1
|
Rocha PN, Gawryszewski FM. Foraging strategy as a route for sexual size dimorphism evolution. Ecol Evol 2024; 14:e70100. [PMID: 39512849 PMCID: PMC11542996 DOI: 10.1002/ece3.70100] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/17/2024] [Revised: 06/30/2024] [Accepted: 07/11/2024] [Indexed: 11/15/2024] Open
Abstract
Female-biased sexual size dimorphism stands as a widespread evolutionary pattern. Fecundity selection, favouring larger females with greater reproductive output, is a leading explanation. However, larger body sizes demand greater energy intake, potentially hindering the evolution of extreme female sizes. Thus, the evolution of more lucrative foraging tactics may allow for an increase in size. Hence, coupled with selection against larger males, fecundity selection should result in larger SSD in species with more lucrative foraging strategies. Crab spiders are sit-and-wait predators that hunt in several plant substrata. Species that forage on flowers or employ prey-luring strategies likely have access to higher food intake than other species. We extracted body size measurements of 614 crab spider species from 43 genera and classified them based on their foraging strategy. Our findings show that foraging strategies that provide higher energy input (EFS) result in larger SSD. Statistical estimates indicate that females have a cephalothorax width 91% larger than males in EFS species, compared to 26% larger females than males in non-EFS species. These differences possibly arise due to larger females and smaller males. The effects on male size reduction might result from scramble competition, whereas the increase in female size is likely due to fecundity selection. These results suggest that the shift towards more lucrative foraging strategies may have been a key event in body size and SSD evolution in crab spiders.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pedro N. Rocha
- Departamento de Zoologia, Evolutionary Ecology LaboratoryUniversidade de BrasíliaBrasíliaBrazil
- Programa de Pós‐Graduação em EcologiaUniversidade de BrasíliaBrasíliaBrazil
| | - Felipe M. Gawryszewski
- Departamento de Zoologia, Evolutionary Ecology LaboratoryUniversidade de BrasíliaBrasíliaBrazil
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Villanueva-Bonilla GA, Mesas YF, de Oliveira Gonzaga M, da Silva Souza H, Brescovit AD, Sobczak JF, Neto JV. Fine-scale habitat selection in tree-dwelling spiders: an experimental approach. THE SCIENCE OF NATURE - NATURWISSENSCHAFTEN 2024; 111:60. [PMID: 39476258 DOI: 10.1007/s00114-024-01947-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/16/2024] [Revised: 10/21/2024] [Accepted: 10/22/2024] [Indexed: 12/07/2024]
Abstract
Habitat selection by spiders is strongly influenced by biotic factors such as the availability and diversity of prey and abiotic factors such as temperature, humidity, and the structural complexity of the habitat. Structural complexity is an aspect that intensely affects species persistence, population stability, and the coexistence of interacting species. Trees comprise a complex set of microhabitats due to their large biomass and heterogeneity of the architectural components of their trunk surface and branches. Spider species that live on trunks have diversified physiological or morphological adaptations that confer advantages in this environment. In this study, we experimentally examined the habitat choice by the tree-dwelling spiders Selenops cocheleti (Selenopidae), Corinna rubripes (Corinnidae), and Loxosceles gaucho (Sicariidae). We found that microhabitat specialization was restricted to trunk architectural characteristics rather than plant taxonomy. Selenops cocheleti and C. rubripes significantly preferred loose barks and holes in the trunks, respectively, showing that both spider species can evaluate the physical structure of the microhabitat on a fine scale. On the other hand, L. gaucho selected crevices and holes near the base of the trunk without giving much importance to the physical characteristics of the microhabitat per se (e.g., depth, height, length). Our findings indicate that for generalist predators like spiders, coexistence relies heavily on spatial segregation driven by distinct habitat preferences, irrespective of their method for capturing prey.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- German Antonio Villanueva-Bonilla
- Departamento de Biologia, Universidade Federal Do Ceará, Av. da Universidade, Benfica, Fortaleza, Ceará, 2853, Brazil.
- Instituto de Biologia, Departamento de Biologia Animal, Universidade Estadual de Campinas, Campinas, São Paulo, SP, Brazil.
| | - Yuri Fanchini Mesas
- Instituto de Biologia, Departamento de Biologia Animal, Universidade Estadual de Campinas, Campinas, São Paulo, SP, Brazil
| | - Marcelo de Oliveira Gonzaga
- Instituto de Biologia, Universidade Federal de Uberlândia, Av. Amazonas 20, Umuarama, Uberlândia, Minas Gerais, Brazil
| | - Hebert da Silva Souza
- Instituto de Biologia, Universidade Federal de Uberlândia, Av. Amazonas 20, Umuarama, Uberlândia, Minas Gerais, Brazil
| | | | - Jober Fernando Sobczak
- Instituto de Ciências Exatas e da Natureza, Universidade Federal da Integração Internacional da Lusofonia Afro-Brasileira, Redenção, Brazil
| | - João Vasconcellos Neto
- Instituto de Biologia, Departamento de Biologia Animal, Universidade Estadual de Campinas, Campinas, São Paulo, SP, Brazil
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Gavini SS, Quintero C. Predation risk and floral rewards: How pollinators balance these conflicts and the consequences on plant fitness. CURRENT RESEARCH IN INSECT SCIENCE 2024; 6:100091. [PMID: 39193177 PMCID: PMC11345579 DOI: 10.1016/j.cris.2024.100091] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/13/2023] [Revised: 07/09/2024] [Accepted: 07/15/2024] [Indexed: 08/29/2024]
Abstract
Foraging behavior of pollinators is shaped by, among other factors, the conflict between maximizing resource intake and minimizing predation risk; yet, empirical studies quantifying variation in both forces are rare, compared to those investigating each separately. Here, we discuss the importance of simultaneously assessing bottom-up and top-down forces in the study of plant-pollinator interactions, and propose a conceptual and testable graphical hypothesis for pollinator foraging behavior and plant fitness outcomes as a function of varying floral rewards and predation risk. In low predation risk scenarios, no noticeable changes in pollinator foraging behavior are expected, with reward levels affecting only the activity threshold. However, as predation risk increases we propose that there is a decrease in foraging behavior, with a steeper decline as plants are more rewarding and profitable. Lastly, in high predation risk scenarios, we expect foraging to approach zero, regardless of floral rewards. Thus, we propose that pollinator foraging behavior follows an inverse S-shape curve, with more pronounced changes in foraging activity at intermediate levels of predation risk, especially in high reward systems. We present empirical evidence that is consistent with this hypothesis. In terms of the consequences for plant fitness, we propose that specialized plant-pollinator systems should be more vulnerable to increased predation risk, with a steeper and faster decline in plant fitness, compared with generalist systems, in which pollinator redundancy can delay or buffer the effect of predators. Moreover, whereas we expect that specialist systems follows a similar inverse S-shape curve, in generalist systems we propose three different scenarios as a function not only of reward level but also compatibility, mating-system, and the interplay between growth form and floral display. The incorporation of trade-offs in pollinator behavior balancing the conflicting demands between feeding and predation risk has a promising future as a key feature enabling the development of more complex foraging models.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sabrina S. Gavini
- INIBIOMA, CONICET-CRUB, Universidad Nacional del Comahue, Quintral 1250, San Carlos de Bariloche, Río Negro, Argentina
| | - Carolina Quintero
- INIBIOMA, CONICET-CRUB, Universidad Nacional del Comahue, Quintral 1250, San Carlos de Bariloche, Río Negro, Argentina
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
England SJ, Robert D. Electrostatic pollination by butterflies and moths. J R Soc Interface 2024; 21:20240156. [PMID: 39044626 PMCID: PMC11267234 DOI: 10.1098/rsif.2024.0156] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/06/2024] [Revised: 04/19/2024] [Accepted: 05/30/2024] [Indexed: 07/25/2024] Open
Abstract
Animals, most notably insects, generally seem to accumulate electrostatic charge in nature. These electrostatic charges will exert forces on other charges in these animals' environments and therefore have the potential to attract or repel other objects, for example, pollen from flowers. Here, we show that butterflies and moths (Lepidoptera) accumulate electrostatic charge while in flight. Then, using finite element analysis, we demonstrate that when within millimetres of a flower, the electrostatic charge of a lepidopteran generates an electric field in excess of 5 kV m-1, and that an electric field of this magnitude is sufficient to elicit contactless pollen transfer from flowers across air gaps onto the body of a butterfly or moth. Furthermore, we see that phylogenetic variations exist in the magnitude and polarity of net charge between different species and families and Lepidoptera. These phylogenetic variations in electrostatic charging correlate with morphological, biogeographical and ecological differences between different clades. Such correlations with biogeographical and ecological differences may reflect evolutionary adaptations towards maximizing or minimizing charge accumulation, in relation to pollination, predation and parasitism, and thus we introduce the idea that electrostatic charging may be a trait upon which evolution can act.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sam J. England
- School of Biological Sciences, Faculty of Life Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
| | - Daniel Robert
- School of Biological Sciences, Faculty of Life Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Martin-Ordas G. Relational reasoning in wild bumblebees revisited: the role of distance. Sci Rep 2023; 13:22311. [PMID: 38102236 PMCID: PMC10724225 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-49840-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/14/2023] [Accepted: 12/12/2023] [Indexed: 12/17/2023] Open
Abstract
In reasoning tasks, non-human animals attend more to relational than to object similarity. It is precisely this focus on relational similarity that has been argued to explain the reasoning gap between humans and other animals. Work with humans has revealed that objects placed near each other are represented to be more similar than objects placed farther apart. Will distance between objects also affect non-human animals' abilities to represent and reason about objects? To test this, wild bumblebees were presented with a spatial reasoning task (with competing object matches) in which the objects or features alone (colour, shape) were placed close together or far apart. Bumblebees spontaneously attended to objects over relations, but only when the objects were far apart. Features alone were not strong enough to drive object matching-suggesting that bumblebees bound colour and shape into their object representations. These findings question whether the ability to focus on and compare objects is what makes human abstract reasoning unique.
