1
|
Flores-Salgado E, Gutiérrez-Koyoc AF. Working Memory and Cross-Linguistic Influence on Vocabulary Acquisition. Brain Sci 2024; 14:796. [PMID: 39199488 PMCID: PMC11352211 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci14080796] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/29/2024] [Revised: 07/24/2024] [Accepted: 07/29/2024] [Indexed: 09/01/2024] Open
Abstract
The purpose of this study was to analyze the cross-linguistic influence of previously learned languages and working memory capacities on the vocabulary performance of two different typological languages. The objectives of this study were (1) to compare the working memory capacities of bilingual adults in relation to the vocabulary performance of two different languages never learned by the participants, and (2) to analyze to what extent the typology of previously learned languages influences working memory capacities in relation to the vocabulary performance of French and Nahuatl. A group of 43 Mexican Spanish college students participated in this experimental study. The participants completed a series of working memory tasks in Nahuatl and French. The results showed that working memory capacities were lower in Nahuatl than in French. Thus, a correlation was found between their first and second language and vocabulary performance in French. We can consider the influence of previously learned languages as a significant factor in vocabulary acquisition in accordance with the participants' working memory capacities.
Collapse
|
2
|
Baron A, Connell K, Kleinman D, Bedore LM, Griffin ZM. Grammatical gender in spoken word recognition in school-age Spanish monolingual and Spanish-English bilingual children. Front Psychol 2024; 15:1295379. [PMID: 39114584 PMCID: PMC11304351 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1295379] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/16/2023] [Accepted: 06/21/2024] [Indexed: 08/10/2024] Open
Abstract
This study examined grammatical gender processing in school-aged children with varying levels of cumulative English exposure. Children participated in a visual world paradigm with a four-picture display where they heard a gendered article followed by a target noun and were in the context where all images were the same gender (same gender), where all of the distractor images were the opposite gender than the target noun (different gender), and where all of the distractor images were the opposite gender, but there was a mismatch in the gendered article and target noun pair. We investigated 51 children (aged 5;0-10;0) who were exposed to Spanish since infancy but varied in their amount of cumulative English exposure. In addition to the visual word paradigm, all children completed an article-noun naming task, a grammaticality judgment task, and standardized vocabulary tests. Parents reported on their child's cumulative English language exposure and current English language use. To investigate the time course of lexical facilitation effects, looks to the target were analyzed with a cluster-based permutation test. The results revealed that all children used gender in a facilitatory way (during the noun region), and comprehension was significantly inhibited when the article-noun pairing was ungrammatical rather than grammatical. Compared to children with less cumulative English exposure, children with more cumulative English exposure looked at the target noun significantly less often overall, and compared to younger children, older children looked at the target noun significantly more often overall. Additionally, children with lower cumulative English exposure looked at target nouns more in the different-gender condition than the same-gender condition for masculine items more than feminine items.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alisa Baron
- Department of Communicative Disorders, University of Rhode Island, Kingston, RI, United States
| | | | - Daniel Kleinman
- Yale Child Study Center, Yale University, New Haven, CT, United States
| | - Lisa M. Bedore
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, United States
| | - Zenzi M. Griffin
- Department of Psychology, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, United States
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Calder SD, Visentin D, Claessen M, Hollingsworth L, Ebbels S, Smith-Lock K, Leitão S. The grammaticality judgement of inflectional morphology in children with and without Developmental Language Disorder. CLINICAL LINGUISTICS & PHONETICS 2024; 38:676-691. [PMID: 37477201 DOI: 10.1080/02699206.2023.2236768] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/26/2023] [Revised: 07/08/2023] [Accepted: 07/10/2023] [Indexed: 07/22/2023]
