Education, Shaoxing University, Shaoxing, People’s Republic of China
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
McDougal E, Riby DM, Hanley M. Profiles of academic achievement and attention in children with and without Autism Spectrum Disorder. RESEARCH IN DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 2020; 106:103749. [PMID: 32858397 DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2020.103749] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/08/2019] [Revised: 07/01/2020] [Accepted: 07/22/2020] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Academic outcomes for autistic individuals are heterogeneous, but the reasons for this are unknown. Attention is known to predict learning in typical development, but there is less evidence about this relationship in Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD), even though attention is reported as atypical in this group. AIMS To investigate reading and maths achievement profiles for children with and without an ASD, focusing on the role of attention in these profiles and to enable a better understanding of individual differences. METHODS Reading, maths and attention abilities of 22 autistic children (6-16 years) and 59 TD children (6-11 years) were measured using standardised assessments. RESULTS A hierarchical cluster analysis that included all children (N = 81) revealed three distinct transdiagnostic subgroups, characterised by children with good, average, and poorer divided attention and academic achievement respectively. Children with poorer attention and achievement displayed relative weaknesses in maths, while children with average or above-average attention and achievement showed no such weakness. CONCLUSIONS The findings provide a novel insight into the relationship between attention and achievement and understanding individual differences in ASD and typical development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emily McDougal
- Department of Psychology, Durham University, South Road, Durham DH1 3LE, UK; Centre for Developmental Disorders, Durham University, South Road, Durham DH1 3LE, UK; Child Life and Health, University of Edinburgh, 20 Sylvan Place, Edinburgh EH9 1UW, UK
| | - Deborah M Riby
- Department of Psychology, Durham University, South Road, Durham DH1 3LE, UK; Centre for Developmental Disorders, Durham University, South Road, Durham DH1 3LE, UK.
| | - Mary Hanley
- Department of Psychology, Durham University, South Road, Durham DH1 3LE, UK; Centre for Developmental Disorders, Durham University, South Road, Durham DH1 3LE, UK
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Ren X, Tong Y, Peng P, Wang T. Critical thinking predicts academic performance beyond general cognitive ability: Evidence from adults and children. INTELLIGENCE 2020. [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2020.101487] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
12
|
Nechtelberger M, Vlasak T, Senft B, Nechtelberger A, Barth A. Assessing Psychological Fitness to Drive for Intoxicated Drivers: Relationships of Cognitive Abilities, Fluid Intelligence, and Personality Traits. Front Psychol 2020; 11:1002. [PMID: 32528377 PMCID: PMC7264114 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/29/2019] [Accepted: 04/22/2020] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Our study explores the relationships between traffic-psychological driving-related personality traits, fluid intelligence, and cognitive abilities for drivers whose driver license has been revoked due to intoxicated driving (alcohol and/or drugs). We were able to show that high significant impacts on cognitive functions derive from the participants' age and fluid intelligence. In addition, driving-related personality traits like emotional instability, sense of responsibility and self-control contributed significantly to some of the cognitive abilities that are important for the fitness to drive. Additionally, mediating effects of fluid intelligence in the model are discussed. Traffic psychologists can use this knowledge in their assessment of drivers, mainly regarding the possible compensation of cognitive deficits regarding the fitness to drive.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Thomas Vlasak
- Department of Psychology, Sigmund Freud University Linz, Linz, Austria
| | - Birgit Senft
- Department of Social Sciences, University of Nicosia, Nicosia, Cyprus
| | | | - Alfred Barth
- Department of Psychology, Sigmund Freud University Linz, Linz, Austria
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Caviola S, Colling LJ, Mammarella IC, Szűcs D. Predictors of mathematics in primary school: Magnitude comparison, verbal and spatial working memory measures. Dev Sci 2020; 23:e12957. [DOI: 10.1111/desc.12957] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/24/2019] [Revised: 11/15/2019] [Accepted: 02/10/2020] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Sara Caviola
- School of Psychology University of Leeds Leeds UK
- Department of Psychology Centre for Neuroscience in Education University of Cambridge Cambridge UK
| | - Lincoln J. Colling
- Department of Psychology Centre for Neuroscience in Education University of Cambridge Cambridge UK
- School of Psychology University of Sussex Brighton UK
| | | | - Dénes Szűcs
- Department of Psychology Centre for Neuroscience in Education University of Cambridge Cambridge UK
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Hellebrekers DMJ, Doorenweerd N, Sweere DJJ, van Kuijk SMJ, Aartsma-Rus AM, Klinkenberg S, Vles JSH, Hendriksen JGM. Longitudinal follow-up of verbal span and processing speed in Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Eur J Paediatr Neurol 2020; 25:120-126. [PMID: 31964551 DOI: 10.1016/j.ejpn.2020.01.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/25/2019] [Revised: 12/18/2019] [Accepted: 01/03/2020] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Neurocognitive deficits are frequently described in Duchenne muscular dystrophy (DMD), but it is unknown how these progress over time. Our aim was to longitudinally assess verbal span capacity and information processing speed in DMD and to explore a genotype-phenotype relation. Verbal span and processing speed scores were available of 28 males with DMD on two time-points, with a mean time interval of 28.34 months (SD = 16.09). The cohort contained of six patients missing only dystrophin isoform Dp427, sixteen missing Dp427 and Dp140, and six were undeterminable. A lower verbal span capacity was found at the first and second assessment, whereas processing speed was normal at both time-points. Post-hoc analyses suggested lower scores on verbal span and processing speed for patients missing Dp427 and Dp140. In DMD, a developmental stagnation in verbal span capacity, irrespective of normal processing speed, is detected through longitudinal follow-up. This appears more pronounced in patients missing Dp427 and Dp140.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Danique M J Hellebrekers
- Kempenhaeghe, Centre for Neurological Learning Disabilities, Heeze, the Netherlands; School for Mental Health & Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, the Netherlands.
| | - Nathalie Doorenweerd
- Department of Radiology, Leiden University Medical Centre, the Netherlands; John Walton Muscular Dystrophy Research Centre, Newcastle University, Newcastle Upon Tyne, United Kingdom
| | - Dirk J J Sweere
- Kempenhaeghe, Centre for Neurological Learning Disabilities, Heeze, the Netherlands
| | - Sander M J van Kuijk
- Department of Clinical Epidemiology and Medical Technology Assessments, Maastricht University Medical Centre, the Netherlands
| | | | - Sylvia Klinkenberg
- Kempenhaeghe, Centre for Neurological Learning Disabilities, Heeze, the Netherlands; School for Mental Health & Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, the Netherlands; Department of Neurology, Maastricht University Medical Centre, the Netherlands
| | - Johan S H Vles
- School for Mental Health & Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, the Netherlands
| | - Jos G M Hendriksen
- Kempenhaeghe, Centre for Neurological Learning Disabilities, Heeze, the Netherlands; School for Mental Health & Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, the Netherlands.
