1
|
Zhou J, Mai Z, Lau E, Lum C, Thian AL, Yip V. Cross- and Within-Language Associations Between Phonological, Lexical, and Grammatical Domains in Mandarin-English Bilingual Preschoolers in Singapore. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2025; 68:636-653. [PMID: 39836448 DOI: 10.1044/2024_jslhr-24-00310] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/22/2025]
Abstract
PURPOSE This study aims to examine the associations of phonological, lexical, and grammatical skills within and between languages in Mandarin-English bilingual preschoolers. METHOD Sixty-three Singaporean Mandarin-English bilingual children aged 3-5 years were assessed for articulation, receptive vocabulary, and receptive grammar using standardized instruments in English and compatible tools in Mandarin. Regression analyses were performed on each language outcome, with other language variables as predictors, controlling for age, nonverbal working memory, and home language environment. RESULTS Phonological and grammatical skills in one language predicted corresponding skills in the other. Phonemes shared across languages showed higher accuracy rates compared to unshared phonemes, while accuracy varied across grammatical structures. Vocabulary did not correlate between languages. It was influenced by household language distribution, with Mandarin vocabulary also correlating with nonverbal working memory. Mandarin grammar positively correlated with the number of native Mandarin speakers at home. Within each language, phonological skills were predicted by vocabulary, while vocabulary and grammar were reciprocally predictive. Cross-language, cross-domain relationships were weak. CONCLUSIONS This study shows domain-specific cross-language associations and language-specific cross-domain associations in Mandarin-English bilingual children, indicating both interdependent and autonomous development. Our findings call for approaches that value the child's full linguistic repertoire and utilize interconnectedness between languages and language domains to enhance bi/multilingual competence. They also highlight the importance of assessing each of the child's languages and considering individual bilingual profiles in research on bilingual language development. SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.28200128.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jiangling Zhou
- Department of Linguistics and Modern Languages, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, China
| | - Ziyin Mai
- Department of Linguistics and Modern Languages, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, China
| | - Elaine Lau
- Research Office, Yew Chung College of Early Childhood Education, Hong Kong SAR, China
| | | | | | - Virginia Yip
- Department of Linguistics and Modern Languages, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, China
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
White MJ, Southwood F. Is home environment predictive of early grammar development? JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2024:1-24. [PMID: 39670477 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000924000527] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/14/2024]
Abstract
Research shows that children's home environment (e.g., the composition of their household and the resources available in it) has an impact on children's language development. However, this research has mostly been conducted among English speakers from the minority world and has often only considered vocabulary size. This exploratory study investigated whether home environment factors are predictive of grammar development in Afrikaans-speaking (n = 117) and English-speaking (n = 102) toddlers in South Africa. Moreover, potential differences between these two language groups were explored. Results showed that home environment factors pertaining to family stability predicted two of the three grammar scores, namely total grammar and complex phrases. Cluster analysis showed distinct patterns of home environment factors between Afrikaans and English-speaking households, illustrating the importance of measuring these factors even across samples from the same country. This study shows that children's home environment is an interconnected system and cautions against oversimplified single-factor approaches.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michelle Jennifer White
- University of Plymouth, School of Psychology, Plymouth, United Kingdom
- Stellenbosch University, Department of General Linguistics, Stellenbosch, South Africa
| | - Frenette Southwood
- Stellenbosch University, Department of General Linguistics, Stellenbosch, South Africa
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Durston L, Clarke MT, Soto G. What relationships exist between nouns and verbs and the use of prepositions, adverbs, and adjectives in children and adolescents who use speech generating devices? Augment Altern Commun 2024; 40:306-313. [PMID: 38775646 DOI: 10.1080/07434618.2024.2348996] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/29/2023] [Revised: 03/22/2024] [Accepted: 04/21/2024] [Indexed: 11/26/2024] Open
Abstract
The relationships between the use of nouns and verbs, and other word classes have been well established in the typical language development literature. However, questions remain as to whether the same relationships are seen in the language use of individuals who use graphic symbol-based augmentative and alternative communication (AAC). The aim of the study was to examine relationships between the use of verbs and nouns, and the use of prepositions, adverbs, and adjectives through a secondary analysis of language transcripts taken from 12 children and adolescents who used aided AAC in conversation with an adult. A series of multiple linear mixed-effect regression analyses showed a positive predictive association between the use of verbs and the use of prepositions and adverbs, as well as a positive predictive relationship between the use of nouns and the use of adjectives. The theoretical and practical implications of these findings are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laura Durston
- Department of Language and Cognition, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Michael T Clarke
- Department of Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences, San Francisco State University, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Gloria Soto
- Department of Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences, San Francisco State University, San Francisco, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Hudson Kam CL, Sadlier-Brown E, Clark S, Jang C, Demmans Epp C, Thomson J. Evaluating English-language morphological awareness assessments. FIRST LANGUAGE 2024; 44:327-344. [PMID: 39100688 PMCID: PMC11293997 DOI: 10.1177/01427237241245500] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 08/06/2024]
Abstract
Many studies have shown that morphological knowledge has effects on reading comprehension separate from other aspects of language knowledge. This has implications for reading instruction and assessment: it suggests that children could have reading comprehension difficulties that are due to a lack of morphological knowledge, and thus, that explicit instruction of morphology might be helpful for them, indeed for all children. To find children who might especially benefit from specific instruction in morphology, we would need good tests of morphological knowledge. We evaluated a set of morphological awareness assessments to determine whether they conclusively tapped into morphological knowledge, and found that it was not possible to be certain that they were accurately targeting morphological knowledge.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carla L. Hudson Kam
- Carla L. Hudson Kam, Department of Linguistics, The University of British Columbia, 2613 West Mall, Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4, Canada.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Fedorenko E, Ivanova AA, Regev TI. The language network as a natural kind within the broader landscape of the human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2024; 25:289-312. [PMID: 38609551 DOI: 10.1038/s41583-024-00802-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 02/23/2024] [Indexed: 04/14/2024]
Abstract
Language behaviour is complex, but neuroscientific evidence disentangles it into distinct components supported by dedicated brain areas or networks. In this Review, we describe the 'core' language network, which includes left-hemisphere frontal and temporal areas, and show that it is strongly interconnected, independent of input and output modalities, causally important for language and language-selective. We discuss evidence that this language network plausibly stores language knowledge and supports core linguistic computations related to accessing words and constructions from memory and combining them to interpret (decode) or generate (encode) linguistic messages. We emphasize that the language network works closely with, but is distinct from, both lower-level - perceptual and motor - mechanisms and higher-level systems of knowledge and reasoning. The perceptual and motor mechanisms process linguistic signals, but, in contrast to the language network, are sensitive only to these signals' surface properties, not their meanings; the systems of knowledge and reasoning (such as the system that supports social reasoning) are sometimes engaged during language use but are not language-selective. This Review lays a foundation both for in-depth investigations of these different components of the language processing pipeline and for probing inter-component interactions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Evelina Fedorenko
- Brain and Cognitive Sciences Department, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA.
- McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA.
- The Program in Speech and Hearing in Bioscience and Technology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA.
| | - Anna A Ivanova
- School of Psychology, Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, GA, USA
| | - Tamar I Regev
- Brain and Cognitive Sciences Department, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA
- McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Soto G, Clarke MT, Savaldi-Harussi G. Relationship between lexicon and grammar in children and youth who use augmentative and alternative communication. Augment Altern Commun 2023; 39:293-301. [PMID: 37671918 DOI: 10.1080/07434618.2023.2237108] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/11/2022] [Accepted: 06/15/2023] [Indexed: 09/07/2023] Open
Abstract
The present study investigated the relationship between lexicon and grammar in individuals who use graphic symbol-based aided augmentative and alternative communication (AAC). Data came from 60 transcripts of generalization sessions that were part of two previous intervention studies, aimed at improving the expressive vocabulary and grammar of 12 children and youth who used graphic symbol-based AAC. The specific aims of the current study were to (a) describe vocabulary composition across different levels of expressive vocabulary and (b) analyze the relationship between global measures of expressive vocabulary and the use of grammar in individuals who use aided AAC. A series of multiple linear mixed effect regression analyses showed a positive predictive association between overall vocabulary size and the use of closed-class words, and a positive relationship between the use of verbs and the use of closed-class words. Additionally, the use of verbs had a significant positive association with the use of inflectional morphology, while the use of nouns did not. Theoretical and practical implications of these findings are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gloria Soto
- Department of Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences, San Francisco State University, San Francisco, California, USA
| | - Michael T Clarke
- Department of Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences, San Francisco State University, San Francisco, California, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
7
|
Li L, Su YE, Hou W, Zhou M, Xie Y, Zou X, Li M. Expressive Language Profiles in a Clinical Screening Sample of Mandarin-Speaking Preschool Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2023; 66:4497-4518. [PMID: 37758191 DOI: 10.1044/2023_jslhr-23-00184] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 09/30/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE This cross-sectional study aimed to depict expressive language profiles and clarify lexical-grammatical interrelationships in Mandarin-speaking preschoolers with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) during the administration of the simplified Chinese Psychoeducational Profile-Third Edition screening. METHOD We collected naturalistic language samples from 81 (74 boys, seven girls) 2- to 7-year-old (Mage = 55.6 months, SD = 15.17) Mandarin-speaking children with ASD in clinician-child interactions. The child participants were divided into five age subgroups with 12-month intervals according to their chronological age. Computer-assisted part-of-speech tagging, constituency analysis, and dependency analysis addressed the developmental trajectories of early lexical and grammatical growth in each age subgroup. RESULTS Significant within-ASD differences were observed in content words, function words, and lexical categories. Nouns and verbs were the predominant lexical categories, while noun types overwhelmed verb types in children over 3 years old. The grammatical development of 5- to 6-year-old Mandarin-speaking children with ASD was better than that of 3- to 4-year-old children. The trends of syntactic structures, grammatical relations, and grammatical complexity in each age group were similar. CONCLUSIONS Mandarin-speaking preschoolers with ASD produce more lexicons with increasing age. They preserve the noun bias as a universal mechanism in early lexical learning. Moreover, their developmental trajectories of grammatical growth were comparable in each age subgroup. In addition, their lexicons and grammar were synchronically developed during early language acquisition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Li Li
- Child Language Lab, School of Foreign Languages, Central South University, Changsha, China
- Data Science Research Center, Duke Kunshan University, Suzhou, China
| | - Yi Esther Su
- Child Language Lab, School of Foreign Languages, Central South University, Changsha, China
| | - Wenwen Hou
- Child Language Lab, School of Foreign Languages, Central South University, Changsha, China
| | - Muyu Zhou
- Child Language Lab, School of Foreign Languages, Central South University, Changsha, China
| | - Yixiang Xie
- School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, China
| | - Xiaobing Zou
- Child Development and Behavior Center, Third Affiliated Hospital of Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou, China
| | - Ming Li
- Data Science Research Center, Duke Kunshan University, Suzhou, China