Abstract
To explore the clinical potential of grammaticality judgement tasks, this study investigated whether a Grammaticality Judgment Task (GJT) of inflectional morphology could differentiate between a clinically selected sample of children with DLD and children in mainstream (i.e. regular education) schools. We also explored the relationship between grammaticality judgement and measures of receptive vocabulary, receptive grammar, and nonword repetition. Children with DLD (n = 30; age range = 69-80 months) and mainstream children in Pre-primary, Year 1, and Year 2 (n = 89, age range = 61-96 months) were assessed on a GJT of regular past tense, third person singular, and possessive 's. The GJT was sensitive to developmental differences in mainstream children and differentiated children with DLD from Year 1 and 2 mainstream children, with DLD results consistent with a one-year delay in performance compared to controls. The GJT was the strongest discriminator of membership to a clinically selected sample of children with DLD (ROC curve analysis, area under the curve = 88%). Receptive grammar, receptive vocabulary, and nonword repetition were related to performance on the GJT. The grammaticality judgement of inflectional morphology shows promise as a reliable indicator of DLD and a measure sensitive to developmental differences in mainstream children. GJTs should continue to be explored for clinical application as a potential tool for both assessment and intervention.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Samuel D Calder
- Health Sciences, College of Health and Medicine, University of Tasmania, Launceston, Australia
- School of Allied Health, Faculty of Health Sciences, Curtin University, Perth, Australia
| | - Denis Visentin
- Health Sciences, College of Health and Medicine, University of Tasmania, Launceston, Australia
| | | | | | - Susan Ebbels
- Moor House Research and Training Institute, Moor House School & College, Oxted, UK
- Language and Cognition, Division of Psychology and Language, University College London, London, UK
| | - Karen Smith-Lock
- School of Allied Health, Faculty of Health Sciences, Curtin University, Perth, Australia
| | - Suze Leitão
- School of Allied Health, Faculty of Health Sciences, Curtin University, Perth, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Vaughn LE, Oetting JB, McDonald JL. Grammaticality Judgments of Tense and Agreement by Child Speakers of African American English: Effects of Clinical Status, Surface Form, and Grammatical Structure. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2023; 66:1755-1770. [PMID: 37120833 PMCID: PMC10457090 DOI: 10.1044/2023_jslhr-22-00431] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/22/2022] [Revised: 02/06/2023] [Accepted: 02/07/2023] [Indexed: 05/11/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE We examined the grammaticality judgments of tense and agreement (T/A) structures by children with and without developmental language disorder (DLD) within African American English (AAE). The children's judgments of T/A forms were also compared to their judgments of two control forms and, for some analyses, examined by surface form (i.e., overt, zero) and type of structure (i.e., BE, past tense, verbal -s). METHOD The judgments were from 91 AAE-speaking kindergartners (DLD = 34; typically developing = 57), elicited using items from the Rice/Wexler Test of Early Grammatical Impairment. The data were analyzed twice, once using General American English as the reference and A' scores and once using AAE as the reference and percentages of acceptability. RESULTS Although the groups differed using both metrics, the percentages of acceptability tied the DLD T/A deficit to judgments of the overt forms, while also revealing a general DLD weakness judging sentences that are ungrammatical in AAE. Judgments of the overt T/A forms by both groups correlated with their productions of these forms and their language test scores, and both groups showed structure-specific form preferences ("is": overt > zero vs. verbal -s: overt = zero). CONCLUSION The findings demonstrate the utility of grammaticality judgment tasks for revealing weaknesses in T/A within AAE-speaking children with DLD, while also calling for more studies using AAE as the dialect reference when designing stimuli and coding systems. SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.22534588.