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Kamel AF, Behery F, Kenawy G, El Ghamrawy T, Ali M, Nasr M, Shaheen M, Shatat R, Baag M. Exploring study skills among university students in Riyadh, Saudi Arabia. SAUDI JOURNAL OF ORAL SCIENCES 2020. [DOI: 10.4103/sjos.sjoralsci_87_19] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022] Open
|
16
|
Oi K. Does degree completion improve non-cognitive skills during early adulthood and adulthood? J Adolesc 2019; 71:50-62. [PMID: 30616223 DOI: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2018.12.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/10/2018] [Revised: 12/05/2018] [Accepted: 12/12/2018] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Non-cognitive skills, particularly in terms of risk-aversion, future-orientation, and conscientiousness, grow with age, and this phenomenon is known as personality maturation. However, significant variability in maturation among individuals exists. The technology of cognitive/non-cognitive skill formation suggests that the growth of non-cognitive skills is contingent on cognitive skills or human capital in general. The completion of formal education is a quintessential form of human capital. The aim of this study is to test whether formal education indeed facilitates the improvement of non-cognitive skills during early adulthood and adulthood.] METHODS: I used data from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health. The study sample consists of 9291 individuals, representative of U.S. adolescents in grades 7 through 12 in 1994. The longitudinal design of the data allowed the repeated measurement of their non-cognitive skills in adolescence (age < 18), early adulthood (between 18 and 25) and then in adulthood (>25). I used Latent Score Difference modeling to examine whether advancement in formal education through degree completion predicts within-individual change in non-cognitive skills in early adulthood and adulthood. RESULTS A steady increase in non-cognitive skills beyond adolescence was found. Independently of academic engagement during high school, parental socio-economic status, and adolescent non-cognitive skills, degree completion reported in early adulthood coincides with gains in non-cognitive skills since adolescence, and this positive feedback repeats itself in adulthood. CONCLUSIONS Continued schooling facilitates personality maturation beyond adolescence. Given the profound effects of non-cognitive skills on various life outcomes, educational opportunities could alleviate social stratification.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katsuya Oi
- Department of Sociology, Northern Arizona University, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Esposito AG, Bauer PJ. Building a knowledge base: Predicting self-derivation through integration in 6- to 10-year-olds. J Exp Child Psychol 2018; 176:55-72. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.07.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/16/2018] [Revised: 07/10/2018] [Accepted: 07/11/2018] [Indexed: 12/27/2022]
|
18
|
Woo M, Kim Y. Cortical Functional Connections and Fluid Intelligence in Adolescent APOE ε4 Carriers. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2018; 44:153-159. [PMID: 28848214 DOI: 10.1159/000479276] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 07/06/2017] [Indexed: 01/07/2023] Open
Abstract
AIMS This study examined differences in corticocortical communication between adolescent ε4 carriers (ε4+) and noncarriers (ε4-) during a fluid intelligence task (Comprehensive Test of Nonverbal Intelligence [CTONI]). METHODS Sixteen ε4+ and 20 ε4- individuals aged 13-15 years performed the CTONI while real-time EEG signals were acquired. Inter- and intrahemispheric coherences were analyzed. RESULTS The ε4+ subjects exhibited lower inter- and intrahemispheric coherences than the ε4- individuals. CONCLUSION ε4 carriers have lower corticocortical communication than noncarriers during an intelligence task, implying that carrying the ε4 allele may reduce brain networking in adolescence, several decades before the onset of Alzheimer disease.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Minjung Woo
- School of Exercise and Sport Science, University of Ulsan, Ulsan, South Korea
| | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Flores-Mendoza C, Escorial S, Herrero O, Colom R. The Dissociation between Adult Intelligence and Personality with Respect to Maltreatment Episodes and Externalizing Behaviors Occurring in Childhood. J Intell 2018; 6:E31. [PMID: 31162458 PMCID: PMC6480752 DOI: 10.3390/jintelligence6030031] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/29/2018] [Revised: 07/02/2018] [Accepted: 07/05/2018] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Here we analyze the simultaneous relationships among five variables. Two refer to childhood (episodes of various forms of maltreatment and externalizing behaviors), whereas three refer to early adulthood (intelligence, personality, and socialization difficulties). The 120 individuals considered for the present report were invited from the 650 schoolchildren participating in the Longitudinal Study of Intelligence and Personality (Minas Gerais, Brazil). The complete sample was recruited in 2002 (T1; mean age = 10.0; standard deviation (SD) = 2.2) and 120 were tested again in 2014-17 (T2; mean age = 23.5; SD = 2.2). Externalizing behaviors were registered at T1, whereas the remaining variables were obtained at T2. These were the main results: (1) externalizing behaviors predict future social effectiveness (as estimated by the general factor of personality derived from the NEO Personality Inventory-Revised (NEO-PI-R) and socialization difficulties computed from the socialization scale (SOC)) and future intelligence performance (as assessed by a set of fluid and crystallized tests); (2) episodes of self-reported childhood maltreatment predict social effectiveness, but not intelligence; (3) maltreatment and externalizing behaviors are unrelated; and (4) social effectiveness (personality) and intelligence are unrelated. Therefore, the findings support the dissociation between adult intelligence and personality with respect to maltreatment episodes and externalizing behaviors occurring in childhood. Implications of these findings for social policies aimed at preventing adult socially ineffective personalities are underscored.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carmen Flores-Mendoza
- Department of Psychology-Laboratory of the Individual Differences Assessment, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, Av. Antonio Carlos, 6627-Pampulha, Belo Horizonte 31270-901, Minas Gerais, Brazil.
| | - Sergio Escorial
- Department of Methodology, Faculty of Psychology, Universidad Complutense, Av. Séneca, 2, 28040 Madrid, Spain.
| | - Oscar Herrero
- Penintentiary Center of Cáceres, Arroyo Valhondo, 1, 10004 Cáceres, Spain.
| | - Roberto Colom
- Department of Biological Psychology and Health, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Ciudad Universitaria de Cantoblanco, 28049 Madrid, Spain.
