1
|
Sibilio S, Mennella R, Gregorio VD, Rocca AL, Urciuolo F, Imparato G, Netti PA. A novel membrane-on-chip guides morphogenesis for the reconstruction of the intestinal crypt-villus axis. Biofabrication 2024; 16:045019. [PMID: 39029501 DOI: 10.1088/1758-5090/ad6599] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/15/2024] [Accepted: 07/19/2024] [Indexed: 07/21/2024]
Abstract
Reconstructing the microscale villous organisation and functionality of the small intestine is essential for developingin vitroplatforms tailored for absorption studies as well as for investigating intestinal morphogenesis in development and disease. However, the current fabrication techniques able to mimic the villus-crypt axis poses significant challenges in terms of reconstruction of the complex 3D microarchitecture. These challenges extend beyond mere structural intricacies to encompass the incorporation of diverse cell types and the management of intricate fluid dynamics within the system. Here, we introduce a novel microfluidic device calledIn-Crypts, which integrates a cell-instructive membrane aimed at inducing and guiding Caco-2 cells morphogenesis. Patterned topographical cues embossed onto the porous membrane induce the formation of a well-organized intestinal epithelium, characterized by proliferating crypt-like domains and differentiated villus-like regions. Notably, our cell-instructive porous membrane effectively sustains stem cells development, faithfully replicating the niche environment ofin vivointestinal crypts thus mirroring the cell biogeography observedin vivo. Moreover, by introducing dynamic fluid flow, we provide a faithful recapitulation of the native microenvironmental shear stress experienced by the intestinal epithelium. This stress plays a crucial role in influencing cell behaviour, differentiation, and overall functionality, thus offering a highly realistic model for studying intestinal physiology and pathology. The resulting intestinal epithelium exhibits significantly denser regions of mucus and microvilli, characteristic typically absent in static cultures, upregulating more than 1.5 of the amount expressed in the classical flattened configuration, enhanced epithelial cell differentiation and increased adsorptive surface area. Hence, the innovative design ofIn-Cryptsproves the critical role of employing a cell-instructive membrane in argument the physiological relevance of organs-on-chips. This aspect, among others, will contribute to a more comprehensive understanding of organism function, directly impacting drug discovery and development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sara Sibilio
- Department of Chemical, Materials and Industrial Production Engineering, University of Naples Federico II, Naples, Italy
- Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Center for Advanced Biomaterials for HealthCare@CRIB, Naples, Italy
| | - Raffaele Mennella
- Department of Chemical, Materials and Industrial Production Engineering, University of Naples Federico II, Naples, Italy
- Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Center for Advanced Biomaterials for HealthCare@CRIB, Naples, Italy
- University of Naples Federico II, Interdisciplinary Research Centre on Biomaterials (CRIB), Naples, Italy
| | - Vincenza De Gregorio
- Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Center for Advanced Biomaterials for HealthCare@CRIB, Naples, Italy
- Department of Biology, University of Naples Federico II, Naples, Italy
| | - Alessia La Rocca
- University of Naples Federico II, Interdisciplinary Research Centre on Biomaterials (CRIB), Naples, Italy
| | - Francesco Urciuolo
- Department of Chemical, Materials and Industrial Production Engineering, University of Naples Federico II, Naples, Italy
- Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Center for Advanced Biomaterials for HealthCare@CRIB, Naples, Italy
- University of Naples Federico II, Interdisciplinary Research Centre on Biomaterials (CRIB), Naples, Italy
| | - Giorgia Imparato
- University of Naples Federico II, Interdisciplinary Research Centre on Biomaterials (CRIB), Naples, Italy
| | - Paolo A Netti
- Department of Chemical, Materials and Industrial Production Engineering, University of Naples Federico II, Naples, Italy
- Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Center for Advanced Biomaterials for HealthCare@CRIB, Naples, Italy
- University of Naples Federico II, Interdisciplinary Research Centre on Biomaterials (CRIB), Naples, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Karpov G, Lin MH, Headley DB, Baker TE. Oscillatory correlates of threat imminence during virtual navigation. Psychophysiology 2024; 61:e14551. [PMID: 38516942 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14551] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/21/2023] [Revised: 01/18/2024] [Accepted: 02/10/2024] [Indexed: 03/23/2024]
Abstract
The Predatory Imminence Continuum Theory proposes that defensive behaviors depend on the proximity of a threat. While the neural mechanisms underlying this proposal are well studied in animal models, it remains poorly understood in humans. To address this issue, we recorded EEG from 24 (15 female) young adults engaged in a first-person virtual reality Risk-Reward interaction task. On each trial, participants were placed in a virtual room and presented with either a threat or reward conditioned stimulus (CS) in the same room location (proximal) or different room location (distal). Behaviorally, all participants learned to avoid the threat-CS, with most using the optimal behavior to actively avoid the proximal threat-CS (88% accuracy) and passively avoid the distal threat-CS (69% accuracy). Similarly, participants learned to actively approach the distal reward-CS (82% accuracy) and to remain passive to the proximal reward-CS (72% accuracy). At an electrophysiological level, we observed a general increase in theta power (4-8 Hz) over the right posterior channel P8 across all conditions, with the proximal threat-CS evoking the largest theta response. By contrast, distal cues induced two bursts of gamma (30-60 Hz) power over midline-parietal channel Pz (200 msec post-cue) and right frontal channel Fp2 (300 msec post-cue). Interestingly, the first burst of gamma power was sensitive to the distal threat-CS and the second burst at channel Fp2 was sensitive to the distal reward-CS. Together, these findings demonstrate that oscillatory processes differentiate between the spatial proximity information during threat and reward encoding, likely optimizing the selection of the appropriate behavioral response.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Galit Karpov
- Center for Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience, Rutgers State University, Newark, New Jersey, USA
| | - Mei-Heng Lin
- Center for Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience, Rutgers State University, Newark, New Jersey, USA
| | - Drew B Headley
- Center for Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience, Rutgers State University, Newark, New Jersey, USA
| | - Travis E Baker
- Center for Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience, Rutgers State University, Newark, New Jersey, USA
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Subramanian DL, Miller AMP, Smith DM. A comparison of hippocampal and retrosplenial cortical spatial and contextual firing patterns. Hippocampus 2024; 34:357-377. [PMID: 38770779 DOI: 10.1002/hipo.23610] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/16/2023] [Revised: 03/22/2024] [Accepted: 05/07/2024] [Indexed: 05/22/2024]
Abstract
The hippocampus (HPC) and retrosplenial cortex (RSC) are key components of the brain's memory and navigation systems. Lesions of either region produce profound deficits in spatial cognition and HPC neurons exhibit well-known spatial firing patterns (place fields). Recent studies have also identified an array of navigation-related firing patterns in the RSC. However, there has been little work comparing the response properties and information coding mechanisms of these two brain regions. In the present study, we examined the firing patterns of HPC and RSC neurons in two tasks which are commonly used to study spatial cognition in rodents, open field foraging with an environmental context manipulation and continuous T-maze alternation. We found striking similarities in the kinds of spatial and contextual information encoded by these two brain regions. Neurons in both regions carried information about the rat's current spatial location, trajectories and goal locations, and both regions reliably differentiated the contexts. However, we also found several key differences. For example, information about head direction was a prominent component of RSC representations but was only weakly encoded in the HPC. The two regions also used different coding schemes, even when they encoded the same kind of information. As expected, the HPC employed a sparse coding scheme characterized by compact, high contrast place fields, and information about spatial location was the dominant component of HPC representations. RSC firing patterns were more consistent with a distributed coding scheme. Instead of compact place fields, RSC neurons exhibited broad, but reliable, spatial and directional tuning, and they typically carried information about multiple navigational variables. The observed similarities highlight the closely related functions of the HPC and RSC, whereas the differences in information types and coding schemes suggest that these two regions likely make somewhat different contributions to spatial cognition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Adam M P Miller
- Department of Psychology, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York, USA
| | - David M Smith
- Department of Psychology, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York, USA
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Laczó M, Svatkova R, Lerch O, Martinkovic L, Zuntychova T, Nedelska Z, Horakova H, Vyhnalek M, Hort J, Laczó J. Spatial navigation questionnaires as a supportive diagnostic tool in early Alzheimer's disease. iScience 2024; 27:109832. [PMID: 38779476 PMCID: PMC11108981 DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2024.109832] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/10/2023] [Revised: 03/14/2024] [Accepted: 04/24/2024] [Indexed: 05/25/2024] Open
Abstract
Impaired spatial navigation is early marker of Alzheimer's disease (AD). We examined ability of self- and informant-reported navigation questionnaires to discriminate between clinically and biomarker-defined participants, and associations of questionnaires with navigation performance, regional brain atrophy, AD biomarkers, and biomarker status. 262 participants (cognitively normal, with subjective cognitive decline, amnestic mild cognitive impairment [aMCI], and mild dementia) and their informants completed three navigation questionnaires. Navigation performance, magnetic resonance imaging volume/thickness of AD-related brain regions, and AD biomarkers were measured. Informant-reported questionnaires distinguished between cognitively normal and impaired participants, and amyloid-β positive and negative aMCI. Lower scores were associated with worse navigation performance, greater atrophy in AD-related brain regions, and amyloid-β status. Self-reported questionnaire scores did not distinguish between the groups and were weakly associated with navigation performance. Other associations were not significant. Informant-reported navigation questionnaires may be a screening tool for early AD reflecting atrophy of AD-related brain regions and AD pathology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Martina Laczó
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Second Faculty of Medicine, Charles University and Motol University Hospital, Prague, Czechia
| | - Radka Svatkova
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Second Faculty of Medicine, Charles University and Motol University Hospital, Prague, Czechia
| | - Ondrej Lerch
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Second Faculty of Medicine, Charles University and Motol University Hospital, Prague, Czechia
| | - Lukas Martinkovic
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Second Faculty of Medicine, Charles University and Motol University Hospital, Prague, Czechia
| | - Terezie Zuntychova
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Second Faculty of Medicine, Charles University and Motol University Hospital, Prague, Czechia
| | - Zuzana Nedelska
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Second Faculty of Medicine, Charles University and Motol University Hospital, Prague, Czechia
| | - Hana Horakova
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Second Faculty of Medicine, Charles University and Motol University Hospital, Prague, Czechia
| | - Martin Vyhnalek
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Second Faculty of Medicine, Charles University and Motol University Hospital, Prague, Czechia
| | - Jakub Hort
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Second Faculty of Medicine, Charles University and Motol University Hospital, Prague, Czechia
| | - Jan Laczó
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Second Faculty of Medicine, Charles University and Motol University Hospital, Prague, Czechia
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Gonçalves-Garcia M, Barto D, Reyna N, Clark BJ, Hamilton DA. The Prominence of Action Sequences and Behavioral Similarity in the Morris Water Task. Perspect Behav Sci 2024; 47:449-470. [PMID: 39099741 PMCID: PMC11294510 DOI: 10.1007/s40614-024-00402-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 03/05/2024] [Indexed: 08/06/2024] Open
Abstract
The question of What is learned when navigating to a place is reinforced has been the subject of considerable debate. Prevailing views emphasize cognitive structures (e.g., maps) or associative learning, which has shaped measurement in spatial navigation tasks (e.g., the Morris water task [MWT]) toward selection of coarse measures that do not capture precise behaviors of individual animals. We analyzed the navigation paths of 15 rats (60 trials each) in the MWT at high temporal resolution (30Hz) and utilized dynamic time warping to quantify the similarity of paths within and between animals. Paths were largely direct, yet suboptimal, and included changes in speed and trajectory that were established early in training and unique to each animal. Individual rats executed similar paths from the same release point from trial to trial, which were distinct from paths executed by other rats as well as paths performed by the same rat from other release points. These observations suggest that rats learn to execute similar path sequences from trial to trial for each release point in the MWT. Occasional spontaneous deviations from the established, unique behavioral sequence, resulted in profound disruption in navigation accuracy. We discuss the potential implications of sequence navigation behaviors for understanding relations between behavior and spatial neural signals such as place cells, grid cells, and head direction cells. Supplementary Information The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s40614-024-00402-8.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mônica Gonçalves-Garcia
- Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, MSC03 2220, 1, Albuquerque, NM 87131 USA
| | - Daniel Barto
- Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, MSC03 2220, 1, Albuquerque, NM 87131 USA
| | - Nicole Reyna
- Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, MSC03 2220, 1, Albuquerque, NM 87131 USA
| | - Benjamin J. Clark
- Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, MSC03 2220, 1, Albuquerque, NM 87131 USA
| | - Derek A. Hamilton
- Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, MSC03 2220, 1, Albuquerque, NM 87131 USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Sigismondi F, Xu Y, Silvestri M, Bottini R. Altered grid-like coding in early blind people. Nat Commun 2024; 15:3476. [PMID: 38658530 PMCID: PMC11043432 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-024-47747-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/10/2023] [Accepted: 04/09/2024] [Indexed: 04/26/2024] Open
Abstract
Cognitive maps in the hippocampal-entorhinal system are central for the representation of both spatial and non-spatial relationships. Although this system, especially in humans, heavily relies on vision, the role of visual experience in shaping the development of cognitive maps remains largely unknown. Here, we test sighted and early blind individuals in both imagined navigation in fMRI and real-world navigation. During imagined navigation, the Human Navigation Network, constituted by frontal, medial temporal, and parietal cortices, is reliably activated in both groups, showing resilience to visual deprivation. However, neural geometry analyses highlight crucial differences between groups. A 60° rotational symmetry, characteristic of a hexagonal grid-like coding, emerges in the entorhinal cortex of sighted but not blind people, who instead show a 90° (4-fold) symmetry, indicative of a square grid. Moreover, higher parietal cortex activity during navigation in blind people correlates with the magnitude of 4-fold symmetry. In sum, early blindness can alter the geometry of entorhinal cognitive maps, possibly as a consequence of higher reliance on parietal egocentric coding during navigation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Yangwen Xu
- Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, 38122, Trento, Italy
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, D-04303, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Mattia Silvestri
- Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, 38122, Trento, Italy
| | - Roberto Bottini
- Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, 38122, Trento, Italy.