Collapse
|
5
|
Acha J, Agirregoikoa A, Barreto-Zarza F, Arranz-Freijo EB. Cognitive predictors of language abilities in primary school children: A cascaded developmental view. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2023; 50:417-436. [PMID: 35193712 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000921000908] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/14/2023]
Abstract
This study investigated the longitudinal relationship between children's domain-general cognitive constraints underlying phonological and sentence processing development in a big sample of typically developing children. 104 children were tested on non-linguistic processing speed, phonological skills (phonological short term memory, phonological knowledge, phonological working memory), and sentence processing abilities (sentence repetition and receptive grammar) in 1st grade (aged 6 to 6.5) and one year later. A cross-lagged structural equation model showed that non-linguistic processing speed was a concurrent predictor of phonological skills, and that phonology had a powerful effect on the child's sentence processing abilities concurrently and longitudinally, providing clear evidence for the role of domain-general processes in the developmental pathway of language. These findings support a cascaded cognitive view of language development and pose important challenges for evaluation and intervention strategies in childhood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joana Acha
- Universidad del País Vasco UPV/EHU
- Biodonostia. Health Research Institute. San Sebastian, Spain
| | | | - Florencia Barreto-Zarza
- Universidad del País Vasco UPV/EHU
- Biodonostia. Health Research Institute. San Sebastian, Spain
| | - Enrique B Arranz-Freijo
- Universidad del País Vasco UPV/EHU
- Biodonostia. Health Research Institute. San Sebastian, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Tribushinina E, Niemann G, Meuwissen J, Mackaaij M, Lahdo G. Teaching foreign language grammar to primary-school children with developmental language disorder: A classroom-based intervention study. JOURNAL OF COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2022; 100:106269. [PMID: 36191574 DOI: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2022.106269] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/23/2022] [Revised: 08/30/2022] [Accepted: 09/26/2022] [Indexed: 06/16/2023]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Children with developmental language disorder (DLD) start learning foreign languages, usually English as a foreign language (EFL), at an increasingly young age. However, current scholarship lacks crucial insights into how children with DLD respond to language learning in classroom settings and how they can be supported in doing so. The purpose of this early efficacy study is to determine whether a business-as-usual curriculum or a new teaching method tailored to the specific needs of pupils with DLD results in (greater) progress in the foreign language (English) and in the school language (Dutch). METHOD The participants were 75 pupils with DLD in the last three years of primary school, learning EFL in special education in the Netherlands. The intervention group (n=41) received 12 lessons following the CodeTaal approach, including metalinguistic instruction of grammar rules, explicit cross-linguistic contrasts and multimodal interaction with the material. The control group (n=34) received their regular English lessons. The study used a pre- to post-test design and compared the performance of the two groups on a Grammaticality Judgment Task (GJT) in English and a narrative task in both English and Dutch. RESULTS Only the intervention group significantly improved in their ability to identify ungrammaticalities in English and generalised the learnt rules to new sentences. Although the performance on the GJT predicted accuracy of English narratives, neither group showed a significant decrease of error rates in English. In contrast, the accuracy of Dutch narratives showed improvement, but only in the intervention group. However, the effects were small and there was significant variability in responsiveness to the intervention. CONCLUSION We conclude that pupils with DLD are able to make progress in foreign language learning in a classroom setting if provided with adequate support.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Geke Niemann
- Utrecht University, Trans 10, Utrecht 3512 JK, the Netherlands
| | - Joyce Meuwissen
- Royal Kentalis, AB-dienst, Nijmeegsebaan 21a, Groesbeek GLD 6561 KE, the Netherlands
| | - Megan Mackaaij
- Utrecht University, Trans 10, Utrecht 3512 JK, the Netherlands
| | - Gabriëlla Lahdo