Collapse
|
6
|
Sinha SK, Dolai A, Roy AB, Manna S, Das A. The Flower Colour Influences Spontaneous Nectaring in Butterflies: a Case Study with Twenty Subtropical Butterflies. NEOTROPICAL ENTOMOLOGY 2023; 52:1027-1040. [PMID: 37819480 DOI: 10.1007/s13744-023-01086-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/14/2022] [Accepted: 09/14/2023] [Indexed: 10/13/2023]
Abstract
Butterflies have a wide spectrum of colour vision, and changes in flower colour influence both the visiting and nectaring (the act of feeding on flower nectar) events of them. However, the spontaneous behavioural response of butterflies while foraging on real flowers is less characterised in wild conditions. Hence, this study intends to investigate flower colour affinity in wild butterflies in relation to nectaring frequency (NF) and nectaring duration (ND). Six distinct flower colours were used to study spontaneous nectaring behaviour in 20 species of subtropical butterflies. Both NF and ND greatly varied in the flower colours they offered. Yellow flowers were frequently imbibed by butterflies for longer durations, followed by orange, while red, pink, white and violet flowers were occasionally nectared in shorter bouts. Though butterflies have a general tendency to nectar on multiple flower colours, the Nymphalids were more biased towards nectaring on yellow flowers, but Papilionids preferred both yellow and orange, while the Pierids were likely to display an equal affinity for yellow, orange and violet flowers as their first order of preference. Even if the blooms are associated with higher nectar concentrations or a significant grade reward, the butterfly may prefer to visit different-coloured flowers instead. Flower colour choice appears to be a generalist phenomenon for butterflies, but their specialist visiting nature was also significant. Nymphalid representatives responded to a wider variety of floral colour affinities than Pierid and Papilionid species. The colour preference of butterflies aids in the identification of flowers during foraging and influences subsequent foraging decisions, which ultimately benefits pollination success. The current information will support the preservation and conservation of butterflies in their natural habitats.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sumit Kumar Sinha
- Entomology Laboratory, Department of Zoology, University of Calcutta, Calcutta, West Bengal, India
| | - Avishek Dolai
- Entomology Laboratory, Department of Zoology, University of Calcutta, Calcutta, West Bengal, India
| | | | - Sourav Manna
- Semiochemical and lipid laboratory, Department of Life Science, Presidency University, Kolkata, West Bengal, India
| | - Amlan Das
- Entomology Laboratory, Department of Zoology, University of Calcutta, Calcutta, West Bengal, India.
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Lo YC, Blamires SJ, Liao CP, Tso IM. Nocturnal and diurnal predator and prey interactions with crab spider color polymorphs. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2023. [DOI: 10.1007/s00265-023-03291-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/04/2023]
|
8
|
Fairnie ALM, Yeo MTS, Gatti S, Chan E, Travaglia V, Walker JF, Moyroud E. Eco-Evo-Devo of petal pigmentation patterning. Essays Biochem 2022; 66:753-768. [PMID: 36205404 PMCID: PMC9750854 DOI: 10.1042/ebc20220051] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/01/2022] [Revised: 09/09/2022] [Accepted: 09/13/2022] [Indexed: 12/13/2022]
Abstract
Colourful spots, stripes and rings decorate the corolla of most flowering plants and fulfil important biotic and abiotic functions. Spatial differences in the pigmentation of epidermal cells can create these patterns. The last few years have yielded new data that have started to illuminate the mechanisms controlling the function, formation and evolution of petal patterns. These advances have broad impacts beyond the immediate field as pigmentation patterns are wonderful systems to explore multiscale biological problems: from understanding how cells make decisions at the microscale to examining the roots of biodiversity at the macroscale. These new results also reveal there is more to petal patterning than meets the eye, opening up a brand new area of investigation. In this mini-review, we summarise our current knowledge on the Eco-Evo-Devo of petal pigmentation patterns and discuss some of the most exciting yet unanswered questions that represent avenues for future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alice L M Fairnie
- The Sainsbury Laboratory, University of Cambridge, Bateman Street, Cambridge CB2 1LR, U.K
| | - May T S Yeo
- The Sainsbury Laboratory, University of Cambridge, Bateman Street, Cambridge CB2 1LR, U.K
- Department of Genetics, Downing Site, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EJ, U.K
| | - Stefano Gatti
- The Sainsbury Laboratory, University of Cambridge, Bateman Street, Cambridge CB2 1LR, U.K
| | - Emily Chan
- The Sainsbury Laboratory, University of Cambridge, Bateman Street, Cambridge CB2 1LR, U.K
| | - Valentina Travaglia
- The Sainsbury Laboratory, University of Cambridge, Bateman Street, Cambridge CB2 1LR, U.K
| | - Joseph F Walker
- The Sainsbury Laboratory, University of Cambridge, Bateman Street, Cambridge CB2 1LR, U.K
| | - Edwige Moyroud
- The Sainsbury Laboratory, University of Cambridge, Bateman Street, Cambridge CB2 1LR, U.K
- Department of Genetics, Downing Site, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EJ, U.K
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Yu L, Xu X, Zhang Z, Painting CJ, Yang X, Li D. Masquerading predators deceive prey by aggressively mimicking bird droppings in a crab spider. Curr Zool 2022; 68:325-334. [PMID: 35592341 PMCID: PMC9113284 DOI: 10.1093/cz/zoab060] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/23/2021] [Accepted: 07/22/2021] [Indexed: 12/02/2022] Open
Abstract
In aggressive mimicry, a predator accesses prey by mimicking the appearance and/or behavior of a harmless or beneficial model in order to avoid being correctly identified by its prey. The crab spider genus Phrynarachne is often cited as a textbook example of masquerading as bird droppings (BDs) in order to avoid predation. However, Phrynarachne spiders may also aggressively mimic BDs in order to deceive potential prey. To date, there is no experimental evidence to support aggressive mimicry in masquerading crab spiders; therefore, we performed a field survey, a manipulative field experiment, and visual modeling to test this hypothesis using Phrynarachne ceylonica. We compared prey-attraction rates among BDs, spiders, and control empty leaves in the field. We found that although all prey combined and agromyzid dipterans, in particular, were attracted to BDs at a higher rate than to spiders, other dipterans and hymenopterans were attracted to BDs at a similar rate as to spiders. Both spiders and BDs attracted insects at a significantly higher rate than did control leaves. As predicted, prey was attracted to experimentally blackened or whitened spiders significantly less frequently than to unmanipulated spiders. Finally, visual modeling suggested that spiders and BDs can be detected by dipterans and hymenopterans against background leaves, but they are indistinguishable from each other. Taken together, our results suggest that insects lured by spiders may misidentify them as BDs, and bird-dropping masquerading may serve as aggressive mimicry in addition to predator avoidance in P. ceylonica.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Long Yu
- State Key Laboratory of Biocatalysis and Enzyme Engineering of China & Centre for Behavioural Ecology & Evolution, School of Life Sciences, Hubei University, Wuhan 430062, Hubei, China
| | - Xin Xu
- State Key Laboratory of Biocatalysis and Enzyme Engineering of China & Centre for Behavioural Ecology & Evolution, School of Life Sciences, Hubei University, Wuhan 430062, Hubei, China
- College of Life Sciences, Hunan Normal University, Changsha 410006, Hunan, China
| | - Zengtao Zhang
- State Key Laboratory of Biocatalysis and Enzyme Engineering of China & Centre for Behavioural Ecology & Evolution, School of Life Sciences, Hubei University, Wuhan 430062, Hubei, China
| | | | - Xiaodong Yang
- CAS Key Laboratory of Tropical Forest Ecology, Xishuangbanna Tropical Botanical Garden, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Yunnan 666303, China