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Hoang TH, Manahan-Vaughan D. Differentiated somatic gene expression is triggered in the dorsal hippocampus and the anterior retrosplenial cortex by hippocampal synaptic plasticity prompted by spatial content learning. Brain Struct Funct 2024; 229:639-655. [PMID: 37690045 PMCID: PMC10978647 DOI: 10.1007/s00429-023-02694-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/05/2023] [Accepted: 08/07/2023] [Indexed: 09/12/2023]
Abstract
Hippocampal afferent inputs, terminating on proximal and distal subfields of the cornus ammonis (CA), enable the functional discrimination of 'what' (item identity) and 'where' (spatial location) elements of a spatial representation. This kind of information is supported by structures such as the retrosplenial cortex (RSC). Spatial content learning promotes the expression of hippocampal synaptic plasticity, particularly long-term depression (LTD). In the CA1 region, this is specifically facilitated by the learning of item-place features of a spatial environment. Gene-tagging, by means of time-locked fluorescence in situ hybridization (FISH) to detect nuclear expression of immediate early genes, can reveal neuronal populations that engage in experience-dependent information encoding. In the current study, using FISH, we examined if learning-facilitated LTD results in subfield-specific information encoding in the hippocampus and RSC. Rats engaged in novel exploration of small items during stimulation of Schaffer collateral-CA1 synapses. This resulted in LTD (> 24 h). FISH, to detect nuclear expression of Homer1a, revealed that the distal-CA1 and proximal-CA3 subcompartments were particularly activated by this event. By contrast, all elements of the proximodistal cornus ammonis-axis showed equal nuclear Homer1a expression following LTD induction solely by means of afferent stimulation. The RSC exhibited stronger nuclear Homer1a expression in response to learning-facilitated LTD, and to novel item-place experience, compared to LTD induced by sole afferent stimulation in CA1. These results show that both the cornus ammonis and RSC engage in differentiated information encoding of item-place learning that is salient enough, in its own right, to drive the expression of hippocampal LTD. These results also reveal a novel role of the RSC in item-place learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thu-Huong Hoang
- Medical Faculty, Department of Neurophysiology, Ruhr University Bochum, Universitätsstr. 150, MA 4/150, 44780, Bochum, Germany
| | - Denise Manahan-Vaughan
- Medical Faculty, Department of Neurophysiology, Ruhr University Bochum, Universitätsstr. 150, MA 4/150, 44780, Bochum, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Cheng N, Dong Q, Zhang Z, Wang L, Chen X, Wang C. Egocentric processing of items in spines, dendrites, and somas in the retrosplenial cortex. Neuron 2024; 112:646-660.e8. [PMID: 38101396 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2023.11.018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/23/2023] [Revised: 08/31/2023] [Accepted: 11/15/2023] [Indexed: 12/17/2023]
Abstract
Egocentric representations of external items are essential for spatial navigation and memory. Here, we explored the neural mechanisms underlying egocentric processing in the retrosplenial cortex (RSC), a pivotal area for memory and navigation. Using one-photon and two-photon calcium imaging, we identified egocentric tuning for environment boundaries in dendrites, spines, and somas of RSC neurons (egocentric boundary cells) in the open-field task. Dendrites with egocentric tuning tended to have similarly tuned spines. We further identified egocentric neurons representing landmarks in a virtual navigation task or remembered cue location in a goal-oriented task, respectively. These neurons formed an independent population with egocentric boundary cells, suggesting that dedicated neurons with microscopic clustering of functional inputs shaped egocentric boundary processing in RSC and that RSC adopted a labeled line code with distinct classes of egocentric neurons responsible for representing different items in specific behavioral contexts, which could lead to efficient and flexible computation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ning Cheng
- Shenzhen Key Laboratory of Precision Diagnosis and Treatment of Depression, Shenzhen-Hong Kong Institute of Brain Science, Shenzhen Institute of Advanced Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shenzhen 518055, China; CAS Key Laboratory of Brain Connectome and Manipulation, the Brain Cognition and Brain Disease Institute, Shenzhen Institute of Advanced Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shenzhen 518055, China; Guangdong Provincial Key Laboratory of Brain Connectome and Behavior, the Brain Cognition and Brain Disease Institute, Shenzhen Institute of Advanced Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shenzhen 518055, China
| | - Qiqi Dong
- Shenzhen Key Laboratory of Precision Diagnosis and Treatment of Depression, Shenzhen-Hong Kong Institute of Brain Science, Shenzhen Institute of Advanced Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shenzhen 518055, China; CAS Key Laboratory of Brain Connectome and Manipulation, the Brain Cognition and Brain Disease Institute, Shenzhen Institute of Advanced Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shenzhen 518055, China; Guangdong Provincial Key Laboratory of Brain Connectome and Behavior, the Brain Cognition and Brain Disease Institute, Shenzhen Institute of Advanced Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shenzhen 518055, China
| | - Zhen Zhang
- CAS Key Laboratory of Brain Connectome and Manipulation, the Brain Cognition and Brain Disease Institute, Shenzhen Institute of Advanced Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shenzhen 518055, China; Guangdong Provincial Key Laboratory of Brain Connectome and Behavior, the Brain Cognition and Brain Disease Institute, Shenzhen Institute of Advanced Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shenzhen 518055, China
| | - Li Wang
- Brain Research Centre, Department of Biology, School of Life Sciences, Southern University of Science and Technology, Shenzhen 518055, China
| | - Xiaojing Chen
- Brain Research Centre, Department of Biology, School of Life Sciences, Southern University of Science and Technology, Shenzhen 518055, China.
| | - Cheng Wang
- Shenzhen Key Laboratory of Precision Diagnosis and Treatment of Depression, Shenzhen-Hong Kong Institute of Brain Science, Shenzhen Institute of Advanced Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shenzhen 518055, China; CAS Key Laboratory of Brain Connectome and Manipulation, the Brain Cognition and Brain Disease Institute, Shenzhen Institute of Advanced Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shenzhen 518055, China; Guangdong Provincial Key Laboratory of Brain Connectome and Behavior, the Brain Cognition and Brain Disease Institute, Shenzhen Institute of Advanced Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shenzhen 518055, China; CAS Centre for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligent Technology, Shanghai, China.
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Parrini M, Tricot G, Caroni P, Spolidoro M. Circuit mechanisms of navigation strategy learning in mice. Curr Biol 2024; 34:79-91.e4. [PMID: 38101403 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2023.11.047] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/21/2023] [Revised: 10/09/2023] [Accepted: 11/22/2023] [Indexed: 12/17/2023]
Abstract
Navigation tasks involve the gradual selection and deployment of increasingly effective searching procedures to reach targets. The brain mechanisms underlying such complex behavior are poorly understood, but their elucidation might provide insights into the systems linking exploration and decision making in complex learning. Here, we developed a trial-by-trial goal-related search strategy analysis as mice learned to navigate identical water mazes encompassing distinct goal-related rules and monitored the strategy deployment process throughout learning. We found that navigation learning involved the following three distinct phases: an early phase during which maze-specific search strategies are deployed in a minority of trials, a second phase of preferential increasing deployment of one search strategy, and a final phase of increasing commitment to this strategy only. The three maze learning phases were affected differently by inhibition of retrosplenial cortex (RSC), dorsomedial striatum (DMS), or dorsolateral striatum (DLS). Through brain region-specific inactivation experiments and gain-of-function experiments involving activation of learning-related cFos+ ensembles, we unraveled how goal-related strategy selection relates to deployment throughout these sequential processes. We found that RSC is critically important for search strategy selection, DMS mediates strategy deployment, and DLS ensures searching consistency throughout maze learning. Notably, activation of specific learning-related ensembles was sufficient to direct strategy selection (RSC) or strategy deployment (DMS) in a different maze. Our results establish a goal-related search strategy deployment approach to dissect unsupervised navigation learning processes and suggest that effective searching in navigation involves evidence-based goal-related strategy direction by RSC, reinforcement-modulated strategy deployment through DMS, and online guidance through DLS.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Martina Parrini
- Friedrich Miescher Institute for Biomedical Research, 4058 Basel, Switzerland
| | - Guillaume Tricot
- Friedrich Miescher Institute for Biomedical Research, 4058 Basel, Switzerland
| | - Pico Caroni
- Friedrich Miescher Institute for Biomedical Research, 4058 Basel, Switzerland.
| | - Maria Spolidoro
- Friedrich Miescher Institute for Biomedical Research, 4058 Basel, Switzerland.
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Simmons CM, Moseley SC, Ogg JD, Zhou X, Johnson M, Wu W, Clark BJ, Wilber AA. A thalamo-parietal cortex circuit is critical for place-action coordination. Hippocampus 2023; 33:1252-1266. [PMID: 37811797 PMCID: PMC10872801 DOI: 10.1002/hipo.23578] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/30/2022] [Revised: 08/28/2023] [Accepted: 09/18/2023] [Indexed: 10/10/2023]
Abstract
The anterior and lateral thalamus (ALT) contains head direction cells that signal the directional orientation of an individual within the environment. ALT has direct and indirect connections with the parietal cortex (PC), an area hypothesized to play a role in coordinating viewer-dependent and viewer-independent spatial reference frames. This coordination between reference frames would allow an individual to translate movements toward a desired location from memory. Thus, ALT-PC functional connectivity would be critical for moving toward remembered allocentric locations. This hypothesis was tested in rats with a place-action task that requires associating an appropriate action (left or right turn) with a spatial location. There are four arms, each offset by 90°, positioned around a central starting point. A trial begins in the central starting point. After exiting a pseudorandomly selected arm, the rat had to displace the correct object covering one of two (left versus right) feeding stations to receive a reward. For a pair of arms facing opposite directions, the reward was located on the left, and for the other pair, the reward was located on the right. Thus, each reward location had a different combination of allocentric location and egocentric action. Removal of an object was scored as correct or incorrect. Trials in which the rat did not displace any objects were scored as "no selection" trials. After an object was removed, the rat returned to the center starting position and the maze was reset for the next trial. To investigate the role of the ALT-PC network, muscimol inactivation infusions targeted bilateral PC, bilateral ALT, or the ALT-PC network. Muscimol sessions were counterbalanced and compared to saline sessions within the same animal. All inactivations resulted in decreased accuracy, but only bilateral PC inactivations resulted in increased non selecting, increased errors, and longer latency responses on the remaining trials. Thus, the ALT-PC circuit is critical for linking an action with a spatial location for successful navigation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christine M Simmons
- Department of Psychology, Program of Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida, USA
| | - Shawn C Moseley
- Department of Psychology, Program of Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida, USA
| | - Jordan D Ogg
- Department of Psychology, Program of Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida, USA
| | - Xinyu Zhou
- Department of Statistics, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida, USA
| | - Madeline Johnson
- Department of Psychology, Program of Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida, USA
| | - Wei Wu
- Department of Statistics, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida, USA
| | - Benjamin J Clark
- Department of Psychology, The University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, New Mexico, USA
| | - Aaron A Wilber
- Department of Psychology, Program of Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida, USA
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Iggena D, Jeung S, Maier PM, Ploner CJ, Gramann K, Finke C. Multisensory input modulates memory-guided spatial navigation in humans. Commun Biol 2023; 6:1167. [PMID: 37963986 PMCID: PMC10646091 DOI: 10.1038/s42003-023-05522-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/05/2023] [Accepted: 10/30/2023] [Indexed: 11/16/2023] Open
Abstract
Efficient navigation is supported by a cognitive map of space. The hippocampus plays a key role for this map by linking multimodal sensory information with spatial memory representations. However, in human navigation studies, the full range of sensory information is often unavailable due to the stationarity of experimental setups. We investigated the contribution of multisensory information to memory-guided spatial navigation by presenting a virtual version of the Morris water maze on a screen and in an immersive mobile virtual reality setup. Patients with hippocampal lesions and matched controls navigated to memorized object locations in relation to surrounding landmarks. Our results show that availability of multisensory input improves memory-guided spatial navigation in both groups. It has distinct effects on navigational behaviour, with greater improvement in spatial memory performance in patients. We conclude that congruent multisensory information shifts computations to extrahippocampal areas that support spatial navigation and compensates for spatial navigation deficits.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Deetje Iggena
- Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Department of Neurology, Augustenburger Platz 1, 13353, Berlin, Germany.
- Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Unter den Linden 6, 10099, Berlin, Germany.