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Xenidou-Dervou I, Van Luit JEH, Kroesbergen EH, Friso-van den Bos I, Jonkman LM, van der Schoot M, van Lieshout ECDM. Cognitive predictors of children's development in mathematics achievement: A latent growth modeling approach. Dev Sci 2018; 21:e12671. [PMID: 29691952 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12671] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/22/2017] [Accepted: 03/06/2018] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
Research has identified various domain-general and domain-specific cognitive abilities as predictors of children's individual differences in mathematics achievement. However, research into the predictors of children's individual growth rates, namely between-person differences in within-person change in mathematics achievement is scarce. We assessed 334 children's domain-general and mathematics-specific early cognitive abilities and their general mathematics achievement longitudinally across four time-points within the first and second grades of primary school. As expected, a constellation of multiple cognitive abilities contributed to the children's starting level of mathematical success. Specifically, latent growth modeling revealed that WM abilities, IQ, counting skills, nonsymbolic and symbolic approximate arithmetic and comparison skills explained individual differences in the children's initial status on a curriculum-based general mathematics achievement test. Surprisingly, however, only one out of all the assessed cognitive abilities was a unique predictor of the children's individual growth rates in mathematics achievement: their performance in the symbolic approximate addition task. In this task, children were asked to estimate the sum of two large numbers and decide if this estimated sum was smaller or larger compared to a third number. Our findings demonstrate the importance of multiple domain-general and mathematics-specific cognitive skills for identifying children at risk of struggling with mathematics and highlight the significance of early approximate arithmetic skills for the development of one's mathematical success. We argue the need for more research focus on explaining children's individual growth rates in mathematics achievement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Iro Xenidou-Dervou
- Mathematics Education Centre, Loughborough University, Loughborough, Leicestershire, UK.,Section of Educational Neuroscience, and LEARN! Research Institute, Faculty of Behavioral and Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Johannes E H Van Luit
- Department of Educational and Learning Sciences, Faculty of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Evelyn H Kroesbergen
- Department of Educational and Learning Sciences, Faculty of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands.,Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Ilona Friso-van den Bos
- Department of Educational and Learning Sciences, Faculty of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Lisa M Jonkman
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
| | - Menno van der Schoot
- Section of Educational Neuroscience, and LEARN! Research Institute, Faculty of Behavioral and Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Ernest C D M van Lieshout
- Section of Educational Neuroscience, and LEARN! Research Institute, Faculty of Behavioral and Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Liu R, Blankenship TL, Broomell APR, Garcia-Meza T, Calkins SD, Bell MA. Executive Function Mediates the Association between Toddler Negative Affectivity and Early Academic Achievement. EARLY EDUCATION AND DEVELOPMENT 2018; 29:641-654. [PMID: 30745790 PMCID: PMC6368268 DOI: 10.1080/10409289.2018.1446880] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/09/2023]
Abstract
RESEARCH FINDINGS We examined the nature of association between toddler negative affectivity (NA) and later academic achievement by testing early childhood executive function (EF) as a mediator that links children's temperament and their performance on standardized math and reading assessments. One hundred eighty-four children (93 boys, 91 girls) participated in our longitudinal study. Children's NA was measured at age 2 and EF at age 4. At age 6, academic achievement in reading and mathematics were assessed using the Woodcock Johnson III Tests of Achievement (Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001). Results indicated that NA at age 2 negatively predicted EF at age 4, which positively predicted mathematics achievement and reading achievement at age 6. Age 4 EF mediated the relation between age 2 NA and age 6 academic achievement on both reading and math. These findings highlight the significance of considering both NA and EF in conversations about children's academic achievement. PRACTICE OR POLICY For children with temperamentally high NA, focusing on efforts to enhance emotion regulation and EF during the preschool years may benefit their later mathematics and reading achievement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ran Liu
- Department of Psychology, 109 Williams Hall, Blacksburg, VA 24061
| | | | | | | | - Susan D Calkins
- The University of North Carolina at Greensboro: Department of Human Development and Family Studies, 248 Stone Building, Greensboro, NC 27402-6170
| | - Martha Ann Bell
- Department of Psychology, 109 Williams Hall, Blacksburg, VA 24061
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Davies LEM, Kuipers MAG, Junger M, Kunst AE. The role of self-control and cognitive functioning in educational inequalities in adolescent smoking and binge drinking. BMC Public Health 2017; 17:714. [PMID: 28915912 PMCID: PMC5603096 DOI: 10.1186/s12889-017-4753-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/15/2016] [Accepted: 09/12/2017] [Indexed: 02/08/2023] Open
Abstract
Background Large differences in substance use between educational levels originate at a young age, but there is limited evidence explaining these inequalities. The aim of this study was to test whether a) smoking and binge drinking are associated with lower levels of self-control and cognitive functioning, and b) associations between educational track and smoking and binge drinking, respectively, are attenuated after controlling for self-control and cognitive functioning. Methods This study used cross-sectional survey data of 15 to 20-year-olds (N = 191) from low, middle, and high educational tracks. We measured regular binge drinking and regular smoking (more than once a month), cognitive functioning (cognitive ability, reaction time and memory span), and self-control. Logistic regression models were used to assess the associations between educational track and smoking and binge drinking controlled for age, gender and social disadvantage, and for self-control and cognitive functioning. Results According to models that controlled for age, gender and social disadvantage only, respondents in the low educational track were more likely to drink heavily (OR = 3.25, 95% CI = 1.48–7.17) and smoke (OR = 5.74, 95% CI = 2.31–14.29) than adolescents in the high educational track. The association between educational track and binge drinking was hardly reduced after adjustment for self-control and cognitive ability (OR = 2.88, 95% CI = 1.09–7.62). Adjustment for self-control and cognitive functioning, especially cognitive ability, weakened the association between education and smoking (OR = 3.40, 95% CI = 1.11–10.37). However, inequalities in smoking remained significant and substantial. Conclusions In this study population, pre-existing variations between adolescents in terms of self-control and cognitive functioning played a minor role in educational inequalities in smoking, but not in binge drinking.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lisa E M Davies
- Department of Public Health, Academic Medical Center - University of Amsterdam, P.O. Box 22660, 1100 DD, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Mirte A G Kuipers