- Utrecht University, Trans 10, Utrecht 3512 JK, the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Sheibani F, Ghoreishi ZS, Nilipour R, Pourshahbaz A, Mohammad Zamani S. Validity and Reliability of a Language Development Scale for Persian-speaking Children Aged 2-6 Years. IRANIAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL SCIENCES 2020; 45:259-268. [PMID: 32801415 PMCID: PMC7395959 DOI: 10.30476/ijms.2020.72538.0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/28/2018] [Revised: 10/10/2018] [Accepted: 10/14/2018] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Language disorders may affect receptive and/or expressive language skills. The use of a validated and reliable assessment tool is essential to assess these skills in children. The present study aimed to develop a valid and reliable language development instrument for Persian-speaking children aged 2-6 years. METHODS The present cross-sectional study was conducted during 2016-2017 in three main Iranian cities, namely Mashhad, Tehran, and Isfahan. The target population was children between the ages of 2 and 6 in various kindergartens and schools. The Persian Language Development Scale (PLDS) was developed by incorporating linguistic characteristics of the Persian language and Iranian culture. Following a number of iterations, including a pilot study of 36 children, the final version of the PLDS tool was used to assess the receptive and expressive language skills of 460 children. The reliability and validity of the PLDS tool were examined. RESULTS The content validity ratio (CRV) of the PLDS tool was 0.85. The tool could differentiate children by age, but not by sex. The test-retest reliability, with 10 days interval, showed a significant correlation between the coefficients of receptive (0.96) and expressive (0.93) scales. The intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) for receptive and expressive scales was 0.93 and 0.98, respectively. The internal consistency, using the KR-21, for the receptive and expressive scales was 0.88 and 0.92, respectively. CONCLUSION A language development scale has been developed to assess receptive and expressive language skills in Iranian children aged 2-6 years. The validity and reliability of the tool were confirmed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Fatemeh Sheibani
- Department of Speech Therapy, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| | - Zahra Sadat Ghoreishi
- Department of Speech Therapy, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| | - Reza Nilipour
- Department of Speech Therapy, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| | - Abbas Pourshahbaz
- Department of Psychology, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| | - Sakineh Mohammad Zamani
- Department of Speech Therapy, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Schneider JM, Abel AD, Ogiela DA, McCord C, Maguire MJ. Developmental differences in the neural oscillations underlying auditory sentence processing in children and adults. BRAIN AND LANGUAGE 2018; 186:17-25. [PMID: 30199760 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandl.2018.09.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/26/2017] [Revised: 08/21/2018] [Accepted: 09/03/2018] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
Although very young children seem to process ongoing language quickly and effortlessly, neuroimaging and behavioral studies reveal that children continue to mature in their language skills through adolescence. During this prolonged development, children likely engage the same basic cognitive processes and neural mechanisms to perform language tasks as adults, but in somewhat different ways. In this study we used time frequency analysis of EEG to identify developmental differences in the engagement of neural oscillations between children (ages 10-12) and adults while listening to naturally-paced sentences. Adults displayed consistent beta changes throughout the sentence compared to children, thought to be related to efficient syntactic integration, and children displayed more broadly distributed theta changes than adults, thought to be related to more effortful semantic integration. Few differences in alpha, related to verbal working memory, existed between groups. These findings shed new light on developmental changes in the neuronal processes underlying language comprehension.
Collapse
|
9
|
Murray LL. Sentence Processing in Aphasia: An Examination of Material-Specific and General Cognitive Factors. JOURNAL OF NEUROLINGUISTICS 2018; 48:26-46. [PMID: 30686860 PMCID: PMC6345386 DOI: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2018.03.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/16/2023]
Abstract
The purpose of this study was to characterize further the nature of sentence processing deficits in acquired aphasia. Adults with aphasia and age-and education-matched adults with no brain damage completed a battery of formal cognitive-linguistic tests and an experimental sentence judgment task, which was performed alone and during focused attention and divided attention or dual-task conditions. The specific aims were to determine whether (a) increased extra-linguistic cognitive demands (i.e., focused and divided conditions) differentially affected the sentence judgement performances of the aphasic and control groups, (b) increased extra- linguistic cognitive demands interact with stimulus parameters (i.e., syntactic complexity, number of propositions) known to influence sentence processing, and (c) syntactic- or material specific resource limitations (e.g., sentence judgment in isolation), general cognitive abilities (e.g., short-term and working memory test scores), or both share a significant relationship with dual-task outcomes. Accuracy, grammatical sensitivity, and reaction time findings were consistent with resource models of aphasia and processing accounts of aphasic syntactic limitations, underscoring the theoretical and clinical importance of acknowledging and specifying the strength and nature of interactions between linguistic and extra-linguistic cognitive processes in not only individuals with aphasia, but also other patient and typical aging populations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laura L Murray