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Everaert E, Boerma T, Selten I, Gerrits E, Houben M, Vorstman J, Wijnen F. Nonverbal Executive Functioning in Relation to Vocabulary and Morphosyntax in Preschool Children With and Without Developmental Language Disorder. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2023; 66:3954-3973. [PMID: 37713541 DOI: 10.1044/2023_jslhr-22-00732] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 09/17/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE Developmental language disorder (DLD) is characterized by persistent and unexplained difficulties in language development. Accumulating evidence shows that children with DLD also present with deficits in other cognitive domains, such as executive functioning (EF). There is an ongoing debate on whether exclusively verbal EF abilities are impaired in children with DLD or whether nonverbal EF is also impaired, and whether these EF impairments are related to their language difficulties. The aims of this study were to (a) compare nonverbal performance of preschoolers with DLD and typically developing (TD) peers, (b) examine how nonverbal EF and language abilities are related, and (c) investigate whether a diagnosis of DLD moderates the relationship between EF and language abilities. METHOD A total of 143 children (nDLD = 65, nTD = 78) participated. All children were between 3 and 6.5 years old and were monolingual Dutch. We assessed nonverbal EF with a visual selective attention task, a visuospatial short-term and working memory task, and a task gauging broad EF abilities. Vocabulary and morphosyntax were each measured with two standardized language tests. We created latent variables for EF, vocabulary, and morphosyntax. RESULTS Analyses showed that children with DLD were outperformed by their TD peers on all nonverbal EF tasks. Nonverbal EF abilities were related to morphosyntactic abilities in both groups, whereas a relationship between vocabulary and EF skills was found in the TD group only. These relationships were not significantly moderated by a diagnosis of DLD. CONCLUSIONS We found evidence for nonverbal EF impairments in preschool children with DLD. Moreover, nonverbal EF and morphosyntactic abilities were significantly related in these children. These findings may have implications for intervention and support the improvement of prognostic accuracy. SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.24121287.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emma Everaert
- Institute for Language Sciences, Utrecht University, Trans 10, the Netherlands
- Department of Pediatrics, Wilhelmina Children's Hospital, University Medical Center Utrecht, the Netherlands
| | - Tessel Boerma
- Institute for Language Sciences, Utrecht University, Trans 10, the Netherlands
- Department of Pediatrics, Wilhelmina Children's Hospital, University Medical Center Utrecht, the Netherlands
| | - Iris Selten
- Institute for Language Sciences, Utrecht University, Trans 10, the Netherlands
- Department of Pediatrics, Wilhelmina Children's Hospital, University Medical Center Utrecht, the Netherlands
| | - Ellen Gerrits
- Institute for Language Sciences, Utrecht University, Trans 10, the Netherlands
- Research Group Speech and Language Therapy: Participation Through Communication, HU University of Applied Sciences, Utrecht, the Netherlands
| | - Michiel Houben
- Department of Pediatrics, Wilhelmina Children's Hospital, University Medical Center Utrecht, the Netherlands
| | - Jacob Vorstman
- Program in Genetics and Genome Biology, Research Institute, and Department of Psychiatry, Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| | - Frank Wijnen
- Institute for Language Sciences, Utrecht University, Trans 10, the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
van Koert M, Leona N, Rispens J, Tijms J, Molen MVD, Grunberg HL, Snellings P. English Grammar Skills in Dutch Grade 4 Children: Examining the Relation Between L1 and L2 Language Skills. JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLINGUISTIC RESEARCH 2023; 52:1737-1753. [PMID: 37184734 PMCID: PMC10520197 DOI: 10.1007/s10936-023-09968-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 03/21/2023] [Indexed: 05/16/2023]
Abstract
Second language proficiency may be related to first language acquisition (Ganschow & Sparks, 1991), but relatively little is known about the relation between first and second language grammatical proficiency in primary school children who are in their first stages of foreign language learning. This study aims to determine whether differences in Dutch and English vocabulary and Dutch grammar skills predict differences in English grammatical proficiency in Dutch speaking children who are in grade 4 in primary school. The selected participants are monolingual Dutch pupils (N = 152), aged 9;0-10;0. To measure the children's vocabulary the PPVT was used in Dutch (Schlichting, 2005) and in English (Dunn & Dunn, 2007). In addition, two grammar tasks in English and one in Dutch of the CELF (Semel et al., 2003) were used. The results show that English vocabulary is a strong predictor of English grammar skills, and that the Dutch vocabulary skills are weaker predictors of English grammar skills. Moreover, Dutch grammar skills predict English grammar skills for one of the grammar tasks. These results are discussed vis-à-vis hypotheses about cross-domain transfer and cross-linguistic transfer (Blom et al., 2012; Cummins, 1979; Ganschow & Sparks, 1991; Paradis, 2011; Sparks, 1995).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | - Jurgen Tijms
- University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Haim M, Bat-El Foux O. Asynchronization at the phonology-morphology interface: A case study of an atypically developing Hebrew-acquiring boy. CLINICAL LINGUISTICS & PHONETICS 2023; 37:802-827. [PMID: 35876441 DOI: 10.1080/02699206.2022.2089912] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/05/2021] [Revised: 05/30/2022] [Accepted: 06/02/2022] [Indexed: 06/15/2023]
Abstract
In this paper, we present a case study of an atypically developing Hebrew-acquiring boy (YV), in comparison with three typically developing boys. Drawing on data from longitudinal studies, we examined the development of two verbal suffixes, -im 'ms.pl.pres' and -ti '1.sg.past', with reference to two prosodic structures that these suffixes assume - word final codas for -im and trisyllabic words for -ti (e.g. bon-ím 'they build', kará-ti 'I read'). We found that YV's developmental trajectory was similar to that of the three boys in both phonology and morphology, each module independently; the deviant phenomena were found at the interface between phonology and morphology. The typically developing boys produced the relevant phonological structures in bare stems before they produced them in suffixed forms. YV, however, proceeded in the opposite order; he produced the final m in -im verbs before mastering word final codas in bare stems; similarly, he produced trisyllabic forms in -ti verbs before mastering them in bare stems. We attribute this deviance to asynchronization between phonological (prosodic) and morphological development. That is, YV's phonology lagged behind his morphology, but this lag did not block the morphological development as it would have in synchronized development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mor Haim
- Department of Linguistics, Tel-Aviv University, Tel-Aviv, Israel
| | - Outi Bat-El Foux
- Department of Linguistics, Tel-Aviv University, Tel-Aviv, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Benítez-Burraco A, Hoshi K, Progovac L. The gradual coevolution of syntactic combinatorics and categorization under the effects of human self-domestication: a proposal. Cogn Process 2023; 24:425-439. [PMID: 37306792 PMCID: PMC10359229 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-023-01140-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/20/2022] [Accepted: 04/15/2023] [Indexed: 06/13/2023]
Abstract
The gradual emergence of syntax has been claimed to be engaged in a feedback loop with Human Self-Domestication (HSD), both processes resulting from, and contributing to, enhanced connectivity in selected cortico-striatal networks, which is the mechanism for attenuating reactive aggression, the hallmark of HSD, but also the mechanism of cross-modality, relevant for syntax. Here, we aim to bridge the gap between these brain changes and further changes facilitated by the gradual complexification of grammars. We propose that increased cross-modality would have enabled and supported, more specifically, a feedback loop between categorization abilities relevant for vocabulary building and the gradual emergence of syntactic structure, including Merge. In brief, an enhanced categorization ability not only brings about more distinct categories, but also a critical number of tokens in each category necessary for Merge to take off in a systematic and productive fashion; in turn, the benefits of expressive capabilities brought about by productive Merge encourage more items to be categorized, and more categories to be formed, thus further potentiating categorization abilities, and with it, syntax again. We support our hypothesis with evidence from the domains of language development and animal communication, but also from biology, neuroscience, paleoanthropology, and clinical linguistics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Antonio Benítez-Burraco
- Department of Spanish, Linguistics and Theory of Literature (Linguistics), Faculty of Philology, University of Seville, Seville, Spain.
| | - Koji Hoshi
- Faculty of Economics, Keio University, Tokyo, Japan
| | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
Eriksson M, Myrberg K. How the communicative development inventories can contribute to clinical assessments of children with speech and language disorders. Front Psychol 2023; 14:1176028. [PMID: 37519399 PMCID: PMC10382198 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1176028] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/28/2023] [Accepted: 06/14/2023] [Indexed: 08/01/2023] Open
Abstract
Introduction The purpose of the present study was to investigate whether information from the Swedish version of the Communicative Development Inventories III (SCDI-III) is informative to the Speech and Language Pathologist (SLP) when examining children with suspected speech and language disorders at a SLP unit. Method Parents to 50 children (25 girls, 25 boys, age 30-80 months) that had been referred to the local SLP unit completed the SCDI-III. Nine children came from multilingual families and 41 children came from monolingual, Swedish speaking homes. The children were diagnosed as having developmental speech disorders (12) or developmental language disorders (33). Five children were not diagnosed with any disorder. Results The results showed that the referred children performed significantly lower on scales for word production, grammar, and metalinguistic awareness, compared to a subset from the norms with a similar age and gender composition. Most children fell below the 10th percentile on word production and grammatical constructions. The intercorrelation between the three scales were in general substantial. Comparisons of children's performance on the vocabulary and grammar scales of SCDI-III, and the medical records revealed 18 cases of discordance that would have motivated further examination. The parents rated sometimes their child's vocabulary and grammar skills as higher and sometime as lower to the medical records. Discussion Limitations due to attrition and sample size were discussed. It was concluded that the SCDI-III can provide valuable information to the examination at the SLP clinic in addition to parent interviews, observations of children, and various tests, and that the potential for adapted versions would be particularly high for examinations of multilingual children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mårten Eriksson
- Faculty of Health and Occupational Studies, Department of Occupational Health Sciences and Psychology, University of Gävle, Gävle, Sweden
| | - Karin Myrberg
- Centre for Research and Development, Region Gävleborg/Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden
- Faculty of Health and Occupational Studies, Department of Caring Sciences, University of Gävle, Gävle, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Visapää M, Munck P, Stolt S. Associations between early lexical composition and pre-reading skills at 5 years - A longitudinal study. Early Hum Dev 2023; 182:105780. [PMID: 37127018 DOI: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2023.105780] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/02/2023] [Revised: 04/25/2023] [Accepted: 04/25/2023] [Indexed: 05/03/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND At the end of the second year, children's lexical compositions (LexC) differ significantly in terms of variety of lexical categories (nouns, verbs, adjectives, closed-class words). The aim of this study was to investigate whether this variation is associated with acquisition of pre-reading skills (PreRS) at 5;0. AIMS To study the associations between LexC at 2;0 and PreRS at 5;0 and to examine the possible explaining value of LexC and lexicon size for PreRS. PARTICIPANTS AND METHODS Participants were 66 healthy, monolingual Finnish speaking children. LexC was measured at 2;0 using the standardized Finnish long form version of the MacArthur Communicative Developmental Inventory (FinCDI). Raw scores and percentages of words were used in the analysis. At 5;0, PreRS variables of letter knowledge, rapid automatized naming (RAN), lexical ability and phonological processing were collected. RESULTS The number of social terms, nouns, verbs, adjectives and closed class words associated significantly with all PreRS except RAN. The percentages of predicates and closed class words were positively associated with PreRS. All LexC variables and lexicon size at 2;0 had significant predictive values for the composite pre-reading score, explaining 19-32 % of the variation. The best model to explain PreRS included the number of nouns as the linguistic variable. A high percentage of social terms at 2;0 proposed weak PreRS at 5;0. CONCLUSIONS LexC at 2;0 is a significant predictor of PreRS at 5;0. Closer examination of lexical composition is important, when assessing lexical skills at the end of the second year.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marianna Visapää
- Faculty of Medicine, Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Unit of Logopedics, University of Helsinki, Haartmaninkatu 3, 00014 Helsinki, Finland; Kommunikaari Cooperative for Speech Therapy, Haartmaninkatu 3, 00014 Helsinki, Finland.
| | - Petriina Munck
- Faculty of Medicine, Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Unit of Logopedics, University of Helsinki, Haartmaninkatu 3, 00014 Helsinki, Finland.
| | - Suvi Stolt
- Faculty of Medicine, Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Unit of Logopedics, University of Helsinki, Haartmaninkatu 3, 00014 Helsinki, Finland.