- Department of Public Health, Academic Medical Center - University of Amsterdam, P.O. Box 22660, 1100 DD, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
| | - Marianne Junger
- Department Industrial Engineering and Business Information Systems (IEBIS), University of Twente, P.O. Box 217, 7500 AE, Enschede, The Netherlands
| | - Anton E Kunst
- Department of Public Health, Academic Medical Center - University of Amsterdam, P.O. Box 22660, 1100 DD, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Barreda-Tarrazona I, Jaramillo-Gutiérrez A, Pavan M, Sabater-Grande G. Individual Characteristics vs. Experience: An Experimental Study on Cooperation in Prisoner's Dilemma. Front Psychol 2017; 8:596. [PMID: 28473787 PMCID: PMC5397528 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00596] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/17/2016] [Accepted: 03/31/2017] [Indexed: 11/27/2022] Open
Abstract
Cooperative behavior is often assumed to depend on individuals' characteristics, such as altruism and reasoning ability. Evidence is mixed about what the precise impact of these characteristics is, as the subjects of study are generally randomly paired, generating a heterogeneous mix of the two characteristics. In this study we ex-ante create four different groups of subjects by factoring their higher or lower than the median scores in both altruism and reasoning ability. Then we use these groups in order to analyze the joint effect of the two characteristics on the individual choice of cooperating and on successful paired cooperation. Subjects belonging to each group play first 10 one-shot prisoner's dilemma (PD) games with ten random partners and then three consecutive 10-round repeated PD games with three random partners. In all games, we elicit players' beliefs regarding cooperation using an incentive compatible method. Individuals with high altruism are more optimistic about the cooperative behavior of the other player in the one-shot game. They also show higher individual cooperation and paired cooperation rates in the first repetitions of this game. Contrary to the one-shot PD games where high reasoning ability reduces the probability of playing cooperatively, the sign of the relationship is inverted in the first repeated PD game, showing that high reasoning ability individuals better adjust their behavior to the characteristics of the game they are playing. In this sense, the joint effect of reasoning ability and altruism is not linear, with reasoning ability counteracting the cooperative effect of altruism in the one-shot game and reinforcing it in the first repeated game. However, experience playing the repeated PD games takes over the two individual characteristics in explaining individual and paired cooperation. Thus, in a (PD) setting, altruism and reasoning ability significantly affect behavior in single encounters, while in repeated interactions individual and paired cooperation reach similarly high levels independently of these individual characteristics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Iván Barreda-Tarrazona
- LEE and Economics Department, Universitat Jaume ICastellón, Spain.,Departments of Management and Economics, Center for Experimental Research in Management and Economics (CERME), Università Ca'FoscariVenezia, Italy
| | | | - Marina Pavan
- LEE and Economics Department, Universitat Jaume ICastellón, Spain
| | | |
Collapse
|
24
|
Williams LH, Drew T. Distraction in diagnostic radiology: How is search through volumetric medical images affected by interruptions? Cogn Res Princ Implic 2017; 2:12. [PMID: 28275705 PMCID: PMC5318487 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-017-0050-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/18/2016] [Accepted: 01/14/2017] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
Observational studies have shown that interruptions are a frequent occurrence in diagnostic radiology. The present study used an experimental design in order to quantify the cost of these interruptions during search through volumetric medical images. Participants searched through chest CT scans for nodules that are indicative of lung cancer. In half of the cases, search was interrupted by a series of true or false math equations. The primary cost of these interruptions was an increase in search time with no corresponding increase in accuracy or lung coverage. This time cost was not modulated by the difficulty of the interruption task or an individual's working memory capacity. Eye-tracking suggests that this time cost was driven by impaired memory for which regions of the lung were searched prior to the interruption. Potential interventions will be discussed in the context of these results.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Trafton Drew
- Department of Psychology, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT USA
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Puchert JI, Dodd N, Viljoen KL. Secondary education as a predictor of aptitude: Implications for selection in the automotive sector. SA JOURNAL OF INDUSTRIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2017. [DOI: 10.4102/sajip.v43i0.1416] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/01/2022] Open
Abstract
Orientation: Details of applicants’ secondary education (incorporating subject choice) could be a useful screening tool when processing large applicant pools. Here, the relationships between secondary education (incorporating subject choice) and the reasoning and visual perceptual speed components of the Differential Aptitude Test are explored.Research purpose: The objective of the study was to determine whether type of secondary education (incorporating subject choice) could be used as a substitute for reasoning (verbal and non-verbal) and/or visual perceptual speed aptitudes in the selection of operators for an automotive plant in South Africa.Motivation for the study: The motivation for this study arose from the evident gap in academic literature as well as the selection needs of the automotive industry.Research design, approach and method: This research adopted a quantitative approach. It involved a non-probability convenience quota sample of 2463 work-seeking applicants for an automotive operator position in South Africa. Participants completed a biographical questionnaire and three subtests from the Differential Aptitude Test battery. The Chi-square test was used to determine the relationship between type of secondary education (incorporating subject choice) and selected cognitive aptitudes.Main findings: The study’s findings revealed statistically and practically significant relationships between type of secondary education (incorporating subject choice), verbal reasoning, non-verbal reasoning and visual perceptual speed. Broad performance levels in the three aptitude subtests employed in this study were significantly associated with the type of matriculation certificate held by applicants. The findings specifically indicated that the secondary education types that included the subjects mathematics or both mathematics and science were associated with higher levels of performance in the three aptitudes. This had consequences for these applicants’ success in the screening process which could lead to enhanced chances of employability.Practical and managerial implications: Applicants’ type of secondary education (incorporating subject choice) could be regarded as a key criterion in human resource selection and be instructive in the screening process. This could reduce the candidate pool prior to more costly psychometric assessments.Contribution or value-add: The findings are specifically relevant to the South African automotive industry in terms of their human resource selection practices. The insights gained from the findings may also be used as a guide to human resource practitioners in the selection of similar level employees in other working contexts. The study makes a case for a multiple-hurdle approach to selection.