- School of Communication Sciences and Disorders Western University
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Bosma E, Heeringa W, Hoekstra E, Versloot A, Blom E. Verbal Working Memory Is Related to the Acquisition of Cross-Linguistic Phonological Regularities. Front Psychol 2017; 8:1487. [PMID: 28955260 PMCID: PMC5600946 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01487] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/20/2017] [Accepted: 08/17/2017] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Closely related languages share cross-linguistic phonological regularities, such as Frisian -âld [ͻ:t] and Dutch -oud [ʱut], as in the cognate pairs kâld [kͻ:t] – koud [kʱut] ‘cold’ and wâld [wͻ:t] – woud [wʱut] ‘forest’. Within Bybee’s (1995, 2001, 2008, 2010) network model, these regularities are, just like grammatical rules within a language, generalizations that emerge from schemas of phonologically and semantically related words. Previous research has shown that verbal working memory is related to the acquisition of grammar, but not vocabulary. This suggests that verbal working memory supports the acquisition of linguistic regularities. In order to test this hypothesis we investigated whether verbal working memory is also related to the acquisition of cross-linguistic phonological regularities. For three consecutive years, 5- to 8-year-old Frisian-Dutch bilingual children (n = 120) were tested annually on verbal working memory and a Frisian receptive vocabulary task that comprised four cognate categories: (1) identical cognates, (2) non-identical cognates that either do or (3) do not exhibit a phonological regularity between Frisian and Dutch, and (4) non-cognates. The results showed that verbal working memory had a significantly stronger effect on cognate category (2) than on the other three cognate categories. This suggests that verbal working memory is related to the acquisition of cross-linguistic phonological regularities. More generally, it confirms the hypothesis that verbal working memory plays a role in the acquisition of linguistic regularities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Evelyn Bosma
- Fryske AkademyLeeuwarden, Netherlands.,Amsterdam Center for Language and Communication, University of AmsterdamAmsterdam, Netherlands.,Leiden University Centre for Linguistics, Leiden UniversityLeiden, Netherlands
| | | | | | - Arjen Versloot
- Fryske AkademyLeeuwarden, Netherlands.,Department of Modern Foreign Languages and Cultures, University of AmsterdamAmsterdam, Netherlands
| | - Elma Blom
- Special Education Cognitive and Motor Disabilities, Department of Education and Pedagogy, Utrecht UniversityUtrecht, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Ellis Weismer S, Davidson MM, Gangopadhyay I, Sindberg H, Roebuck H, Kaushanskaya M. The role of nonverbal working memory in morphosyntactic processing by children with specific language impairment and autism spectrum disorders. J Neurodev Disord 2017; 9:28. [PMID: 28690687 PMCID: PMC5496437 DOI: 10.1186/s11689-017-9209-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/24/2017] [Accepted: 06/21/2017] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Both children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) and children with specific language impairment (SLI) have been shown to have difficulties with grammatical processing. A comparison of these two populations with neurodevelopmental disorders was undertaken to examine similarities and differences in the mechanisms that may underlie grammatical processing. Research has shown that working memory (WM) is recruited during grammatical processing. The goal of this study was to examine morphosyntactic processing on a grammatical judgment task in children who varied in clinical diagnosis and language abilities and to assess the extent to which performance is predicted by nonverbal working memory (WM). Two theoretical perspectives were evaluated relative to performance on the grammatical judgment task-the "working memory" account and the "wrap-up" account. These accounts make contrasting predictions about the detection of grammatical errors occurring early versus late in the sentence. METHODS Participants were 84 school-age children with SLI (n = 21), ASD (n = 27), and typical development (TD, n = 36). Performance was analyzed based on diagnostic group as well as language status (normal language, NL, n = 54, and language impairment, LI, n = 30). A grammatical judgment task was used in which the position of the error in the sentence (early versus late) was manipulated. A