1
|
Biotti F, Barker M, Carr L, Pickard H, Brewer R, Murphy J. The effects of the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic on self-reported interoception and mental health. PLoS One 2025; 20:e0314272. [PMID: 39854384 PMCID: PMC11759990 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0314272] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/14/2024] [Accepted: 11/08/2024] [Indexed: 01/26/2025] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Atypical interoception has been observed across multiple mental health conditions, including anxiety disorders and depression. Evidence suggests that not only pathological anxiety, but also heightened levels of state anxiety and stress are associated with interoceptive functioning. This study aimed to investigate the effects of the recent Coronavirus SARS-CoV-2 pandemic on self-reported interoception and mental health, and their relationship. METHODS Self-report measures of interoceptive attention and accuracy, anxiety, stress and depression taken during the pandemic (at three time points) were compared to the same measures taken from comparable samples prior to the pandemic. In the sample collected during the pandemic, the relationship between interoceptive and mental health measures and focus on COVID-19-related news and information, propensity to take objective measures of COVID-19 symptoms, and subjective beliefs concerning COVID-19 symptoms was assessed. Finally, a cross-lagged panel model (CLPM) was used to test directional relationships between self-reported interoceptive and mental health measures across three time points. RESULTS Higher self-reported anxiety was associated with a) increased self-reported attention to bodily signals, b) increased focus on COVID-19-related news and information, c) propensity to take objective measures of COVID-19 symptoms, and d) reduced self-reported interoceptive accuracy for bodily signals participants believed were associated with COVID-19. The CLPM revealed a mutual and comparable directional effect from T1 to T2 between interoceptive attention and measures of mental health. CONCLUSIONS Implications of these findings are discussed in the light of existing models and newly proposed accounts of the relationship between interoception and mental health.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Federica Biotti
- Barts and the London Faculty of Medicine and Dentistry, Centre for Psychiatry and Mental Health, Wolfson Institute of Population Health, Queen Mary, University of London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Melissa Barker
- Department of Psychology Royal Holloway, University of London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Lara Carr
- Department of Psychology Royal Holloway, University of London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Hannah Pickard
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck, University of London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Rebecca Brewer
- Department of Psychology Royal Holloway, University of London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Jennifer Murphy
- Department of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Siegel P, Peterson BS. Advancing the treatment of anxiety disorders in transition-age youth: a review of the therapeutic effects of unconscious exposure. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2025; 66:98-121. [PMID: 39128857 PMCID: PMC11652263 DOI: 10.1111/jcpp.14037] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 04/04/2024] [Indexed: 08/13/2024]
Abstract
BACKGROUND The real-world effectiveness of exposure-based therapies for youth depends on the willingness and ability of young people to tolerate confronting their fears, which can be experienced as highly aversive and create problems with treatment engagement and acceptance. Recently, neuroscientific research on the nonconscious basis of fear has been translated into novel exposure interventions that bypass conscious processing of feared stimuli and that thus do not cause phobic youth to experience distress. We present a review of these unconscious exposure interventions. METHODS A PRISMA-based search yielded 20 controlled experiments based on three paradigms that tested if fear-related responses could be reduced without conscious awareness in highly phobic, transition-age youth: 14 randomized controlled trials (RCTs), 5 fMRI studies (1 was also an RCT), 4 psychophysiological studies (3 were also RCTs), and 1 ERP study. We conducted meta-analyses of outcomes where feasible. RESULTS Unconscious exposure interventions significantly (1) reduced avoidance behavior (range of Cohen's d = 0.51-0.95) and self-reported fear (d = 0.45-1.25) during in vivo exposure to the feared situation; (2) reduced neurobiological indicators of fear (d = 0.54-0.62) and concomitant physiological arousal (d = 0.55-0.64); (3) activated neural systems supporting fear regulation more strongly than visible exposure to the same stimuli (d = 1.2-1.5); (4) activated regions supporting fear regulation that mediated the reduction of avoidance behavior (d = 0.70); (5) evoked ERPs suggesting encoding of extinction memories (d = 2.13); and (6) had these effects without inducing autonomic arousal or subjective fear. CONCLUSIONS Unconscious exposure interventions significantly reduce a variety of symptomatic behaviors with mostly moderate effect sizes in transition-age youth with specific phobias. fMRI and physiological findings establish a neurophysiological basis for this efficacy, and suggest it occurs through extinction learning. Unconscious exposure was well tolerated, entirely unassociated with drop out, and is highly scalable for clinical practice. However, a number of limitations must be addressed to assess potential clinical impacts, including combining unconscious exposure with exposure therapy to boost treatment acceptance and efficacy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paul Siegel
- Department of Psychology, Purchase College, State University of New York, Purchase, NY, USA
- Department of Psychiatry, Keck School of Medicine, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
| | - Bradley S. Peterson
- Department of Psychiatry, Keck School of Medicine, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
- Department of Psychiatry, Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, Institute for the Developing Mind, Los Angeles, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Mei Y, Becker B, Leppänen PHT, Lei Y. Exploring the 'black box' of anxiety: An ERP study of non-consciously triggered fear generalization. Behav Res Ther 2024; 178:104552. [PMID: 38718631 DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2024.104552] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/19/2022] [Revised: 04/24/2024] [Accepted: 04/25/2024] [Indexed: 06/11/2024]
Abstract
Individuals with anxiety disorders frequently display heightened fear responses, even in situations where there is no imminent danger. We hypothesize that these irrational fear responses are related to automatic processing of fear generalization. The initial automatic detection of stimuli often operates at a non-conscious level. However, whether fear generalization can occur when the cues are not perceived consciously remains unclear. The current study investigated the neurocognitive mechanisms underlying fear conditioning and its non-conscious and conscious generalization using a backward masking paradigm, combined with analysis of event-related potentials from electroencephalographic recordings. Behaviorally, participants showed heightened shock expectancy in response to non-conscious perceived generalization stimuli compared to those perceived consciously. Nonetheless, participants could not consciously distinguish between danger and safe cues in non-conscious trials. Physiologically, danger cues evoked larger frontal N1 amplitudes than safety cues in non-conscious trials, suggesting enhanced attention vigilance towards danger cues in the early sensory processing stage. Meanwhile, when fear generalization was conscious, it was accompanied by a larger P2 amplitude, indicating attention orientation or stimulus evaluation. In addition, fear conditioning was associated with sustained discrimination on P2, P3, and LPP. These findings collectively suggest that non-conscious fear generalization occurs at the neural level, yet additional control conditions are required to confirm this phenomenon on the US expectancy. Thus, non-consciously fear generalization may represent a mechanism that could trigger automatic irrational fear, highlighting the need for further research to explore therapeutic targets in anxiety disorders.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ying Mei