| | - Sein Jeung
- Technische Universität Berlin, Department of Biological Psychology and Neuroergonomics, Fasanenstraße 1, 10623, Berlin, Germany
- Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Kavli Institute for Systems Neuroscience, Olav Kyrres gate 9,7030, Trondheim, Norway
- Max-Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Stephanstraße 1a, 04103, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Patrizia M Maier
- Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Department of Neurology, Augustenburger Platz 1, 13353, Berlin, Germany
- Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Unter den Linden 6, 10099, Berlin, Germany
| | - Christoph J Ploner
- Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Department of Neurology, Augustenburger Platz 1, 13353, Berlin, Germany
| | - Klaus Gramann
- Technische Universität Berlin, Department of Biological Psychology and Neuroergonomics, Fasanenstraße 1, 10623, Berlin, Germany
- University of California, San Diego, Center for Advanced Neurological Engineering, 9500 Gilman Dr, La Jolla, CA, 92093, USA
| | - Carsten Finke
- Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Department of Neurology, Augustenburger Platz 1, 13353, Berlin, Germany
- Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Unter den Linden 6, 10099, Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Mertens PEC, Marchesi P, Ruikes TR, Oude Lohuis M, Krijger Q, Pennartz CMA, Lansink CS. Coherent mapping of position and head direction across auditory and visual cortex. Cereb Cortex 2023; 33:7369-7385. [PMID: 36967108 PMCID: PMC10267650 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhad045] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/07/2022] [Revised: 01/31/2023] [Accepted: 02/01/2023] [Indexed: 09/21/2024] Open
Abstract
Neurons in primary visual cortex (V1) may not only signal current visual input but also relevant contextual information such as reward expectancy and the subject's spatial position. Such contextual representations need not be restricted to V1 but could participate in a coherent mapping throughout sensory cortices. Here, we show that spiking activity coherently represents a location-specific mapping across auditory cortex (AC) and lateral, secondary visual cortex (V2L) of freely moving rats engaged in a sensory detection task on a figure-8 maze. Single-unit activity of both areas showed extensive similarities in terms of spatial distribution, reliability, and position coding. Importantly, reconstructions of subject position based on spiking activity displayed decoding errors that were correlated between areas. Additionally, we found that head direction, but not locomotor speed or head angular velocity, was an important determinant of activity in AC and V2L. By contrast, variables related to the sensory task cues or to trial correctness and reward were not markedly encoded in AC and V2L. We conclude that sensory cortices participate in coherent, multimodal representations of the subject's sensory-specific location. These may provide a common reference frame for distributed cortical sensory and motor processes and may support crossmodal predictive processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paul E C Mertens
- Center for Neuroscience, Faculty of Science, Swammerdam Institute for Life Sciences, University of Amsterdam, Science Park 904, Amsterdam 1098 XH, The Netherlands
| | - Pietro Marchesi
- Center for Neuroscience, Faculty of Science, Swammerdam Institute for Life Sciences, University of Amsterdam, Science Park 904, Amsterdam 1098 XH, The Netherlands
| | - Thijs R Ruikes
- Center for Neuroscience, Faculty of Science, Swammerdam Institute for Life Sciences, University of Amsterdam, Science Park 904, Amsterdam 1098 XH, The Netherlands
| | - Matthijs Oude Lohuis
- Center for Neuroscience, Faculty of Science, Swammerdam Institute for Life Sciences, University of Amsterdam, Science Park 904, Amsterdam 1098 XH, The Netherlands
- Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Foundation, Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Quincy Krijger
- Center for Neuroscience, Faculty of Science, Swammerdam Institute for Life Sciences, University of Amsterdam, Science Park 904, Amsterdam 1098 XH, The Netherlands
| | - Cyriel M A Pennartz
- Center for Neuroscience, Faculty of Science, Swammerdam Institute for Life Sciences, University of Amsterdam, Science Park 904, Amsterdam 1098 XH, The Netherlands
- Research Priority Program Brain and Cognition, University of Amsterdam, Science Park 904, Amsterdam 1098 XH, The Netherlands
| | - Carien S Lansink
- Center for Neuroscience, Faculty of Science, Swammerdam Institute for Life Sciences, University of Amsterdam, Science Park 904, Amsterdam 1098 XH, The Netherlands
- Research Priority Program Brain and Cognition, University of Amsterdam, Science Park 904, Amsterdam 1098 XH, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Kira S, Safaai H, Morcos AS, Panzeri S, Harvey CD. A distributed and efficient population code of mixed selectivity neurons for flexible navigation decisions. Nat Commun 2023; 14:2121. [PMID: 37055431 PMCID: PMC10102117 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-023-37804-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/07/2021] [Accepted: 03/30/2023] [Indexed: 04/15/2023] Open
Abstract
Decision-making requires flexibility to rapidly switch one's actions in response to sensory stimuli depending on information stored in memory. We identified cortical areas and neural activity patterns underlying this flexibility during virtual navigation, where mice switched navigation toward or away from a visual cue depending on its match to a remembered cue. Optogenetics screening identified V1, posterior parietal cortex (PPC), and retrosplenial cortex (RSC) as necessary for accurate decisions. Calcium imaging revealed neurons that can mediate rapid navigation switches by encoding a mixture of a current and remembered visual cue. These mixed selectivity neurons emerged through task learning and predicted the mouse's choices by forming efficient population codes before correct, but not incorrect, choices. They were distributed across posterior cortex, even V1, and were densest in RSC and sparsest in PPC. We propose flexibility in navigation decisions arises from neurons that mix visual and memory information within a visual-parietal-retrosplenial network.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shinichiro Kira
- Department of Neurobiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Houman Safaai
- Department of Neurobiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
- Neural Computation Laboratory, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Rovereto, Italy
| | - Ari S Morcos
- Department of Neurobiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Stefano Panzeri
- Neural Computation Laboratory, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Rovereto, Italy
- Department of Excellence for Neural Information Processing, Center for Molecular Neurobiology (ZMNH), University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf (UKE), Hamburg, Germany
| | | |
Collapse
|
14
|
Alexander AS, Place R, Starrett MJ, Chrastil ER, Nitz DA. Rethinking retrosplenial cortex: Perspectives and predictions. Neuron 2023; 111:150-175. [PMID: 36460006 PMCID: PMC11709228 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2022.11.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 22.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/09/2022] [Revised: 08/09/2022] [Accepted: 11/06/2022] [Indexed: 12/03/2022]
Abstract
The last decade has produced exciting new ideas about retrosplenial cortex (RSC) and its role in integrating diverse inputs. Here, we review the diversity in forms of spatial and directional tuning of RSC activity, temporal organization of RSC activity, and features of RSC interconnectivity with other brain structures. We find that RSC anatomy and dynamics are more consistent with roles in multiple sensorimotor and cognitive processes than with any isolated function. However, two more generalized categories of function may best characterize roles for RSC in complex cognitive processes: (1) shifting and relating perspectives for spatial cognition and (2) prediction and error correction for current sensory states with internal representations of the environment. Both functions likely take advantage of RSC's capacity to encode conjunctions among sensory, motor, and spatial mapping information streams. Together, these functions provide the scaffold for intelligent actions, such as navigation, perspective taking, interaction with others, and error detection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrew S Alexander
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, MA 02215, USA
| | - Ryan Place
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA
| | - Michael J Starrett
- Department of Neurobiology & Behavior, University of California, Irvine, Irvine, CA 92697, USA
| | - Elizabeth R Chrastil
- Department of Neurobiology & Behavior, University of California, Irvine, Irvine, CA 92697, USA; Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California, Irvine, Irvine, CA 92697, USA.
| | - Douglas A Nitz
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Rogojin A, Gorbet DJ, Hawkins KM, Sergio LE. Differences in structural MRI and diffusion tensor imaging underlie visuomotor performance declines in older adults with an increased risk for Alzheimer's disease. Front Aging Neurosci 2023; 14:1054516. [PMID: 36711200 PMCID: PMC9877535 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.1054516] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/26/2022] [Accepted: 12/26/2022] [Indexed: 01/13/2023] Open
Abstract
Introduction Visuomotor impairments have been demonstrated in preclinical AD in individuals with a positive family history of dementia and APOE e4 carriers. Previous behavioral findings have also reported sex-differences in performance of visuomotor tasks involving a visual feedback reversal. The current study investigated the relationship between grey and white matter changes and non-standard visuomotor performance, as well as the effects of APOE status, family history of dementia, and sex on these brain-behavior relationships. Methods Older adults (n = 49) with no cognitive impairments completed non-standard visuomotor tasks involving a visual feedback reversal, plane-change, or combination of the two. Participants with a family history of dementia or who were APOE e4 carriers were considered at an increased risk for AD. T1-weighted anatomical scans were used to quantify grey matter volume and thickness, and diffusion tensor imaging measures were used to quantify white matter integrity. Results In APOE e4 carriers, grey and white matter structural measures were associated with visuomotor performance. Regression analyses showed that visuomotor deficits were predicted by lower grey matter thickness and volume in areas of the medial temporal lobe previously implicated in visuomotor control (entorhinal and parahippocampal cortices). This finding was replicated in the diffusion data, where regression analyses revealed that lower white matter integrity (lower FA, higher MD, higher RD, higher AxD) was a significant predictor of worse visuomotor performance in the forceps minor, forceps major, cingulum, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (IFOF), inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF), superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF), and uncinate fasciculus (UF). Some of these tracts overlap with those important for visuomotor integration, namely the forceps minor, forceps major, SLF, IFOF, and ILF. Conclusion These findings suggest that measuring the dysfunction of brain networks underlying visuomotor control in early-stage AD may provide a novel behavioral target for dementia risk detection that is easily accessible, non-invasive, and cost-effective. The results also provide insight into the structural differences in inferior parietal lobule that may underlie previously reported sex-differences in performance of the visual feedback reversal task.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alica Rogojin
- School of Kinesiology and Health Science, York University, Toronto, ON, Canada,Centre for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, ON, Canada,Vision: Science to Applications (VISTA) Program, York University, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Diana J. Gorbet
- School of Kinesiology and Health Science, York University, Toronto, ON, Canada,Centre for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Kara M. Hawkins
- School of Kinesiology and Health Science, York University, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Lauren E. Sergio
- School of Kinesiology and Health Science, York University, Toronto, ON, Canada,Centre for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, ON, Canada,*Correspondence: Lauren E. Sergio, ✉
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Jeung S, Hilton C, Berg T, Gehrke L, Gramann K. Virtual Reality for Spatial Navigation. Curr Top Behav Neurosci 2023; 65:103-129. [PMID: 36512288 DOI: 10.1007/7854_2022_403] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/17/2023]
Abstract
Immersive virtual reality (VR) allows its users to experience physical space in a non-physical world. It has developed into a powerful research tool to investigate the neural basis of human spatial navigation as an embodied experience. The task of wayfinding can be carried out by using a wide range of strategies, leading to the recruitment of various sensory modalities and brain areas in real-life scenarios. While traditional desktop-based VR setups primarily focus on vision-based navigation, immersive VR setups, especially mobile variants, can efficiently account for motor processes that constitute locomotion in the physical world, such as head-turning and walking. When used in combination with mobile neuroimaging methods, immersive VR affords a natural mode of locomotion and high immersion in experimental settings, designing an embodied spatial experience. This in turn facilitates ecologically valid investigation of the neural underpinnings of spatial navigation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sein Jeung
- Department of Biological Psychology and Neuroergonomics, Technische Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany
- Kavli Institute for Systems Neuroscience, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Christopher Hilton
- Department of Biological Psychology and Neuroergonomics, Technische Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Timotheus Berg
- Department of Biological Psychology and Neuroergonomics, Technische Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Lukas Gehrke
- Department of Biological Psychology and Neuroergonomics, Technische Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Klaus Gramann
- Department of Biological Psychology and Neuroergonomics, Technische Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany.
- Center for Advanced Neurological Engineering, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Linking global top-down views to first-person views in the brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2022; 119:e2202024119. [PMID: 36322732 PMCID: PMC9659407 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2202024119] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/25/2023] Open
Abstract
Humans and other animals have a remarkable capacity to translate their position from one spatial frame of reference to another. The ability to seamlessly move between top-down and first-person views is important for navigation, memory formation, and other cognitive tasks. Evidence suggests that the medial temporal lobe and other cortical regions contribute to this function. To understand how a neural system might carry out these computations, we used variational autoencoders (VAEs) to reconstruct the first-person view from the top-down view of a robot simulation, and vice versa. Many latent variables in the VAEs had similar responses to those seen in neuron recordings, including location-specific activity, head direction tuning, and encoding of distance to local objects. Place-specific responses were prominent when reconstructing a first-person view from a top-down view, but head direction-specific responses were prominent when reconstructing a top-down view from a first-person view. In both cases, the model could recover from perturbations without retraining, but rather through remapping. These results could advance our understanding of how brain regions support viewpoint linkages and transformations.
Collapse
|
18
|
Vijayabaskaran S, Cheng S. Navigation task and action space drive the emergence of egocentric and allocentric spatial representations. PLoS Comput Biol 2022; 18:e1010320. [PMID: 36315587 PMCID: PMC9648855 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1010320] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/21/2022] [Revised: 11/10/2022] [Accepted: 10/18/2022] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
In general, strategies for spatial navigation could employ one of two spatial reference frames: egocentric or allocentric. Notwithstanding intuitive explanations, it remains unclear however under what circumstances one strategy is chosen over another, and how neural representations should be related to the chosen strategy. Here, we first use a deep reinforcement learning model to investigate whether a particular type of navigation strategy arises spontaneously during spatial learning without imposing a bias onto the model. We then examine the spatial representations that emerge in the network to support navigation. To this end, we study two tasks that are ethologically valid for mammals—guidance, where the agent has to navigate to a goal location fixed in allocentric space, and aiming, where the agent navigates to a visible cue. We find that when both navigation strategies are available to the agent, the solutions it develops for guidance and aiming are heavily biased towards the allocentric or the egocentric strategy, respectively, as one might expect. Nevertheless, the agent can learn both tasks using either type of strategy. Furthermore, we find that place-cell-like allocentric representations emerge preferentially in guidance when using an allocentric strategy, whereas egocentric vector representations emerge when using an egocentric strategy in aiming. We thus find that alongside the type of navigational strategy, the nature of the task plays a pivotal role in the type of spatial representations that emerge. Most species rely on navigation in space to find water, food, and mates, as well as to return home. When navigating, humans and animals can use one of two reference frames: one based on stable landmarks in the external environment, such as moving due north and then east, or one centered on oneself, such as moving forward and turning left. However, it remains unclear how these reference frames are chosen and interact in navigation tasks, as well as how they are supported by representations in the brain. We therefore modeled two navigation tasks that would each benefit from using one of these reference frames, and trained an artificial agent to learn to solve them through trial and error. Our results show that when given the choice, the agent leveraged the appropriate reference frame to solve the task, but surprisingly could also use the other reference frame when constrained to do so. We also show that the representations that emerge to enable the agent to solve the tasks exist on a spectrum, and are more complex than commonly thought. These representations reflect both the task and reference frame being used, and provide useful insights for the design of experimental tasks to study the use of navigational strategies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Sen Cheng
- Faculty of Computer Science, Ruhr University Bochum, Bochum, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Liu J, Singh AK, Lin CT. Predicting the Quality of Spatial Learning via Virtual Global Landmarks. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng 2022; 30:2418-2425. [PMID: 35981074 DOI: 10.1109/tnsre.2022.3199713] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
Analyzing the effects landmarks have on spatial learning is an active area of research in the study of human navigation processes and one that is key to understanding the links between human brain dynamics, landmark encoding, and spatial learning outcomes. This article presents a study on whether electroencephalography (EEG) signals related to virtual global landmarks combined with deep learning can be used to predict the accuracy and efficacy of spatial learning. Virtual global landmarks are silhouettes of actual landmarks projected into the navigator's vision via a heads-up display. They serve as a notable frame of reference in addition to the local landmarks we all typically use for route navigation. From a mobile virtual reality scenario involving 55 participants, the results of the study suggest that the EEG data associated with those who were exposed to global landmarks shows a visibly better capacity for predicting the quality of spatial learning levels than those who were not. As such, the EEG features associated with processing VGLs have a greater functional relation to the quality of spatial learning. This finding opens up a future direction of enquiry into landmark encoding and navigational ability. It may also provide a potential avenue for the early diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease.