Collapse
|
26
|
Edalati H, Krank MD. Childhood Maltreatment and Development of Substance Use Disorders: A Review and a Model of Cognitive Pathways. TRAUMA, VIOLENCE & ABUSE 2016; 17:454-467. [PMID: 25964275 DOI: 10.1177/1524838015584370] [Citation(s) in RCA: 55] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/04/2023]
Abstract
Exposure to childhood maltreatment (CM) is associated with increased risk for developing substance use disorders (SUDs). CM exerts negative effects on cognitive abilities including intellectual performance, memory, attention, and executive function. Parallel cognitive impairments have been observed in SUDs. Hence, limited studies have examined the mediating effect of cognitive impairments in the relationship between CM and SUDs. In addition, most studies used concurrent self-report assessments in adult populations. Longitudinal studies that investigated the long-term consequences of CM on psychopathology, including SUDs, throughout childhood, adolescence, and adulthood are rare. Thus, the underlying developmental pathways between CM and SUDs are not clearly understood. In this article, we review the evidence that cognitive impairments mediate, at least in part, the relationship between CM and development of SUDs and propose a model that explains how CM increases the risk for SUDs through the development of a cognitive framework of vulnerability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hanie Edalati
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Kelowna, British Columbia, Canada
| | - Marvin D Krank
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Kelowna, British Columbia, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Ibáñez MI, Sabater-Grande G, Barreda-Tarrazona I, Mezquita L, López-Ovejero S, Villa H, Perakakis P, Ortet G, García-Gallego A, Georgantzís N. Take the Money and Run: Psychopathic Behavior in the Trust Game. Front Psychol 2016; 7:1866. [PMID: 27965606 PMCID: PMC5125304 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01866] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/07/2016] [Accepted: 11/10/2016] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
We study the association among different sources of individual differences such as personality, cognitive ability and risk attitudes with trust and reciprocate behavior in an incentivized experimental binary trust game in a sample of 220 (138 females) undergraduate students. The game involves two players, player 1 (P1) and player 2 (P2). In the first stage, P1 decides whether to trust and let P2 decide, or to secure an egalitarian payoff for both players. If P1 trusts P2, the latter can choose between a symmetric payoff that is double than the secure alternative discarded by P1, and an asymmetric payoff in which P2 earns more than in any other case but makes P1 worse off. Before the main experiment, we obtained participants' scores for Abstract Reasoning (AR), risk attitudes, basic personality characteristics, and specific traits such as psychopathy and impulsivity. During the main experiment, we measured Heart Rate (HR) and ElectroDermal Activity (EDA) variation to account for emotional arousal caused by the decision and feedback processes. Our main findings indicate that, on one hand, P1 trust behavior associates to positive emotionality and, specifically, to the extraversion's warmth facet. In addition, the impulsivity facet of positive urgency also favors trust behavior. No relation to trusting behavior was found for either other major personality aspects or risk attitudes. The physiological results show that participants scoring high in psychopathy exhibit increased EDA and reduced evoked HR deceleration at the moment in which they are asked to decide whether or not to trust. Regarding P2, we find that AR ability and mainly low disagreeable disinhibition favor reciprocal behavior. Specifically, lack of reciprocity significantly relates with a psychopathic, highly disinhibited and impulsive personality. Thus, the present study suggests that personality characteristics would play a significant role in different behaviors underlying cooperation, with extraversion/positive emotionality being more relevant for initiating cooperation, and low disagreeable disinhibition for maintaining it.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Manuel I Ibáñez
- Department of Basic and Clinical Psychology, Universitat Jaume ICastelló, Spain; Centre for Biomedical Research Network on Mental Health, Instituto de Salud Carlos IIIMadrid, Spain
| | - Gerardo Sabater-Grande
- Laboratory of Experimental Economics and Economics Department, Universitat Jaume I Castellón, Spain
| | - Iván Barreda-Tarrazona
- Laboratory of Experimental Economics and Economics Department, Universitat Jaume I Castellón, Spain
| | - Laura Mezquita
- Department of Basic and Clinical Psychology, Universitat Jaume I Castelló, Spain
| | - Sandra López-Ovejero
- Laboratory of Experimental Economics and Economics Department, Universitat Jaume I Castellón, Spain
| | - Helena Villa
- Department of Basic and Clinical Psychology, Universitat Jaume I Castelló, Spain
| | - Pandelis Perakakis
- Laboratory of Experimental Economics and Economics Department, Universitat Jaume ICastellón, Spain; Centro de Investigación Mente, Cerebro y Comportamiento, Universidad de GranadaGranada, Spain
| | - Generós Ortet
- Department of Basic and Clinical Psychology, Universitat Jaume ICastelló, Spain; Centre for Biomedical Research Network on Mental Health, Instituto de Salud Carlos IIIMadrid, Spain
| | - Aurora García-Gallego
- Laboratory of Experimental Economics and Economics Department, Universitat Jaume I Castellón, Spain
| | - Nikolaos Georgantzís
- Laboratory of Experimental Economics and Economics Department, Universitat Jaume ICastellón, Spain; School of Agriculture, Policy and Development, University of ReadingReading, UK
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Abstract
Working memory capacity is one of the most frequently measured individual difference constructs in cognitive psychology and related fields. However, implementation of complex span and other working memory measures is generally time-consuming for administrators and examinees alike. Because researchers often must manage the tension between limited testing time and measuring numerous constructs reliably, a short and effective measure of working memory capacity would often be a major practical benefit in future research efforts. The current study developed a shortened computerized domain-general measure of working memory capacity by representatively sampling items from three existing complex working memory span tasks: operation span, reading span, and symmetry span. Using a large archival data set (Study 1, N = 4,845), we developed and applied a principled strategy for developing the reduced measure, based on testing a series of confirmatory factor analysis models. Adequate fit indices from these models lent support to this strategy. The resulting shortened measure was then administered to a second independent sample (Study 2, N = 172), demonstrating that the new measure saves roughly 15 min (30%) of testing time on average, and even up to 25 min depending on the test-taker. On the basis of these initial promising findings, several directions for future research are discussed.