- Institute for Brain and Psychological Sciences, Sichuan Normal University, 610066, China; Centre of Excellence for Learning Dynamics and Intervention Research, Faculty of Education and Psychology, University of Jyväskylä, Jyväskylä, FI-40014, Finland
| | - Benjamin Becker
- State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China; Department of Psychology, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China
| | - Paavo H T Leppänen
- Centre of Excellence for Learning Dynamics and Intervention Research, Faculty of Education and Psychology, University of Jyväskylä, Jyväskylä, FI-40014, Finland
| | - Yi Lei
- Institute for Brain and Psychological Sciences, Sichuan Normal University, 610066, China.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Chen Y, Chen S, Sun Z, Zhang X, Yuan X, Wang L, Jiang Y. Rapid Unconscious Acquisition of Conditioned Fear with Low-Spatial-Frequency but Emotionally Neutral Stimuli. RESEARCH (WASHINGTON, D.C.) 2023; 6:0181. [PMID: 37383220 PMCID: PMC10298222 DOI: 10.34133/research.0181] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/26/2023] [Accepted: 06/02/2023] [Indexed: 06/30/2023]
Abstract
It has long been proposed that emotionally "prepared" (i.e., fear-related) stimuli are privileged in the unconscious acquisition of conditioned fear. However, as fear processing is suggested to highly depend on the coarse, low-spatial-frequency (LSF) components of the fear-related stimuli, it is plausible that LSF may play a unique role in the unconscious fear conditioning even with emotionally neutral stimuli. Here, we provided empirical evidence that, following classical fear conditioning, an invisible, emotionally neutral conditioned stimulus (CS+) with LSF, but not with high spatial frequency (HSF), can rapidly elicit stronger skin conductance responses (SCRs) and larger pupil diameters than its CS- counterpart. In comparison, consciously perceived emotionally neutral CS+ with LSF and HSF elicited comparable SCRs. Taken together, these results support that the unconscious fear conditioning does not necessarily entail emotionally prepared stimuli but prioritizes LSF information processing and highlight the crucial distinctions between the unconscious and the conscious fear learning. These findings not only coincide with the postulation that a rapid, spatial-frequency-dependent subcortical route is engaged in unconscious fear processing but also suggest the existence of multiple routes for conscious fear processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yujie Chen
- State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China
- Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China
- Chinese Institute for Brain Research, Beijing 102206, China
| | - Si Chen
- Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China
- CAS Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China
| | - Zhongju Sun
- State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China
- Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China
- Chinese Institute for Brain Research, Beijing 102206, China
| | - Xilei Zhang
- Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China
- CAS Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China
| | - Xiangyong Yuan
- State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China
- Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China
- Chinese Institute for Brain Research, Beijing 102206, China
| | - Liang Wang
- Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China
- CAS Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China
| | - Yi Jiang
- State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China
- Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China
- Chinese Institute for Brain Research, Beijing 102206, China
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Gunderson C, ten Brinke L, Sokol-Hessner P. When the body knows: Interoceptive accuracy enhances physiological but not explicit differentiation between liars and truth-tellers. PERSONALITY AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2023. [DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2022.112039] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/24/2022]
|
6
|
Critchley HD, Sherrill SP, Ewing DL, van Praag CG, Habash-Bailey H, Quadt L, Eccles JA, Meeten F, Jones AM, Garfinkel SN. Cardiac interoception in patients accessing secondary mental health services: A transdiagnostic study. Auton Neurosci 2023; 245:103072. [PMID: 36709619 DOI: 10.1016/j.autneu.2023.103072] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/05/2022] [Revised: 12/19/2022] [Accepted: 01/08/2023] [Indexed: 01/15/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Abnormalities in the regulation of physiological arousal and interoceptive processing are implicated in the expression and maintenance of specific psychiatric conditions and symptoms. We undertook a cross-sectional characterisation of patients accessing secondary mental health services, recording measures relating to cardiac physiology and interoception, to understand how physiological state and interoceptive ability relate transdiagnostically to affective symptoms. METHODS Participants were patients (n = 258) and a non-clinical comparison group (n = 67). Clinical diagnoses spanned affective disorders, complex personality presentations and psychoses. We first tested for differences between patient and non-clinical participants in terms of cardiac physiology and interoceptive ability, considering interoceptive tasks and a self-report measure. We then tested for correlations between cardiac and interoceptive measures and affective symptoms. Lastly, we explored group differences across recorded clinical diagnoses. RESULTS Patients exhibited lower performance accuracy and confidence in heartbeat discrimination and lower heartbeat tracking confidence relative to comparisons. In patients, greater anxiety and depression predicted greater self-reported interoceptive sensibility and a greater mismatch between performance accuracy and sensibility. This effect was not observed in comparison participants. Significant differences between patient groups were observed for heart rate variability (HRV) although post hoc differences were not significant after correction for multiple comparisons. Finally, accuracy in heartbeat tracking was significantly lower in schizophrenia compared to other diagnostic groups. CONCLUSIONS The multilevel characterisation presented here identified certain physiological and interoceptive differences associated with psychiatric symptoms and diagnoses. The clinical stratification and therapeutic targeting of interoceptive mechanisms is therefore of potential value in treating certain psychiatric conditions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hugo D Critchley
- Department of Neuroscience, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland; Sussex Partnership NHS Foundation Trust, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland; Sussex Neuroscience, University of Sussex, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland.