Collapse
|
20
|
Liu J, Singh AK, Wunderlich A, Gramann K, Lin CT. Redesigning navigational aids using virtual global landmarks to improve spatial knowledge retrieval. NPJ SCIENCE OF LEARNING 2022; 7:17. [PMID: 35853945 PMCID: PMC9296625 DOI: 10.1038/s41539-022-00132-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/15/2021] [Accepted: 07/01/2022] [Indexed: 06/15/2023]
Abstract
Although beacon- and map-based spatial strategies are the default strategies for navigation activities, today's navigational aids mostly follow a beacon-based design where one is provided with turn-by-turn instructions. Recent research, however, shows that our reliance on these navigational aids is causing a decline in our spatial skills. We are processing less of our surrounding environment and relying too heavily on the instructions given. To reverse this decline, we need to engage more in map-based learning, which encourages the user to process and integrate spatial knowledge into a cognitive map built to benefit flexible and independent spatial navigation behaviour. In an attempt to curb our loss of skills, we proposed a navigation assistant to support map-based learning during active navigation. Called the virtual global landmark (VGL) system, this augmented reality (AR) system is based on the kinds of techniques used in traditional orienteering. Specifically, a notable landmark is always present in the user's sight, allowing the user to continuously compute where they are in relation to that specific location. The efficacy of the unit as a navigational aid was tested in an experiment with 27 students from the University of Technology Sydney via a comparison of brain dynamics and behaviour. From an analysis of behaviour and event-related spectral perturbation, we found that participants were encouraged to process more spatial information with a map-based strategy where a silhouette of the compass-like landmark was perpetually in view. As a result of this technique, they consistently navigated with greater efficiency and better accuracy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jia Liu
- CIBCI Centre, Australian AI Institute, School of Computer Science, Faculty of Engineering and Information Technology, University of Technology Sydney, Ultimo, NSW, Australia
| | - Avinash Kumar Singh
- CIBCI Centre, Australian AI Institute, School of Computer Science, Faculty of Engineering and Information Technology, University of Technology Sydney, Ultimo, NSW, Australia.
| | - Anna Wunderlich
- Biological Psychology and Neuroergonomics, Berlin Institute of Technology, Berlin, Germany
| | - Klaus Gramann
- CIBCI Centre, Australian AI Institute, School of Computer Science, Faculty of Engineering and Information Technology, University of Technology Sydney, Ultimo, NSW, Australia
- Biological Psychology and Neuroergonomics, Berlin Institute of Technology, Berlin, Germany
| | - Chin-Teng Lin
- CIBCI Centre, Australian AI Institute, School of Computer Science, Faculty of Engineering and Information Technology, University of Technology Sydney, Ultimo, NSW, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Laczó M, Martinkovic L, Lerch O, Wiener JM, Kalinova J, Matuskova V, Nedelska Z, Vyhnalek M, Hort J, Laczó J. Different Profiles of Spatial Navigation Deficits In Alzheimer’s Disease Biomarker-Positive Versus Biomarker-Negative Older Adults With Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment. Front Aging Neurosci 2022; 14:886778. [PMID: 35721017 PMCID: PMC9201637 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.886778] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/28/2022] [Accepted: 04/29/2022] [Indexed: 12/22/2022] Open
Abstract
BackgroundSpatial navigation impairment is a promising cognitive marker of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) that can reflect the underlying pathology.ObjectivesWe assessed spatial navigation performance in AD biomarker positive older adults with amnestic mild cognitive impairment (AD aMCI) vs. those AD biomarker negative (non-AD aMCI), and examined associations between navigation performance, MRI measures of brain atrophy, and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) biomarkers.MethodsA total of 122 participants with AD aMCI (n = 33), non-AD aMCI (n = 31), mild AD dementia (n = 28), and 30 cognitively normal older adults (CN) underwent cognitive assessment, brain MRI (n = 100 had high-quality images for volumetric analysis) and three virtual navigation tasks focused on route learning (body-centered navigation), wayfinding (world-centered navigation) and perspective taking/wayfinding. Cognitively impaired participants underwent CSF biomarker assessment [amyloid-β1–42, total tau, and phosphorylated tau181 (p-tau181)] and amyloid PET imaging (n = 47 and n = 45, respectively), with a subset having both (n = 19).ResultsIn route learning, AD aMCI performed worse than non-AD aMCI (p < 0.001), who performed similarly to CN. In wayfinding, aMCI participants performed worse than CN (both p ≤ 0.009) and AD aMCI performed worse than non-AD aMCI in the second task session (p = 0.032). In perspective taking/wayfinding, aMCI participants performed worse than CN (both p ≤ 0.001). AD aMCI and non-AD aMCI did not differ in conventional cognitive tests. Route learning was associated with parietal thickness and amyloid-β1–42, wayfinding was associated with posterior medial temporal lobe (MTL) volume and p-tau181 and perspective taking/wayfinding was correlated with MRI measures of several brain regions and all CSF biomarkers.ConclusionAD biomarker positive and negative older adults with aMCI had different profiles of spatial navigation deficits that were associated with posterior MTL and parietal atrophy and reflected AD pathology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Martina Laczó
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Charles University, Second Faculty of Medicine and Motol University Hospital, Prague, Czechia
| | - Lukas Martinkovic
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Charles University, Second Faculty of Medicine and Motol University Hospital, Prague, Czechia
| | - Ondrej Lerch
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Charles University, Second Faculty of Medicine and Motol University Hospital, Prague, Czechia
- International Clinical Research Center, St. Anne’s University Hospital Brno, Brno, Czechia
| | - Jan M. Wiener
- Department of Psychology, Ageing and Dementia Research Centre, Bournemouth University, Poole, United Kingdom
| | - Jana Kalinova
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Charles University, Second Faculty of Medicine and Motol University Hospital, Prague, Czechia
| | - Veronika Matuskova
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Charles University, Second Faculty of Medicine and Motol University Hospital, Prague, Czechia
- International Clinical Research Center, St. Anne’s University Hospital Brno, Brno, Czechia
| | - Zuzana Nedelska
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Charles University, Second Faculty of Medicine and Motol University Hospital, Prague, Czechia
- International Clinical Research Center, St. Anne’s University Hospital Brno, Brno, Czechia
| | - Martin Vyhnalek
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Charles University, Second Faculty of Medicine and Motol University Hospital, Prague, Czechia
- International Clinical Research Center, St. Anne’s University Hospital Brno, Brno, Czechia
| | - Jakub Hort
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Charles University, Second Faculty of Medicine and Motol University Hospital, Prague, Czechia
- International Clinical Research Center, St. Anne’s University Hospital Brno, Brno, Czechia
| | - Jan Laczó
- Memory Clinic, Department of Neurology, Charles University, Second Faculty of Medicine and Motol University Hospital, Prague, Czechia
- International Clinical Research Center, St. Anne’s University Hospital Brno, Brno, Czechia
- *Correspondence: Jan Laczó
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Using virtual global landmark to improve incidental spatial learning. Sci Rep 2022; 12:6744. [PMID: 35469001 PMCID: PMC9039035 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-10855-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/02/2021] [Accepted: 04/14/2022] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
To reduce the decline of spatial cognitive skills caused by the increasing use of automated GPS navigation, the virtual global landmark (VGL) system is proposed to help people naturally improve their sense of direction. Designed to accompany a heads-up navigation system, VGL system constantly displays silhouette of global landmarks in the navigator’s vision as a notable frame of reference. This study exams how VGL system impacts incidental spatial learning, i.e., subconscious spatial knowledge acquisition. We asked 55 participants to explore a virtual environment and then draw a map of what they had explored while capturing electroencephalogram (EEG) signals and eye activity. The results suggest that, with the VGL system, participants paid more attention during exploration and performed significantly better at the map drawing task—a result that indicates substantially improved incidental spatial learning. This finding might kickstart a redesigning navigation aids, to teach users to learn a route rather than simply showing them the way.
Collapse
|
23
|
Sacco K, Ronga I, Perna P, Cicerale A, Del Fante E, Sarasso P, Geminiani GC. A Virtual Navigation Training Promotes the Remapping of Space in Allocentric Coordinates: Evidence From Behavioral and Neuroimaging Data. Front Hum Neurosci 2022; 16:693968. [PMID: 35479185 PMCID: PMC9037151 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.693968] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/13/2021] [Accepted: 03/02/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Allocentric space representations demonstrated to be crucial to improve visuo-spatial skills, pivotal in every-day life activities and for the development and maintenance of other cognitive abilities, such as memory and reasoning. Here, we present a series of three different experiments: Experiment 1, Discovery sample (23 young male participants); Experiment 2, Neuroimaging and replicating sample (23 young male participants); and Experiment 3 (14 young male participants). In the experiments, we investigated whether virtual navigation stimulates the ability to form spatial allocentric representations. With this aim, we used a novel 3D videogame (MindTheCity!), focused on the navigation of a virtual town. We verified whether playing at MindTheCity! enhanced the performance on spatial representational tasks (pointing to a specific location in space) and on a spatial memory test (asking participant to remember the location of specific objects). Furthermore, to uncover the neural mechanisms underlying the observed effects, we performed a preliminary fMRI investigation before and after the training with MindTheCity!. Results show that our virtual training enhances the ability to form allocentric representations and spatial memory (Experiment 1). Experiments 2 and 3 confirmed the behavioral results of Experiment 1. Furthermore, our preliminary neuroimaging and behavioral results suggest that the training activates brain circuits involved in higher-order mechanisms of information encoding, triggering the activation of broader cognitive processes and reducing the working load on memory circuits (Experiments 2 and 3).
Collapse
|
24
|
Stacho M, Manahan-Vaughan D. Mechanistic flexibility of the retrosplenial cortex enables its contribution to spatial cognition. Trends Neurosci 2022; 45:284-296. [DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2022.01.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/21/2021] [Revised: 12/17/2021] [Accepted: 01/27/2022] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
|
25
|
Jeffery K, Guo W, Ball D, Rodriguez-Sanchez J. Visual imagination and cognitive mapping of a virtual building. JOURNAL OF NAVIGATION 2022; 75:1-14. [PMID: 35418722 PMCID: PMC7612610 DOI: 10.1017/s0373463321000588] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/14/2023]
Abstract
We investigated the contribution of visual imagination to the cognitive mapping of a building when initial exploration was simulated either visually by using a passive video walk-through, or mentally by using verbal guidance. Building layout had repeating elements with either rotational or mirror symmetry. Cognitive mapping of the virtual building, determined using questionnaires and map drawings, was present following verbal guidance but inferior to that following video guidance. Mapping was not affected by the building's structural symmetry. However, notably, it correlated with small-scale mental rotation scores for both video and verbal guidance conditions. There was no difference between males and females. A common factor that may have influenced cognitive mapping was the availability of visual information about the relationships of the building elements, either directly perceived (during the video walk-through) or imagined (during the verbal walk-through and/or during recall). Differences in visual imagination, particularly mental rotation, may thus account for some of the individual variance in cognitive mapping of complex built environments, which is relevant to how designers provide navigation-relevant information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kate Jeffery
- Experimental Psychology, University College London, London, UK
| | - Wanying Guo
- Experimental Psychology, University College London, London, UK
| | - Danny Ball
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
26
|
Motanis H, Khorasani LN, Giza CC, Harris NG. Peering into the Brain through the Retrosplenial Cortex to Assess Cognitive Function of the Injured Brain. Neurotrauma Rep 2021; 2:564-580. [PMID: 34901949 PMCID: PMC8655812 DOI: 10.1089/neur.2021.0044] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
The retrosplenial cortex (RSC) is a posterior cortical area that has been drawing increasing interest in recent years, with a growing number of studies studying its contribution to cognitive and sensory functions. From an anatomical perspective, it has been established that the RSC is extensively and often reciprocally connected with the hippocampus, neocortex, and many midbrain regions. Functionally, the RSC is an important hub of the default-mode network. This endowment, with vast anatomical and functional connections, positions the RSC to play an important role in episodic memory, spatial and contextual learning, sensory-cognitive activities, and multi-modal sensory information processing and integration. Additionally, RSC dysfunction has been reported in cases of cognitive decline, particularly in Alzheimer's disease and stroke. We review the literature to examine whether the RSC can act as a cortical marker of persistent cognitive dysfunction after traumatic brain injury (TBI). Because the RSC is easily accessible at the brain's surface using in vivo techniques, we argue that studying RSC network activity post-TBI can shed light into the mechanisms of less-accessible brain regions, such as the hippocampus. There is a fundamental gap in the TBI field about the microscale alterations occurring post-trauma, and by studying the RSC's neuronal activity at the cellular level we will be able to design better therapeutic tools. Understanding how neuronal activity and interactions produce normal and abnormal activity in the injured brain is crucial to understanding cognitive dysfunction. By using this approach, we expect to gain valuable insights to better understand brain disorders like TBI.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Helen Motanis
- UCLA Brain Injury Research Center, Department of Neurosurgery, Geffen Medical School, UCLA Mattel Children's Hospital, University of California at Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California, USA
| | - Laila N. Khorasani
- UCLA Brain Injury Research Center, Department of Neurosurgery, Geffen Medical School, UCLA Mattel Children's Hospital, University of California at Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California, USA
| | - Christopher C. Giza
- UCLA Brain Injury Research Center, Department of Neurosurgery, Geffen Medical School, UCLA Mattel Children's Hospital, University of California at Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California, USA
- Department of Pediatrics, UCLA Mattel Children's Hospital, University of California at Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California, USA
| | - Neil G. Harris
- UCLA Brain Injury Research Center, Department of Neurosurgery, Geffen Medical School, UCLA Mattel Children's Hospital, University of California at Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California, USA
- Intellectual Development and Disabilities Research Center, UCLA Mattel Children's Hospital, University of California at Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California, USA
- *Address correspondence to: Neil G. Harris, PhD, Department of Neurosurgery, University of California at Los Angeles, Wasserman Building, 300 Stein Plaza, Room 551, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA;
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Shan X, Contreras MP, Mendez M, Born J, Inostroza M. Unfolding of spatial representation at systems level in infant rats. Hippocampus 2021; 32:121-133. [PMID: 34786798 DOI: 10.1002/hipo.23392] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/17/2020] [Revised: 10/25/2021] [Accepted: 11/01/2021] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
Abstract
Spatial representations enable navigation from early life on. However, the brain regions essential to form spatial representations, like the hippocampus, are considered functionally immature before weaning. Here, we examined the formation of representations of space in rat pups on postnatal day (PD) 16, using a simple habituation paradigm where the pups were exposed to an arena on three occasions, separated by ~140 min. Whereas on the first two occasions the arena was the same, on the third "test" occasion either proximal cues (Prox group), or distal cues (Dist group), or proximal and distal cues (Prox-Dist group), or no cues (No-change group) were rearranged. Locomotion (distance traveled) was used as behavioral measure of habituation, and c-Fos expression to measure regional brain activity at test. Locomotion generally decreased across the first two occasions. At test, it reached a minimum in the No-change group, indicating familiarity with the spatial conditions. By contrast, the Prox-Dist group displayed a significant increase in locomotion which was less robust in the Prox group and absent in the Dist group, a pattern suggesting that the pups relied more on proximal than distal cues during spatial exploration. c-Fos activity in the No-change group was significantly suppressed in the hippocampus (CA1, CA3, dentate gyrus) but simultaneously enhanced in the prelimbic area (PL) of the medial prefrontal cortex, compared with untreated Home-cage controls, pointing to a possible involvement of the PL in regulating locomotion in familiar spaces. By contrast, in both Prox-Dist and Prox groups c-Fos activity was enhanced in hippocampal CA1 and CA3 regions, suggesting these regions might be particularly involved in regulating exploration of spatial novelty. Our findings show that functional representations of space at a systems level are formed already in pre-weanling rats.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xia Shan
- Institute of Medical Psychology and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany.,Graduate School of Neural & Behavioral Science, International Max Planck Research School, Tübingen, Germany
| | - María P Contreras
- Institute of Medical Psychology and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany.,Graduate School of Neural & Behavioral Science, International Max Planck Research School, Tübingen, Germany
| | - Marta Mendez
- Laboratory of Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, Instituto de Neurociencias del Principado de Asturias (INEUROPA), University of Oviedo, Plaza Feijoo, Oviedo, Spain
| | - Jan Born
- Institute of Medical Psychology and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany.,German Center for Diabetes Research (DZD), Institute for Diabetes Research & Metabolic Diseases of the Helmholtz Center Munich at the University Tübingen (IDM), Tübingen, Germany.,Werner Reichert Center for Integrative Neuroscience, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| | - Marion Inostroza
- Institute of Medical Psychology and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Stimmell AC, Xu Z, Moseley SC, Benthem SD, Fernandez DM, Dang JV, Santos-Molina LF, Anzalone RA, Garcia-Barbon CL, Rodriguez S, Dixon JR, Wu W, Wilber AA. Tau Pathology Profile Across a Parietal-Hippocampal Brain Network Is Associated With Spatial Reorientation Learning and Memory Performance in the 3xTg-AD Mouse. FRONTIERS IN AGING 2021; 2. [PMID: 34746919 PMCID: PMC8570590 DOI: 10.3389/fragi.2021.655015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
In early Alzheimer's disease (AD) spatial navigation is one of the first impairments to emerge; however, the precise cause of this impairment is unclear. Previously, we showed that, in a mouse model of tau and amyloid beta (Aβ) aggregation, getting lost represents, at least in part, a failure to use distal cues to get oriented in space and that impaired parietal-hippocampal network level plasticity during sleep may underlie this spatial disorientation. However, the relationship between tau and amyloid beta aggregation in this brain network and impaired spatial orientation has not been assessed. Therefore, we used several approaches, including canonical correlation analysis and independent components analysis tools, to examine the relationship between pathology profile across the parietal-hippocampal brain network and spatial reorientation learning and memory performance. We found that consistent with the exclusive impairment in 3xTg-AD 6-month female mice, only 6-month female mice had an ICA identified pattern of tau pathology across the parietal-hippocampal network that were positively correlated with behavior. Specifically, a higher density of pTau positive cells predicted worse spatial learning and memory. Surprisingly, despite a lack of impairment relative to controls, 3-month female, as well as 6- and 12- month male mice all had patterns of tau pathology across the parietal-hippocampal brain network that are predictive of spatial learning and memory performance. However, the direction of the effect was opposite, a negative correlation, meaning that a higher density of pTau positive cells predicted better performance. Finally, there were not significant group or region differences in M78 density at any of the ages examined and ICA analyses were not able to identify any patterns of 6E10 staining across brain regions that were significant predictors of behavioral performance. Thus, the pattern of pTau staining across the parietal-hippocampal network is a strong predictor of spatial learning and memory performance, even for mice with low levels of tau accumulation and intact spatial re-orientation learning and memory. This suggests that AD may cause spatial disorientation as a result of early tau accumulation in the parietal-hippocampal network.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alina C Stimmell
- Department of Psychology, Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, United States
| | - Zishen Xu
- Department of Statistics, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, United States
| | - Shawn C Moseley
- Department of Psychology, Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, United States
| | - Sarah D Benthem
- Department of Psychology, Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, United States
| | - Diana M Fernandez
- Department of Psychology, Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, United States
| | - Jessica V Dang
- Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United States
| | - Luis F Santos-Molina
- Department of Psychology, Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, United States
| | - Rosina A Anzalone
- Department of Psychology, Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, United States
| | - Carolina L Garcia-Barbon
- Department of Psychology, Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, United States
| | - Stephany Rodriguez
- Department of Psychology, Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, United States
| | - Jessica R Dixon
- Department of Psychology, Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, United States
| | - Wei Wu
- Department of Statistics, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, United States
| | - Aaron A Wilber
- Department of Psychology, Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, United States
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Mao D, Avila E, Caziot B, Laurens J, Dickman JD, Angelaki DE. Spatial modulation of hippocampal activity in freely moving macaques. Neuron 2021; 109:3521-3534.e6. [PMID: 34644546 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.09.032] [Citation(s) in RCA: 57] [Impact Index Per Article: 14.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/07/2021] [Revised: 07/30/2021] [Accepted: 09/14/2021] [Indexed: 02/08/2023]
Abstract
The hippocampal formation is linked to spatial navigation, but there is little corroboration from freely moving primates with concurrent monitoring of head and gaze stances. We recorded neural activity across hippocampal regions in rhesus macaques during free foraging in an open environment while tracking their head and eye. Theta activity was intermittently present at movement onset and modulated by saccades. Many neurons were phase-locked to theta, with few showing phase precession. Most neurons encoded a mixture of spatial variables beyond place and grid tuning. Spatial representations were dominated by facing location and allocentric direction, mostly in head, rather than gaze, coordinates. Importantly, eye movements strongly modulated neural activity in all regions. These findings reveal that the macaque hippocampal formation represents three-dimensional (3D) space using a multiplexed code, with head orientation and eye movement properties being dominant during free exploration.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dun Mao
- Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY 10003, USA; Department of Neuroscience, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX 77030, USA.