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Rezaii N, Ren B, Quimby M, Hochberg D, Dickerson BC. Less is more in language production: an information-theoretic analysis of agrammatism in primary progressive aphasia. Brain Commun 2023; 5:fcad136. [PMID: 37324242 PMCID: PMC10263269 DOI: 10.1093/braincomms/fcad136] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/17/2022] [Revised: 03/06/2023] [Accepted: 04/21/2023] [Indexed: 06/17/2023] Open
Abstract
Agrammatism is a disorder of language production characterized by short, simplified sentences, the omission of function words, an increased use of nouns over verbs and a higher use of heavy verbs. Despite observing these phenomena for decades, the accounts of agrammatism have not converged. Here, we propose and test the hypothesis that the lexical profile of agrammatism results from a process that opts for words with a lower frequency of occurrence to increase lexical information. Furthermore, we hypothesize that this process is a compensatory response to patients' core deficit in producing long, complex sentences. In this cross-sectional study, we analysed speech samples of patients with primary progressive aphasia (n = 100) and healthy speakers (n = 65) as they described a picture. The patient cohort included 34 individuals with the non-fluent variant, 41 with the logopenic variant and 25 with the semantic variant of primary progressive aphasia. We first analysed a large corpus of spoken language and found that the word types preferred by patients with agrammatism tend to have lower frequencies of occurrence than less preferred words. We then conducted a computational simulation to examine the impact of word frequency on lexical information as measured by entropy. We found that strings of words that exclude highly frequent words have a more uniform word distribution, thereby increasing lexical entropy. To test whether the lexical profile of agrammatism results from their inability to produce long sentences, we asked healthy speakers to produce short sentences during the picture description task. We found that, under this constrained condition, a similar lexical profile of agrammatism emerged in the short sentences of healthy individuals, including fewer function words, more nouns than verbs and more heavy verbs than light verbs. This lexical profile of short sentences resulted in their lower average word frequency than unconstrained sentences. We extended this finding by showing that, in general, shorter sentences get packaged with lower-frequency words as a basic property of efficient language production, evident in the language of healthy speakers and all primary progressive aphasia variants.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Neguine Rezaii
- Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02129, USA
| | - Boyu Ren
- Department of Psychiatry, Laboratory for Psychiatric Biostatistics, McLean Hospital, Belmont, MA 02478, USA
| | - Megan Quimby
- Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02129, USA
| | - Daisy Hochberg
- Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02129, USA
| | - Bradford C Dickerson
- Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02129, USA
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Liu J, Yu F, Feng C, Li S. The Pausing Strategies in Chinese Preschool Children's Narratives. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2023; 66:431-443. [PMID: 36626390 DOI: 10.1044/2022_jslhr-21-00542] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/17/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE Language production, a dynamic process involving real-time language processing, is crucial for children's language and communication development. To explore the early development of children's real-time language production, this study investigated Chinese preschool children's pausing strategies in narratives and their associations with verbal working memory and vocabulary abilities. METHOD A picture-elicited narrative task was employed. Sixty Mandarin-speaking children aged 4-5 years were asked to tell a story according to the book Frog, Where Are You? The pausing types and positions in narratives were coded and analyzed. Additionally, children's verbal working memory and vocabulary were measured. RESULTS The results showed that 4- to 5-year-old children prefer to use silent pauses and tend to produce pauses within clauses. The total frequency of pausing decreases with age and shows a significant gender difference. Girls prefer to use within-clause pauses, whereas boys prefer to use between-clause pauses. More importantly, children's pausing frequency is closely associated with their verbal working memory and vocabulary, in which working memory plays a more important role. CONCLUSIONS This study is a first-step exploration of pausing strategies in 4- to 5-year-old Chinese children's narratives. The developmental characteristics of pausing strategies shown in typically developing children serve as a crucial reference for interventions for children with language deficits.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jiehan Liu
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
- Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
| | - Fan Yu
- Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
| | - Chen Feng
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
- Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
| | - Su Li
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
- Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Word learning in ASD: the sensorimotor, the perceptual and the symbolic. JOURNAL OF CULTURAL COGNITIVE SCIENCE 2023. [DOI: 10.1007/s41809-022-00117-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/09/2023]
Abstract
AbstractWord learning requires successful pairing of form and meaning. A common hypothesis about the process of word learning is that initially, infants work on identifying the phonological segments corresponding to words (speech analysis), and subsequently map those segments onto meaning. A range of theories have been proposed to account for the underlying mechanisms and factors in this remarkable achievement. While some are mainly concerned with the sensorimotor affordances and perceptual properties of referents out in the world, other theories emphasize the importance of language as a system, and the relations among language units (other words or syntax). Recent approaches inspired by neuro-science suggest that the storage and processing of word meanings is supported by neural systems subserving both the representation of conceptual knowledge and its access and use (Lambon Ralph et al., Nature Reviews Neuroscience 18:42–55, 2017). Developmental disorders have been attested to impact on different aspects of word learning. While impaired word knowledge is not a hallmark of Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), and remains largely understudied in this population, there is evidence that there are, sometimes subtle, problems in that domain, reflected in both how such knowledge is acquired and how words are used (Vulchanova et al., Word knowledge and word usage: A cross-disciplinary guide to the mental lexicon, Mouton De Gruyter, 2020). In addition, experimental evidence suggests that children with autism present with specific problems in categorizing the referents of linguistic labels leading to subsequent problems with using those labels (Hartley and Allen, Autism 19:570–579, 2015). Furthermore, deficits have been reported in some of the underlying mechanisms, biases and use of cues in word learning, such as e.g., object shape (Field et al., Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 46:1210–1219, 2016; Tek et al., Autism Research 1:208–222, 2008). Finally, it is likely that symbol use might be impaired in ASD, however, the direction of the causal relationship between social and communication impairment in autism and symbolic skills is still an open question (Allen and Lewis, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 45:1–3, 2015; Allen and Butler, British Journal of Developmental Psychology 38:345–362, 2020; Wainwright et al., Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 50:2941–2956, 2020). Further support for impaired symbol formation in autism comes from the well-attested problems with figurative, non-literal language use (e.g., metaphors, idioms, hyperbole, irony) (Vulchanova et al., Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9:24, 2015). Here we propose that embodied theories of cognition which link perceptual experience with conceptual knowledge (see Eigsti, Frontiers in Psychology 4:224, 2013; Klin et al., Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 358:345–360, 2003) might be useful in explaining the difficulty in symbolic understanding that individuals with autism face during the word learning process.
Collapse
|
17
|
Klein CC, Berger P, Goucha T, Friederici AD, Grosse Wiesmann C. Children’s syntax is supported by the maturation of BA44 at 4 years, but of the posterior STS at 3 years of age. Cereb Cortex 2022; 33:5426-5435. [PMID: 36408641 PMCID: PMC10152089 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhac430] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/06/2022] [Revised: 09/07/2022] [Accepted: 09/26/2022] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Abstract
Within the first years of life, children learn major aspects of their native language. However, the ability to process complex sentence structures, a core faculty in human language called syntax, emerges only slowly. A milestone in syntax acquisition is reached around the age of 4 years, when children learn a variety of syntactic concepts. Here, we ask which maturational changes in the child’s brain underlie the emergence of syntactically complex sentence processing around this critical age. We relate markers of cortical brain maturation to 3- and 4-year-olds’ sentence processing in contrast to other language abilities. Our results show that distinct cortical brain areas support sentence processing in the two age groups. Sentence production abilities at 3 years were associated with increased surface area in the most posterior part of the left superior temporal sulcus, whereas 4-year-olds showed an association with cortical thickness in the left posterior part of Broca’s area, i.e. BA44. The present findings suggest that sentence processing abilities rely on the maturation of distinct cortical regions in 3- compared to 4-year-olds. The observed shift to more mature regions involved in processing syntactically complex sentences may underlie behavioral milestones in syntax acquisition at around 4 years.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cheslie C Klein
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences Department of Neuropsychology, , Stephanstraße 1a, Leipzig 04103 , Germany
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences Research Group Milestones of Early Cognitive Development, , Stephanstraße 1a, Leipzig 04103 , Germany
| | - Philipp Berger
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences Department of Neuropsychology, , Stephanstraße 1a, Leipzig 04103 , Germany
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences Research Group Milestones of Early Cognitive Development, , Stephanstraße 1a, Leipzig 04103 , Germany
| | - Tomás Goucha
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences Department of Neuropsychology, , Stephanstraße 1a, Leipzig 04103 , Germany
| | - Angela D Friederici
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences Department of Neuropsychology, , Stephanstraße 1a, Leipzig 04103 , Germany
| | - Charlotte Grosse Wiesmann
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences Research Group Milestones of Early Cognitive Development, , Stephanstraße 1a, Leipzig 04103 , Germany
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Fedorenko E, Ryskin R, Gibson E. Agrammatic output in non-fluent, including Broca's, aphasia as a rational behavior. APHASIOLOGY 2022; 37:1981-2000. [PMID: 38213953 PMCID: PMC10782888 DOI: 10.1080/02687038.2022.2143233] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/11/2022] [Accepted: 10/31/2022] [Indexed: 01/13/2024]
Abstract
Background Speech of individuals with non-fluent, including Broca's, aphasia is often characterized as "agrammatic" because their output mostly consists of nouns and, to a lesser extent, verbs and lacks function words, like articles and prepositions, and correct morphological endings. Among the earliest accounts of agrammatic output in the early 1900s was the "economy of effort" idea whereby agrammatic output is construed as a way of coping with increases in the cost of language production. This idea resurfaced in the 1980s, but in general, the field of language research has largely focused on accounts of agrammatism that postulated core deficits in syntactic knowledge. Aims We here revisit the economy of effort hypothesis in light of increasing emphasis in cognitive science on rational and efficient behavior. Main contribution The critical idea is as follows: there is a cost per unit of linguistic output, and this cost is greater for patients with non-fluent aphasia. For a rational agent, this increase leads to shorter messages. Critically, the informative parts of the message should be preserved and the redundant ones (like the function words and inflectional markers) should be omitted. Although economy of effort is unlikely to provide a unifying account of agrammatic output in all patients-the relevant population is too heterogeneous and the empirical landscape too complex for any single-factor explanation-we argue that the idea of agrammatic output as a rational behavior was dismissed prematurely and appears to provide a plausible explanation for a large subset of the reported cases of expressive aphasia. Conclusions The rational account of expressive agrammatism should be evaluated more carefully and systematically. On the basic research side, pursuing this hypothesis may reveal how the human mind and brain optimize communicative efficiency in the presence of production difficulties. And on the applied side, this construal of expressive agrammatism emphasizes the strengths of some patients to flexibly adapt utterances in order to communicate in spite of grammatical difficulties; and focusing on these strengths may be more effective than trying to "fix" their grammar.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Evelina Fedorenko
- Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Brain & Cognitive Sciences Department
- Massachusetts Institute of Technology, McGovern Institute for Brain Research
- Speech and Hearing in Bioscience and Technology program at Harvard University
| | - Rachel Ryskin
- University of California at Merced, Cognitive & Information Sciences Department
| | - Edward Gibson
- Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Brain & Cognitive Sciences Department
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Macdonald KT, Francis DJ, Hernandez AE, Castilla-Earls AP, Cirino PT. Characterization of English and Spanish language proficiency among middle school English learners with reading difficulties. BILINGUALISM (CAMBRIDGE, ENGLAND) 2022; 25:899-912. [PMID: 36644407 PMCID: PMC9838822 DOI: 10.1017/s1366728922000104] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/17/2023]