| | - Samantha P Sherrill
- Department of Neuroscience, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland; Sussex Neuroscience, University of Sussex, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland
| | - Donna L Ewing
- Department of Neuroscience, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland; School of Humanities and Social Science, University of Brighton, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland
| | - Cassandra Gould van Praag
- Department of Neuroscience, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland; Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland
| | - Haniah Habash-Bailey
- Department of Neuroscience, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland; School of Psychology, University of Sussex, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland
| | - Lisa Quadt
- Department of Neuroscience, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland; Sussex Neuroscience, University of Sussex, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland
| | - Jessica A Eccles
- Department of Neuroscience, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland; Sussex Partnership NHS Foundation Trust, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland; Sussex Neuroscience, University of Sussex, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland
| | - Fran Meeten
- Department of Neuroscience, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland; School of Psychology, University of Sussex, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland
| | - Anna-Marie Jones
- Sussex Partnership NHS Foundation Trust, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland
| | - Sarah N Garfinkel
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Pollatos O, Mönkemöller K, Groppe K, Elsner B. Interoceptive accuracy is associated with benefits in decision making in children. Front Psychol 2023; 13:1070037. [PMID: 36743603 PMCID: PMC9893641 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1070037] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/14/2022] [Accepted: 12/22/2022] [Indexed: 01/20/2023] Open
Abstract
Introduction Decision making results not only from logical analyses, but seems to be further guided by the ability to perceive somatic information (interoceptive accuracy). Relations between interoceptive accuracy and decision making have been exclusively studied in adults and with regard to complex, uncertain situations (as measured by the Iowa Gambling Task, IGT). Methods In the present study, 1454 children (6-11 years) were examined at two time points (approximately 1 year apart) using an IGT as well as a delay-of-gratification task for sweets-items and toy-items. Interoceptive accuracy was measured using a child-adapted version of the Heartbeat Perception Task. Results The present results revealed that children with higher, as compared to lower, interoceptive accuracy showed more advantageous choices in the IGT and delayed more sweets-items, but not toy-items, in a delay-of-gratification task at time point 2 but not at time point 1. However, no longitudinal relation between interoceptive accuracy and decision making 1 year later could be shown. Discussion Results indicate that interoceptive accuracy relates to decision-making abilities in situations of varying complexity already in middle childhood, and that this link might consolidate across the examined 1-year period. Furthermore, the association of interoceptive accuracy and the delay of sweets-items might have implications for the regulation of body weight at a later age.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Olga Pollatos
- Clinical and Health Psychology, Institute of Education and Psychology, Ulm University, Ulm, Germany
| | - Karla Mönkemöller
- Clinical and Health Psychology, Institute of Education and Psychology, Ulm University, Ulm, Germany,*Correspondence: Karla Mönkemöller, ✉
| | - Karoline Groppe
- Evangelisches Krankenhaus Königin Elisabeth Herzberge, Berlin, Germany
| | - Birgit Elsner
- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Human Sciences, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Hübner AM, Trempler I, Schubotz RI. Interindividual differences in interoception modulate behavior and brain responses in emotional inference. Neuroimage 2022; 261:119524. [PMID: 35907498 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119524] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/11/2022] [Revised: 07/26/2022] [Accepted: 07/26/2022] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
Abstract
Emotional experiences are proposed to arise from contextualized perception of bodily responses, also referred to as interoceptive inferences. The recognition of emotions benefits from adequate access to one's own interoceptive information. However, direct empirical evidence of interoceptive inferences and their neural basis is still lacking. In the present fMRI study healthy volunteers performed a probabilistic emotion classification task with videotaped dynamically unfolding facial expressions. In a first step, we aimed to determine functional areas involved in the processing of dynamically unfolding emotional expressions. We then tested whether individuals with higher interoceptive accuracy (IAcc), as assessed by the Heartbeat detection task (HDT), or higher interoceptive sensitivity (IS), as assessed by the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness, Version 2 (MAIA-2), benefit more from the contextually given likelihood of emotional valence and whether brain regions reflecting individual IAcc and/or IS play a role in this. Individuals with higher IS benefitted more from the biased probability of emotional valence. Brain responses to more predictable emotions elicited a bilateral activity pattern comprising the inferior frontal gyrus and the posterior insula. Importantly, individual IAcc scores positively covaried with brain responses to more surprising and less predictable emotional expressions in the insula and caudate nucleus. We show for the first time that IAcc score is associated with enhanced processing of interoceptive prediction errors, particularly in the anterior insula. A higher IS score seems more likely to be associated with a stronger weighting of attention to interoceptive changes processed by the posterior insula and ventral prefrontal cortex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Ima Trempler
- Department of Psychology, University of Muenster, Germany; Otto-Creutzfeldt-Center for Cognitive and Behavioral Neuroscience, University of Muenster, Germany
| | - Ricarda I Schubotz
- Department of Psychology, University of Muenster, Germany; Otto-Creutzfeldt-Center for Cognitive and Behavioral Neuroscience, University of Muenster, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Quadt L, Critchley H, Nagai Y. Cognition, emotion, and the central autonomic network. Auton Neurosci 2022; 238:102948. [PMID: 35149372 DOI: 10.1016/j.autneu.2022.102948] [Citation(s) in RCA: 52] [Impact Index Per Article: 17.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/15/2021] [Revised: 12/05/2021] [Accepted: 01/16/2022] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
The demands of both mental and physical activity are integrated with the dynamic control of internal bodily states. The set of neural interactions that supports autonomic regulation extends beyond afferent-efferent homeostatic reflexes (interoceptive feedback, autonomic action) to encompass allostatic policies reflecting more abstract and predictive mental representations, often accessed as conscious thoughts and feelings. Historically and heuristically, reason is contrasted with passion, cognition with emotion, and 'cold' with 'hot' cognition. These categories are themselves arbitrary and blurred. Investigations of psychological processes have been generally pursued during states of musculoskeletal quiescence and are thus relatively insensitive to autonomic interaction with attentional, perceptual, mnemonic and decision-making processes. Autonomic psychophysiology has nevertheless highlighted the bidirectional coupling of distinct cognitive domains to the internal states of bodily arousal. More powerfully perhaps, in the context of emotion, autonomically mediated changes in inner bodily physiological states are viewed as intrinsic constituents of the expression of emotions, while their feedback representation is proposed to underpin emotional and motivational feelings. Here, we review the brain systems, encapsulated by the notion of central autonomic network, that provide the interface between cognitive, emotional and autonomic state. These systems span the neuraxis, overlap with the more general governance of behaviour, and represent district levels of proximity to survival-related imperatives. We touch upon the conceptual relevance of prediction and surprise to understanding the integration of cognition and emotion with autonomic control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lisa Quadt
- BSMS Department of Neuroscience, University of Brighton and University of Sussex, UK; Sussex Neuroscience, University of Sussex, UK
| | - Hugo Critchley
- BSMS Department of Neuroscience, University of Brighton and University of Sussex, UK; Sussex Neuroscience, University of Sussex, UK; Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, University of Sussex, UK; Sussex Partnership NHS Foundation Trust, UK.