| | - Eric Avila
- Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY 10003, USA
| | - Baptiste Caziot
- Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY 10003, USA
| | - Jean Laurens
- Ernst Strüngmann Institute (ESI) for Neuroscience in Cooperation with Max Planck Society, Frankfurt, Germany
| | - J David Dickman
- Department of Neuroscience, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX 77030, USA
| | - Dora E Angelaki
- Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY 10003, USA; Department of Neuroscience, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX 77030, USA; Tandon School of Engineering, New York University, New York, NY 11201, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Sun W, Choi I, Stoyanov S, Senkov O, Ponimaskin E, Winter Y, Pakan JMP, Dityatev A. Context value updating and multidimensional neuronal encoding in the retrosplenial cortex. Nat Commun 2021; 12:6045. [PMID: 34663792 PMCID: PMC8523535 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-021-26301-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/10/2019] [Accepted: 09/24/2021] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
Abstract
The retrosplenial cortex (RSC) has diverse functional inputs and is engaged by various sensory, spatial, and associative learning tasks. We examine how multiple functional aspects are integrated on the single-cell level in the RSC and how the encoding of task-related parameters changes across learning. Using a visuospatial context discrimination paradigm and two-photon calcium imaging in behaving mice, a large proportion of dysgranular RSC neurons was found to encode multiple task-related dimensions while forming context-value associations across learning. During reversal learning requiring increased cognitive flexibility, we revealed an increased proportion of multidimensional encoding neurons that showed higher decoding accuracy for behaviorally relevant context-value associations. Chemogenetic inactivation of RSC led to decreased behavioral context discrimination during learning phases in which context-value associations were formed, while recall of previously formed associations remained intact. RSC inactivation resulted in a persistent positive behavioral bias in valuing contexts, indicating a role for the RSC in context-value updating.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Weilun Sun
- grid.424247.30000 0004 0438 0426Molecular Neuroplasticity, German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Magdeburg, Germany ,grid.418723.b0000 0001 2109 6265Center for Behavioral Brain Sciences (CBBS), Magdeburg, Germany
| | - Ilseob Choi
- grid.424247.30000 0004 0438 0426Molecular Neuroplasticity, German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Magdeburg, Germany ,grid.418723.b0000 0001 2109 6265Center for Behavioral Brain Sciences (CBBS), Magdeburg, Germany
| | - Stoyan Stoyanov
- grid.424247.30000 0004 0438 0426Molecular Neuroplasticity, German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Magdeburg, Germany
| | - Oleg Senkov
- grid.424247.30000 0004 0438 0426Molecular Neuroplasticity, German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Magdeburg, Germany
| | - Evgeni Ponimaskin
- grid.10423.340000 0000 9529 9877Department of Cellular Neurophysiology, Hannover Medical School, Hannover, Germany
| | - York Winter
- grid.7468.d0000 0001 2248 7639Institute for Biology, Humboldt University, Berlin, Germany
| | - Janelle M. P. Pakan
- grid.418723.b0000 0001 2109 6265Center for Behavioral Brain Sciences (CBBS), Magdeburg, Germany ,grid.5807.a0000 0001 1018 4307Institute of Cognitive Neurology and Dementia Research, Otto-von-Guericke University, Magdeburg, Germany ,grid.424247.30000 0004 0438 0426German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Magdeburg, Germany
| | - Alexander Dityatev
- grid.424247.30000 0004 0438 0426Molecular Neuroplasticity, German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Magdeburg, Germany ,grid.418723.b0000 0001 2109 6265Center for Behavioral Brain Sciences (CBBS), Magdeburg, Germany ,grid.5807.a0000 0001 1018 4307Medical Faculty, Otto-von-Guericke University, Magdeburg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Perry BAL, Lomi E, Mitchell AS. Thalamocortical interactions in cognition and disease: the mediodorsal and anterior thalamic nuclei. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2021; 130:162-177. [PMID: 34216651 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.05.032] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/31/2020] [Revised: 04/12/2021] [Accepted: 05/17/2021] [Indexed: 01/15/2023]
Abstract
The mediodorsal thalamus (MD) and anterior thalamic nuclei (ATN) are two adjacent brain nodes that support our ability to make decisions, learn, update information, form and retrieve memories, and find our way around. The MD and PFC work in partnerships to support cognitive processes linked to successful learning and decision-making, while the ATN and extended hippocampal system together coordinate the encoding and retrieval of memories and successful spatial navigation. Yet, while these distinctions may appear to be segregated, both the MD and ATN together support our higher cognitive functions as they regulate and are influenced by interconnected fronto-temporal neural networks and subcortical inputs. Our review focuses on recent studies in animal models and in humans. This evidence is re-shaping our understanding of the importance of MD and ATN cortico-thalamocortical pathways in influencing complex cognitive functions. Given the evidence from clinical settings and neuroscience research labs, the MD and ATN should be considered targets for effective treatments in neuropsychiatric diseases and disorders and neurodegeneration.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brook A L Perry
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Oxford University, The Tinsley Building, Mansfield Road, OX1 3SR, United Kingdom
| | - Eleonora Lomi
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Oxford University, The Tinsley Building, Mansfield Road, OX1 3SR, United Kingdom
| | - Anna S Mitchell
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Oxford University, The Tinsley Building, Mansfield Road, OX1 3SR, United Kingdom.
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
A Thalamic Reticular Circuit for Head Direction Cell Tuning and Spatial Navigation. Cell Rep 2021; 31:107747. [PMID: 32521272 DOI: 10.1016/j.celrep.2020.107747] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/04/2019] [Revised: 02/13/2020] [Accepted: 05/18/2020] [Indexed: 01/13/2023] Open
Abstract
As we navigate in space, external landmarks and internal information guide our movement. Circuit and synaptic mechanisms that integrate these cues with head-direction (HD) signals remain, however, unclear. We identify an excitatory synaptic projection from the presubiculum (PreS) and the multisensory-associative retrosplenial cortex (RSC) to the anterodorsal thalamic reticular nucleus (TRN), so far classically implied in gating sensory information flow. In vitro, projections to TRN involve AMPA/NMDA-type glutamate receptors that initiate TRN cell burst discharge and feedforward inhibition of anterior thalamic nuclei. In vivo, chemogenetic anterodorsal TRN inhibition modulates PreS/RSC-induced anterior thalamic firing dynamics, broadens the tuning of thalamic HD cells, and leads to preferential use of allo- over egocentric search strategies in the Morris water maze. TRN-dependent thalamic inhibition is thus an integral part of limbic navigational circuits wherein it coordinates external sensory and internal HD signals to regulate the choice of search strategies during spatial navigation.
Collapse
|
33
|
Gehrke L, Gramann K. Single-trial regression of spatial exploration behavior indicates posterior EEG alpha modulation to reflect egocentric coding. Eur J Neurosci 2021; 54:8318-8335. [PMID: 33609299 DOI: 10.1111/ejn.15152] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/29/2020] [Revised: 12/24/2020] [Accepted: 02/17/2021] [Indexed: 12/29/2022]
Abstract
Learning to navigate uncharted terrain is a key cognitive ability that emerges as a deeply embodied process, with eye movements and locomotion proving most useful to sample the environment. We studied healthy human participants during active spatial learning of room-scale virtual reality (VR) mazes. In the invisible maze task, participants wearing a wireless electroencephalography (EEG) headset were free to explore their surroundings, only given the objective to build and foster a mental spatial representation of their environment. Spatial uncertainty was resolved by touching otherwise invisible walls that were briefly rendered visible inside VR, similar to finding your way in the dark. We showcase the capabilities of mobile brain/body imaging using VR, demonstrating several analysis approaches based on general linear models (GLMs) to reveal behavior-dependent brain dynamics. Confirming spatial learning via drawn sketch maps, we employed motion capture to image spatial exploration behavior describing a shift from initial exploration to subsequent exploitation of the mental representation. Using independent component analysis, the current work specifically targeted oscillations in response to wall touches reflecting isolated spatial learning events arising in deep posterior EEG sources located in the retrosplenial complex. Single-trial regression identified significant modulation of alpha oscillations by the immediate, egocentric, exploration behavior. When encountering novel walls, as well as with increasing walking distance between subsequent touches when encountering novel walls, alpha power decreased. We conclude that these oscillations play a prominent role during egocentric evidencing of allocentric spatial hypotheses.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lukas Gehrke
- Biopsychology and Neuroergonomics, Institute of Psychology and Ergonomics, Berlin, Germany
| | - Klaus Gramann
- Biopsychology and Neuroergonomics, Institute of Psychology and Ergonomics, Berlin, Germany.,Center for Advanced Neurological Engineering, University of California San Diego, San Diego, CA, USA.,School of Computer Science, University of Technology Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
van Wijngaarden JBG, Babl SS, Ito HT. Entorhinal-retrosplenial circuits for allocentric-egocentric transformation of boundary coding. eLife 2020; 9:e59816. [PMID: 33138915 PMCID: PMC7609058 DOI: 10.7554/elife.59816] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/09/2020] [Accepted: 10/15/2020] [Indexed: 12/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Spatial navigation requires landmark coding from two perspectives, relying on viewpoint-invariant and self-referenced representations. The brain encodes information within each reference frame but their interactions and functional dependency remains unclear. Here we investigate the relationship between neurons in the rat's retrosplenial cortex (RSC) and entorhinal cortex (MEC) that increase firing near boundaries of space. Border cells in RSC specifically encode walls, but not objects, and are sensitive to the animal's direction to nearby borders. These egocentric representations are generated independent of visual or whisker sensation but are affected by inputs from MEC that contains allocentric spatial cells. Pharmaco- and optogenetic inhibition of MEC led to a disruption of border coding in RSC, but not vice versa, indicating allocentric-to-egocentric transformation. Finally, RSC border cells fire prospective to the animal's next motion, unlike those in MEC, revealing the MEC-RSC pathway as an extended border coding circuit that implements coordinate transformation to guide navigation behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Susanne S Babl
- Institute of Neurophysiology, Neuroscience Center, Goethe UniversityFrankfurtGermany
| | - Hiroshi T Ito
- Max Planck Institute for Brain ResearchFrankfurtGermany
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
Abstract
Several types of neurons involved in spatial navigation and memory encode the distance and direction (that is, the vector) between an agent and items in its environment. Such vectorial information provides a powerful basis for spatial cognition by representing the geometric relationships between the self and the external world. Here, we review the explicit encoding of vectorial information by neurons in and around the hippocampal formation, far from the sensory periphery. The parahippocampal, retrosplenial and parietal cortices, as well as the hippocampal formation and striatum, provide a plethora of examples of vector coding at the single neuron level. We provide a functional taxonomy of cells with vectorial receptive fields as reported in experiments and proposed in theoretical work. The responses of these neurons may provide the fundamental neural basis for the (bottom-up) representation of environmental layout and (top-down) memory-guided generation of visuospatial imagery and navigational planning.