Abstract
Among bilinguals, language-related variables such as first and second language proficiency and balance may be related to important cognitive and academic outcomes, but approaches to characterizing these variables are inconsistent, particularly among at-risk samples of children. The current study employed comprehensive language assessment of English and Spanish language skills and contrasted various approaches to the characterization of language among at-risk ELs in middle school (N = 161). Specifically, we contrasted variable-centered and person-centered approaches, and convergence between objective and self-report measures. Findings support a two-factor structure of English and Spanish language skills in this population, three profiles of students (balanced, moderately unbalanced-higher Spanish, and very unbalanced-higher English), convergence between variable-centered and person-centered approaches, and mixed support for subjective indices of usage. Results provide a foundation from which to examine the roles of L1 and L2 proficiency as well as balance in important cognitive and academic outcomes in this at-risk and understudied population.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kelly T Macdonald
- Department of Psychology, Texas Institute for Measurement, Evaluation, and Statistics, University of Houston, Houston, Texas
| | - David J Francis
- Department of Psychology, Texas Institute for Measurement, Evaluation, and Statistics, University of Houston, Houston, Texas
| | - Arturo E Hernandez
- Department of Psychology, Texas Institute for Measurement, Evaluation, and Statistics, University of Houston, Houston, Texas
| | - Anny P Castilla-Earls
- Department of Communication Disorders and Sciences, University of Houston, Houston, Texas
| | - Paul T Cirino
- Department of Psychology, Texas Institute for Measurement, Evaluation, and Statistics, University of Houston, Houston, Texas
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Finley S. Morphological cues as an aid to word learning: a cross-situational word learning study. JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2022. [DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2022.2113087] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/15/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Sara Finley
- Department of Psychology, Pacific Lutheran University, Tacoma, WA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Dudschig C. Are control processes domain-general? A replication of 'To adapt or not to adapt? The question of domain-general cognitive control' (Kan et al. 2013). ROYAL SOCIETY OPEN SCIENCE 2022; 9:210550. [PMID: 35911207 PMCID: PMC9326271 DOI: 10.1098/rsos.210550] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/31/2021] [Accepted: 07/01/2022] [Indexed: 06/15/2023]
Abstract
Conflict and conflict adaptation are well-studied phenomena in experimental psychology. Standard tasks investigating causes and outcomes of conflict during information processing include the Stroop, the Flanker and the Simon task. Interestingly, recent research efforts have moved toward investigating whether conflict in one task domain influences information processing in another task domain, typically referred to as cross-task conflict adaptation. These transfer effects are of central importance for theories about our cognitive architecture, as they are interpreted as pointing towards domain-general cognitive mechanisms. Given the importance of these cross-task transfer effects, the current paper targets at replicating one of the key findings. Specifically, Kan et al. (Kan et al. 2013 Cognition 129, 637-651) showed that reading syntactically ambiguous sentences result in processing adjustments in subsequent Stroop trials. This result is in line with the idea that conflict monitoring works in a domain overarching manner. The present paper presents two replication studies: (i) exact replication: identical sentence-reading task intermixed with stimulus-based Stroop task and (ii) conceptual replication: identical sentence-reading task intermixed with response-based Stroop task. Power calculations were based on the original paper. Both experiments were pre-registered. Despite the experiments being closely designed according to the original study, there was no evidence supporting the hypothesis regarding cross-domain conflict adaptation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carolin Dudschig
- Fachbereich Psychologie, University Tübingen, Schleichstr. 4, 72076 Tübingen
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Eshghi M, Adatorwovor R, Preisser JS, Crais ER, Zajac DJ. Lexicogrammatical skills in 2-year-old children with and without repaired cleft palate. CLINICAL LINGUISTICS & PHONETICS 2022; 36:528-546. [PMID: 34263689 PMCID: PMC8760352 DOI: 10.1080/02699206.2021.1941263] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/04/2020] [Revised: 05/11/2021] [Accepted: 05/14/2021] [Indexed: 06/05/2023]
Abstract
The purpose of the current research was to compare the lexical-grammatical skills of two-year-old children with and without repaired cleft palate (CP), accounting for the effect of variables such as vocabulary size at 18 months of age, maternal education level, and gender. Participants included 52 children with CP and 25 typically developing (TD) children. The CDI-WS was employed to measure vocabulary and grammatical skills. Significant differences were observed between the CP and TD groups with respect to the number of words, word forms (irregular nouns and verbs), word endings (overuse of plural (-s) and past tense (-ed) markers), the mean number of morphemes in their three longest utterances (M3L), and sentence complexity. In addition, compared to TD children, significantly smaller proportions of children with CP were observed to use words to talk about past and future events or use words to talk about an absent object. The difference between the CP and TD groups in terms of the size of vocabulary at 24 months of age remained statistically significant in the multivariable model. Among all predictors, the size of vocabulary at 18 months of age was identified as the most robust precursor of lexical and grammatical skills at 24 months of age. Gender was identified as a predictor of the M3L measure as an index for syntactic ability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marziye Eshghi
- Ph.D., Postdoctoral Research Associate, Speech and Feeding Disorders Lab, MGH Institute of Health Professions
| | - Reuben Adatorwovor
- Ph.D., Assistant Professor, Department of Biostatistics, College of Public Health, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky
| | - John S. Preisser
- Ph.D, Research Professor, Department of Biostatistics, Gillings School of Global Public Health, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
| | - Elizabeth R. Crais
- Ph.D, Professor, Division of Speech and Hearing Sciences, School of Medicine, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
| | - David J. Zajac
- Ph.D, CCC-SLP, Professor, Department of Craniofacial and Surgical Care, Associate Director, Speech-Language Pathology, Craniofacial Center, School of Dentistry, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Vulchanova M, Vulchanov V, Sorace A, Suarez-Gomez C, Guijarro-Fuentes P. Editorial: The Notion of the Native Speaker Put to the Test: Recent Research Advances. Front Psychol 2022; 13:875740. [PMID: 35422733 PMCID: PMC9003014 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.875740] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/14/2022] [Accepted: 03/08/2022] [Indexed: 12/02/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Mila Vulchanova
- Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway
| | | | | | - Cristina Suarez-Gomez
- Department of Spanish, Modern and Classic Philology, University of the Balearic Islands, Palma, Spain
| | - Pedro Guijarro-Fuentes
- Department of Spanish, Modern and Classic Philology, University of the Balearic Islands, Palma, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Creaghe N, Quinn S, Kidd E. Symbolic play provides a fertile context for language development. INFANCY 2021; 26:980-1010. [PMID: 34297890 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12422] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/24/2020] [Revised: 06/21/2021] [Accepted: 06/29/2021] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
In this study, we test the hypothesis that symbolic play represents a fertile context for language acquisition because its inherent ambiguity elicits communicative behaviors that positively influence development. Infant-caregiver dyads (N = 54) participated in two 20-minute play sessions six months apart (Time 1 = 18 months, Time 2 = 24 months). During each session, the dyads played with two sets of toys that elicited either symbolic or functional play. The sessions were transcribed and coded for several features of dyadic interaction and language; infants' linguistic proficiency was measured via parental report. The two contexts elicited different communicative and linguistic behaviors. Notably, the symbolic play condition resulted in significantly greater conversational turn-taking than functional play, and also resulted in the greater use of questions and mimetics in infant-directed speech (IDS). In contrast, caregivers used more imperative clauses in functional play. Correlational and regression analyses showed that frequent properties of symbolic play (i.e., turn-taking, yes-no questions, mimetics) were positively related to infants' language proficiency, whereas frequent features of functional play (i.e., imperatives in IDS) were negatively related. The results provide evidence supporting the hypothesis that symbolic play is a fertile context for language development, driven by the need to negotiate meaning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Noëlie Creaghe
- Research School of Psychology, The Australian National University, Canberra, ACT, Australia.,ARC Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language, Canberra, ACT, Australia
| | - Sara Quinn
- Research School of Psychology, The Australian National University, Canberra, ACT, Australia
| | - Evan Kidd
- Research School of Psychology, The Australian National University, Canberra, ACT, Australia.,ARC Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language, Canberra, ACT, Australia.,Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands.,Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Sansavini A, Zuccarini M, Gibertoni D, Bello A, Caselli MC, Corvaglia L, Guarini A. Language Profiles and Their Relation to Cognitive and Motor Skills at 30 Months of Age: An Online Investigation of Low-Risk Preterm and Full-Term Children. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2021; 64:2715-2733. [PMID: 34215160 DOI: 10.1044/2021_jslhr-20-00636] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/13/2023]
Abstract
Purpose Wide interindividual variability characterizes language development in the general and at-risk populations of up to 3 years of age. We adopted a complex approach that considers multiple aspects of lexical and grammatical skills to identify language profiles in low-risk preterm and full-term children. We also investigated biological and environmental predictors and relations between language profiles and cognitive and motor skills. Method We enrolled 200 thirty-month-old Italian-speaking children-consisting of 100 low-risk preterm and 100 comparable full-term children. Parents filled out the Italian version of the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories Infant and Toddler Short Forms (word comprehension, word production, and incomplete and complete sentence production), Parent Report of Children's Abilities-Revised (cognitive score), and Early Motor Questionnaire (fine motor, gross motor, perception-action, and total motor scores) questionnaires. Results A latent profile analysis identified four profiles: poor (21%), with lowest receptive and expressive vocabulary and absent or limited word combination and phonological accuracy; weak (22.5%), with average receptive but limited expressive vocabulary, incomplete sentences, and absent or limited phonological accuracy; average (25%), with average receptive and expressive vocabulary, use of incomplete and complete sentences, and partial phonological accuracy; and advanced (31.5%), with highest expressive vocabulary, complete sentence production, and phonological accuracy. Lower cognitive and motor scores characterized the poor profile, and lower cognitive and perception-action scores characterized the weak profile. Having a nonworking mother and a father with lower education increased the probability of a child's assignment to the poor profile, whereas being small for gestational age at birth increased it for the weak profile. Conclusions These findings suggest a need for a person-centered and cross-domain approach to identifying children with language weaknesses and implementing timely interventions. An online procedure for data collection and data-driven analyses based on multiple lexical and grammatical skills appear to be promising methodological innovations. Supplemental Material https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.14818179.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Dino Gibertoni
- Department of Biomedical and Neuromotor Sciences, University of Bologna, Italy
| | | | | | - Luigi Corvaglia
- Department of Medical and Surgical Sciences, University of Bologna, Italy
- Neonatology and Neonatal Intensive Care Unit, S. Orsola-Malpighi Hospital, Bologna, Italy
| | - Annalisa Guarini
- Department of Psychology "Renzo Canestrari", University of Bologna, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Casillas M, Brown P, Levinson SC. Early language experience in a Papuan community. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2021; 48:792-814. [PMID: 32988426 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000920000549] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
The rate at which young children are directly spoken to varies due to many factors, including (a) caregiver ideas about children as conversational partners and (b) the organization of everyday life. Prior work suggests cross-cultural variation in rates of child-directed speech is due to the former factor, but has been fraught with confounds in comparing postindustrial and subsistence farming communities. We investigate the daylong language environments of children (0;0-3;0) on Rossel Island, Papua New Guinea, a small-scale traditional community where prior ethnographic study demonstrated contingency-seeking child interaction styles. In fact, children were infrequently directly addressed and linguistic input rate was primarily affected by situational factors, though children's vocalization maturity showed no developmental delay. We compare the input characteristics between this community and a Tseltal Mayan one in which near-parallel methods produced comparable results, then briefly discuss the models and mechanisms for learning best supported by our findings.