| | - Yoko Nagai
- BSMS Department of Neuroscience, University of Brighton and University of Sussex, UK; Sussex Neuroscience, University of Sussex, UK
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Brewer R, Murphy J, Bird G. Atypical interoception as a common risk factor for psychopathology: A review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2021; 130:470-508. [PMID: 34358578 PMCID: PMC8522807 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.07.036] [Citation(s) in RCA: 79] [Impact Index Per Article: 19.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/16/2020] [Revised: 07/09/2021] [Accepted: 07/31/2021] [Indexed: 02/06/2023]
Abstract
The inadequacy of a categorial approach to mental health diagnosis is now well-recognised, with many authors, diagnostic manuals and funding bodies advocating a dimensional, trans-diagnostic approach to mental health research. Variance in interoception, the ability to perceive one's internal bodily state, is reported across diagnostic boundaries, and is associated with atypical functioning across symptom categories. Drawing on behavioural and neuroscientific evidence, we outline current research on the contribution of interoception to numerous cognitive and affective abilities (in both typical and clinical populations), and describe the interoceptive atypicalities seen in a range of psychiatric conditions. We discuss the role that interoception may play in the development and maintenance of psychopathology, as well as the ways in which interoception may differ across clinical presentations. A number of important areas for further research on the role of interoception in psychopathology are highlighted.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rebecca Brewer
- Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London, United Kingdom
| | - Jennifer Murphy
- Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London, United Kingdom.
| | - Geoffrey Bird
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, United Kingdom; Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Hübner AM, Trempler I, Gietmann C, Schubotz RI. Interoceptive sensibility predicts the ability to infer others' emotional states. PLoS One 2021; 16:e0258089. [PMID: 34613976 PMCID: PMC8494315 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0258089] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/16/2020] [Accepted: 09/20/2021] [Indexed: 01/09/2023] Open
Abstract
Emotional sensations and inferring another's emotional states have been suggested to depend on predictive models of the causes of bodily sensations, so-called interoceptive inferences. In this framework, higher sensibility for interoceptive changes (IS) reflects higher precision of interoceptive signals. The present study examined the link between IS and emotion recognition, testing whether individuals with higher IS recognize others' emotions more easily and are more sensitive to learn from biased probabilities of emotional expressions. We recorded skin conductance responses (SCRs) from forty-six healthy volunteers performing a speeded-response task, which required them to indicate whether a neutral facial expression dynamically turned into a happy or fearful expression. Moreover, varying probabilities of emotional expressions by their block-wise base rate aimed to generate a bias for the more frequently encountered emotion. As a result, we found that individuals with higher IS showed lower thresholds for emotion recognition, reflected in decreased reaction times for emotional expressions especially of high intensity. Moreover, individuals with increased IS benefited more from a biased probability of an emotion, reflected in decreased reaction times for expected emotions. Lastly, weak evidence supporting a differential modulation of SCR by IS as a function of varying probabilities was found. Our results indicate that higher interoceptive sensibility facilitates the recognition of emotional changes and is accompanied by a more precise adaptation to emotion probabilities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Amelie M. Hübner
- Department of Psychology, University of Muenster, Muenster, Germany
| | - Ima Trempler
- Department of Psychology, University of Muenster, Muenster, Germany
- Otto-Creutzfeldt-Center for Cognitive and Behavioural Neuroscience, University of Muenster, Muenster, Germany
| | - Corinna Gietmann
- Department of Psychology, University of Muenster, Muenster, Germany
| | - Ricarda I. Schubotz
- Department of Psychology, University of Muenster, Muenster, Germany
- Otto-Creutzfeldt-Center for Cognitive and Behavioural Neuroscience, University of Muenster, Muenster, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Homan P, Lau HL, Levy I, Raio CM, Bach DR, Carmel D, Schiller D. Evidence for a minimal role of stimulus awareness in reversal of threat learning. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2021; 28:95-103. [PMID: 33593928 PMCID: PMC7888237 DOI: 10.1101/lm.050997.119] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/29/2019] [Accepted: 12/02/2020] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
In an ever-changing environment, survival depends on learning which stimuli represent threat, and also on updating such associations when circumstances shift. It has been claimed that humans can acquire physiological responses to threat-associated stimuli even when they are unaware of them, but the role of awareness in updating threat contingencies remains unknown. This complex process-generating novel responses while suppressing learned ones-relies on distinct neural mechanisms from initial learning, and has only been shown with awareness. Can it occur unconsciously? Here, we present evidence that threat reversal may not require awareness. Participants underwent classical threat conditioning to visual stimuli that were suppressed from awareness. One of two images was paired with an electric shock; halfway through the experiment, contingencies were reversed and the shock was paired with the other image. Despite variations in suppression across participants, we found that physiological responses reflected changes in stimulus-threat pairings independently of stimulus awareness. These findings suggest that unconscious affective processing may be sufficiently flexible to adapt to changing circumstances.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Philipp Homan
- Psychiatric University Hospital Zurich, University of Zurich, 8032 Zurich, Switzerland
| | - H Lee Lau
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Zurich, 8032 Zurich, Switzerland.,Department of Neuroscience, University of Zurich, 8032 Zurich, Switzerland.,Friedman Brain Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029, USA
| | - Ifat Levy
- Department of Comparative Medicine, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029, USA.,Department of Neuroscience, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029, USA.,Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut 06520, USA
| | - Candace M Raio
- New York University Grossman School of Medicine, New York, New York 10016, USA
| | - Dominik R Bach
- Computational Psychiatry Research, Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, University of Zurich, 8032 Zurich, Switzerland.,Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, London WC1N 3BG, United Kingdom
| | - David Carmel
- School of Psychology, Victoria University of Wellington, Kelburn Parade, Wellington 6012, New Zealand
| | - Daniela Schiller