Collapse
|
36
|
Bermudez-Contreras E, Clark BJ, Wilber A. The Neuroscience of Spatial Navigation and the Relationship to Artificial Intelligence. Front Comput Neurosci 2020; 14:63. [PMID: 32848684 PMCID: PMC7399088 DOI: 10.3389/fncom.2020.00063] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/31/2019] [Accepted: 05/28/2020] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Recent advances in artificial intelligence (AI) and neuroscience are impressive. In AI, this includes the development of computer programs that can beat a grandmaster at GO or outperform human radiologists at cancer detection. A great deal of these technological developments are directly related to progress in artificial neural networks-initially inspired by our knowledge about how the brain carries out computation. In parallel, neuroscience has also experienced significant advances in understanding the brain. For example, in the field of spatial navigation, knowledge about the mechanisms and brain regions involved in neural computations of cognitive maps-an internal representation of space-recently received the Nobel Prize in medicine. Much of the recent progress in neuroscience has partly been due to the development of technology used to record from very large populations of neurons in multiple regions of the brain with exquisite temporal and spatial resolution in behaving animals. With the advent of the vast quantities of data that these techniques allow us to collect there has been an increased interest in the intersection between AI and neuroscience, many of these intersections involve using AI as a novel tool to explore and analyze these large data sets. However, given the common initial motivation point-to understand the brain-these disciplines could be more strongly linked. Currently much of this potential synergy is not being realized. We propose that spatial navigation is an excellent area in which these two disciplines can converge to help advance what we know about the brain. In this review, we first summarize progress in the neuroscience of spatial navigation and reinforcement learning. We then turn our attention to discuss how spatial navigation has been modeled using descriptive, mechanistic, and normative approaches and the use of AI in such models. Next, we discuss how AI can advance neuroscience, how neuroscience can advance AI, and the limitations of these approaches. We finally conclude by highlighting promising lines of research in which spatial navigation can be the point of intersection between neuroscience and AI and how this can contribute to the advancement of the understanding of intelligent behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Benjamin J. Clark
- Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, United States
| | - Aaron Wilber
- Department of Psychology, Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, United States
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
Benthem SD, Skelin I, Moseley SC, Stimmell AC, Dixon JR, Melilli AS, Molina L, McNaughton BL, Wilber AA. Impaired Hippocampal-Cortical Interactions during Sleep in a Mouse Model of Alzheimer's Disease. Curr Biol 2020; 30:2588-2601.e5. [PMID: 32470367 PMCID: PMC7356567 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.04.087] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/13/2019] [Revised: 03/11/2020] [Accepted: 04/29/2020] [Indexed: 01/23/2023]
Abstract
Spatial learning is impaired in humans with preclinical Alzheimer's disease (AD). We reported similar impairments in 3xTg-AD mice learning a spatial reorientation task. Memory reactivation during sleep is critical for learning-related plasticity, and memory consolidation is correlated with hippocampal sharp wave ripple (SWR) density, cortical delta waves (DWs), cortical spindles, and the temporal coupling of these events-postulated as physiological substrates for memory consolidation. Further, hippocampal-cortical discoordination is prevalent in individuals with AD. Thus, we hypothesized that impaired memory consolidation mechanisms in hippocampal-cortical networks could account for spatial memory deficits. We assessed sleep architecture, SWR-DW dynamics, and memory reactivation in a mouse model of tauopathy and amyloidosis implanted with a recording array targeting isocortex and hippocampus. Mice underwent daily recording sessions of rest-task-rest while learning the spatial reorientation task. We assessed memory reactivation by matching activity patterns from the approach to the unmarked reward zone to patterns during slow-wave sleep (SWS). AD mice had more SWS, but reduced SWR density. The increased SWS compensated for reduced SWR density so there was no reduction in SWR number. In control mice, spindles were phase-coupled with DWs, and hippocampal SWR-cortical DW coupling was strengthened in post-task sleep and was correlated with performance on the spatial reorientation task the following day. However, in AD mice, SWR-DW and spindle-DW coupling were impaired. Thus, reduced SWR-DW coupling may cause impaired learning in AD, and spindle-DW coupling during short rest-task-rest sessions may serve as a biomarker for early AD-related changes in these brain dynamics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah D Benthem
- Department of Psychology and Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL 32306, USA.
| | - Ivan Skelin
- Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, University of California Irvine, Irvine, CA 92697, USA
| | - Shawn C Moseley
- Department of Psychology and Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL 32306, USA
| | - Alina C Stimmell
- Department of Psychology and Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL 32306, USA
| | - Jessica R Dixon
- Department of Psychology and Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL 32306, USA
| | - Andreza S Melilli
- Department of Psychology and Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL 32306, USA
| | - Leonardo Molina
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, AB T1K 3M4, Canada
| | - Bruce L McNaughton
- Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, University of California Irvine, Irvine, CA 92697, USA; Department of Neuroscience, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, AB T1K 3M4, Canada
| | - Aaron A Wilber
- Department of Psychology and Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL 32306, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
38
|
Nau M, Navarro Schröder T, Frey M, Doeller CF. Behavior-dependent directional tuning in the human visual-navigation network. Nat Commun 2020; 11:3247. [PMID: 32591544 PMCID: PMC7320013 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-020-17000-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/26/2019] [Accepted: 06/05/2020] [Indexed: 01/06/2023] Open
Abstract
The brain derives cognitive maps from sensory experience that guide memory formation and behavior. Despite extensive efforts, it still remains unclear how the underlying population activity unfolds during spatial navigation and how it relates to memory performance. To examine these processes, we combined 7T-fMRI with a kernel-based encoding model of virtual navigation to map world-centered directional tuning across the human cortex. First, we present an in-depth analysis of directional tuning in visual, retrosplenial, parahippocampal and medial temporal cortices. Second, we show that tuning strength, width and topology of this directional code during memory-guided navigation depend on successful encoding of the environment. Finally, we show that participants' locomotory state influences this tuning in sensory and mnemonic regions such as the hippocampus. We demonstrate a direct link between neural population tuning and human cognition, where high-level memory processing interacts with network-wide visuospatial coding in the service of behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matthias Nau
- Kavli Institute for Systems Neuroscience, Centre for Neural Computation, The Egil and Pauline Braathen and Fred Kavli Centre for Cortical Microcircuits, NTNU, Trondheim, Norway.
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany.
| | - Tobias Navarro Schröder
- Kavli Institute for Systems Neuroscience, Centre for Neural Computation, The Egil and Pauline Braathen and Fred Kavli Centre for Cortical Microcircuits, NTNU, Trondheim, Norway
| | - Markus Frey
- Kavli Institute for Systems Neuroscience, Centre for Neural Computation, The Egil and Pauline Braathen and Fred Kavli Centre for Cortical Microcircuits, NTNU, Trondheim, Norway
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Christian F Doeller
- Kavli Institute for Systems Neuroscience, Centre for Neural Computation, The Egil and Pauline Braathen and Fred Kavli Centre for Cortical Microcircuits, NTNU, Trondheim, Norway.
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
Mao D, Molina LA, Bonin V, McNaughton BL. Vision and Locomotion Combine to Drive Path Integration Sequences in Mouse Retrosplenial Cortex. Curr Biol 2020; 30:1680-1688.e4. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.02.070] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/03/2019] [Revised: 01/30/2020] [Accepted: 02/24/2020] [Indexed: 01/30/2023]
|
40
|
Angelaki DE, Ng J, Abrego AM, Cham HX, Asprodini EK, Dickman JD, Laurens J. A gravity-based three-dimensional compass in the mouse brain. Nat Commun 2020; 11:1855. [PMID: 32296057 PMCID: PMC7160108 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-020-15566-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/25/2019] [Accepted: 03/12/2020] [Indexed: 02/07/2023] Open
Abstract
Gravity sensing provides a robust verticality signal for three-dimensional navigation. Head direction cells in the mammalian limbic system implement an allocentric neuronal compass. Here we show that head-direction cells in the rodent thalamus, retrosplenial cortex and cingulum fiber bundle are tuned to conjunctive combinations of azimuth and tilt, i.e. pitch or roll. Pitch and roll orientation tuning is anchored to gravity and independent of visual landmarks. When the head tilts, azimuth tuning is affixed to the head-horizontal plane, but also uses gravity to remain anchored to the allocentric bearings in the earth-horizontal plane. Collectively, these results demonstrate that a three-dimensional, gravity-based, neural compass is likely a ubiquitous property of mammalian species, including ground-dwelling animals. Head direction neurons constitute the brain’s compass, and are classically known to indicate head orientation in the horizontal plane. Here, the authors show that head direction neurons form a three-dimensional compass that can also indicate head tilt, and anchors to gravity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dora E Angelaki
- Center for Neural Science and Tandon School of Engineering, New York University, New York, NY, USA. .,Department of Neuroscience, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, USA.
| | - Julia Ng
- Department of Neuroscience, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, USA
| | - Amada M Abrego
- Department of Neuroscience, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, USA
| | - Henry X Cham
- Department of Neuroscience, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, USA
| | - Eftihia K Asprodini
- Department of Pharmacology, Faculty of Medicine, School of Health Sciences, University of Thessaly, Larissa, Greece
| | - J David Dickman
- Department of Neuroscience, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, USA.,Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Rice University, Houston, TX, USA
| | - Jean Laurens
- Department of Neuroscience, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, USA
| |
Collapse
|
41
|
Alexander AS, Carstensen LC, Hinman JR, Raudies F, Chapman GW, Hasselmo ME. Egocentric boundary vector tuning of the retrosplenial cortex. SCIENCE ADVANCES 2020; 6:eaaz2322. [PMID: 32128423 PMCID: PMC7035004 DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.aaz2322] [Citation(s) in RCA: 111] [Impact Index Per Article: 22.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/22/2019] [Accepted: 11/27/2019] [Indexed: 05/17/2023]
Abstract
The retrosplenial cortex is reciprocally connected with multiple structures implicated in spatial cognition, and damage to the region itself produces numerous spatial impairments. Here, we sought to characterize spatial correlates of neurons within the region during free exploration in two-dimensional environments. We report that a large percentage of retrosplenial cortex neurons have spatial receptive fields that are active when environmental boundaries are positioned at a specific orientation and distance relative to the animal itself. We demonstrate that this vector-based location signal is encoded in egocentric coordinates, is localized to the dysgranular retrosplenial subregion, is independent of self-motion, and is context invariant. Further, we identify a subpopulation of neurons with this response property that are synchronized with the hippocampal theta oscillation. Accordingly, the current work identifies a robust egocentric spatial code in retrosplenial cortex that can facilitate spatial coordinate system transformations and support the anchoring, generation, and utilization of allocentric representations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrew S. Alexander
- Center for Systems Neuroscience, Boston University, 610 Commonwealth Ave., Boston, MA 02215, USA
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, 64 Cummington Mall, Boston, MA 02215, USA
- Corresponding author.