| | - Daiqin Li
- Department of Biological Sciences, National University of Singapore, 14 Science Drive 4, 117543, Singapore
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Schmalz F, El Jundi B, Rössler W, Strube-Bloss M. Categorizing Visual Information in Subpopulations of Honeybee Mushroom Body Output Neurons. Front Physiol 2022; 13:866807. [PMID: 35574496 PMCID: PMC9092450 DOI: 10.3389/fphys.2022.866807] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/31/2022] [Accepted: 04/08/2022] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Multisensory integration plays a central role in perception, as all behaviors usually require the input of different sensory signals. For instance, for a foraging honeybee the association of a food source includes the combination of olfactory and visual cues to be categorized as a flower. Moreover, homing after successful foraging using celestial cues and the panoramic scenery may be dominated by visual cues. Hence, dependent on the context, one modality might be leading and influence the processing of other modalities. To unravel the complex neural mechanisms behind this process we studied honeybee mushroom body output neurons (MBON). MBONs represent the first processing level after olfactory-visual convergence in the honeybee brain. This was physiologically confirmed in our previous study by characterizing a subpopulation of multisensory MBONs. These neurons categorize incoming sensory inputs into olfactory, visual, and olfactory-visual information. However, in addition to multisensory units a prominent population of MBONs was sensitive to visual cues only. Therefore, we asked which visual features might be represented at this high-order integration level. Using extracellular, multi-unit recordings in combination with visual and olfactory stimulation, we separated MBONs with multisensory responses from purely visually driven MBONs. Further analysis revealed, for the first time, that visually driven MBONs of both groups encode detailed aspects within this individual modality, such as light intensity and light identity. Moreover, we show that these features are separated by different MBON subpopulations, for example by extracting information about brightness and wavelength. Most interestingly, the latter MBON population was tuned to separate UV-light from other light stimuli, which were only poorly differentiated from each other. A third MBON subpopulation was neither tuned to brightness nor to wavelength and encoded the general presence of light. Taken together, our results support the view that the mushroom body, a high-order sensory integration, learning and memory center in the insect brain, categorizes sensory information by separating different behaviorally relevant aspects of the multisensory scenery and that these categories are channeled into distinct MBON subpopulations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Fabian Schmalz
- Behavioral Physiology and Sociobiology (Zoology II), Biozentrum, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany
| | - Basil El Jundi
- Behavioral Physiology and Sociobiology (Zoology II), Biozentrum, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany
| | - Wolfgang Rössler
- Behavioral Physiology and Sociobiology (Zoology II), Biozentrum, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany
| | - Martin Strube-Bloss
- Department of Biological Cybernetics and Theoretical Biology, University of Bielefeld, Bielefeld, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Kemp DJ, Edwards W, White TE. Captivating color: evidence for optimal stimulus design in a polymorphic prey lure. Behav Ecol 2022. [DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arac034] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Abstract
Many species – humans included – employ color as an instrument of deception. One intriguing example of this resides in the conspicuous abstract color patterns displayed on the bodies of female orb weaving spiders. These displays increase prey interception rates and thereby function at least as visual lures. Their chromatic properties however vary extensively, both across and within species, with discrete forms often co-existing in the manner of a stable polymorphism. Variation is principally expressed in terms of signal hue (color per se), but it is unclear how attractiveness scales with this property and if extant morphs are maximally attractive relative to a graded range of potential alternatives. We examined these questions by assessing catch rates among color-manipulated females of the dimorphic jeweled spider Gasteracantha fornicata in their natural webs. The manipulation altered dorsal appearance in a manner akin to adding six new variants of their existing white/yellow phenotypes. This magnified the natural variation in stimulus hue independently of chroma (saturation) across a range spanning most of the color spectrum. Catch rate varied across treatments in simple accordance with how greatly stimulus hue deviated from either of the two extant spider phenotypes. Predictions based on fly-perceived chromatic and achromatic background contrast were clearly unsupported despite dipterans constituting ~60 % of identifiable prey. This study supports the importance of signal coloration per se in G. fornicata and suggests that extant lure phenotypes reside in a broadly optimal spectral range for stimulating their aggregate prey community.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Darrell J Kemp
- Department of Biological Sciences, Macquarie University, North Ryde, New South Wales, Australia
| | - Will Edwards
- College of Science and Engineering, James Cook University, Cairns, Queensland, Australia
| | - Thomas E White
- School of Life and Environmental Sciences, University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Xu X, Yu L, Li F, Wang B, Liu F, Li D. Phylogenetic placement and species delimitation of the crab spider genus Phrynarachne (Araneae: Thomisidae) from China. Mol Phylogenet Evol 2022; 173:107521. [DOI: 10.1016/j.ympev.2022.107521] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/11/2021] [Revised: 03/24/2022] [Accepted: 04/11/2022] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
|
13
|
Rodríguez-Morales D, Tapia-McClung H, Robledo-Ospina LE, Rao D. Colour and motion affect a dune wasp's ability to detect its cryptic spider predators. Sci Rep 2021; 11:15442. [PMID: 34326422 PMCID: PMC8322161 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-021-94926-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/20/2021] [Accepted: 07/19/2021] [Indexed: 02/07/2023] Open
Abstract
Ambush predators depend on cryptic body colouration, stillness and a suitable hunting location to optimise the probability of prey capture. Detection of cryptic predators, such as crab spiders, by flower seeking wasps may also be hindered by wind induced movement of the flowers themselves. In a beach dune habitat, Microbembex nigrifrons wasps approaching flowerheads of the Palafoxia lindenii plant need to evaluate the flowers to avoid spider attack. Wasps may detect spiders through colour and movement cues. We tracked the flight trajectories of dune wasps as they approached occupied and unoccupied flowers under two movement conditions; when the flowers were still or moving. We simulated the appearance of the spider and the flower using psychophysical visual modelling techniques and related it to the decisions made by the wasp to land or avoid the flower. Wasps could discriminate spiders only at a very close range, and this was reflected in the shape of their trajectories. Wasps were more prone to making errors in threat assessment when the flowers are moving. Our results suggest that dune wasp predation risk is augmented by abiotic conditions such as wind and compromises their early detection capabilities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dulce Rodríguez-Morales
- Instituto de Biotecnología y Ecología Aplicada (INBIOTECA), Universidad Veracruzana, Xalapa, Veracruz, Mexico
| | - Horacio Tapia-McClung
- Instituto de Investigación en Inteligencia Artificial (IIIA), Universidad Veracruzana, Xalapa, Veracruz, Mexico
| | - Luis E Robledo-Ospina
- Red de Ecoetología, Instituto de Ecología A.C., Carretera antigua a Coatepec No. 351, Xalapa, Veracruz, Mexico
| | - Dinesh Rao
- Instituto de Biotecnología y Ecología Aplicada (INBIOTECA), Universidad Veracruzana, Xalapa, Veracruz, Mexico.