Collapse
|
29
|
Baniqued PL, Allen CM, Kranz MB, Johnson K, Sipolins A, Dickens C, Ward N, Geyer A, Kramer AF. Working Memory, Reasoning, and Task Switching Training: Transfer Effects, Limitations, and Great Expectations? PLoS One 2015; 10:e0142169. [PMID: 26555341 PMCID: PMC4640538 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0142169] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/28/2015] [Accepted: 10/19/2015] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Although some studies have shown that cognitive training can produce improvements to untrained cognitive domains (far transfer), many others fail to show these effects, especially when it comes to improving fluid intelligence. The current study was designed to overcome several limitations of previous training studies by incorporating training expectancy assessments, an active control group, and "Mind Frontiers," a video game-based mobile program comprised of six adaptive, cognitively demanding training tasks that have been found to lead to increased scores in fluid intelligence (Gf) tests. We hypothesize that such integrated training may lead to broad improvements in cognitive abilities by targeting aspects of working memory, executive function, reasoning, and problem solving. Ninety participants completed 20 hour-and-a-half long training sessions over four to five weeks, 45 of whom played Mind Frontiers and 45 of whom completed visual search and change detection tasks (active control). After training, the Mind Frontiers group improved in working memory n-back tests, a composite measure of perceptual speed, and a composite measure of reaction time in reasoning tests. No training-related improvements were found in reasoning accuracy or other working memory tests, nor in composite measures of episodic memory, selective attention, divided attention, and multi-tasking. Perceived self-improvement in the tested abilities did not differ between groups. A general expectancy difference in problem-solving was observed between groups, but this perceived benefit did not correlate with training-related improvement. In summary, although these findings provide modest evidence regarding the efficacy of an integrated cognitive training program, more research is needed to determine the utility of Mind Frontiers as a cognitive training tool.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pauline L. Baniqued
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
| | - Courtney M. Allen
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
| | - Michael B. Kranz
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
| | - Kathryn Johnson
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
| | - Aldis Sipolins
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
| | - Charles Dickens
- Aptima, Inc., Woburn, Massachusetts, United States of America
| | - Nathan Ward
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
| | - Alexandra Geyer
- Aptima, Inc., Woburn, Massachusetts, United States of America
| | - Arthur F. Kramer
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Are impulsivity and intelligence truly related constructs? Evidence based on the fixed-links model. PERSONALITY AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2015. [DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.04.049] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
31
|
Bosco F, Allen DG, Singh K. Executive Attention: An Alternative Perspective on General Mental Ability, Performance, and Subgroup Differences. PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY 2015. [DOI: 10.1111/peps.12099] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
32
|
Dörrenbächer S, Müller PM, Tröger J, Kray J. Dissociable effects of game elements on motivation and cognition in a task-switching training in middle childhood. Front Psychol 2014; 5:1275. [PMID: 25431564 PMCID: PMC4230167 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01275] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/23/2014] [Accepted: 10/21/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Although motivational reinforcers are often used to enhance the attractiveness of trainings of cognitive control in children, little is known about how such motivational manipulations of the setting contribute to separate gains in motivation and cognitive-control performance. Here we provide a framework for systematically investigating the impact of a motivational video-game setting on the training motivation, the task performance, and the transfer success in a task-switching training in middle-aged children (8-11 years of age). We manipulated both the type of training (low-demanding/single-task training vs. high-demanding/task-switching training) as well as the motivational setting (low-motivational/without video-game elements vs. high-motivational/with video-game elements) separately from another. The results indicated that the addition of game elements to a training setting enhanced the intrinsic interest in task practice, independently of the cognitive demands placed by the training type. In the task-switching group, the high-motivational training setting led to an additional enhancement of task and switching performance during the training phase right from the outset. These motivation-induced benefits projected onto the switching performance in a switching situation different from the trained one (near-transfer measurement). However, in structurally dissimilar cognitive tasks (far-transfer measurement), the motivational gains only transferred to the response dynamics (speed of processing). Hence, the motivational setting clearly had a positive impact on the training motivation and on the paradigm-specific task-switching abilities; it did not, however, consistently generalize on broad cognitive processes. These findings shed new light on the conflation of motivation and cognition in childhood and may help to refine guidelines for designing adequate training interventions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sandra Dörrenbächer
- Department of Psychology, Development of Language, Learning and Action, Saarland University Saarbrücken, Germany
| | - Philipp M Müller
- Department of Psychology, Development of Language, Learning and Action, Saarland University Saarbrücken, Germany
| | - Johannes Tröger
- Department of Psychology, Development of Language, Learning and Action, Saarland University Saarbrücken, Germany
| | - Jutta Kray
- Department of Psychology, Development of Language, Learning and Action, Saarland University Saarbrücken, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Klaczynski PA. Heuristics and biases: interactions among numeracy, ability, and reflectiveness predict normative responding. Front Psychol 2014; 5:665. [PMID: 25071639 PMCID: PMC4078194 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00665] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/31/2014] [Accepted: 06/09/2014] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