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Zurich, 8032 Zurich, Switzerland.,Department of Neuroscience, University of Zurich, 8032 Zurich, Switzerland.,Friedman Brain Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York 10029, USA
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Mendoza-Medialdea MT, Ruiz-Padial E. Understanding the capture of exogenous attention by disgusting and fearful stimuli: The role of interoceptive accuracy. Int J Psychophysiol 2021; 161:53-63. [PMID: 33453302 DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2021.01.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/06/2020] [Revised: 11/16/2020] [Accepted: 01/05/2021] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
The aim of the study was to explore the role of interoceptive accuracy (IA) on exogenous attention to disgusting and fearful distractors of a main concurrent task. Participants were thirty university students previously identified as high (N = 16) or normal IA according their performance in a heartbeat detection task. Event-related potentials and behavioural responses were recorded. The results showed that disgusting stimuli capture exogenous attention in a first stage as reflected by the augmented amplitude of the P100 component of the ERPs in high IA participants. Fearful distractors may capture attention in a later moment in all participants as revealed by a marginally significant effect on the amplitude of N200. At behavioural level, disgusting distractors provoked a higher number of errors than neutral in normal IA participants. The time course of the effect of disgust and fearful eliciting distractors on exogenous attention appeared to depend on the individual characteristic of participants.
Collapse
|
14
|
Marcos JL, Marcos A. Concurrent Electrodermal and Eyeblink Conditioning With Masked and Unmasked Stimuli. J PSYCHOPHYSIOL 2021. [DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000259] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
Abstract. The aim of this study was to determine if contingency awareness between the conditioned (CS) and unconditioned stimulus (US) is necessary for concurrent electrodermal and eyeblink conditioning to masked stimuli. An angry woman’s face (CS+) and a fearful face (CS−) were presented for 23 milliseconds (ms) and followed by a neutral face as a mask. A 98 dB noise burst (US) was administered 477 ms after CS+ offset to elicit both electrodermal and eyeblink responses. For the unmasking conditioning a 176 ms blank screen was inserted between the CS and the mask. Contingency awareness was assessed using trial-by-trial ratings of US-expectancy in a post-conditioning phase. The results showed acquisition of differential electrodermal and eyeblink conditioning in aware, but not in unaware participants. Acquisition of differential eyeblink conditioning required more trials than electrodermal conditioning. These results provided strong evidence of the causal role of contingency awareness on differential eyeblink and electrodermal conditioning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Azahara Marcos
- Department of Neurology, Hospital “Infanta Cristina”, Madrid, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Eng GK, Collins KA, Brown C, Ludlow M, Tobe RH, Iosifescu DV, Stern ER. Dimensions of interoception in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Obsessive Compuls Relat Disord 2020; 27:100584. [PMID: 33194538 PMCID: PMC7665060 DOI: 10.1016/j.jocrd.2020.100584] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/04/2023]
Abstract
Interoceptive sensibility (IS) refers to the subjective experience of perceiving and being aware of one's internal body sensations, and is typically evaluated using self-report questionnaires or confidence ratings. Here we evaluated IS in 81 patients with OCD and 76 controls using the Multidimensional Scale of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA), which contains 8 subscales assessing adaptive and maladaptive responses to sensation. Compared to controls, OCD patients showed hyperawareness of body sensations. Patients also demonstrated a more maladaptive profile of IS characterized by greater distraction from and worry about unpleasant sensations, and reduced tendency to experience the body as safe and trustworthy. These findings were independent of medication status and comorbidities in the patient group. Correlational analyses showed that subscales of the MAIA were differentially associated with OCD symptom dimensions. These findings indicate that patients with OCD show abnormality of IS that is independent of confounding factors related to medication and comorbidities and associated with different OCD symptom dimensions. Future work would benefit from examining neural correlates of these effects and evaluating whether dimensions of IS are impacted by treatments for the disorder.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Goi Khia Eng
- Department of Psychiatry, New York University School of
Medicine, New York, NY
- Nathan S. Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research,
Orangeburg, NY
| | - Katherine A. Collins
- Nathan S. Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research,
Orangeburg, NY
- Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount
Sinai, New York, NY
| | - Carina Brown
- Department of Psychiatry, New York University School of
Medicine, New York, NY
- Nathan S. Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research,
Orangeburg, NY
| | - Molly Ludlow
- Nathan S. Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research,
Orangeburg, NY
| | - Russell H. Tobe
- Nathan S. Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research,
Orangeburg, NY
| | - Dan V. Iosifescu
- Department of Psychiatry, New York University School of
Medicine, New York, NY
- Nathan S. Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research,
Orangeburg, NY
| | - Emily R. Stern
- Department of Psychiatry, New York University School of
Medicine, New York, NY
- Nathan S. Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research,
Orangeburg, NY
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Mertens G, Engelhard IM. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the evidence for unaware fear conditioning. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2020; 108:254-268. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.11.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/11/2019] [Revised: 11/12/2019] [Accepted: 11/15/2019] [Indexed: 01/23/2023]
|
17
|
Forkmann T, Volz-Sidiropoulou E, Helbing T, Drüke B, Mainz V, Rath D, Gauggel S, Teismann T. Sense it and use it: interoceptive accuracy and sensibility in suicide ideators. BMC Psychiatry 2019; 19:334. [PMID: 31675999 PMCID: PMC6825340 DOI: 10.1186/s12888-019-2322-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/22/2019] [Accepted: 10/14/2019] [Indexed: 01/28/2023] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Interoceptive deficits have been found to be associated with suicidal ideation and behavior. However, an objective measure of interoceptive accuracy has not been investigated in participants with suicide ideation, by now. This study aimed at investigating interoceptive accuracy and sensibility in persons with and without suicide ideation (SI) while controlling for severity of depressive symptoms. METHOD Ninety-five participants (age: M = 34.8, SD = 11.6, n = 56 female [58.9%]; n = 51 patients with a Major Depressive Disorder and n = 44 healthy participants) were assessed for interoceptive accuracy and sensibility, depression and SI. RESULTS Twenty-five participants (26%) reported SI. They showed interoceptive accuracy comparable to persons without SI (t = -.81, p = .422), but significantly lower interoceptive sensibility. After controlling for severity of depressive symptoms in a hierarchical linear regression analysis, most associations between interoceptive sensibility and SI disappeared. CONCLUSION Results suggest that suicide ideators do not lack the ability to perceive their own bodily signals but they feel less able to use them in a way that is advantageous for them. Differences between suicide ideators and non-ideators appear to be largely driven by depressive symptoms (depression bias).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas Forkmann