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
|
15
|
Benoit AD, Kalisz S. Predator Effects on Plant-Pollinator Interactions, Plant Reproduction, Mating Systems, and Evolution. ANNUAL REVIEW OF ECOLOGY, EVOLUTION, AND SYSTEMATICS 2020. [DOI: 10.1146/annurev-ecolsys-012120-094926] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
Plants are the foundation of the food web and therefore interact directly and indirectly with myriad organisms at higher trophic levels. They directly provide nourishment to mutualistic and antagonistic primary consumers (e.g., pollinators and herbivores), which in turn are consumed by predators. These interactions produce cascading indirect effects on plants (either trait-mediated or density-mediated). We review how predators affect plant-pollinator interactions and thus how predators indirectly affect plant reproduction, fitness, mating systems, and trait evolution. Predators can influence pollinator abundance and foraging behavior. In many cases, predators cause pollinators to visit plants less frequently and for shorter durations. This decline in visitation can lead to pollen limitation and decreased seed set. However, alternative outcomes can result due to differences in predator, pollinator, and plant functional traits as well as due to altered interaction networks with plant enemies. Furthermore, predators may indirectly affect the evolution of plant traits and mating systems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Amanda D. Benoit
- Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, Tennessee 37996, USA;,
| | - Susan Kalisz
- Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, Tennessee 37996, USA;,
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Conspicuous colours in a polymorphic orb-web spider: evidence of predator avoidance but not prey attraction. Anim Behav 2020. [DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2020.08.022] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
17
|
Su Q, Qi L, Zhang W, Yun Y, Zhao Y, Peng Y. Biodiversity Survey of Flower-Visiting Spiders Based on Literature Review and Field Study. ENVIRONMENTAL ENTOMOLOGY 2020; 49:673-682. [PMID: 32281625 DOI: 10.1093/ee/nvaa022] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/25/2019] [Indexed: 05/26/2023]
Abstract
Many arthropods exhibit flower-visiting behavior, including a variety of spider species. However, as spiders are assumed to be strictly predatory, flower-visiting spiders are an often neglected group. We conducted a systematic biodiversity study of flower-visiting spiders based on published papers and field surveys. Most previous studies have focused on the herbivorous behavior of flower-visiting spiders (nectivory or pollinivory) and their effects on host flowers (tritrophic interactions with flower-visiting insects). In our field survey, we utilized standard transect walks (active sampling) and colored pan traps (passive sampling) to investigate species occurrence, diurnal and seasonal variation, and flower color preference of flower-visiting spiders. From the transect walks, crab spider species were found to be the dominant flower-visiting spiders and, based on all spider species, juvenile visitors were significantly more common than adults. Furthermore, in terms of spider number and species richness, tulips were the preferred flower to visit. For the pan traps, wolf spiders were found to be the dominant spider species. No significant differences were observed in the number of spiders caught in different colored pans, suggesting that color may not be an important flower trait in regard to spider preference. To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first to propose the term 'flower-visiting spiders' and conduct a systematic investigation of their diversity. However, this is preliminary research and further studies are required, especially as biodiversity is often closely linked to survey sites and ecotopes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Qichen Su
- State Key Laboratory of Biocatalysis and Enzyme Engineering of China, School of Life Sciences, Hubei University, Wuhan, China
| | - Lin Qi
- State Key Laboratory of Biocatalysis and Enzyme Engineering of China, School of Life Sciences, Hubei University, Wuhan, China
| | - Wei Zhang
- State Key Laboratory of Biocatalysis and Enzyme Engineering of China, School of Life Sciences, Hubei University, Wuhan, China
| | - Yueli Yun
- State Key Laboratory of Biocatalysis and Enzyme Engineering of China, School of Life Sciences, Hubei University, Wuhan, China
| | - Yao Zhao
- State Key Laboratory of Biocatalysis and Enzyme Engineering of China, School of Life Sciences, Hubei University, Wuhan, China
| | - Yu Peng
- State Key Laboratory of Biocatalysis and Enzyme Engineering of China, School of Life Sciences, Hubei University, Wuhan, China
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Lichtenberg EM, Heiling JM, Bronstein JL, Barker JL. Noisy communities and signal detection: why do foragers visit rewardless flowers? Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2020; 375:20190486. [PMID: 32420846 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0486] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Floral communities present complex and shifting resource landscapes for flower-foraging animals. Strong similarities among the floral displays of different plant species, paired with high variability in reward distributions across time and space, can weaken correlations between floral signals and reward status. As a result, it should be difficult for foragers to discriminate between rewarding and rewardless flowers. Building on signal detection theory in behavioural ecology, we use hypothetical probability density functions to examine graphically how plant signals pose challenges to forager decision-making. We argue that foraging costs associated with incorrect acceptance of rewardless flowers and incorrect rejection of rewarding ones interact with community-level reward availability to determine the extent to which rewardless and rewarding species should overlap in flowering time. We discuss the evolutionary consequences of these phenomena from both the forager and the plant perspectives. This article is part of the theme issue 'Signal detection theory in recognition systems: from evolving models to experimental tests'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elinor M Lichtenberg
- Department of Integrative Biology, University of Texas, Austin, TX, USA.,Department of Biological Sciences and Advanced Environmental Research Institute, University of North Texas, Denton, TX, USA
| | - Jacob M Heiling
- Department of Biological Science, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, USA
| | - Judith L Bronstein
- Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA
| | - Jessica L Barker
- The Behavioural Insights Team, UK.,Interacting Minds Centre, Aarhus University, Denmark
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Pembury Smith MQR, Ruxton GD. Camouflage in predators. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 2020; 95:1325-1340. [DOI: 10.1111/brv.12612] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/02/2020] [Revised: 04/24/2020] [Accepted: 04/28/2020] [Indexed: 12/29/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | - Graeme D. Ruxton
- School of Biology University of St Andrews, Dyers Brae House, St Andrews Fife KY16 9TH U.K
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
|
21
|
Rodríguez-Gironés MA, Jiménez OM. Encounters with predators fail to trigger predator avoidance in bumblebees, Bombus terrestris (Hymenoptera: Apidae). Biol J Linn Soc Lond 2019. [DOI: 10.1093/biolinnean/blz155] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022]
Abstract
Abstract
Many species must learn to identify their predators, but little is known about the effect of direct encounters on the development of predator avoidance. We asked whether bumblebees, Bombus terrestris, learn to avoid predators, whether learning depends on the conspicuousness of predators and whether bumblebees learn to identify predators or simply to avoid dangerous patches. To answer these questions, we allowed bumblebees to forage in an enclosed meadow of 15 artificial flowers containing a yellow female crab spider, Thomisus onustus. Flowers were yellow in half of the trials and white in the other half. Spiders could remain at the same flower throughout the experiment or swap flowers between bee foraging bouts. Of the 60 bees used in the experiment, eight were killed by the spiders and nine stopped foraging without finishing the trial. Death or refusal to forage typically occurred early in the trial. Regardless of the treatment, the probability of landing at the spider-harbouring flower increased with time. Previous encounters with heterospecific individuals can therefore be a poor source of information about their predatory nature.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Miguel A Rodríguez-Gironés