Collapse
|
27
|
Cheong Y, Uehara I. Segmentation of Rhythmic Units in Word Speech by Japanese Infants and Toddlers. Front Psychol 2021; 12:626662. [PMID: 33897533 PMCID: PMC8062928 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.626662] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/06/2020] [Accepted: 03/08/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
When infants and toddlers are confronted with sequences of sounds, they are required to segment the sounds into meaningful units to achieve sufficient understanding. Rhythm has been regarded as a crucial cue for segmentation of speech sounds. Although previous intermodal methods indicated that infants and toddlers could detect differences in speech sounds based on stress-timed and syllable-timed units, these methods could not clearly indicate how infants and toddlers perform sound segmentation. Thus, the present study examined whether Japanese infants and toddlers could segment word speech sounds comprising basic morae (i.e., rhythm units similar to syllables), on the basis of concurrent basic mora units within syllable units, using the new intermodal matching procedure. The results indicated that, regardless of their ages and linguistic abilities, Japanese infants and toddlers aged 6-25 months tended to segment Japanese words comprising basic morae sounds on the basis of concurrent basic mora units within syllable units. This implies that infants' and toddlers' use of syllable units for segmentation of speech sounds at an early age could be evident among many infants and toddlers learning various languages. Although this finding should be interpreted carefully, the present study demonstrated the utility of the new intermodal matching procedure for examining segmentation of speech sounds and word sounds by infants and toddlers, on the basis of specific rhythm units.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yeonju Cheong
- Department of Psychology, Ochanomizu University, Tokyo, Japan
| | - Izumi Uehara
- Department of Psychology, Ochanomizu University, Tokyo, Japan
- Institute for Education and Human Development, Ochanomizu University, Tokyo, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Avila-Varela DS, Arias-Trejo N, Mani N. A longitudinal study of the role of vocabulary size in priming effects in early childhood. J Exp Child Psychol 2021; 205:105071. [PMID: 33529992 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2020.105071] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/10/2020] [Revised: 12/10/2020] [Accepted: 12/13/2020] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Studies on lexical development in young children often suggest that the organization of the early lexicon may vary with age and increasing vocabulary size. In the current study, we explicitly examined this suggestion in further detail using a longitudinal study of the development of phonological and semantic priming effects in the same group of toddlers at three different ages. In particular, our longitudinal design allows us to disentangle effects of increasing age and vocabulary size on priming and the extent to which vocabulary size may predict later priming effects. We tested phonological and semantic priming effects in monolingual German infants at 18, 21, and 24 months of age. We used the intermodal preferential looking paradigm combined with eye tracking to measure the influence of phonologically and semantic related/unrelated primes on target recognition. We found that phonological priming effects were predicted by participants' current vocabulary size even after controlling for participants' age and participants' early vocabulary size. Semantic priming effects were, in contrast, not predicted by vocabulary size. Finally, we also found a relationship between early phonological priming effects and later semantic priming effects as well as between early semantic priming effects and later phonological priming effects, potentially suggesting (limited) consistency in lexical structure across development. Taken together, these results highlight the important role of vocabulary size in the development of priming effects in early childhood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniela S Avila-Varela
- Psychology of Language Research Group, Georg-August-Universität Göttingen, 37073 Göttingen, Germany; Leibniz ScienceCampus "Primate Cognition", 37077 Göttingen, Germany; Center for Brain and Cognition, Department of Technology, Universitat Pompeu Fabra, 08005 Barcelona, Spain.
| | - Natalia Arias-Trejo
- Laboratorio de Psicolingüística, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Copilco Universidad, Coyoacan 04510, Ciudad de México, México
| | - Nivedita Mani
- Psychology of Language Research Group, Georg-August-Universität Göttingen, 37073 Göttingen, Germany; Leibniz ScienceCampus "Primate Cognition", 37077 Göttingen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Foster-Cohen S, van Bysterveldt A, Papp V. The roles of language use and vocabulary size in the emergence of word-combining in children with complex neurodevelopmental disabilities. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2021; 48:202-214. [PMID: 32460930 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000920000136] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
Parent report data on 82 preschool children with complex neurodevelopmental disabilities including Down syndrome, dyspraxia, autism, and global developmental delay suggests communicative language use must reach a threshold level before vocabulary size becomes the best predictor of word combining. Using the Language Use Inventory and the MacArthur-Bates CDI (with sign vocabulary option), statistical modelling using regression trees and random forests suggests that, despite high linear correlations between variables, (1) pragmatic ability, particularly children's emerging ability to talk about things, themselves and others is a significantly better predictor of the earliest word combining than vocabulary size; and (2) vocabulary size becomes a better predictor of later word combining, once this pragmatic base has been established.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Susan Foster-Cohen
- University of Canterbury, New Zealand Institute of Language, Brain and Behaviour
- The Champion Centre, Christchurch, New Zealand
| | - Anne van Bysterveldt
- University of Canterbury, New Zealand Institute of Language, Brain and Behaviour
| | - Viktoria Papp
- University of Canterbury, New Zealand Institute of Language, Brain and Behaviour
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Chai JH, Lo CH, Mayor J. A Bayesian-Inspired Item Response Theory-Based Framework to Produce Very Short Versions of MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2020; 63:3488-3500. [PMID: 32897770 DOI: 10.1044/2020_jslhr-20-00361] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
Purpose This study introduces a framework to produce very short versions of the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories (CDIs) by combining the Bayesian-inspired approach introduced by Mayor and Mani (2019) with an item response theory-based computerized adaptive testing that adapts to the ability of each child, in line with Makransky et al. (2016). Method We evaluated the performance of our approach-dynamically selecting maximally informative words from the CDI and combining parental response with prior vocabulary data-by conducting real-data simulations using four CDI versions having varying sample sizes on Wordbank-the online repository of digitalized CDIs: American English (a very large data set), Danish (a large data set), Beijing Mandarin (a medium-sized data set), and Italian (a small data set). Results Real-data simulations revealed that correlations exceeding .95 with full CDI administrations were reached with as few as 15 test items, with high levels of reliability, even when languages (e.g., Italian) possessed few digitalized administrations on Wordbank. Conclusions The current approach establishes a generic framework that produces very short (less than 20 items) adaptive early vocabulary assessments-hence considerably reducing their administration time. This approach appears to be robust even when CDIs have smaller samples in online repositories, for example, with around 50 samples per month-age.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jun Ho Chai
- University of Nottingham Malaysia, Semenyih, Selangor
- Jun Ho Chai and Chang Huan Lo share first authorship
| | - Chang Huan Lo
- University of Nottingham Malaysia, Semenyih, Selangor
- Jun Ho Chai and Chang Huan Lo share first authorship
| | | |
Collapse
|
31
|
Fedorenko E, Blank IA, Siegelman M, Mineroff Z. Lack of selectivity for syntax relative to word meanings throughout the language network. Cognition 2020; 203:104348. [PMID: 32569894 DOI: 10.1101/477851] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/23/2018] [Revised: 05/14/2020] [Accepted: 05/31/2020] [Indexed: 05/25/2023]
Abstract
To understand what you are reading now, your mind retrieves the meanings of words and constructions from a linguistic knowledge store (lexico-semantic processing) and identifies the relationships among them to construct a complex meaning (syntactic or combinatorial processing). Do these two sets of processes rely on distinct, specialized mechanisms or, rather, share a common pool of resources? Linguistic theorizing, empirical evidence from language acquisition and processing, and computational modeling have jointly painted a picture whereby lexico-semantic and syntactic processing are deeply inter-connected and perhaps not separable. In contrast, many current proposals of the neural architecture of language continue to endorse a view whereby certain brain regions selectively support syntactic/combinatorial processing, although the locus of such "syntactic hub", and its nature, vary across proposals. Here, we searched for selectivity for syntactic over lexico-semantic processing using a powerful individual-subjects fMRI approach across three sentence comprehension paradigms that have been used in prior work to argue for such selectivity: responses to lexico-semantic vs. morpho-syntactic violations (Experiment 1); recovery from neural suppression across pairs of sentences differing in only lexical items vs. only syntactic structure (Experiment 2); and same/different meaning judgments on such sentence pairs (Experiment 3). Across experiments, both lexico-semantic and syntactic conditions elicited robust responses throughout the left fronto-temporal language network. Critically, however, no regions were more strongly engaged by syntactic than lexico-semantic processing, although some regions showed the opposite pattern. Thus, contra many current proposals of the neural architecture of language, syntactic/combinatorial processing is not separable from lexico-semantic processing at the level of brain regions-or even voxel subsets-within the language network, in line with strong integration between these two processes that has been consistently observed in behavioral and computational language research. The results further suggest that the language network may be generally more strongly concerned with meaning than syntactic form, in line with the primary function of language-to share meanings across minds.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Evelina Fedorenko
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, MIT, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA; McGovern Institute for Brain Research, MIT, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA.