visual WM task (N-back) was administered and the ability of WM to predict morphosyntactic processing was assessed. RESULTS Groups differed significantly in their sensitivity to grammatical errors (TD > SLI and NL > LI) but did not differ in nonverbal WM. Overall, children in all groups were more sensitive and quicker at detecting errors occurring late in the sentence than early in the sentence. Nonverbal WM predicted morphosyntactic processing across groups, but the specific profile of association between WM and early versus late error detection was reversed for children with and without language impairment. CONCLUSIONS Findings primarily support a "wrap up" account whereby the accumulating sentence context for errors positioned late in the sentence (rather than early) appeared to facilitate morphosyntactic processing. Although none of the groups displayed deficits in visual WM, individual differences in these nonverbal WM resources predicted proficiency in morphosyntactic processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Susan Ellis Weismer
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53706 USA
- Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53706 USA
| | - Meghan M. Davidson
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53706 USA
- Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53706 USA
- Callier Center for Communication Disorders, University of Texas at Dallas, Dallas, TX 75235 USA
| | - Ishanti Gangopadhyay
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53706 USA
- Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53706 USA
| | - Heidi Sindberg
- Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53706 USA
| | - Hettie Roebuck
- Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53706 USA
| | - Margarita Kaushanskaya
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53706 USA
- Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53706 USA
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Paradis J, Tulpar Y, Arppe A. Chinese L1 children's English L2 verb morphology over time: individual variation in long-term outcomes. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2016; 43:553-580. [PMID: 26915494 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000915000562] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/05/2023]
Abstract
This study examined accuracy in production and grammaticality judgements of verb morphology by eighteen Chinese-speaking children learning English as a second language (L2) followed longitudinally from four to six years of exposure to English, and who began to learn English at age 4;2. Children's growth in accuracy with verb morphology reached a plateau by six years, where 11/18 children did not display native-speaker levels of accuracy for one or more morphemes. Variation in children's accuracy with verb morphology was predicted by their English vocabulary size and verbal short-term memories primarily, and quality and quantity of English input at home secondarily. This study shows that even very young L2 learners might not all catch up to native speakers in this time frame and that non-age factors play a role in determining individual variation in child L2 learners' long-term outcomes with English morphology.
Collapse
|
13
|
Verhagen J, Leseman P. How do verbal short-term memory and working memory relate to the acquisition of vocabulary and grammar? A comparison between first and second language learners. J Exp Child Psychol 2016; 141:65-82. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.06.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 79] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/15/2014] [Revised: 06/25/2015] [Accepted: 06/30/2015] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
14
|
The role of nonverbal working memory in morphosyntactic processing by school-aged monolingual and bilingual children. J Exp Child Psychol 2015; 142:171-94. [PMID: 26550957 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.09.025] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/21/2015] [Revised: 09/24/2015] [Accepted: 09/25/2015] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
The current study examined the relationship between nonverbal working memory and morphosyntactic processing in monolingual native speakers of English and bilingual speakers of English and Spanish. We tested 42 monolingual children and 42 bilingual children between the ages of 8 and 10years matched on age and nonverbal IQ. Children were administered an auditory Grammaticality Judgment task in English to measure morphosyntactic processing and a visual N-Back task and Corsi Blocks task to measure nonverbal working memory capacity. Analyses revealed that monolinguals were more sensitive to English morphosyntactic information than bilinguals, but the groups did not differ in reaction times or response bias. Furthermore, higher nonverbal working memory capacity was associated with greater sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations in bilinguals but not in monolinguals. The findings suggest that nonverbal working memory skills link more tightly to syntactic processing in populations with lower levels of language knowledge.