| | - Lucas C. Carstensen
- Center for Systems Neuroscience, Boston University, 610 Commonwealth Ave., Boston, MA 02215, USA
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, 64 Cummington Mall, Boston, MA 02215, USA
- Graduate Program for Neuroscience, Boston University, 610 Commonwealth Ave., Boston, MA 02215, USA
| | - James R. Hinman
- Center for Systems Neuroscience, Boston University, 610 Commonwealth Ave., Boston, MA 02215, USA
| | - Florian Raudies
- Center for Systems Neuroscience, Boston University, 610 Commonwealth Ave., Boston, MA 02215, USA
| | - G. William Chapman
- Center for Systems Neuroscience, Boston University, 610 Commonwealth Ave., Boston, MA 02215, USA
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, 64 Cummington Mall, Boston, MA 02215, USA
| | - Michael E. Hasselmo
- Center for Systems Neuroscience, Boston University, 610 Commonwealth Ave., Boston, MA 02215, USA
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, 64 Cummington Mall, Boston, MA 02215, USA
- Graduate Program for Neuroscience, Boston University, 610 Commonwealth Ave., Boston, MA 02215, USA
| |
Collapse
|
42
|
Angelaki DE, Laurens J. The head direction cell network: attractor dynamics, integration within the navigation system, and three-dimensional properties. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2020; 60:136-144. [PMID: 31877492 PMCID: PMC7002189 DOI: 10.1016/j.conb.2019.12.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/28/2019] [Revised: 12/02/2019] [Accepted: 12/04/2019] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Knowledge of head direction cell function has progressed remarkably in recent years. The predominant theory that they form an attractor has been confirmed by several experiments. Candidate pathways that may convey visual input have been identified. The pre-subicular circuitry that conveys head direction signals to the medial entorhinal cortex, potentially sustaining path integration by grid cells, has been resolved. Although the neuronal substrate of the attractor remains unknown in mammals, a simple head direction network, whose structure is astoundingly similar to neuronal models theorized decades earlier, has been identified in insects. Finally, recent experiments have revealed that these cells do not encode head direction in the horizontal plane only, but also in vertical planes, thus providing a 3D orientation signal.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dora E Angelaki
- Department of Neuroscience, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas, USA; Center for Neural Science and Tandon School of Engineering, New York University, NY, USA
| | - Jean Laurens
- Department of Neuroscience, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas, USA; Ernst Strüngmann Institute for Neuroscience, Frankfurt, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
43
|
Schöberl F, Pradhan C, Irving S, Buerger K, Xiong G, Kugler G, Kohlbecher S, Engmann J, Werner P, Brendel M, Schneider E, Perneczky R, Jahn K, la Fougère C, Bartenstein P, Brandt T, Dieterich M, Zwergal A. Real-space navigation testing differentiates between amyloid-positive and -negative aMCI. Neurology 2020; 94:e861-e873. [PMID: 31896617 DOI: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000008758] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/25/2019] [Accepted: 09/05/2019] [Indexed: 01/08/2023] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE To distinguish between patients with amyloid-positive (A+) and -negative (A-) amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) by simultaneously investigating navigation performance, visual exploration behavior, and brain activations during a real-space navigation paradigm. METHODS Twenty-one patients with aMCI were grouped into A+ (n = 11) and A- cases by amyloid-PET imaging and amyloid CSF levels and compared to 15 healthy controls. Neuropsychological deficits were quantified by use of the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease-plus cognitive battery. All participants performed a navigation task in which they had to find items in a realistic spatial environment and had to apply egocentric and allocentric route planning strategies. 18F-fluorodeoxyglucose was injected at the start to detect navigation-induced brain activations. Subjects wore a gaze-controlled, head-fixed camera that recorded their visual exploration behavior. RESULTS A+ patients performed worse during egocentric and allocentric navigation compared to A- patients and controls (p < 0.001). Both aMCI subgroups used fewer shortcuts, moved more slowly, and stayed longer at crossings. Word-list learning, figural learning, and Trail-Making tests did not differ in the A+ and A- subgroups. A+ patients showed a reduced activation of the right hippocampus, retrosplenial, and parietal cortex during navigation compared to A- patients (p < 0.005). CONCLUSIONS A+ patients with aMCI perform worse than A- patients with aMCI in egocentric and allocentric route planning because of a more widespread impairment of their cerebral navigation network. Navigation testing in real space is a promising approach to identify patients with aMCI with underlying Alzheimer pathology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Florian Schöberl
- From the Department of Neurology (F.S., J.E., P.W., A.Z., M.D.), University Hospital, German Center for Vertigo and Balance Disorders (F.S., C.P., S.I., G.X., G.K., S.K., E.S., K.J., C.l.F., P.B., T.B., M.D., A.Z.), DSGZ, Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research (K.B.), ISD, University Hospital, Department of Nuclear Medicine (G.X., M.B., P.B.), Department of Psychiatry (R.P.), and Clinical Neurosciences (T.B.), Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich; German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (K.B., R.P., M.D.), DZNE, Munich; Institute for Medical Technology (E.S.), Brandenburg University of Technology Cottbus-Senftenberg; Munich Cluster of Systems Neurology (R.P., P.B., M.D.), SyNergy, Germany; Ageing Epidemiology Research Unit (R.P.), School of Public Health, Imperial College, London, UK; Neurological Hospital (K.J.), Schön Klinik Bad Aibling; and Department of Nuclear Medicine (C.l.F.), Eberhard Karl University of Tübingen, Germany
| | - Cauchy Pradhan
- From the Department of Neurology (F.S., J.E., P.W., A.Z., M.D.), University Hospital, German Center for Vertigo and Balance Disorders (F.S., C.P., S.I., G.X., G.K., S.K., E.S., K.J., C.l.F., P.B., T.B., M.D., A.Z.), DSGZ, Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research (K.B.), ISD, University Hospital, Department of Nuclear Medicine (G.X., M.B., P.B.), Department of Psychiatry (R.P.), and Clinical Neurosciences (T.B.), Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich; German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (K.B., R.P., M.D.), DZNE, Munich; Institute for Medical Technology (E.S.), Brandenburg University of Technology Cottbus-Senftenberg; Munich Cluster of Systems Neurology (R.P., P.B., M.D.), SyNergy, Germany; Ageing Epidemiology Research Unit (R.P.), School of Public Health, Imperial College, London, UK; Neurological Hospital (K.J.), Schön Klinik Bad Aibling; and Department of Nuclear Medicine (C.l.F.), Eberhard Karl University of Tübingen, Germany
| | - Stephanie Irving
- From the Department of Neurology (F.S., J.E., P.W., A.Z., M.D.), University Hospital, German Center for Vertigo and Balance Disorders (F.S., C.P., S.I., G.X., G.K., S.K., E.S., K.J., C.l.F., P.B., T.B., M.D., A.Z.), DSGZ, Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research (K.B.), ISD, University Hospital, Department of Nuclear Medicine (G.X., M.B., P.B.), Department of Psychiatry (R.P.), and Clinical Neurosciences (T.B.), Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich; German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (K.B., R.P., M.D.), DZNE, Munich; Institute for Medical Technology (E.S.), Brandenburg University of Technology Cottbus-Senftenberg; Munich Cluster of Systems Neurology (R.P., P.B., M.D.), SyNergy, Germany; Ageing Epidemiology Research Unit (R.P.), School of Public Health, Imperial College, London, UK; Neurological Hospital (K.J.), Schön Klinik Bad Aibling; and Department of Nuclear Medicine (C.l.F.), Eberhard Karl University of Tübingen, Germany
| | - Katharina Buerger
- From the Department of Neurology (F.S., J.E., P.W., A.Z., M.D.), University Hospital, German Center for Vertigo and Balance Disorders (F.S., C.P., S.I., G.X., G.K., S.K., E.S., K.J., C.l.F., P.B., T.B., M.D., A.Z.), DSGZ, Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research (K.B.), ISD, University Hospital, Department of Nuclear Medicine (G.X., M.B., P.B.), Department of Psychiatry (R.P.), and Clinical Neurosciences (T.B.), Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich; German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (K.B., R.P., M.D.), DZNE, Munich; Institute for Medical Technology (E.S.), Brandenburg University of Technology Cottbus-Senftenberg; Munich Cluster of Systems Neurology (R.P., P.B., M.D.), SyNergy, Germany; Ageing Epidemiology Research Unit (R.P.), School of Public Health, Imperial College, London, UK; Neurological Hospital (K.J.), Schön Klinik Bad Aibling; and Department of Nuclear Medicine (C.l.F.), Eberhard Karl University of Tübingen, Germany
| | - Guoming Xiong
- From the Department of Neurology (F.S., J.E., P.W., A.Z., M.D.), University Hospital, German Center for Vertigo and Balance Disorders (F.S., C.P., S.I., G.X., G.K., S.K., E.S., K.J., C.l.F., P.B., T.B., M.D., A.Z.), DSGZ, Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research (K.B.), ISD, University Hospital, Department of Nuclear Medicine (G.X., M.B., P.B.), Department of Psychiatry (R.P.), and Clinical Neurosciences (T.B.), Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich; German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (K.B., R.P., M.D.), DZNE, Munich; Institute for Medical Technology (E.S.), Brandenburg University of Technology Cottbus-Senftenberg; Munich Cluster of Systems Neurology (R.P., P.B., M.D.), SyNergy, Germany; Ageing Epidemiology Research Unit (R.P.), School of Public Health, Imperial College, London, UK; Neurological Hospital (K.J.), Schön Klinik Bad Aibling; and Department of Nuclear Medicine (C.l.F.), Eberhard Karl University of Tübingen, Germany
| | - Günter Kugler
- From the Department of Neurology (F.S., J.E., P.W., A.Z., M.D.), University Hospital, German Center for Vertigo and Balance Disorders (F.S., C.P., S.I., G.X., G.K., S.K., E.S., K.J., C.l.F., P.B., T.B., M.D., A.Z.), DSGZ, Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research (K.B.), ISD, University Hospital, Department of Nuclear Medicine (G.X., M.B., P.B.), Department of Psychiatry (R.P.), and Clinical Neurosciences (T.B.), Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich; German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (K.B., R.P., M.D.), DZNE, Munich; Institute for Medical Technology (E.S.), Brandenburg University of Technology Cottbus-Senftenberg; Munich Cluster of Systems Neurology (R.P., P.B., M.D.), SyNergy, Germany; Ageing Epidemiology Research Unit (R.P.), School of Public Health, Imperial College, London, UK; Neurological Hospital (K.J.), Schön Klinik Bad Aibling; and Department of Nuclear Medicine (C.l.F.), Eberhard Karl University of Tübingen, Germany
| | - Stefan Kohlbecher
- From the Department of Neurology (F.S., J.E., P.W., A.Z., M.D.), University Hospital, German Center for Vertigo and Balance Disorders (F.S., C.P., S.I., G.X., G.K., S.K., E.S., K.J., C.l.F., P.B., T.B., M.D., A.Z.), DSGZ, Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research (K.B.), ISD, University Hospital, Department of Nuclear Medicine (G.X., M.B., P.B.), Department of Psychiatry (R.P.), and Clinical Neurosciences (T.B.), Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich; German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (K.B., R.P., M.D.), DZNE, Munich; Institute for Medical Technology (E.S.), Brandenburg University of Technology Cottbus-Senftenberg; Munich Cluster of Systems Neurology (R.P., P.B., M.D.), SyNergy, Germany; Ageing Epidemiology Research Unit (R.P.), School of Public Health, Imperial College, London, UK; Neurological Hospital (K.J.), Schön Klinik Bad Aibling; and Department of Nuclear Medicine (C.l.F.), Eberhard Karl University of Tübingen, Germany
| | - Julia Engmann
- From the Department of Neurology (F.S., J.E., P.W., A.Z., M.D.), University Hospital, German Center for Vertigo and Balance Disorders (F.S., C.P., S.I., G.X., G.K., S.K., E.S., K.J., C.l.F., P.B., T.B., M.D., A.Z.), DSGZ, Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research (K.B.), ISD, University Hospital, Department of Nuclear Medicine (G.X., M.B., P.B.), Department of Psychiatry (R.P.), and Clinical Neurosciences (T.B.), Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich; German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (K.B., R.P., M.D.), DZNE, Munich; Institute for Medical Technology (E.S.), Brandenburg University of Technology Cottbus-Senftenberg; Munich Cluster of Systems Neurology (R.P., P.B., M.D.), SyNergy, Germany; Ageing Epidemiology Research Unit (R.P.), School of Public Health, Imperial College, London, UK; Neurological Hospital (K.J.), Schön Klinik Bad Aibling; and Department of Nuclear Medicine (C.l.F.), Eberhard Karl University of Tübingen, Germany
| | - Philipp Werner
- From the Department of Neurology (F.S., J.E., P.W., A.Z., M.D.), University Hospital, German Center for Vertigo and Balance Disorders (F.S., C.P., S.I., G.X., G.K., S.K., E.S., K.J., C.l.F., P.B., T.B., M.D., A.Z.), DSGZ, Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research (K.B.), ISD, University Hospital, Department of Nuclear Medicine (G.X., M.B., P.B.), Department of Psychiatry (R.P.), and Clinical Neurosciences (T.B.), Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich; German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (K.B., R.P., M.D.), DZNE, Munich; Institute for Medical Technology (E.S.), Brandenburg University of Technology Cottbus-Senftenberg; Munich Cluster of Systems Neurology (R.P., P.B., M.D.), SyNergy, Germany; Ageing Epidemiology Research Unit (R.P.), School of Public Health, Imperial College, London, UK; Neurological Hospital (K.J.), Schön Klinik Bad Aibling; and Department of Nuclear Medicine (C.l.F.), Eberhard Karl University of Tübingen, Germany
| | - Matthias Brendel
- From the Department of Neurology (F.S., J.E., P.W., A.Z., M.D.), University Hospital, German Center for Vertigo and Balance Disorders (F.S., C.P., S.I., G.X., G.K., S.K., E.S., K.J., C.l.F., P.B., T.B., M.D., A.Z.), DSGZ, Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research (K.B.), ISD, University Hospital, Department of Nuclear Medicine (G.X., M.B., P.B.), Department of Psychiatry (R.P.), and Clinical Neurosciences (T.B.), Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich; German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (K.B., R.P., M.D.), DZNE, Munich; Institute for Medical Technology (E.S.), Brandenburg University of Technology Cottbus-Senftenberg; Munich Cluster of Systems Neurology (R.P., P.B., M.D.), SyNergy, Germany; Ageing Epidemiology Research Unit (R.P.), School of Public Health, Imperial College, London, UK; Neurological Hospital (K.J.), Schön Klinik Bad Aibling; and Department of Nuclear Medicine (C.l.F.), Eberhard Karl University of Tübingen, Germany
| | - Erich Schneider
- From the Department of Neurology (F.S., J.E., P.W., A.Z., M.D.), University Hospital, German Center for Vertigo and Balance Disorders (F.S., C.P., S.I., G.X., G.K., S.K., E.S., K.J., C.l.F., P.B., T.B., M.D., A.Z.), DSGZ, Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research (K.B.), ISD, University Hospital, Department of Nuclear Medicine (G.X., M.B., P.B.), Department of Psychiatry (R.P.), and Clinical Neurosciences (T.B.), Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich; German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (K.B., R.P., M.D.), DZNE, Munich; Institute for Medical Technology (E.S.), Brandenburg University of Technology Cottbus-Senftenberg; Munich Cluster of Systems Neurology (R.P., P.B., M.D.), SyNergy, Germany; Ageing Epidemiology Research Unit (R.P.), School of Public Health, Imperial College, London, UK; Neurological Hospital (K.J.), Schön Klinik Bad Aibling; and Department of Nuclear Medicine (C.l.F.), Eberhard Karl University of Tübingen, Germany
| | - Robert Perneczky
- From the Department of Neurology (F.S., J.E., P.W., A.Z., M.D.), University Hospital, German Center for Vertigo and Balance Disorders (F.S., C.P., S.I., G.X., G.K., S.K., E.S., K.J., C.l.F., P.B., T.B., M.D., A.Z.), DSGZ, Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research (K.B.), ISD, University Hospital, Department of Nuclear Medicine (G.X., M.B., P.B.), Department of Psychiatry (R.P.), and Clinical Neurosciences (T.B.), Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich; German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (K.B., R.P., M.D.), DZNE, Munich; Institute for Medical Technology (E.S.), Brandenburg University of Technology Cottbus-Senftenberg; Munich Cluster of Systems Neurology (R.P., P.B., M.D.), SyNergy, Germany; Ageing Epidemiology Research Unit (R.P.), School of Public Health, Imperial College, London, UK; Neurological Hospital (K.J.), Schön Klinik Bad Aibling; and Department of Nuclear Medicine (C.l.F.), Eberhard Karl University of Tübingen, Germany
| | - Klaus Jahn
- From the Department of Neurology (F.S., J.E., P.W., A.Z., M.D.), University Hospital, German Center for Vertigo and Balance Disorders (F.S., C.P., S.I., G.X., G.K., S.K., E.S., K.J., C.l.F., P.B., T.B., M.D., A.Z.), DSGZ, Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research (K.B.), ISD, University Hospital, Department of Nuclear Medicine (G.X., M.B., P.B.), Department of Psychiatry (R.P.), and Clinical Neurosciences (T.B.), Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich; German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (K.B., R.P., M.D.), DZNE, Munich; Institute for Medical Technology (E.S.), Brandenburg University of Technology Cottbus-Senftenberg; Munich Cluster of Systems Neurology (R.P., P.B., M.D.), SyNergy, Germany; Ageing Epidemiology Research Unit (R.P.), School of Public Health, Imperial College, London, UK; Neurological Hospital (K.J.), Schön Klinik Bad Aibling; and Department of Nuclear Medicine (C.l.F.), Eberhard Karl University of Tübingen, Germany
| | - Christian la Fougère