| | - Idan Asher Blank
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, MIT, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA; Department of Psychology, UCLA, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA
| | - Matthew Siegelman
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, MIT, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA; Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027, USA
| | - Zachary Mineroff
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, MIT, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA; Eberly Center for Teaching Excellence & Educational Innovation, CMU, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Fedorenko E, Blank IA, Siegelman M, Mineroff Z. Lack of selectivity for syntax relative to word meanings throughout the language network. Cognition 2020; 203:104348. [PMID: 32569894 PMCID: PMC7483589 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104348] [Citation(s) in RCA: 77] [Impact Index Per Article: 15.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/23/2018] [Revised: 05/14/2020] [Accepted: 05/31/2020] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
Abstract
To understand what you are reading now, your mind retrieves the meanings of words and constructions from a linguistic knowledge store (lexico-semantic processing) and identifies the relationships among them to construct a complex meaning (syntactic or combinatorial processing). Do these two sets of processes rely on distinct, specialized mechanisms or, rather, share a common pool of resources? Linguistic theorizing, empirical evidence from language acquisition and processing, and computational modeling have jointly painted a picture whereby lexico-semantic and syntactic processing are deeply inter-connected and perhaps not separable. In contrast, many current proposals of the neural architecture of language continue to endorse a view whereby certain brain regions selectively support syntactic/combinatorial processing, although the locus of such "syntactic hub", and its nature, vary across proposals. Here, we searched for selectivity for syntactic over lexico-semantic processing using a powerful individual-subjects fMRI approach across three sentence comprehension paradigms that have been used in prior work to argue for such selectivity: responses to lexico-semantic vs. morpho-syntactic violations (Experiment 1); recovery from neural suppression across pairs of sentences differing in only lexical items vs. only syntactic structure (Experiment 2); and same/different meaning judgments on such sentence pairs (Experiment 3). Across experiments, both lexico-semantic and syntactic conditions elicited robust responses throughout the left fronto-temporal language network. Critically, however, no regions were more strongly engaged by syntactic than lexico-semantic processing, although some regions showed the opposite pattern. Thus, contra many current proposals of the neural architecture of language, syntactic/combinatorial processing is not separable from lexico-semantic processing at the level of brain regions-or even voxel subsets-within the language network, in line with strong integration between these two processes that has been consistently observed in behavioral and computational language research. The results further suggest that the language network may be generally more strongly concerned with meaning than syntactic form, in line with the primary function of language-to share meanings across minds.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Evelina Fedorenko
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, MIT, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA; McGovern Institute for Brain Research, MIT, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA.
| | - Idan Asher Blank
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, MIT, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA; Department of Psychology, UCLA, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA
| | - Matthew Siegelman
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, MIT, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA; Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027, USA
| | - Zachary Mineroff
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, MIT, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA; Eberly Center for Teaching Excellence & Educational Innovation, CMU, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Martzoukou M, Nousia A, Marinis T. Narrative Abilities of Adults' With Down Syndrome as a Window to Their Morphosyntactic, Socio-Cognitive, and Prosodic Abilities. Front Psychol 2020; 11:2060. [PMID: 32982854 PMCID: PMC7479217 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02060] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/28/2020] [Accepted: 07/24/2020] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Down syndrome (DS) is the most common developmental disorder characterized by mild to moderate intellectual disability. Several studies have reported poor language and prosodic skills and contradictory results regarding individuals’ with DS socio-cognitive skills, whereas most of them have focused on children with DS. The present study attempts to explore adults’ with DS language, socio-cognitive and prosodic abilities via the use of story-retellings. Twenty adults with DS and two groups of TD children, one matched to their expressive vocabulary (TD-EVT) and the other matched to their non-verbal mental age (TD-RCPM), took part in the present study. Participants listened to a story while viewing a wordless picture PowerPoint presentation on a computer screen, and then, they were instructed to retell the story while viewing the pictures for a second time. Each participant listened to two stories, one with “lively” and one with “flat” prosody. Results revealed that adults’ with DS performance was comparable with the one presented by the TD-RCPM group, whereas the TD-EVT group performed significantly better in almost all variables. Individuals’ with DS re-narrations, however, contained significantly less complement clauses and internal state terms (related or not related to Theory of Mind–ToM) compared to the re-narrations of both control groups. In contrast, the group with DS performed similarly to both control groups in comprehension questions related to main characters’ internal state terms and significantly better compared to the TD-RCPM group in questions related to ToM. In terms of prosody, all three groups performed significantly better on story structure and comprehension questions when prosody was “lively” compared “flat” prosody. DS group’s re-narrations did not contain enough internal state terms, not due to their inability in recognizing them, but due to their poor morphosyntactic abilities, which did not allow them to find the proper means to express the main characters’ internal states. Prosody facilitated participants with DS in the comprehension and re-narration. This suggests that intervention programs based on prosody could support the language skills of adults with DS.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maria Martzoukou
- School of Health Sciences, University of Ioannina, Ioannina, Greece
| | - Anastasia Nousia
- School of Health Sciences, University of Ioannina, Ioannina, Greece
| | - Theodoros Marinis
- Department of Linguistics, University of Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany.,School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Reading, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Orizaba L, Gorman BK, Fiestas CE, Bingham GE, Terry NP. Examination of Narrative Language at Microstructural and Macrostructural Levels in Spanish-Speaking Preschoolers. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch 2020; 51:428-440. [PMID: 32097088 DOI: 10.1044/2019_lshss-19-00103] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Purpose The purpose of this study was to examine narrative language samples of Spanish-speaking preschoolers to analyze changes in microstructural and macrostructural skills in their first language (L1) from fall to spring, relationships between narrative and vocabulary skills in L1, and the extent to which fall skills predict spring performance. Method Participants included 40 Spanish-speaking children who were enrolled in community-based preschool programs. Narrative language retells from the fall and spring were examined, and microstructural and macrostructural components were analyzed using the Narrative Assessment Protocol-Spanish and the Narrative Scoring Scheme, respectively. Participants also completed an assessment of expressive vocabulary in Spanish. Results The results indicated different degrees of change in microstructural and macrostructural elements, change from fall to spring in some but not all elements measured, shifting patterns of association between L1 vocabulary and narrative skills, and variable prediction of spring scores. Conclusion Results from this study enhance professionals' understanding of Spanish-speaking preschoolers' narrative language skills in L1 and considerations for assessing and monitoring progress at different points in the academic year.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lorena Orizaba
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Elmhurst College, IL
| | - Brenda K Gorman
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Elmhurst College, IL
| | | | - Gary E Bingham
- Department of Early Childhood and Elementary Education, Georgia State University, Atlanta
| | | |
Collapse
|
35
|
McCauley SM. Towards an integrated, single-system account of language development as skill learning. JOURNAL OF COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2020; 83:105942. [PMID: 31575393 DOI: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2019.105942] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/12/2019] [Revised: 09/19/2019] [Accepted: 09/23/2019] [Indexed: 06/10/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Stewart M McCauley
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Wendell Johnson Speech and Hearing Center, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, 52242, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Eshghi M, Adatorwovor R, Preisser JS, Crais ER, Zajac DJ. Vocabulary Growth From 18 to 24 Months of Age in Children With and Without Repaired Cleft Palate. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2019; 62:3413-3430. [PMID: 31437085 PMCID: PMC6808344 DOI: 10.1044/2019_jslhr-l-18-0207] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/23/2018] [Revised: 10/22/2018] [Accepted: 05/27/2019] [Indexed: 05/10/2023]
Abstract
Purpose This study investigated vocabulary growth from 18 to 24 months of age in young children with repaired cleft palate (CP), children with otitis media, and typically developing (TD) children. In addition, the contributions of factors such as hearing level, middle ear status, size of consonant inventory, maternal education level, and gender to the development of expressive vocabulary were explored. Method Vocabulary size of 40 children with repaired CP, 29 children with otitis media, and 25 TD children was measured using the parent report on MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories: Words and Sentences (Fenson et al., 2007) at 18 and 24 months of age. All participants underwent sound field audiometry at 12 months of age and tympanometry at 18 months of age. A multiple linear regression with and without covariates was used to model vocabulary growth from 18 to 24 months of age across the 3 groups. Results Children with CP produced a significantly smaller number of words at 24 months of age and showed significantly slower rate of vocabulary growth from 18 to 24 months of age when compared to TD children (p < .05). Although middle ear status was found to predict vocabulary growth from 18 to 24 months of age across the 3 groups (p < .05), the confidence interval was large, suggesting the effect should be interpreted with caution. Conclusions Children with CP showed slower expressive vocabulary growth relative to their age-matched TD peers. Middle ear status may be associated with development of vocabulary skills for some children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marziye Eshghi
- Speech and Feeding Disorders Lab, MGH Institute of Health Professions, Boston, MA
| | - Reuben Adatorwovor
- Department of Biostatistics, Gillings School of Global Public Health,University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
| | - John S. Preisser
- Department of Biostatistics, Gillings School of Global Public Health,University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
| | - Elizabeth R. Crais
- Department of Dental Ecology, School of Dentistry, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
| | - David J. Zajac
- Division of Craniofacial and Surgical Sciences, Adams School of Dentistry, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
Siegelman M, Blank IA, Mineroff Z, Fedorenko E. An Attempt to Conceptually Replicate the Dissociation between Syntax and Semantics during Sentence Comprehension. Neuroscience 2019; 413:219-229. [PMID: 31200104 PMCID: PMC6661197 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2019.06.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/18/2018] [Revised: 05/31/2019] [Accepted: 06/03/2019] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
Is sentence structure processed by the same neural and cognitive resources that are recruited for processing word meanings, or do structure and meaning rely on distinct resources? Linguistic theorizing and much behavioral evidence suggest tight integration between lexico-semantic and syntactic representations and processing. However, most current proposals of the neural architecture of language continue to postulate a distinction between the two. One of the earlier and most cited pieces of neuroimaging evidence in favor of this dissociation comes from a paper by Dapretto and Bookheimer (1999). Using a sentence-meaning judgment task, Dapretto & Bookheimer observed two distinct peaks within the left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG): one was more active during a lexico-semantic manipulation, and the other during a syntactic manipulation. Although the paper is highly cited, no attempt has been made, to our knowledge, to replicate the original finding. We report an fMRI study that attempts to do so. Using a combination of whole-brain, group-level ROI, and participant-specific functional ROI approaches, we fail to replicate the original dissociation. In particular, whereas parts of LIFG respond reliably more strongly during lexico-semantic than syntactic processing, no part of LIFG (including in the region defined around the peak reported by Dapretto & Bookheimer) shows the opposite pattern. We speculate that the original result was a false positive, possibly driven by a small subset of participants or items that biased a fixed-effects analysis with low power.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matthew Siegelman
- MIT, Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences; Columbia University, Department of Psychology
| | - Idan A Blank
- MIT, Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences; UCLA, Department of Psychology
| | | | - Evelina Fedorenko
- MIT, Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences; MIT, McGovern Institute for Brain Research; MGH, Department of Psychiatry.