Collapse
|
15
|
Vugs B, Knoors H, Cuperus J, Hendriks M, Verhoeven L. Interactions between working memory and language in young children with specific language impairment (SLI). Child Neuropsychol 2015; 22:955-78. [DOI: 10.1080/09297049.2015.1058348] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
16
|
Noonan NB, Redmond SM, Archibald LMD. Contributions of children's linguistic and working memory proficiencies to their judgments of grammaticality. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2014; 57:979-989. [PMID: 24686570 DOI: 10.1044/2014_jslhr-l-12-0225] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/03/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE The authors explored the cognitive mechanisms involved in language processing by systematically examining the performance of children with deficits in the domains of working memory and language. METHOD From a database of 370 school-age children who had completed a grammaticality judgment task, groups were identified with a co-occurring language and working memory impairment (LI-WMI; n = 18) or specific language impairment (SLI) with typical working memory skills ( n = 60) and matched control groups. Correct and incorrect use of grammatical markers occurred either early or late in sentence stimuli, imposing a greater working memory load for late-marker sentences. RESULTS Children with SLI showed a lower preference for grammatical items than typically developing controls, regardless of error marker position. Children with LI-WMI demonstrated a performance pattern modulated by error marker position: Their preference for grammatical items was lower than typically developing controls for late but not early marker sentences. CONCLUSION This pattern of results suggests that there are distinct and dissociable impacts of working memory and linguistic skills on metalinguistic functioning through a grammatical judgment task.
Collapse
|
17
|
Pawłowska M, Robinson S, Seddoh A. Detection of lexical and morphological anomalies by children with and without language impairment. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2014; 57:236-246. [PMID: 24687474 DOI: 10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0241)] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/03/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE The abilities of 5-year-old children with and without language impairment (LI) to detect anomalies involving lexical items and grammatical morphemes in stories were compared. The influence of sentence versus discourse context on lexical anomaly detection rates was explored. METHOD The participants were read 3 story scripts and asked to detect the anomalies embedded in them. RESULTS Typically developing (TD) children outperformed their peers with LI across the board. For both groups, lexical anomalies were easier to detect than morphological anomalies. Similarly, anomalous nouns were easier for both groups to detect compared with anomalous verbs. The latter presented a particular challenge to the children with LI. Both groups had greater difficulty with lexical anomaly detection in discourse relative to sentence context. CONCLUSION These outcomes suggest that children's ability to detect anomalies is sensitive to the nature of the anomaly and the linguistic context involved in anomaly detection. Future research may address the relative role of linguistic and cognitive factors in anomaly detection.
Collapse
|
18
|
Chiat S, Roy P. Early predictors of language and social communication impairments at ages 9-11 years: a follow-up study of early-referred children. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2013; 56:1824-1836. [PMID: 23926296 DOI: 10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0249)] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/02/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE In this study, the authors aimed to evaluate hypotheses that early sociocognition will predict later social communication and early phonology will predict later morphosyntax in clinically referred preschoolers. METHOD Participants were 108 children ages 9-11 years who had been referred to clinical services with concerns about language at age 2½-3½ years. Predictors at Time 1 (T1) were measures of sociocognition, word/nonword repetition, and receptive language. Outcome measures at Time 3 (T3) included a social communication questionnaire completed by parents and tests of nonword repetition, morphosyntax, and receptive language. RESULTS Group- and case-level analyses revealed early sociocognition to be the strongest predictor of social communication problems, which by T3 affected almost one third of the sample. At the group level, early phonology, which was a significant problem for the majority of children at T1, was a weak predictor of morphosyntax at T3. However, at the case level the majority of children with poor morphosyntax and nonword repetition at outcome had had very low repetition scores at T1. CONCLUSIONS In early language referrals, it is important to identify and address sociocognitive problems, a considerable risk for later social communication and autism spectrum disorders. The majority of early-referred children had phonological problems, often severe, but these require further investigation to determine their longer term significance for language.