- 0000 0001 2187 5445grid.5718.bDepartment of Clinical Psychology, University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany
| | - Eftychia Volz-Sidiropoulou
- 0000 0000 8653 1507grid.412301.5Institute of Medical Psychology and Medical Sociology, University Hospital of RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany
| | - Trientje Helbing
- 0000 0000 8653 1507grid.412301.5Institute of Medical Psychology and Medical Sociology, University Hospital of RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany
| | - Barbara Drüke
- 0000 0000 8653 1507grid.412301.5Institute of Medical Psychology and Medical Sociology, University Hospital of RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany
| | - Verena Mainz
- 0000 0000 8653 1507grid.412301.5Institute of Medical Psychology and Medical Sociology, University Hospital of RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany
| | - Dajana Rath
- 0000 0001 2187 5445grid.5718.bDepartment of Clinical Psychology, University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany
| | - Siegfried Gauggel
- 0000 0000 8653 1507grid.412301.5Institute of Medical Psychology and Medical Sociology, University Hospital of RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany
| | - Tobias Teismann
- Mental Health Research and Treatment Center, Department of Psychology, Ruhr-Universität Bochum, Bochum, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation improves interoceptive accuracy. Neuropsychologia 2019; 134:107201. [PMID: 31562863 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107201] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/15/2019] [Revised: 08/31/2019] [Accepted: 09/20/2019] [Indexed: 12/25/2022]
Abstract
How can interoceptive accuracy, i.e. the objective ability to identify interoceptive signals, be improved? In the present study, we investigated whether non-invasive stimulation of the auricular branch of the vagus nerve (taVNS) modulates cardiac interoceptive accuracy, interoceptive sensibility, i.e. confidence in the identification of bodily signals, and interoceptive awareness, i.e. the capacity to evaluate one's ability in the objective task. Using a single-blind within-subjects design we compared participants' performance on the heartbeat counting task and on the heartbeat discrimination task during active and sham taVNS stimulation. Results revealed improved accuracy during active taVNS on the heartbeat discrimination task but not on the heartbeat counting task. Participants were also more confident during active stimulation, but interoceptive awareness was not modulated by taVNS. These findings show that taVNS can modulate interoceptive processing and suggest its potential as a tool to investigate body-brain interactions.
Collapse
|
19
|
Shalev I. Motivated Cue Integration in Alexithymia: Improving Interoception and Emotion Information Processing by Awareness-of-Sensation Techniques. Front Psychiatry 2019; 10:329. [PMID: 31133902 PMCID: PMC6524402 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00329] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/28/2019] [Accepted: 04/29/2019] [Indexed: 12/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Recent findings indicate that alexithymia is the result of a multidomain, multidimensional failure of interoception. Whereas much of the literature addresses the cognitive and affective aspects of alexithymia, less is known about the association between the failure of interoception and the process of motivated cue integration. The theory of motivated cue integration integrates high-level control processes with low-level embodied and contextual cues, suggesting that selective attention to internal and contextual cues results in the creation of meaning that, in turn, influences judgment and action generation. Conceptualized as a special case of the cue integration problem, alexithymia may be associated with restricted access to emotional cues, indicating impaired connectivity between low-level embodied cues and top-down goals and values. This problem may also be viewed as a means substitution problem, indicating the individual's need for alternative multisensory information. Based on this reasoning, interventions that exploit awareness-of-sensation techniques (e.g., mindfulness, experiential approach, focusing) may help to improve the distinction between bodily sensation and interpretation and to create meaning of situational state by substitution of inaccessible affective cues with alternative cues. Accordingly, clinicians and neuropsychologists can help individuals who suffer from alexithymia by training them to use awareness-of-sensation techniques and directing their attention to alternative multisensory cues as well as alternative cognitive configurations (e.g., mental images). Integrating peripheral cues in the moment-by-moment generation of meaning and self-regulation can improve affective judgment through the exchange of inaccessible affective cues with alternative ones.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Idit Shalev
- Laboratory for Embodiment and Self-Regulation, Department of Psychology, Ariel University, Ariel, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Herman AM, Critchley HD, Duka T. Risk-Taking and Impulsivity: The Role of Mood States and Interoception. Front Psychol 2018; 9:1625. [PMID: 30210421 PMCID: PMC6123387 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01625] [Citation(s) in RCA: 61] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/02/2018] [Accepted: 08/14/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Objectives: The consequences of impulsive decisions and actions represent a major source of concern to the health and well-being of individuals and society. It is, therefore, crucial to understand the factors which contribute to impulsive behaviors. Here, we examined how personality traits of behavioral tendencies, interoceptive sensibility as well as transient mood states predict behavioral performance on impulsivity and risk-taking tasks. Method: 574 (121 males; age 18–45) individuals completed self-report personality measures of impulsivity, reward sensitivity, punishment avoidance as well as interoceptive sensibility, undertook a mood assessment and performed a set of cognitive tasks: delay discounting (temporal impulsivity), probability discounting (risk-taking), and reflection impulsivity task. Data were interrogated using principal component analysis, correlations and regression analyses to test mutual relationships between personality traits, interoceptive sensibility, mood state and impulsive behaviors. Results: We observed a clear separation of measures used, both trait and behavioral. Namely, sensation-seeking, reward sensitivity and probability discounting reflected risk-taking. These were separate from measures associated with impulsivity, both trait (negative and positive urgency, premeditation, perseverance) and behavioral (delayed discounting and reflection impulsivity). This separation was further highlighted by their relationship with the current emotional state: positive affect was associated with increased risk-taking tendencies and risky decision-making, while negative emotions were related to heightened impulsivity measures. Interoceptive sensibility was only associated with negative emotions component. Conclusion: Our findings support the proposal that risk-taking and impulsivity represent distinct constructs that are differentially affected by current mood states. This novel insight enhances our understanding of impulsive behaviors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aleksandra M Herman