| |
Collapse
|
38
|
Monaghan P, Schoetensack C, Rebuschat P. A Single Paradigm for Implicit and Statistical Learning. Top Cogn Sci 2019; 11:536-554. [PMID: 31338980 PMCID: PMC6852402 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12439] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/10/2018] [Revised: 05/04/2019] [Accepted: 05/27/2019] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Implicit learning generally refers to the acquisition of structures that, like knowledge of natural language grammar, are not available to awareness. In contrast, statistical learning has frequently been related to learning language structures that are explicitly available, such as vocabulary. In this paper, we report an experimental paradigm that enables testing of both classic implicit and statistical learning in language. The paradigm employs an artificial language comprising sentences that accompany visual scenes that they represent, thus combining artificial grammar learning with cross-situational statistical learning of vocabulary. We show that this methodology enables a comparison between acquisition of grammar and vocabulary, and the influences on their learning. We show that both grammar and vocabulary are promoted by explicit information about the language structure, that awareness of structure affects acquisition during learning, and awareness precedes learning, but is not distinctive at the endpoint of learning. The two traditions of learning-implicit and statistical-can be conjoined in a single paradigm to explore both the phenomenological and learning consequences of statistical structural knowledge.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Padraic Monaghan
- Department of EnglishUniversity of Amsterdam
- Department of PsychologyLancaster University
| | | | - Patrick Rebuschat
- Department of Linguistics and English LanguageLancaster University
- LEAD Graduate School and Research NetworkUniversity of Tübingen
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
Davidson D, Vanegas SB, Hilvert E, Rainey VR, Misiunaite I. Examination of monolingual (English) and bilingual (English/Spanish; English/Urdu) children's syntactic awareness. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2019; 46:682-706. [PMID: 30868992 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000919000059] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/09/2023]
Abstract
In this study, monolingual (English) and bilingual (English/Spanish, English/Urdu) five- and six-year-old children completed a grammaticality judgment test in order to assess their awareness of the grammaticality of two types of syntactic constructions in English: word order and gender representation. All children were better at detecting grammatically correct and incorrect word order constructions than gender constructions, regardless of language group. In fact, bilingualism per se did not impact the results as much as receptive vocabulary range. For example, children with the highest receptive vocabulary scores were more accurate in detecting incorrect word order constructions (i.e., word order violations, semantic anomalies) and incorrect gender agreement than children in the lower receptive vocabulary ranges. However, no differences were found between the ranges for ambiguous gender constructions. These results highlight the importance of receptive vocabulary ability on syntactic awareness performance, regardless of language group.
Collapse
|
40
|
Cadime I, Moreira CS, Santos AL, Silva C, Ribeiro I, Viana FL. The development of vocabulary and grammar: a longitudinal study of European Portuguese-speaking toddlers. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2019; 46:653-681. [PMID: 30868990 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000919000060] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/09/2023]
Abstract
The goals of this study were to analyze the growth and stability of vocabulary, mean length of the three longest utterances (MLLUw), and sentence complexity in European Portuguese-speaking children aged 1;4-2;6, to explore differences in growth as a function of personal and family-related variables, and to investigate the inter-relationships among the three language dimensions. Fifty-one European Portuguese-speaking toddlers were longitudinally assessed at 1;4, 1;9, 2;1, and 2;6, through parent reports. Exponential growth models best described acquisition patterns during this period, but the vocabulary growth accelerated across the full age-range, whereas the growth of grammar dimensions accelerated mainly after 1;9. High variability was observed in the scores, but the toddlers' relative positions were mostly stable over time. Gender approached significance as a predictor of vocabulary growth. Maternal educational level did not predict the growth of any of the three language dimensions. Both vocabulary and MLLUw predicted sentence complexity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Irene Cadime
- Research Centre on Child Studies,University of Minho,Portugal
| | - Célia S Moreira
- Psychology Research Center (CIPsi), University of Minho,Portugal
| | - Ana Lúcia Santos
- Centre of Linguistics of the University of Lisbon (CLUL),Portugal
| | - Carla Silva
- Research Centre on Child Studies,University of Minho,Portugal
| | - Iolanda Ribeiro
- Psychology Research Center (CIPsi), University of Minho,Portugal
| | | |
Collapse
|
41
|
Chilosi AM, Pfanner L, Pecini C, Salvadorini R, Casalini C, Brizzolara D, Cipriani P. Which linguistic measures distinguish transient from persistent language problems in Late Talkers from 2 to 4 years? A study on Italian speaking children. RESEARCH IN DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 2019; 89:59-68. [PMID: 30947105 DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2019.03.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/27/2018] [Revised: 03/11/2019] [Accepted: 03/12/2019] [Indexed: 06/09/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND In spite of the large literature on Late Talkers (LTs) it's still unclear which factors predict outcome in children younger than 3 years old. AIMS To identify the early language characteristics of LTs whose outcome was either a transient delay or a Developmental Language Disorder (DLD). METHODS AND PROCEDURES 50 LTs were assessed both by indirect and direct measures of expressive and receptive language at three time points between 2 and 4 years of age. OUTCOMES AND RESULTS At the first evaluation, all LTs had an expressive language delay; 61% also had delayed early syntactic comprehension. Three different linguistic outcomes emerged: children who caught up with their peers ("Late Bloomers") at age 3; children with slow language recovery ("Slow Learners") at age 4 and children at risk of DLD. The linguistic measures that differentiated the groups changed with age. By 28 months, impaired syntactic comprehension differentiated children at risk of DLD at 4 years of age, from the other two groups. By 36 months, the discrepancy between vocabulary size and age was larger in children with persistent language difficulties compared to both "Late Bloomers" and "Slow Learners". Expressive grammar differentiated the groups significantly by age 3 with difficulties in this domain still persisting in children with DLD at age 4. CONCLUSIONS An early syntactic comprehension delay was a predictive index of DLD in LTs, suggesting the importance of evaluating this language component when assessing LT toddlers. IMPLICATIONS LTs with receptive-expressive language delay around 24-30 months could benefit from an early language intervention.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A M Chilosi
- Department of Developmental Neuroscience IRCCS "Stella Maris Foundation" Scientific Institute, Pisa, Italy.
| | - L Pfanner
- Department of Developmental Neuroscience IRCCS "Stella Maris Foundation" Scientific Institute, Pisa, Italy
| | - C Pecini
- Department of Education, Languages, Intercultures, Literatures and Psychology, University of Florence, Italy
| | - R Salvadorini
- Department of Developmental Neuroscience IRCCS "Stella Maris Foundation" Scientific Institute, Pisa, Italy
| | - C Casalini
- Department of Developmental Neuroscience IRCCS "Stella Maris Foundation" Scientific Institute, Pisa, Italy
| | - D Brizzolara
- Department of Developmental Neuroscience IRCCS "Stella Maris Foundation" Scientific Institute, Pisa, Italy
| | - P Cipriani
- Department of Developmental Neuroscience IRCCS "Stella Maris Foundation" Scientific Institute, Pisa, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
42
|
Brito NH, Fifer WP, Amso D, Barr R, Bell MA, Calkins S, Flynn A, Montgomery-Downs HE, Oakes LM, Richards JE, Samuelson LM, Colombo J. Beyond the Bayley: Neurocognitive Assessments of Development During Infancy and Toddlerhood. Dev Neuropsychol 2019; 44:220-247. [PMID: 30616391 PMCID: PMC6399032 DOI: 10.1080/87565641.2018.1564310] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/22/2018] [Revised: 12/15/2018] [Accepted: 12/17/2018] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
The use of global, standardized instruments is conventional among clinicians and researchers interested in assessing neurocognitive development. Exclusively relying on these tests for evaluating effects may underestimate or miss specific effects on early cognition. The goal of this review is to identify alternative measures for possible inclusion in future clinical trials and interventions evaluating early neurocognitive development. The domains included for consideration are attention, memory, executive function, language, and socioemotional development. Although domain-based tests are limited, as psychometric properties have not yet been well-established, this review includes tasks and paradigms that have been reliably used across various developmental psychology laboratories.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Natalie H Brito
- a Department of Applied Psychology , New York University , New York , NY , USA
| | - William P Fifer
- b Division of Developmental Neuroscience , New York State Psychiatric Institute , New York , NY , USA
| | - Dima Amso
- c Department of Cognitive, Linguistic, and Psychological Sciences , Brown University , Providence , RI , USA
| | - Rachel Barr
- d Department of Psychology , Georgetown University , Washington , DC , USA
| | - Martha Ann Bell
- e Department of Psychology , Virginia Tech , Blacksburg , VA , USA
| | - Susan Calkins
- f Department of Human Development and Family Studies , University of North Carolina at Greensboro , Greensboro , NC , USA
| | - Albert Flynn
- g School of Food and Nutritional Sciences , University College Cork , Cork , Ireland
| | | | - Lisa M Oakes
- i Department of Psychology , University of California , Davis , CA , USA
| | - John E Richards
- j Department of Psychology , University of South Carolina , Columbia , SC , USA
| | | | - John Colombo
- l Department of Psychology , University of Kansas , Lawrence , KS , USA
| |
Collapse
|
43
|
Smolík F, Bláhová V. Czech 23-month-olds use gender agreement to anticipate upcoming nouns. J Exp Child Psychol 2018; 178:251-265. [PMID: 30415147 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.10.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/01/2017] [Revised: 10/08/2018] [Accepted: 10/09/2018] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
Children acquiring Dutch, French, and Spanish can use gender of articles to facilitate the processing of upcoming nouns. The current study examined whether a similar effect can be found for bound gender-marking agreement morphemes in Czech, a language without obligatory articles. The experiment was designed so that the anticipatory effects of gender-marking morphemes before the head noun onset could be observed. In a preferential looking experiment, 33 children (aged 21-24 months) were shown picture pairs that could be labeled with masculine and feminine nouns. They heard a phrase comprising a demonstrative, an adjective, and a noun, where the first two elements were inflected for gender. The inflections were either correct (matching the noun gender) or incorrect (mismatched). Children were also given offline receptive grammar and vocabulary tasks. The group of children as a whole did not show significant differences in looking behavior between the experimental conditions. When split by the grammar task, the high-scoring children showed significant differences between looks toward the target noun in the matched and mismatched conditions even before the onset of the target noun. No significant difference was observed in the low-scoring group and in the groups split by vocabulary. Results suggest that knowledge of the gender system is just emerging before the second birthday and that more advanced children can use gender information encoded in bound morphemes to actively anticipate the upcoming nouns.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Filip Smolík
- Institute of Psychology of the Czech Academy of Sciences, 110 00 Prague 1, Czech Republic.
| | - Veronika Bláhová
- Institute of Psychology of the Czech Academy of Sciences, 110 00 Prague 1, Czech Republic
| |
Collapse
|
44
|
Mayberry RI, Kluender R. Rethinking the critical period for language: New insights into an old question from American Sign Language. BILINGUALISM (CAMBRIDGE, ENGLAND) 2018; 21:886-905. [PMID: 30643489 PMCID: PMC6329394 DOI: 10.1017/s1366728917000724] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/01/2023]
Abstract
The hypothesis that children surpass adults in long-term second-language proficiency is accepted as evidence for a critical period for language. However, the scope and nature of a critical period for language has been the subject of considerable debate. The controversy centers on whether the age-related decline in ultimate second-language proficiency is evidence for a critical period or something else. Here we argue that age-onset effects for first vs. second language outcome are largely different. We show this by examining psycholinguistic studies of ultimate attainment in L2 vs. L1 learners, longitudinal studies of adolescent L1 acquisition, and neurolinguistic studies of late L2 and L1 learners. This research indicates that L1 acquisition arises from post-natal brain development interacting with environmental linguistic experience. By contrast, L2 learning after early childhood is scaffolded by prior childhood L1 acquisition, both linguistically and neurally, making it a less clear test of the critical period for language.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Robert Kluender
- Department of Linguistics, University of California San Diego
| |
Collapse
|
45
|
Obeid R, Brooks PJ. Associations Between Manual Dexterity and Language Ability in School-Age Children. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch 2018; 49:982-994. [DOI: 10.1044/2018_lshss-17-0124] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/14/2017] [Accepted: 06/03/2018] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Purpose
We aimed to determine whether individual differences in manual dexterity are associated with specific language skills (nonword repetition, receptive vocabulary, and receptive grammar) after controlling for nonverbal abilities (visual–spatial working memory and intelligence).