- Estación Experimental de Zonas Áridas, Spanish National Research Council (CSIC), Carretera de Sacrament s/n, La Cañada de San Urbano, Almeria, Spain
| | - Olga M Jiménez
- Estación Experimental de Zonas Áridas, Spanish National Research Council (CSIC), Carretera de Sacrament s/n, La Cañada de San Urbano, Almeria, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Stefani V, Alves VN, Lange D. Induced indirect defence in a spider-plant system mediated by pericarpial nectaries. AUSTRAL ECOL 2019. [DOI: 10.1111/aec.12766] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Vanessa Stefani
- Laboratório de História Natural e Reprodutiva de Artrópodes (LHINRA); Pós-graduação em Ecologia e Conservação de Recursos Naturais; Universidade Federal de Uberlândia; Uberlândia 38400-902 Brazil
| | - Vinícius Nunes Alves
- Pós-graduação em Ecologia e Conservação dos Recursos Naturais; Universidade Federal de Uberlândia; Uberlândia MG Brazil
| | - Denise Lange
- Pós-graduação em Recursos Naturais e Sustentabilidade; Universidade Tecnológica Federal do Paraná; Santa Helena PR Brazil
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Colour preferences of Tetragonula carbonaria Sm. stingless bees for colour morphs of the Australian native orchid Caladenia carnea. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 2019; 205:347-361. [DOI: 10.1007/s00359-019-01346-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/18/2018] [Revised: 05/08/2019] [Accepted: 05/13/2019] [Indexed: 01/09/2023]
|
24
|
Ajuria Ibarra H, Kinahan M, Marcetteau J, Mehigan AJR, Ziegelmeier RO, Reader T. The significance of prey avoidance behavior for the maintenance of a predator color polymorphism. Behav Ecol 2019. [DOI: 10.1093/beheco/ary129] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Helena Ajuria Ibarra
- School of Life Sciences, University of Nottingham, University Park, Nottingham, UK
| | - Michael Kinahan
- School of Life Sciences, University of Nottingham, University Park, Nottingham, UK
| | - Julien Marcetteau
- School of Life Sciences, University of Nottingham, University Park, Nottingham, UK
| | - Andrew J R Mehigan
- School of Life Sciences, University of Nottingham, University Park, Nottingham, UK
| | - Ross O Ziegelmeier
- School of Life Sciences, University of Nottingham, University Park, Nottingham, UK
| | - Tom Reader
- School of Life Sciences, University of Nottingham, University Park, Nottingham, UK
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Gavini SS, Quintero C, Tadey M. Ecological role of a flower-dwelling predator in a tri-trophic interaction in northwestern Patagonia. ACTA OECOLOGICA-INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF ECOLOGY 2019. [DOI: 10.1016/j.actao.2018.12.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/02/2023]
|
26
|
Shrestha M, Garcia JE, Chua JHJ, Howard SR, Tscheulin T, Dorin A, Nielsen A, Dyer AG. Fluorescent Pan Traps Affect the Capture Rate of Insect Orders in Different Ways. INSECTS 2019; 10:insects10020040. [PMID: 30717089 PMCID: PMC6410105 DOI: 10.3390/insects10020040] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/06/2018] [Revised: 01/15/2019] [Accepted: 01/21/2019] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
To monitor and quantify the changes in pollinator communities over time, it is important to have robust survey techniques of insect populations. Pan traps allow for the assessment of the relative insect abundance in an environment and have been promoted by the Food and Agricultural Organization (FAO) as an efficient data collection methodology. It has been proposed that fluorescent pan traps are particularly useful, as it has been suggested that they capture high numbers of insects in an unbiased fashion. We use a simultaneous presentation of fluorescent and non-fluorescent pan trap colours to assess how flower-visiting insects of different orders respond to visual stimuli and reveal a significant interaction between trap fluorescence and captured insect type. In particular, Coleoptera (beetles) and Lepidoptera (butterflies and moths) were captured significantly more frequently by fluorescent traps, whilst Dipterans (flies) were captured significantly less frequently by this type of pan trap. Hymenopterans (bees and wasps) showed no significant difference in their preference for fluorescent or non-fluorescent traps. Our results reveal that the use of fluorescent pan traps may differently bias insect capture rates when compared to the typical experience of colour flower-visiting insects in natural environments. Correction factors may, therefore, be required for interpreting insect pan trap data collected with different methodologies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mani Shrestha
- School of Media and Communication, RMIT University, Melbourne, VIC 3001, Australia.
- Faculty of Information Technology, Monash University, Melbourne, VIC 3800, Australia.
| | - Jair E Garcia
- School of Media and Communication, RMIT University, Melbourne, VIC 3001, Australia
| | - Justin H J Chua
- School of Media and Communication, RMIT University, Melbourne, VIC 3001, Australia
| | - Scarlett R Howard
- School of Media and Communication, RMIT University, Melbourne, VIC 3001, Australia
| | - Thomas Tscheulin
- Laboratory of Biogeography and Ecology, Department of Geography, University of the Aegean University Hill, GR-81100 Mytilene, Greece
| | - Alan Dorin
- Faculty of Information Technology, Monash University, Melbourne, VIC 3800, Australia
| | - Anders Nielsen
- Centre for Ecological and Evolutionary Synthesis (CEES), Dept. of Biosciences, University of Oslo, P.O. Box 1066 Blindern, 0316 Oslo, Norway
| | - Adrian G Dyer
- School of Media and Communication, RMIT University, Melbourne, VIC 3001, Australia
- Department of Physiology, Monash University, Melbourne, VIC 3800, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Telles FJ, Gonzálvez FG, Rodríguez-Gironés MA, Freitas L. The effect of a flower-dwelling predator on a specialized pollination system. Biol J Linn Soc Lond 2018. [DOI: 10.1093/biolinnean/bly184] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Francismeire Jane Telles
- Programa de Pós-Graduação em Ecologia e Conservação de Recursos Naturais, Universidade Federal de Uberlândia, Uberlândia - MG, Brazil
| | | | | | - Leandro Freitas
- Jardim Botânico do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro - RJ, Brazil
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Lam WN, Tan HTW. The crab spider-pitcher plant relationship is a nutritional mutualism that is dependent on prey-resource quality. J Anim Ecol 2018; 88:102-113. [PMID: 30303527 DOI: 10.1111/1365-2656.12915] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/05/2018] [Accepted: 09/28/2018] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Nutritional mutualisms are one of the three major categories of mutualisms and involve the provision of limiting nutrients (resources) to one species by another. It was recently shown in laboratory experiments that two species of pitcher-dwelling crab spiders (Thomisidae), Thomisus nepenthiphilus and Misumenops nepenthicola, increased capture rates of flesh flies (Sarcophagidae) for their host, Nepenthes gracilis. The spiders ambushed pitcher-visiting flesh flies and dropped their carcasses into pitchers after consuming them. The consumption of shared prey-resources by crab spiders and pitcher plants presents the possibility of parasitism between them. However, ecologically generalizable mechanisms that predict the context-dependent outcomes of such mutualisms are not known. The effectiveness framework (mutualism effectiveness = quality × quantity) is useful for examining the total effect of mutualisms, but its quality component can be difficult to define. We identify the crab spider-pitcher plant interaction as a type of resource conversion mutualism and propose that the quality component in such interactions is the amount of the underlying resource contained in each unit of resource processed. We then used the crab spider-pitcher plant interaction to test the hypothesis that resource conversion mutualisms are more beneficial to the nutrient recipient when operating through high-quality resources (i.e., large prey, in this interaction). We sampled the prey and inquilines of 107 N. gracilis upper pitches in situ and analysed the differences between pitchers that were inhabited or uninhabited by crab spiders, and the differences between nutritional contents of prey that were consumed by crab spiders or not. Pitchers inhabited by T. nepenthiphilus contained higher numbers of several prey taxa, many of which were flying insects. Consumption by T. nepenthiphilus reduced the nutrient contents in all prey examined. Overall, T. nepenthiphilus-assisted prey capture is likely to result in a net nutrient gain for N. gracilis that is proportional to the size of prey consumed by T. nepenthiphilus. Our results suggest that resource conversion mutualisms are more likely to operate through high-quality resources, since the nutrient-processing species necessarily reduces the quality of the resource it processes while increasing its availability to the nutrient recipient species.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Weng Ngai Lam