Collapse
|
19
|
Ingvalson EM, McClelland JL, Holt LL. Predicting Native English-Like Performance by Native Japanese Speakers. JOURNAL OF PHONETICS 2011; 39:571-584. [PMID: 22021941 PMCID: PMC3196605 DOI: 10.1016/j.wocn.2011.03.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/16/2023]
Abstract
This study tested the predictions of the Speech Learning Model (SLM, Flege, 1988) on the case of native Japanese (NJ) speakers' perception and production of English /ɹ / and /l/. NJ speakers' degree of foreign accent, intelligibility of /ɹ -l/ productions, and ability to perceive natural speech /ɹ -l/ were assessed as a function of length of residency in North America, age of arrival in North America, years of student status in an English environment, and percentage of Japanese usage. Additionally, the extent to which NJ speakers' utilized the F3 onset cue when differentiating /ɹ -l/ in perception and production was assessed, this cue having previously been shown to be the most reliable indicator of category membership. As predicted, longer residencies predicted more native English-like accents, more intelligible productions, and more accurate natural speech identifications; however, no changes were observed in F3 reliance, indicating that though performance improves it does so through reliance on other cues.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Erin M. Ingvalson
- Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh PA
- Corresponding Author: Communication Sciences and Disorders, Northwestern University, 2240 Campus Drive, Evanston, Illinois 60208, Tel: 847-491-2430, Fax: 847-491-2429,
| | | | - Lori L. Holt
- Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh PA
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Smith PA. Attention, working memory, and grammaticality judgment in typical young adults. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2011; 54:918-931. [PMID: 21106695 DOI: 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/10-0009)] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/30/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE To examine resource allocation and sentence processing, this study examined the effects of auditory distraction on grammaticality judgment (GJ) of sentences varied by semantics (reversibility) and short-term memory requirements. METHOD Experiment 1: Typical young adult females (N = 60) completed a whole-sentence GJ task in distraction (Quiet, Noise, or Talk). Participants judged grammaticality of Passive sentences varied by sentence (length), grammaticality, and reversibility. Reaction time (RT) data were analyzed using a mixed analysis of variance. Experiment 2: A similar group completed a self-paced reading GJ task using the similar materials. RESULTS Experiment 1: Participants responded faster to Bad and to Nonreversible sentences, and in the Talk distraction. The slowest RTs were noted for Good-Reversible-Padded sentences in the Quiet condition. Experiment 2: Distraction did not differentially affect RTs for sentence components. Verb RTs were slower for Reversible sentences. CONCLUSIONS Results suggest that narrative distraction affected GJ, but by speeding responses, not slowing them. Sentence variables of memory and reversibility slowed RTs, but narrative distraction resulted in faster processing times regardless of individual sentence variables. More explicit, deliberate tasks (self-paced reading) resulted in less effect from distraction. Results are discussed in terms of recent theories about auditory distraction.
Collapse
|
21
|
Davidson D, Raschke VR, Pervez J. Syntactic awareness in young monolingual and bilingual (Urdu–English) children. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2010. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.07.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
22
|
Differences in the cognitive demands of word order, plural, and subject-verb agreement constructions. Psychon Bull Rev 2009; 15:980-4. [PMID: 18926992 DOI: 10.3758/pbr.15.5.980] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The syntactic devices of subject-verb-object word order, regular plurals, and subject-verb agreement differ in age of acquisition and susceptibility to error within language-disordered populations. In the present article, the performance of adults on a grammaticality judgment task is used to explore whether such differences are related to working memory (both in terms of an externally imposed load and individual differences in capacity) and phonological ability. The results show that word order, the earliest acquired and most resilient device, is not affected by load, memory span, or phonological ability. Plurals are affected marginally by load and significantly by phonological ability. Agreement, the last acquired and least resilient device, is affected by load, memory span, and phonological ability. Thus, consistent with a processing-based explanation, later acquired and less resilient devices have higher working memory and phonological demands.
Collapse
|