In Stanovich's (2009a, 2011) dual-process theory, analytic processing occurs in the algorithmic and reflective minds. Thinking dispositions, indexes of reflective mind functioning, are believed to regulate operations at the algorithmic level, indexed by general cognitive ability. General limitations at the algorithmic level impose constraints on, and affect the adequacy of, specific strategies and abilities (e.g., numeracy). In a study of 216 undergraduates, the hypothesis that thinking dispositions and general ability moderate the relationship between numeracy (understanding of mathematical concepts and attention to numerical information) and normative responses on probabilistic heuristics and biases (HB) problems was tested. Although all three individual difference measures predicted normative responses, the numeracy-normative response association depended on thinking dispositions and general ability. Specifically, numeracy directly affected normative responding only at relatively high levels of thinking dispositions and general ability. At low levels of thinking dispositions, neither general ability nor numeric skills related to normative responses. Discussion focuses on the consistency of these findings with the hypothesis that the implementation of specific skills is constrained by limitations at both the reflective level and the algorithmic level, methodological limitations that prohibit definitive conclusions, and alternative explanations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paul A. Klaczynski
- Decision Making and Development, School of Psychological Science, University of Northern ColoradoGreeley, CO, USA
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Impulsivity, intelligence, and academic performance: Testing the interaction hypothesis. PERSONALITY AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2014. [DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
35
|
Omizzolo C, Scratch SE, Stargatt R, Kidokoro H, Thompson DK, Lee KJ, Cheong J, Neil J, Inder TE, Doyle LW, Anderson PJ. Neonatal brain abnormalities and memory and learning outcomes at 7 years in children born very preterm. Memory 2013; 22:605-15. [PMID: 23805915 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2013.809765] [Citation(s) in RCA: 79] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/17/2022]
Abstract
Using prospective longitudinal data from 198 very preterm and 70 full term children, this study characterised the memory and learning abilities of very preterm children at 7 years of age in both verbal and visual domains. The relationship between the extent of brain abnormalities on neonatal magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and memory and learning outcomes at 7 years of age in very preterm children was also investigated. Neonatal MRI scans were qualitatively assessed for global, white-matter, cortical grey-matter, deep grey-matter, and cerebellar abnormalities. Very preterm children performed less well on measures of immediate memory, working memory, long-term memory, and learning compared with term-born controls. Neonatal brain abnormalities, and in particular deep grey-matter abnormality, were associated with poorer memory and learning performance at 7 years in very preterm children. Findings support the importance of cerebral neonatal pathology for predicting later memory and learning function.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cristina Omizzolo
- a Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, Royal Children's Hospital , Parkville , VIC , Australia
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
36
|
Psychoticism and Disruptive Behavior can be also Good Predictors of School Achievement. SPANISH JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY 2013; 16:E13. [DOI: 10.1017/sjp.2013.3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
AbstractThe relations of Gf (Standard Progressive Matrices Raven), Gc (verbal scale of Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Version), personality dimensions (Eysenck Personality Questionnaire-Junior Version), and disruptive behavior (TDAH scale) with school achievement (measured by TDE test and PISA test) were investigated. Two samples of students (total N = 534) representing a broad range of socioeconomic status (SES) participated in this study. Path models were conducted. The results demonstrated that (1) in both samples no sex differences related to school achievement were found; (2) in the first sample, after controlling for age and SES differences, Gf and psychoticism predicted (.38 and −.13, respectively) school achievement (measured by TDE test); (3) in the second sample, after controlling for SES differences to which additional measures were administered, Gf and Gc positively predicted (.22 and .40, respectively) school achievement (measured by PISA test). In addition, psychoticism and disruptive behavior also predicted school performance (−.14 and −.28, respectively). Some theoretical and practical implications are discussed.
Collapse
|
37
|
Surányi Z, Hitchcock DB, Hittner JB, Vargha A, Urbán R. Different types of sensation seeking. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT 2013. [DOI: 10.1177/0165025413483221] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Previous research on sensation seeking (SS) was dominated by a variable-oriented approach indicating that SS level has a linear relation with a host of problem behaviors. Our aim was to provide a person-oriented methodology—a probabilistic clustering—that enables examination of both inter- and intra-individual differences in not only the level, but also in the pattern of SS. We have applied model-based clustering to a four-semester long longitudinal high school survey ( N = 3334) and to a cross-sectional university survey ( N = 438). The results indicated that impulsive patterns are linked to negative outcomes whereas non-impulsive patterns are associated with positive outcomes. Our study aims to serve as a methodological example on how to apply model-based clustering to examine different types of sensation-seeking patterns. This modern clustering method allows for probabilistic categorization, with continous typicality scores besides cluster membership variables. These typicality scores turned out to have higher temporal stability than simple categorical membership variables did.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | - András Vargha
- Károli Gáspár University of the Reformed Church, Budapest, Hungary
| | | |
Collapse
|
38
|
Krumm S, Lipnevich AA, Schmidt-Atzert L, Bühner M. Relational integration as a predictor of academic achievement. LEARNING AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2012. [DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2012.05.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
|
39
|
Processing speed and intelligence as predictors of school achievement: Mediation or unique contribution? INTELLIGENCE 2012. [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2012.01.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
|
40
|
Kagitcibasi C, Biricik D. Generational gains on the Draw-a-Person IQ scores: A three-decade comparison from Turkey. INTELLIGENCE 2011. [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2011.06.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
41
|
Pesonen AK, Räikkönen K, Kajantie E, Heinonen K, Osmond C, Barker DJP, Forsén T, Eriksson JG. Inter-generational social mobility following early life stress. Ann Med 2011; 43:320-8. [PMID: 21366512 DOI: 10.3109/07853890.2010.549146] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
INTRODUCTION. Socio-economic position (SEP) is a powerful source of health inequality. Less is known of early life conditions that may determine the course of adult SEP. We tested if early life stress (ELS) due to a separation from the parents during World War II predicts adult SEP, trajectories of incomes across the entire working career, and inter-generational social mobility. MATERIALS AND METHODS. Participants (n = 10,702) were from the Helsinki Birth Cohort Study 1934-44. Compared to the non-separated, the separated individuals attained a lower SEP in adulthood. The separated whose fathers were manual workers were less likely to be upwardly mobile from paternal occupation category to higher categories of own occupation, education, and incomes. The separated whose fathers had junior and senior clerical occupations were more likely to be downwardly mobile. Comparison of trajectories of incomes across adulthood showed that the difference between the separated and the non-separated grew larger across time, such that among the separated the incomes decreased. CONCLUSIONS. This life-course study shows that severe ELS due to a separation from parents in childhood is associated with socio-economic disadvantage in adult life. Even high initial SEP in childhood may not protect from the negative effects of ELS.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anu-Katriina Pesonen