- Department of Biological Sciences, National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore
| | - Hugh T W Tan
- Department of Biological Sciences, National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Rodríguez-Morales D, Rico-Gray V, García-Franco JG, Ajuria-Ibarra H, Hernández-Salazar LT, Robledo-Ospina LE, Rao D. Context-dependent crypsis: a prey's perspective of a color polymorphic predator. Naturwissenschaften 2018; 105:36. [PMID: 29754205 DOI: 10.1007/s00114-018-1562-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/21/2018] [Revised: 05/04/2018] [Accepted: 05/07/2018] [Indexed: 12/11/2022]
Abstract
Many animals use body coloration as a strategy to communicate with conspecifics, prey, and predators. Color is a trade-off for some species, since they should be visible to conspecifics but cryptic to predators and prey. Some flower-dwelling predators, such as crab spiders, are capable of choosing the color of flowers where they ambush flower visitors and pollinators. In order to avoid being captured, visitors evaluate flowers visually before landing. The crab spider Mecaphesa dubia is a polymorphic species (white/purple color morphs), which inhabits the flower heads of a dune plant, Palafoxia lindenii. Using full-spectrum photography of spiders and flowers, we evaluated how honeybees perceived the spiders at different distances. Using visual modeling, we obtained the chromatic and achromatic contrasts of the spiders on flower heads as perceived by honeybees. Purple morphs were found mainly on the receptacle area and white morphs were equally likely to be found in the flowers and receptacle. According to theoretical modeling, white morphs were visible to honeybees from a distance of 10 cm in receptacle area but appeared to be cryptic in the flower area. Purple morphs were cryptic on the receptacle and less so when they were on the flowers. Spiders on flower heads are predicted to be more easily detected by honeybees using chromatic contrast. Our study shows that the conspicuousness of flower dwelling spiders to honeybees depends on the color morph, the distance of observation, and the position of spider on the flower head.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D Rodríguez-Morales
- Instituto de Neuroetología, Universidad Veracruzana, Av. Luis Castelazo, Col. Industrial Animas, Xalapa, 91190, Veracruz, Mexico.
| | - V Rico-Gray
- Instituto de Neuroetología, Universidad Veracruzana, Av. Luis Castelazo, Col. Industrial Animas, Xalapa, 91190, Veracruz, Mexico
| | - J G García-Franco
- Red de Ecología Funcional, Instituto de Ecología, A.C. Carretera Antigua a Coatepec 351, El Haya, Xalapa, 91070, Veracruz, Mexico
| | - H Ajuria-Ibarra
- Instituto de Biotecnologia y Ecologia Aplicada, Universidad Veracruzana, Av. Culturas Veracruzanas No. 101, Xalapa, 91090, Veracruz, Mexico
| | - L T Hernández-Salazar
- Instituto de Neuroetología, Universidad Veracruzana, Av. Luis Castelazo, Col. Industrial Animas, Xalapa, 91190, Veracruz, Mexico
| | - L E Robledo-Ospina
- Red de Ecoetología, Instituto de Ecología, A.C. Carretera antigua a Coatepec No. 351, Xalapa, 91070, Veracruz, Mexico.,Grupo de Aracnología, Universidad de Caldas, Calle 65 No. 26-10, Manizales, Colombia
| | - D Rao
- Instituto de Biotecnologia y Ecologia Aplicada, Universidad Veracruzana, Av. Culturas Veracruzanas No. 101, Xalapa, 91090, Veracruz, Mexico
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Vieira C, Ramires EN, Vasconcellos-Neto J, Poppi RJ, Romero GQ. Crab Spider Lures Prey In Flowerless Neighborhoods. Sci Rep 2017; 7:9188. [PMID: 28835630 PMCID: PMC5569008 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-017-09456-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/03/2017] [Accepted: 07/26/2017] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
One fundamental question in prey luring systems is to understand how visual signals are interpreted by the receiver. Predators lure prey by falsely imitating the signal of a model, or may exploit sensory preferences of the receivers, which search for rewarding signals. Crab spiders reflect ultraviolet (UV) light, ambush pollinators on flowers, and manipulate flower UV signals altering the behavior and response of prey. Whereas crab spiders typically depend on flowers to forage, adult Epicadus heterogaster departs from this standard behavior by preying on pollinators upon green leaves, even in the absence of flowers nearby. This species has a conspicuous abdomen resembling the shape of a flower, which may reflect UV signals similar to that of flowers, and thus attract pollinators. Nevertheless, no empirical evidence is available that E. heterogaster foraging on leaves mimics flowers, nor how this crab spider interacts with its prey. Field and laboratory experiments demonstrated that UV reflection of adult E. heterogaster is the main signal responsible for the attraction of pollinators. This is the first study to demonstrate that a crab spider attracts pollinators regardless of flower UV signal, which may represent an evolutionary pathway beyond the dependence of flowers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Camila Vieira
- Pós-graduação em Ecologia, Universidade Estadual de Campinas (UNICAMP), CP 6109, CEP 13083-970, Campinas-SP, Brazil
| | - Eduardo N Ramires
- Departamento Acadêmico de Química e Biologia, Universidade Tecnológica Federal do Paraná, CEP 80230-901, Curitiba-PR, Brazil
| | - João Vasconcellos-Neto
- Departamento de Biologia Animal, IB, Universidade Estadual de Campinas, CP 6109, CEP 13083-970, Campinas-SP, Brazil
| | - Ronei J Poppi
- Instituto de Química, IQ, Universidade Estadual de Campinas, CEP 13083-970, Campinas-SP, Brazil
| | - Gustavo Q Romero
- Departamento de Biologia Animal, IB, Universidade Estadual de Campinas, CP 6109, CEP 13083-970, Campinas-SP, Brazil.
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Weigel BL. Digest: Floral ambush in the sun: Evolution of flower-dwelling and UV-white reflectance in crab spiders*. Evolution 2017. [DOI: 10.1111/evo.13294] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Brooke L. Weigel
- Committee on Evolutionary Biology; University of Chicago; 1025 E. 57th Street Chicago Illinois 60637
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Gawryszewski FM, Calero-Torralbo MA, Gillespie RG, Rodríguez-Gironés MA, Herberstein ME. Correlated evolution between coloration and ambush site in predators with visual prey lures. Evolution 2017; 71:2010-2021. [DOI: 10.1111/evo.13271] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/26/2016] [Revised: 04/01/2017] [Accepted: 05/02/2017] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Felipe M. Gawryszewski
- Department of Biology; Macquarie University; Sydney NSW 2109 Australia
- Departmento de Genética; Universidade Federal de Goiás; Goiânia GO Brazil
| | - Miguel A. Calero-Torralbo
- Department of Functional and Evolutionary Ecology; Estación Experimental de Zonas Áridas (CSIC); Almeria Spain
| | - Rosemary G. Gillespie
- Department of Environmental Science, University of California; Berkeley California 94720 USA
| | - Miguel A. Rodríguez-Gironés
- Department of Functional and Evolutionary Ecology; Estación Experimental de Zonas Áridas (CSIC); Almeria Spain
| | | |
Collapse
|
33
|
Females are the brighter sex: Differences in external fluorescence across sexes and life stages of a crab spider. PLoS One 2017; 12:e0175667. [PMID: 28467416 PMCID: PMC5414973 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0175667] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/02/2016] [Accepted: 03/29/2017] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Fluorescence is increasingly recognized to be widespread in nature. In particular, some arachnids fluoresce externally, and in spiders the hemolymph fluoresces. In this study, we examined the external fluorescence and the fluorophores of different sexes and life stages of the crab spider Misumena vatia (Clerk 1757), a sit-and-wait predator that feeds on insects as they visit flowers. We designed novel instrumentation to measure external fluorescence in whole specimens. We found that although males and females possess internal fluorophores with similar properties, the external expression of fluorescence varies across sexes and life stages. Spiders fluoresce brightly as immatures. Females maintain their brightness to adulthood, whereas males become increasingly dim as they mature. We suggest that external fluorescence likely contributes to visual signaling in these animals, and that it differs between the sexes as a result of differences in foraging ecology and behavior.