Method
We assessed manual dexterity using the pegboard task and examined relationships with verbal and nonverbal abilities in a diverse community sample of children (
N
= 63, mean age = 8;2 [year;months], range: 6;0–10;8) varying in language ability (Comprehensive Evaluation of Language Fundamentals–Fourth Edition core language score
M
= 105, range: 62–126; Semel, Wiig, & Secord, 2003).
Results
Correlational analyses indicated significant relationships between manual dexterity and performance on tests of nonword repetition, receptive vocabulary, receptive grammar, and nonverbal intelligence, after controlling for multiple comparisons. In regression analyses, manual dexterity remained a significant predictor of nonword repetition after controlling for nonverbal abilities and age. In contrast, manual dexterity was no longer significant in predicting receptive vocabulary or grammar when nonverbal intelligence was included as a factor in the model.
Conclusions
These findings build on prior work implicating poor fine motor control in child language disorders by identifying a robust relationship between manual dexterity and nonword repetition. Relationships between manual dexterity and receptive language abilities appear to be indirect and mediated by nonword repetition. For clinicians, the results underscore the importance of screening children with poor fine motor control for concomitant language impairments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rita Obeid
- Psychology Department, The Graduate Center, City University of New York, NY
- The College of Staten Island and The Graduate Center, City University of New York, NY
| | - Patricia J. Brooks
- Psychology Department, The Graduate Center, City University of New York, NY
- The College of Staten Island and The Graduate Center, City University of New York, NY
| |
Collapse
|
46
|
Baron A, Bedore LM, Peña ED, Lovgren-Uribe SD, López AA, Villagran E. Production of Spanish Grammatical Forms in U.S. Bilingual Children. AMERICAN JOURNAL OF SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY 2018; 27:975-987. [PMID: 29801102 PMCID: PMC6195028 DOI: 10.1044/2018_ajslp-17-0074] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/23/2017] [Revised: 09/13/2017] [Accepted: 01/31/2018] [Indexed: 05/26/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE The purpose of this analysis was to understand how grammatical morpheme production in Spanish for typically developing Spanish-English bilingual children relates to mean length of utterance in words (MLUw) and the extent to which different bilingual profiles influence order of grammatical morpheme acquisition. METHOD Participants included 228 Spanish-English bilingual children ages 4;0-7;6 (years;months). Grammatical morpheme accuracy was evaluated using an experimental version of the Bilingual English-Spanish Assessment (Peña, Gutiérrez-Clellen, Iglesias, Goldstein, & Bedore, 2014). MLUw data were calculated from children's narrative samples. Production accuracy of plural nouns, singular and plural definite articles, preterite tense, imperfect aspect, direct object clitics, prepositions, subjunctive, and conjunctions was calculated and analyzed as a function of MLUw in Spanish. Level of accuracy on these forms was compared for Spanish-dominant and English-dominant groups. RESULTS Accuracy was significantly associated with MLUw. The relative difficulty of Spanish grammatical morphemes is highly similar across different bilingual profiles. CONCLUSIONS There are common elements of Spanish that are easy (imperfect, plural nouns, singular articles, conjunctions), medium (plural articles, preterite), or hard (prepositions, direct object clitics, subjunctive), regardless of whether a child is a Spanish-dominant or English-dominant bilingual.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alisa Baron
- Department of Communication Sciences & Disorders, The University of Texas at Austin
| | - Lisa M. Bedore
- Department of Communication Sciences & Disorders, The University of Texas at Austin
| | | | | | - Amanda A. López
- Department of Communication Sciences & Disorders, The University of Texas at Austin
| | - Elizabeth Villagran
- Department of Communication Sciences & Disorders, The University of Texas at Austin
| |
Collapse
|
47
|
Jones SD, Brandt S. Auditory Lexical Decisions in Developmental Language Disorder: A Meta-Analysis of Behavioral Studies. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2018; 61:1766-1783. [PMID: 29984371 DOI: 10.1044/2018_jslhr-l-17-0447] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/04/2017] [Accepted: 03/23/2018] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE Despite the apparent primacy of syntactic deficits, children with developmental language disorder (DLD) often also evidence lexical impairments. In particular, it has been argued that this population have difficulty forming lexical representations that are detailed enough to support effective spoken word processing. In order to better understand this deficit, a meta-analysis of studies testing children with DLD in the auditory lexical decision task was conducted. The objective was to provide summary effect size estimates for accuracy and response time measures for comparisons to age- and language-matched control groups. METHOD Two thousand three hundred seventy-two records were initially identified through electronic searches and expert consultation, with this cohort reduced to 9 through duplicate removal and the application of eligibility and quality criteria. The final study cohort included 499 children aged 3;8-11;4 (years;months). RESULTS Multivariate analysis suggests that children with DLD were significantly less accurate in the auditory lexical decision task than age-matched controls. For the response time estimate, however, confidence intervals for the same group comparison crossed 0, suggesting no reliable difference between groups. Confidence intervals also crossed 0 for language-matched control estimates for both accuracy and response time, suggesting no reliable difference between groups on either measure. CONCLUSION Results broadly support the hypothesis that children with DLD have difficulty in forming detailed lexical representations relative to age- though not language-matched peers. However, further work is required to determine the performance profiles of potential subgroups and the impact of manipulating different lexical characteristics, such as the position and degree of nonword error, phonotactic probability, and semantic network size.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Samuel David Jones
- Department of Linguistics and English Language, Lancaster University, United Kingdom
| | - Silke Brandt
- Department of Linguistics and English Language, Lancaster University, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
48
|
DeNigris D, Brooks PJ. The Role of Language in Temporal Cognition in 6- to 10-Year-Old Children. JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 2018. [DOI: 10.1080/15248372.2018.1483372] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/14/2022]
|
49
|
Perry LK, Prince EB, Valtierra AM, Rivero-Fernandez C, Ullery MA, Katz LF, Laursen B, Messinger DS. A year in words: The dynamics and consequences of language experiences in an intervention classroom. PLoS One 2018; 13:e0199893. [PMID: 29979740 PMCID: PMC6034821 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0199893] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/01/2017] [Accepted: 06/17/2018] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Children from low SES backgrounds hear, on average, fewer words at home than those from high SES backgrounds. This word gap is associated with widening achievement differences in children’s language abilities and school readiness. However relatively little is known about adult and child speech in childcare settings, in which approximately 30% of American children are enrolled. We examined the influence of teacher and peer language input on children’s in-class language use and language development in an intervention classroom for low-SES, high-risk 2- to 3-year-olds. Over the course of a year, day-long recordings of the classroom were collected weekly with LENA recorders. Using LENA software algorithms, we found that language input from peers was positively related to children’s in-class language use, both in-the-moment and over the course of each day, as were the number of conversational turns in which children and teachers engaged Both peer input and conversational turns with teachers were also positively related to children’s language development rates, as indexed by increases in vocabulary size. Together these results indicate the importance of child-specific rates of classroom language input in the language development of high-risk, preschoolers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lynn K. Perry
- Department of Psychology, University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL, United States of America
- * E-mail:
| | - Emily B. Prince
- Department of Psychology, University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL, United States of America
| | - Adriana M. Valtierra
- Department of Psychology, University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL, United States of America
| | | | - Mary Anne Ullery
- Linda Ray Intervention Center, University of Miami, Miami, FL, United States of America
| | - Lynne F. Katz
- Department of Psychology, University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL, United States of America
- Linda Ray Intervention Center, University of Miami, Miami, FL, United States of America
| | - Brett Laursen
- Department of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University, Fort Lauderdale, FL, United States of America
| | - Daniel S. Messinger
- Department of Psychology, University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL, United States of America
- Department of Pediatrics, University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL, United States of America
- Department of Electrical & Computer Engineering, University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL, United States of America
- Department of Music Engineering, University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
50
|
Simon-Cereijido G, Méndez LI. Using Language-Specific and Bilingual Measures to Explore Lexical-Grammatical Links in Young Latino Dual-Language Learners. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch 2018; 49:537-550. [PMID: 29625426 DOI: 10.1044/2018_lshss-17-0058] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/09/2017] [Accepted: 01/08/2018] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Purpose This study examined the nature of the relation between language-specific vocabulary and conceptual lexical-semantic skills with grammatical abilities within and across languages in preschool Latino dual language learners (DLLs). Method Sixty-one typically developing, Spanish-English speaking DLLs from preschools serving low-income families participated in the study. Lexical, semantic, and grammar skills were assessed toward the end of the fall in both Spanish and English using normative and researcher-developed assessment instruments. Hierarchical linear regressions using baseline cross-sectional data were completed to determine the association of language-specific vocabulary and bilingual lexical and semantic abilities to grammatical skills measured by sentence repetition tasks in Spanish and English both within and across languages. Results Results from the study revealed that a considerable percentage of the variance in the grammatical ability of these Latino DLL preschoolers in both Spanish and English was explained by lexical variables in the same language (54% in English and 16% in Spanish). In the strong language (Spanish), bilingual semantic skills also played a role, explaining an additional 8% of the variance. Conceptual vocabulary was a significant predictor of English grammar in the model that excluded the language-specific vocabulary measures. Conclusions These findings suggest that grammatical skills in the Latino preschoolers examined in the study are strongly related to language-specific measures of vocabulary. In contrast, no evidence supporting the relation between vocabulary and grammar skills across languages was observed. Findings from this study provide insight into the impact of bilingual lexical-semantic knowledge on the grammatical skills of dual-language preschool children developing language abilities in their 2 languages. Clinical implications are also discussed.
Collapse
|