- Department of Clinical Medicine, Hospital for Children and Adolescents, Helsinki University Central Hospital and University of Helsinki, Finland
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
42
|
Hassanbeigi A, Askari J, Nakhjavani M, Shirkhoda S, Barzegar K, Mozayyan MR, Fallahzadeh H. The relationship between study skills and academic performance of university students. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2011. [DOI: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.10.276] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/14/2022]
|
43
|
Tran UN, O'Callaghan MJ, Mamun AA, Najman JM, Williams GM, Bor W. Relationship between childhood short stature and academic achievement in adolescents and young adults--a longitudinal study. J Paediatr Child Health 2010; 46:660-7. [PMID: 21077977 DOI: 10.1111/j.1440-1754.2010.01816.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
AIM To determine if short stature at 14 or 21 years and patterns of 'catch-up' growth from 5 to 14 or 21 years are related to academic achievement in adolescents. METHODS The Mater University of Queensland Study of Pregnancy is a longitudinal study of 7223 singleton infants born between 1981 and 1984. Data were available for cross-sectional analyses of 3785 adolescents of whom 2149 were seen as young adults. Longitudinal patterns of growth were examined for 2936 subjects from 5 to 14 years and 1753 subjects from 5 to 21 years. RESULTS Adolescents or young adults with height <10th centile had a lower mean Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT) score in adolescence and at 21 years than those of normal height (2.7 and 3.0 points, respectively) and increased odds of a WRAT score <85 (1.57 and 1.87, respectively) and learning difficulties (1.61 and 1.78, respectively). For growth patterns from 5 to 14 years, adolescents short at 5 years, irrespective of height at 14 years, had a lower mean WRAT score and increased odds of WRAT score <85 and learning difficulties. However, for growth patterns from 5 to 21 years, only the group short at both ages had increased learning difficulties. CONCLUSIONS Youth short at 14 years or at 21 years and those persistently short have an increased prevalence of academic difficulties. Catch-up growth by 21, although not 14 years, was associated with improved outcomes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Uyen N Tran
- Queensland Health, Paediatrics and Child Health University of Queensland, Queensland, Australia.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
44
|
Early trajectories of interparental conflict and externalizing problems as predictors of social competence in preadolescence. Dev Psychopathol 2010; 22:527-37. [PMID: 20576176 DOI: 10.1017/s0954579410000258] [Citation(s) in RCA: 80] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
Consistent with developmental cascade notions, the present study investigated (a) associations between trajectories of interparental conflict and early externalizing problems during childhood and (b) early trajectories of externalizing problems as a pathway by which interparental conflict impacts children's social competence in preadolescence. Participants were 235 children and their parents and teachers. Children were assessed annually for 3 years, beginning when they were in kindergarten. Parents provided reports of interparental conflict and child externalizing problems. Children's social competence (prosocial behavior, social problems) was assessed approximately 5 years later via parent and teacher reports. Results from parallel process models indicated that changes in interparental conflict were positively associated with changes in externalizing problems during childhood. Further, demonstrating pathways consistent with notions of developmental cascades, early trajectories of externalizing problems accounted for the longitudinal link between early trajectories of interparental conflict and children's social problems in preadolescence.
Collapse
|
45
|
KYTTÄLÄ MINNA, AUNIO PIRJO, HAUTAMÄKI JARKKO. Working memory resources in young children with mathematical difficulties. Scand J Psychol 2010; 51:1-15. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00736.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
|
46
|
Steinmayr R, Ziegler M, Träuble B. Do intelligence and sustained attention interact in predicting academic achievement? LEARNING AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2010. [DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2009.10.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 50] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
|
47
|
Is smoking in pregnancy an independent predictor of academic difficulties at 14years of age? A birth cohort study. Early Hum Dev 2010; 86:71-6. [PMID: 20117888 DOI: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2009.12.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/05/2009] [Revised: 09/17/2009] [Accepted: 12/20/2009] [Indexed: 02/07/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Studies of the effects of maternal smoking during pregnancy have reported inconsistent findings in relation to measures of offspring cognitive functioning. Few studies, however, have examined learning outcomes in adolescents, as opposed to IQ. AIM To examine the association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and academic performance among adolescent offspring. STUDY DESIGN Population-based birth cohort study. SUBJECTS 7223 mothers and children were enrolled in the Mater-University of Queensland Study of Pregnancy in Brisbane (Australia) from 1981 to 1984. Analyses were restricted to the 4294 mothers and children for whom all information was reported at 14-year follow-up. OUTCOME MEASURES Reports of academic performance of 14-year-old offspring in English, Science and Mathematics with different patterns of maternal smoking (never smoked, smoked before and/or after pregnancy but not during pregnancy, or smoked during pregnancy). RESULTS Low academic achievement was more common only in those whose mothers had smoked during pregnancy. Effect sizes were, however, small. The adjusted mean difference in total learning score for smoking before and/or after pregnancy but not during pregnancy, and for smoking during pregnancy were -0.18 (-0.58, 0.22) and -0.40 (-0.69, -0.12). Similarly, the adjusted odds ratios were 0.9 (0. 65, 1.24) and 1.35 (1.07, 1.70). CONCLUSION Maternal smoking during pregnancy is a preventable prenatal risk factor associated with small decrements in offspring academic performance that continue into adolescence.
Collapse
|
48
|
Working Memory Span and E-Learning: The Effect of Personalization Techniques on Learners’ Performance. USER MODELING, ADAPTATION, AND PERSONALIZATION 2010. [DOI: 10.1007/978-3-642-13470-8_8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/09/2023]
|
49
|
|
50
|
Childhood intelligence, locus of control and behaviour disturbance as determinants of intergenerational social mobility: British Cohort Study 1970. INTELLIGENCE 2009. [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2009.04.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 43] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
|