Collapse
|
34
|
Akkaynak D, Siemann LA, Barbosa A, Mäthger LM. Changeable camouflage: how well can flounder resemble the colour and spatial scale of substrates in their natural habitats? ROYAL SOCIETY OPEN SCIENCE 2017; 4:160824. [PMID: 28405370 PMCID: PMC5383827 DOI: 10.1098/rsos.160824] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/22/2016] [Accepted: 02/03/2017] [Indexed: 06/07/2023]
Abstract
Flounder change colour and pattern for camouflage. We used a spectrometer to measure reflectance spectra and a digital camera to capture body patterns of two flounder species camouflaged on four natural backgrounds of different spatial scale (sand, small gravel, large gravel and rocks). We quantified the degree of spectral match between flounder and background relative to the situation of perfect camouflage in which flounder and background were assumed to have identical spectral distribution. Computations were carried out for three biologically relevant observers: monochromatic squid, dichromatic crab and trichromatic guitarfish. Our computations present a new approach to analysing datasets with multiple spectra that have large variance. Furthermore, to investigate the spatial match between flounder and background, images of flounder patterns were analysed using a custom program originally developed to study cuttlefish camouflage. Our results show that all flounder and background spectra fall within the same colour gamut and that, in terms of different observer visual systems, flounder matched most substrates in luminance and colour contrast. Flounder matched the spatial scales of all substrates except for rocks. We discuss findings in terms of flounder biology; furthermore, we discuss our methodology in light of hyperspectral technologies that combine high-resolution spectral and spatial imaging.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Derya Akkaynak
- Marine Biological Laboratory, Bell Center, Woods Hole, MA 02543, USA
- Department of Mechanical Engineering, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA
- Oceanography and Applied Ocean Science, Woods Hole Oceanographic Institution, Woods Hole, MA 02543, USA
| | - Liese A. Siemann
- Marine Biological Laboratory, Bell Center, Woods Hole, MA 02543, USA
- Coonamessett Farm Foundation, 277 Hatchville Road, East Falmouth, MA 02536, USA
| | - Alexandra Barbosa
- Marine Biological Laboratory, Bell Center, Woods Hole, MA 02543, USA
| | - Lydia M. Mäthger
- Marine Biological Laboratory, Bell Center, Woods Hole, MA 02543, USA
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
Corcobado G, Herberstein ME, Pekár S. The role of ultraviolet colour in the assessment of mimetic accuracy between Batesian mimics and their models: a case study using ant-mimicking spiders. Naturwissenschaften 2016; 103:90. [PMID: 27722878 DOI: 10.1007/s00114-016-1410-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/09/2015] [Revised: 06/27/2016] [Accepted: 09/17/2016] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
The use of ultraviolet (UV) cues for intra- and inter-specific communication is common in many animal species. Still, the role of UV signals under some predator-prey contexts, such as Batesian mimicry, is not clear. Batesian mimicry is a defensive strategy by which a palatable species (the mimic) resembles an unpalatable or noxious species (the model) to avoid predation. This strategy has evolved independently in many different taxa that are predated by species capable of UV perception. Moreover, there is considerable variation in how accurately Batesian mimics resemble their models across species. Our aim was to investigate how UV colour contributed to mimetic accuracy using several ant-mimicking spider species as a case study. We measured the reflectance spectrum (300-700 nm) for several species of mimics and models, and we tested whether they differ in visible and UV colour. We modelled whether two different predators could discriminate between mimics and models using colour information. We found that generally, ant-mimicking spiders differed significantly from their ant models in UV colour and that information from the visible range of light cannot be extrapolated into the UV. Our modelling suggested that wasps should be able to discriminate between mimics and models combining information from visible and the UV light, whereas birds may not discriminate between them. Thus, we show that UV colour can influence mimic accuracy and we discuss its potential role in Batesian mimicry. We conclude that colour, especially in the UV range, should be taken into account when measuring mimetic accuracy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Guadalupe Corcobado
- Department of Botany and Zoology, Faculty of Sciences, Masaryk University, Kotlářská 2, 61137, Brno, Czech Republic.
| | - Marie E Herberstein
- Department of Biological Sciences, Macquarie University, North Ryde, Sydney, NSW, 2109, Australia
| | - Stano Pekár
- Department of Botany and Zoology, Faculty of Sciences, Masaryk University, Kotlářská 2, 61137, Brno, Czech Republic
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Reverté S, Retana J, Gómez JM, Bosch J. Pollinators show flower colour preferences but flowers with similar colours do not attract similar pollinators. ANNALS OF BOTANY 2016; 118:249-257. [PMID: 27325897 PMCID: PMC4970366 DOI: 10.1093/aob/mcw103] [Citation(s) in RCA: 56] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/08/2015] [Revised: 11/29/2015] [Accepted: 04/15/2016] [Indexed: 05/29/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND AND AIMS Colour is one of the main floral traits used by pollinators to locate flowers. Although pollinators show innate colour preferences, the view that the colour of a flower may be considered an important predictor of its main pollinators is highly controversial because flower choice is highly context-dependent, and initial innate preferences may be overridden by subsequent associative learning. Our objective is to establish whether there is a relationship between flower colour and pollinator composition in natural communities. METHODS We measured the flower reflectance spectrum and pollinator composition in four plant communities (85 plant species represented by 109 populations, and 32 305 plant-pollinator interactions in total). Pollinators were divided into six taxonomic groups: bees, ants, wasps, coleopterans, dipterans and lepidopterans. KEY RESULTS We found consistent associations between pollinator groups and certain colours. These associations matched innate preferences experimentally established for several pollinators and predictions of the pollination syndrome theory. However, flowers with similar colours did not attract similar pollinator assemblages. CONCLUSIONS The explanation for this paradoxical result is that most flower species are pollination generalists. We conclude that although pollinator colour preferences seem to condition plant-pollinator interactions, the selective force behind these preferences has not been strong enough to mediate the appearance and maintenance of tight colour-based plant-pollinator associations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sara Reverté
- CREAF, Cerdanyola del Vallès, 08193 Barcelona, Spain
| | - Javier Retana
- CREAF, Cerdanyola del Vallès, 08193 Barcelona, Spain Universitat Autònoma Barcelona, Cerdanyola del Vallès, 08193 Barcelona, Spain
| | - José M Gómez
- CSIC, Estación Experimental de Zonas Áridas, Almería, Spain Universidad de Granada, Granada, Spain
| | - Jordi Bosch
- CREAF, Cerdanyola del Vallès, 08193 Barcelona, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
|
38
|
Painting CJ, Rajamohan G, Chen Z, Zeng H, Li D. It takes two peaks to tango: the importance of UVB and UVA in sexual signalling in jumping spiders. Anim Behav 2016. [DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.12.030] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
|
39
|
Abstract
James O'Hanlon introduces the orchid mantis, a species of preying mantis that resembles flowers.