- From the Department of Neurology (F.S., J.E., P.W., A.Z., M.D.), University Hospital, German Center for Vertigo and Balance Disorders (F.S., C.P., S.I., G.X., G.K., S.K., E.S., K.J., C.l.F., P.B., T.B., M.D., A.Z.), DSGZ, Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research (K.B.), ISD, University Hospital, Department of Nuclear Medicine (G.X., M.B., P.B.), Department of Psychiatry (R.P.), and Clinical Neurosciences (T.B.), Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich; German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (K.B., R.P., M.D.), DZNE, Munich; Institute for Medical Technology (E.S.), Brandenburg University of Technology Cottbus-Senftenberg; Munich Cluster of Systems Neurology (R.P., P.B., M.D.), SyNergy, Germany; Ageing Epidemiology Research Unit (R.P.), School of Public Health, Imperial College, London, UK; Neurological Hospital (K.J.), Schön Klinik Bad Aibling; and Department of Nuclear Medicine (C.l.F.), Eberhard Karl University of Tübingen, Germany
| | - Peter Bartenstein
- From the Department of Neurology (F.S., J.E., P.W., A.Z., M.D.), University Hospital, German Center for Vertigo and Balance Disorders (F.S., C.P., S.I., G.X., G.K., S.K., E.S., K.J., C.l.F., P.B., T.B., M.D., A.Z.), DSGZ, Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research (K.B.), ISD, University Hospital, Department of Nuclear Medicine (G.X., M.B., P.B.), Department of Psychiatry (R.P.), and Clinical Neurosciences (T.B.), Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich; German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (K.B., R.P., M.D.), DZNE, Munich; Institute for Medical Technology (E.S.), Brandenburg University of Technology Cottbus-Senftenberg; Munich Cluster of Systems Neurology (R.P., P.B., M.D.), SyNergy, Germany; Ageing Epidemiology Research Unit (R.P.), School of Public Health, Imperial College, London, UK; Neurological Hospital (K.J.), Schön Klinik Bad Aibling; and Department of Nuclear Medicine (C.l.F.), Eberhard Karl University of Tübingen, Germany
| | - Thomas Brandt
- From the Department of Neurology (F.S., J.E., P.W., A.Z., M.D.), University Hospital, German Center for Vertigo and Balance Disorders (F.S., C.P., S.I., G.X., G.K., S.K., E.S., K.J., C.l.F., P.B., T.B., M.D., A.Z.), DSGZ, Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research (K.B.), ISD, University Hospital, Department of Nuclear Medicine (G.X., M.B., P.B.), Department of Psychiatry (R.P.), and Clinical Neurosciences (T.B.), Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich; German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (K.B., R.P., M.D.), DZNE, Munich; Institute for Medical Technology (E.S.), Brandenburg University of Technology Cottbus-Senftenberg; Munich Cluster of Systems Neurology (R.P., P.B., M.D.), SyNergy, Germany; Ageing Epidemiology Research Unit (R.P.), School of Public Health, Imperial College, London, UK; Neurological Hospital (K.J.), Schön Klinik Bad Aibling; and Department of Nuclear Medicine (C.l.F.), Eberhard Karl University of Tübingen, Germany
| | - Marianne Dieterich
- From the Department of Neurology (F.S., J.E., P.W., A.Z., M.D.), University Hospital, German Center for Vertigo and Balance Disorders (F.S., C.P., S.I., G.X., G.K., S.K., E.S., K.J., C.l.F., P.B., T.B., M.D., A.Z.), DSGZ, Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research (K.B.), ISD, University Hospital, Department of Nuclear Medicine (G.X., M.B., P.B.), Department of Psychiatry (R.P.), and Clinical Neurosciences (T.B.), Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich; German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (K.B., R.P., M.D.), DZNE, Munich; Institute for Medical Technology (E.S.), Brandenburg University of Technology Cottbus-Senftenberg; Munich Cluster of Systems Neurology (R.P., P.B., M.D.), SyNergy, Germany; Ageing Epidemiology Research Unit (R.P.), School of Public Health, Imperial College, London, UK; Neurological Hospital (K.J.), Schön Klinik Bad Aibling; and Department of Nuclear Medicine (C.l.F.), Eberhard Karl University of Tübingen, Germany
| | - Andreas Zwergal
- From the Department of Neurology (F.S., J.E., P.W., A.Z., M.D.), University Hospital, German Center for Vertigo and Balance Disorders (F.S., C.P., S.I., G.X., G.K., S.K., E.S., K.J., C.l.F., P.B., T.B., M.D., A.Z.), DSGZ, Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research (K.B.), ISD, University Hospital, Department of Nuclear Medicine (G.X., M.B., P.B.), Department of Psychiatry (R.P.), and Clinical Neurosciences (T.B.), Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich; German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (K.B., R.P., M.D.), DZNE, Munich; Institute for Medical Technology (E.S.), Brandenburg University of Technology Cottbus-Senftenberg; Munich Cluster of Systems Neurology (R.P., P.B., M.D.), SyNergy, Germany; Ageing Epidemiology Research Unit (R.P.), School of Public Health, Imperial College, London, UK; Neurological Hospital (K.J.), Schön Klinik Bad Aibling; and Department of Nuclear Medicine (C.l.F.), Eberhard Karl University of Tübingen, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
44
|
Jin W, Qin H, Zhang K, Chen X. Spatial Navigation. ADVANCES IN EXPERIMENTAL MEDICINE AND BIOLOGY 2020; 1284:63-90. [PMID: 32852741 DOI: 10.1007/978-981-15-7086-5_7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/13/2022]
Abstract
The hippocampus is critical for spatial navigation. In this review, we focus on the role of the hippocampus in three basic strategies used for spatial navigation: path integration, stimulus-response association, and map-based navigation. First, the hippocampus is not required for path integration unless the path of path integration is too long and complex. The hippocampus provides mnemonic support when involved in the process of path integration. Second, the hippocampus's involvement in stimulus-response association is dependent on how the strategy is conducted. The hippocampus is not required for the habit form of stimulus-response association. Third, while the hippocampus is fully engaged in map-based navigation, the shared characteristics of place cells, grid cells, head direction cells, and other spatial encoding cells, which are detected in the hippocampus and associated areas, offer a possibility that there is a stand-alone allocentric space perception (or mental representation) of the environment outside and independent of the hippocampus, and the spatially specific firing patterns of these spatial encoding cells are the unfolding of the intermediate stages of the processing of this allocentric spatial information when conveyed into the hippocampus for information storage or retrieval. Furthermore, the presence of all the spatially specific firing patterns in the hippocampus and the related neural circuits during the path integration and map-based navigation support such a notion that in essence, path integration is the same allocentric space perception provided with only idiothetic inputs. Taken together, the hippocampus plays a general mnemonic role in spatial navigation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wenjun Jin
- Brain Research Center and State Key Laboratory of Trauma, Burns, and Combined Injury, Third Military Medical University, Chongqing, China.
| | - Han Qin
- Brain Research Center and State Key Laboratory of Trauma, Burns, and Combined Injury, Third Military Medical University, Chongqing, China
| | - Kuan Zhang
- Brain Research Center and State Key Laboratory of Trauma, Burns, and Combined Injury, Third Military Medical University, Chongqing, China
| | - Xiaowei Chen
- Brain Research Center and State Key Laboratory of Trauma, Burns, and Combined Injury, Third Military Medical University, Chongqing, China
| |
Collapse
|
45
|
Xu Z, Wu W, Winter SS, Mehlman ML, Butler WN, Simmons CM, Harvey RE, Berkowitz LE, Chen Y, Taube JS, Wilber AA, Clark BJ. A Comparison of Neural Decoding Methods and Population Coding Across Thalamo-Cortical Head Direction Cells. Front Neural Circuits 2019; 13:75. [PMID: 31920565 PMCID: PMC6914739 DOI: 10.3389/fncir.2019.00075] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/13/2019] [Accepted: 11/12/2019] [Indexed: 12/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Head direction (HD) cells, which fire action potentials whenever an animal points its head in a particular direction, are thought to subserve the animal's sense of spatial orientation. HD cells are found prominently in several thalamo-cortical regions including anterior thalamic nuclei, postsubiculum, medial entorhinal cortex, parasubiculum, and the parietal cortex. While a number of methods in neural decoding have been developed to assess the dynamics of spatial signals within thalamo-cortical regions, studies conducting a quantitative comparison of machine learning and statistical model-based decoding methods on HD cell activity are currently lacking. Here, we compare statistical model-based and machine learning approaches by assessing decoding accuracy and evaluate variables that contribute to population coding across thalamo-cortical HD cells.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zishen Xu
- Department of Statistics, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, United States
| | - Wei Wu
- Department of Statistics, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, United States
| | - Shawn S. Winter
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, United States
| | - Max L. Mehlman
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, United States
| | - William N. Butler
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, United States
| | - Christine M. Simmons
- Department of Psychology, Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, United States
| | - Ryan E. Harvey
- Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, United States
| | - Laura E. Berkowitz
- Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, United States
| | - Yang Chen
- Department of Statistics, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, United States
| | - Jeffrey S. Taube
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, United States
| | - Aaron A. Wilber
- Department of Psychology, Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, United States
| | - Benjamin J. Clark
- Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, United States
| |
Collapse
|
46
|
Harvey RE, Berkowitz LE, Hamilton DA, Clark BJ. The effects of developmental alcohol exposure on the neurobiology of spatial processing. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2019; 107:775-794. [PMID: 31526818 PMCID: PMC6876993 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.09.018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/16/2019] [Revised: 08/02/2019] [Accepted: 09/11/2019] [Indexed: 01/20/2023]
Abstract
The consumption of alcohol during gestation is detrimental to the developing central nervous system. One functional outcome of this exposure is impaired spatial processing, defined as sensing and integrating information pertaining to spatial navigation and spatial memory. The hippocampus, entorhinal cortex, and anterior thalamus are brain regions implicated in spatial processing and are highly susceptible to the effects of developmental alcohol exposure. Some of the observed effects of alcohol on spatial processing may be attributed to changes at the synaptic to circuit level. In this review, we first describe the impact of developmental alcohol exposure on spatial behavior followed by a summary of the development of brain areas involved in spatial processing. We then provide an examination of the consequences of prenatal and early postnatal alcohol exposure in rodents on hippocampal, anterior thalamus, and entorhinal cortex-dependent spatial processing from the cellular to behavioral level. We conclude by highlighting several unanswered questions which may provide a framework for future investigation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ryan E Harvey
- Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, United States
| | - Laura E Berkowitz
- Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, United States
| | - Derek A Hamilton
- Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, United States
| | - Benjamin J Clark
- Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
47
|
Stimmell AC, Baglietto-Vargas D, Moseley SC, Lapointe V, Thompson LM, LaFerla FM, McNaughton BL, Wilber AA. Impaired Spatial Reorientation in the 3xTg-AD Mouse Model of Alzheimer's Disease. Sci Rep 2019; 9:1311. [PMID: 30718609 PMCID: PMC6361963 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-018-37151-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/23/2018] [Accepted: 11/15/2018] [Indexed: 01/08/2023] Open
Abstract
In early Alzheimer's disease (AD) spatial navigation is impaired; however, the precise cause of this impairment is unclear. Recent evidence suggests that getting lost is one of the first impairments to emerge in AD. It is possible that getting lost represents a failure to use distal cues to get oriented in space. Therefore, we set out to look for impaired use of distal cues for spatial orientation in a mouse model of amyloidosis (3xTg-AD). To do this, we trained mice to shuttle to the end of a track and back to an enclosed start box to receive a water reward. Then, mice were trained to stop in an unmarked reward zone to receive a brain stimulation reward. The time required to remain in the zone for a reward was increased across training, and the track was positioned in a random start location for each trial. We found that 6-month female, but not 3-month female, 6-month male, or 12-month male, 3xTg-AD mice were impaired. 6-month male and female mice had only intracellular pathology and male mice had less pathology, particularly in the dorsal hippocampus. Thus, AD may cause spatial disorientation as a result of impaired use of landmarks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alina C Stimmell
- Department of Psychology, Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida, USA.
| | | | - Shawn C Moseley
- Department of Psychology, Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida, USA
| | - Valérie Lapointe
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta, Canada
| | - Lauren M Thompson
- Department of Psychology, Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida, USA
| | - Frank M LaFerla
- Neurobiology and Behavior, University of California Irvine, Irvine, California, USA
| | - Bruce L McNaughton
- Neurobiology and Behavior, University of California Irvine, Irvine, California, USA
- Department of Neuroscience, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta, Canada
| | - Aaron A Wilber
- Department of Psychology, Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
48
|
Nelson AJD, Hindley EL, Vann SD, Aggleton JP. When is the rat retrosplenial cortex required for stimulus integration? Behav Neurosci 2018; 132:366-377. [PMID: 30321026 PMCID: PMC6188469 DOI: 10.1037/bne0000267] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/15/2018] [Revised: 06/05/2018] [Accepted: 06/18/2018] [Indexed: 12/16/2022]
Abstract
The rodent retrosplenial cortex is known to be vital for spatial cognition, but evidence has also pointed to a role in processing nonspatial information. It has been suggested that the retrosplenial cortex may serve as a site of integration of incoming sensory information. To examine this proposal, the current set of experiments assessed the impact of excitotoxic lesions in the retrosplenial cortex on two behavioral tasks that tax animals' ability to process multiple and overlapping environmental stimuli. In Experiment 1, rats with retrosplenial lesions acquired a negative patterning discrimination, a form of configural learning that can be solved only by learning the conjunction of cues. Subsequent transfer tests confirmed that both the lesion and control animals had solved the task by using configural representations. Furthermore, in Experiment 2, a 2nd cohort of retrosplenial lesion animals successfully acquired conditioned inhibition. Nevertheless, the same animals failed a subsequent summation test that assesses the ability to transfer what has been learned about one stimulus to another stimulus in the absence of reinforcement. Taken together, these results suggest that in the nonspatial domain, the retrosplenial cortex is not required for forming associations between multiple or overlapping environmental stimuli and, consequently, retrosplenial engagement in such processes is more selective than was previously envisaged. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2018 APA, all rights reserved).
Collapse
|
49
|
Solari N, Hangya B. Cholinergic modulation of spatial learning, memory and navigation. Eur J Neurosci 2018; 48:2199-2230. [PMID: 30055067 PMCID: PMC6174978 DOI: 10.1111/ejn.14089] [Citation(s) in RCA: 60] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/24/2018] [Revised: 06/25/2018] [Accepted: 07/23/2018] [Indexed: 01/02/2023]
Abstract
Spatial learning, including encoding and retrieval of spatial memories as well as holding spatial information in working memory generally serving navigation under a broad range of circumstances, relies on a network of structures. While central to this network are medial temporal lobe structures with a widely appreciated crucial function of the hippocampus, neocortical areas such as the posterior parietal cortex and the retrosplenial cortex also play essential roles. Since the hippocampus receives its main subcortical input from the medial septum of the basal forebrain (BF) cholinergic system, it is not surprising that the potential role of the septo-hippocampal pathway in spatial navigation has been investigated in many studies. Much less is known of the involvement in spatial cognition of the parallel projection system linking the posterior BF with neocortical areas. Here we review the current state of the art of the division of labour within this complex 'navigation system', with special focus on how subcortical cholinergic inputs may regulate various aspects of spatial learning, memory and navigation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicola Solari
- Lendület Laboratory of Systems NeuroscienceDepartment of Cellular and Network NeurobiologyInstitute of Experimental MedicineHungarian Academy of SciencesBudapestHungary
| | - Balázs Hangya
- Lendület Laboratory of Systems NeuroscienceDepartment of Cellular and Network NeurobiologyInstitute of Experimental MedicineHungarian Academy of SciencesBudapestHungary
| |
Collapse
|