Collapse
|
40
|
Nakata K, Shigemiya Y. Body-colour variation in an orb-web spider and its effect on predation success. Biol J Linn Soc Lond 2015. [DOI: 10.1111/bij.12640] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Kensuke Nakata
- Tokyo Keizai University; Minami-cho, 1-7-34 Kokubunji Tokyo 185-8502 Japan
- Kyoto Women's University; Kitahiyoshi-cho 35 Higashiyama-ku Kyoto 605-8501 Japan
| | - Yusuke Shigemiya
- Faculty of Environment and Architecture; Nagasaki Institute for Applied Science; Abamachi 536 Nagasaki 851-0193 Japan
| |
Collapse
|
41
|
|
42
|
Mokkonen M, Lindstedt C. The evolutionary ecology of deception. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 2015; 91:1020-1035. [PMID: 26118820 DOI: 10.1111/brv.12208] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/21/2014] [Revised: 05/29/2015] [Accepted: 06/05/2015] [Indexed: 12/15/2022]
Abstract
Through dishonest signals or actions, individuals often misinform others to their own benefit. We review recent literature to explore the evolutionary and ecological conditions for deception to be more likely to evolve and be maintained. We identify four conditions: (1) high misinformation potential through perceptual constraints of perceiver; (2) costs and benefits of responding to deception; (3) asymmetric power relationships between individuals and (4) exploitation of common goods. We discuss behavioural and physiological mechanisms that form a deception continuum from secrecy to overt signals. Deceptive tactics usually succeed by being rare and are often evolving under co-evolutionary arms races, sometimes leading to the evolution of polymorphism. The degree of deception can also vary depending on the environmental conditions. Finally, we suggest a conceptual framework for studying deception and highlight important questions for future studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mikael Mokkonen
- Department of Biological and Environmental Science, University of Jyväskylä, PO Box 35, Jyväskylä 40014, Finland. .,Department of Biological Sciences, Simon Fraser University, 8888 University Drive, Burnaby, BC V5A 1S6, Canada.
| | - Carita Lindstedt
- Department of Biological and Environmental Science, Centre of Excellence in Biological Interactions, University of Jyväskylä, PO Box 35, Jyväskylä 40014, Finland
| |
Collapse
|
43
|
LeVan KE, Holway DA. Ant–aphid interactions increase ant floral visitation and reduce plant reproduction via decreased pollinator visitation. Ecology 2015. [DOI: 10.1890/14-0058.1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
|
44
|
Kemp DJ, Herberstein ME, Fleishman LJ, Endler JA, Bennett ATD, Dyer AG, Hart NS, Marshall J, Whiting MJ. An integrative framework for the appraisal of coloration in nature. Am Nat 2015; 185:705-24. [PMID: 25996857 DOI: 10.1086/681021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 167] [Impact Index Per Article: 16.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/03/2022]
Abstract
The world in color presents a dazzling dimension of phenotypic variation. Biological interest in this variation has burgeoned, due to both increased means for quantifying spectral information and heightened appreciation for how animals view the world differently than humans. Effective study of color traits is challenged by how to best quantify visual perception in nonhuman species. This requires consideration of at least visual physiology but ultimately also the neural processes underlying perception. Our knowledge of color perception is founded largely on the principles gained from human psychophysics that have proven generalizable based on comparative studies in select animal models. Appreciation of these principles, their empirical foundation, and the reasonable limits to their applicability is crucial to reaching informed conclusions in color research. In this article, we seek a common intellectual basis for the study of color in nature. We first discuss the key perceptual principles, namely, retinal photoreception, sensory channels, opponent processing, color constancy, and receptor noise. We then draw on this basis to inform an analytical framework driven by the research question in relation to identifiable viewers and visual tasks of interest. Consideration of the limits to perceptual inference guides two primary decisions: first, whether a sensory-based approach is necessary and justified and, second, whether the visual task refers to perceptual distance or discriminability. We outline informed approaches in each situation and discuss key challenges for future progress, focusing particularly on how animals perceive color. Given that animal behavior serves as both the basic unit of psychophysics and the ultimate driver of color ecology/evolution, behavioral data are critical to reconciling knowledge across the schools of color research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Darrell J Kemp
- Department of Biological Sciences, Macquarie University, North Ryde, New South Wales 2109, Australia
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
45
|
|
46
|
Mizuno T, Yamaguchi S, Yamamoto I, Yamaoka R, Akino T. "Double-trick" visual and chemical mimicry by the juvenile orchid mantis hymenopus coronatus used in predation of the oriental honeybee apis cerana. Zoolog Sci 2014; 31:795-801. [PMID: 25483791 DOI: 10.2108/zs140126] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
It has long been hypothesized that the flower-like appearance of the juvenile orchid mantis is used as visual camouflage to capture flower-visiting insects, although it is doubtful whether such morphological resemblance alone could increase their success in hunting. We confirmed that juvenile female orchid mantes often succeed in capturing oriental honeybees, while adult females often fail. Since most of the honeybees approached the juveniles from the front, we hypothesized that juvenile orchid mantes might attract honeybees by emitting some volatile chemical cues. Gas chromatography-mass spectrometry analyses revealed that the mantes' mandibular adducts contained 3-hydroxyoctanoic acid (3HOA) and 10-hydroxy-(E)-2-decenoic acid (10HDA), both of which are also features of the pheromone communication of the oriental honeybee. We also successfully detected 3HOA emitted in the head space air only at the time when the juvenile mantes were attempting to capture their prey. Field bioassay showed that the Oriental Honeybee predominantly preferred to visit dummies impregnated with a mixture of the appropriate amounts and ratios of 3HOA and 10HDA. We therefore conclude that the juvenile mantes utilize these as allelochemicals to trick and attract oriental honeybees.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Takafumi Mizuno
- 1 Applied Entomology Laboratory, Center for Bioresources Field Science, Kyoto Institute of Technology, Saga-ippongi-cho 1, Kyoto 616-8354, Japan
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
47
|
Barry KL, White TE, Rathnayake DN, Fabricant SA, Herberstein ME. Sexual signals for the colour‐blind: cryptic female mantids signal quality through brightness. Funct Ecol 2014. [DOI: 10.1111/1365-2435.12363] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Katherine L. Barry
- Department of Biological Sciences Macquarie University Sydney NSW 2109 Australia
| | - Thomas E. White
- Department of Biological Sciences Macquarie University Sydney NSW 2109 Australia
| | | | - Scott A. Fabricant
- Department of Biological Sciences Macquarie University Sydney NSW 2109 Australia
| | - Marie E. Herberstein
- Department of Biological Sciences Macquarie University Sydney NSW 2109 Australia
| |
Collapse
|
48
|
Habitat selection in a deceptive predator: maximizing resource availability and signal efficacy. Behav Ecol 2014. [DOI: 10.1093/beheco/aru179] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
|
49
|
Pecháček P, Stella D, Keil P, Kleisner K. Environmental effects on the shape variation of male ultraviolet patterns in the Brimstone butterfly (Gonepteryx rhamni, Pieridae, Lepidoptera). THE SCIENCE OF NATURE - NATURWISSENSCHAFTEN 2014; 101:1055-63. [DOI: 10.1007/s00114-014-1244-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/16/2014] [Revised: 09/18/2014] [Accepted: 09/19/2014] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
50
|
Ajuria Ibarra H, Reader T. Female-limited colour polymorphism in the crab spiderSynema globosum(Araneae: Thomisidae). Biol J Linn Soc Lond 2014. [DOI: 10.1111/bij.12338] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Helena Ajuria Ibarra
- School of Life Sciences; University Park; University of Nottingham; Nottingham NG7 2RD UK
| | - Tom Reader
- School of Life Sciences; University Park; University of Nottingham; Nottingham NG7 2RD UK
| |
Collapse
|