- Behavioral and Clinical Neuroscience, School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, United Kingdom.,Sussex Addiction and Intervention Centre, University of Sussex, Brighton, United Kingdom
| | - Hugo D Critchley
- Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, University of Sussex, Brighton, United Kingdom.,Brighton and Sussex Medical School, Brighton, United Kingdom
| | - Theodora Duka
- Behavioral and Clinical Neuroscience, School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, United Kingdom.,Sussex Addiction and Intervention Centre, University of Sussex, Brighton, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Murphy J, Catmur C, Bird G. Alexithymia is associated with a multidomain, multidimensional failure of interoception: Evidence from novel tests. J Exp Psychol Gen 2017; 147:398-408. [PMID: 29154612 PMCID: PMC5824617 DOI: 10.1037/xge0000366] [Citation(s) in RCA: 118] [Impact Index Per Article: 14.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/27/2022]
Abstract
Interoception, the perception of the body’s internal state, contributes to numerous aspects of higher-order cognition. Several theories suggest a causal role for atypical interoception in specific psychiatric disorders, including a recent claim that atypical interoception represents a transdiagnostic impairment across disorders characterized by reduced perception of one’s own emotion (alexithymia). Such theories are supported predominantly by evidence from only one interoceptive domain (cardiac); however, evidence of domain-specific interoceptive ability highlights the need to assess interoception in noncardiac domains. Using novel interoceptive tasks, we demonstrate that individuals high in alexithymic traits show a reduced propensity to utilize interoceptive cues to gauge respiratory output (Experiment 1), reduced accuracy on tasks of muscular effort (Experiment 2), and taste sensitivity (Experiment 3), unrelated to any co-occurring autism, depression, or anxiety. Results suggest that alexithymia reflects a multidomain, multidimensional failure of interoception, which is consistent with theories suggesting that atypical interoception may underpin both symptom commonalities between psychiatric disorders and heterogeneity within disorders.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jennifer Murphy
- MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, King's College London
| | - Caroline Catmur
- Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London
| | - Geoffrey Bird
- MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, King's College London
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Stern ER, Grimaldi SJ, Muratore A, Murrough J, Leibu E, Fleysher L, Goodman WK, Burdick KE. Neural correlates of interoception: Effects of interoceptive focus and relationship to dimensional measures of body awareness. Hum Brain Mapp 2017; 38:6068-6082. [PMID: 28901713 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.23811] [Citation(s) in RCA: 71] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/02/2017] [Revised: 09/01/2017] [Accepted: 09/05/2017] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
Interoception has been defined as the sensing of the physiological condition of the body, with interoceptive sensibility (IS) characterizing an individual's self-reported awareness of internal sensation. IS is a multidimensional construct including not only the tendency to be aware of sensation but also how sensations are interpreted, regulated, and used to inform behavior, with different dimensions relating to different aspects of health and disease. Here we investigated neural mechanisms of interoception when healthy individuals attended to their heartbeat and skin temperature, and examined the relationship between neural activity during interoception and individual differences in self-reported IS using the Multidimensional Scale of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA). Consistent with prior work, interoception activated a network involving insula and sensorimotor regions but also including occipital, temporal, and prefrontal cortex. Differences based on interoceptive focus (heartbeat vs skin temperature) were found in insula, sensorimotor regions, occipital cortex, and limbic areas. Factor analysis of MAIA dimensions revealed 3 dissociable components of IS in our dataset, only one of which was related to neural activity during interoception. Reduced scores on the third factor, which reflected reduced ability to control attention to body sensation and increased tendency to distract from and worry about aversive sensations, was associated with greater activation in many of the same regions as those involved in interoception, including insula, sensorimotor, anterior cingulate, and temporal cortex. These data suggest that self-rated interoceptive sensibility is related to altered activation in regions involved in monitoring body state, which has implications for disorders associated with abnormality of interoception. Hum Brain Mapp 38:6068-6082, 2017. © 2017 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emily R Stern
- Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai (ISMMS), New York, New York.,Fishberg Department of Neuroscience and Friedman Brain Institute, ISMMS, New York, New York
| | - Stephanie J Grimaldi
- Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai (ISMMS), New York, New York
| | | | - James Murrough
- Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai (ISMMS), New York, New York.,Fishberg Department of Neuroscience and Friedman Brain Institute, ISMMS, New York, New York
| | - Evan Leibu
- Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai (ISMMS), New York, New York
| | - Lazar Fleysher
- Fishberg Department of Neuroscience and Friedman Brain Institute, ISMMS, New York, New York.,Department of Radiology, ISMMS, New York, New York
| | - Wayne K Goodman
- Department of Psychiatry, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas
| | - Katherine E Burdick
- Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai (ISMMS), New York, New York.,Fishberg Department of Neuroscience and Friedman Brain Institute, ISMMS, New York, New York.,Department of Psychiatry, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Siegel P, Warren R, Wang Z, Yang J, Cohen D, Anderson JF, Murray L, Peterson BS. Less is more: Neural activity during very brief and clearly visible exposure to phobic stimuli. Hum Brain Mapp 2017; 38:2466-2481. [PMID: 28165171 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.23533] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/04/2016] [Revised: 01/18/2017] [Accepted: 01/19/2017] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Research on automatic processes in fear has emphasized the provocation of fear responses rather than their attenuation. We have previously shown that the repeated presentation of feared images without conscious awareness via backward masking reduces avoidance of a live tarantula in spider-phobic participants. Herein we investigated the neural basis for these adaptive effects of masked exposure. 21 spider-phobic and 21 control participants, identified by a psychiatric interview, fear questionnaire, and approaching a live tarantula, viewed stimuli in each of three conditions: (1) very brief exposure (VBE) to masked images of spiders, severely limited awareness; (2) clearly visible exposure (CVE) to spiders, full awareness; and (3) masked images of flowers (control), severely limited awareness. Only VBE to masked spiders generated neural activity more strongly in phobic than in control participants, within subcortical fear, attention, higher-order language, and vision systems. Moreover, VBE activated regions that support fear processing in phobic participants without causing them to experience fear consciously. Counter-intuitively, CVE to the same spiders generated stronger neural activity in control rather than phobic participants within these and other systems. CVE deactivated regions supporting fear regulation and caused phobic participants to experience fear. CVE-induced activations also correlated with measures of explicit fear ratings, whereas VBE-induced activations correlated with measures of implicit fear (color-naming interference of spider words). These multiple dissociations between the effects of VBE and CVE to spiders suggest that limiting awareness of exposure to phobic stimuli through visual masking paradoxically facilitates their processing, while simultaneously minimizing the experience of fear. Hum Brain Mapp 38:2466-2481, 2017. © 2017 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Zhishun Wang
- New York State Psychiatric Institute, Columbia University, New York, New York
| | - Jie Yang
- New York State Psychiatric Institute, Columbia University, New York, New York
| | - Don Cohen
- New York University, New York, New York
| | | | | | - Bradley S Peterson
- Children's Hospital Los Angeles & Keck School of Medicine, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Murphy J, Brewer R, Catmur C, Bird G. Interoception and psychopathology: A developmental neuroscience perspective. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2017; 23:45-56. [PMID: 28081519 PMCID: PMC6987654 DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.12.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 210] [Impact Index Per Article: 26.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/16/2016] [Revised: 12/19/2016] [Accepted: 12/19/2016] [Indexed: 12/15/2022] Open
Abstract
Interoception refers to the perception of the physiological condition of the body, including hunger, temperature, and heart rate. There is a growing appreciation that interoception is integral to higher-order cognition. Indeed, existing research indicates an association between low interoceptive sensitivity and alexithymia (a difficulty identifying one's own emotion), underscoring the link between bodily and emotional awareness. Despite this appreciation, the developmental trajectory of interoception across the lifespan remains under-researched, with clear gaps in our understanding. This qualitative review and opinion paper provides a brief overview of interoception, discussing its relevance for developmental psychopathology, and highlighting measurement issues, before surveying the available work on interoception across four stages of development: infancy, childhood, adolescence and late adulthood. Where gaps in the literature addressing the development of interoception exist, we draw upon the association between alexithymia and interoception, using alexithymia as a possible marker of atypical interoception. Evidence indicates that interoceptive ability varies across development, and that this variance correlates with established age-related changes in cognition and with risk periods for the development of psychopathology. We suggest a theory within which atypical interoception underlies the onset of psychopathology and risky behaviour in adolescence, and the decreased socio-emotional competence observed in late adulthood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jennifer Murphy
- MRC Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK.
| | - Rebecca Brewer
- MRC Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK; School of Psychology, The University of East London, London, UK
| | - Caroline Catmur
- MRC Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
| | - Geoffrey Bird
- MRC Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK; Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, UCL, London, UK; Dept of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Ainley V, Apps MAJ, Fotopoulou A, Tsakiris M. 'Bodily precision': a predictive coding account of individual differences in interoceptive accuracy. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2016; 371:rstb.2016.0003. [PMID: 28080962 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 143] [Impact Index Per Article: 15.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 07/26/2016] [Indexed: 12/11/2022] Open
Abstract
Individuals differ in their awareness of afferent information from within their bodies, which is typically assessed by a heartbeat perception measure of 'interoceptive accuracy' (IAcc). Neural and behavioural correlates of this trait have been investigated, but a theoretical explanation has yet to be presented. Building on recent models that describe interoception within the free energy/predictive coding framework, this paper applies similar principles to IAcc, proposing that individual differences in IAcc depend on 'precision' in interoceptive systems, i.e. the relative weight accorded to 'prior' representations and 'prediction errors' (that part of incoming interoceptive sensation not accounted for by priors), at various levels within the cortical hierarchy and between modalities. Attention has the effect of optimizing precision both within and between sensory modalities. Our central assumption is that people with high IAcc are able, with attention, to prioritize interoception over other sensory modalities and can thus adjust the relative precision of their interoceptive priors and prediction errors, where appropriate, given their personal history. This characterization explains key findings within the interoception literature; links results previously seen as unrelated or contradictory; and may have important implications for understanding cognitive, behavioural and psychopathological consequences of both high and low interoceptive awareness.This article is part of the themed issue 'Interoception beyond homeostasis: affect, cognition and mental health'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Vivien Ainley
- Laboratory of Action and Body, Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London, Egham TW20 0EX, UK
| | - Matthew A J Apps
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3UD, UK
| | - Aikaterini Fotopoulou
- Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, Gower Street, London WC1E 6B, UK
| | - Manos Tsakiris
- Laboratory of Action and Body, Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London, Egham TW20 0EX, UK
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Kandasamy N, Garfinkel SN, Page L, Hardy B, Critchley HD, Gurnell M, Coates JM. Interoceptive Ability Predicts Survival on a London Trading Floor. Sci Rep 2016; 6:32986. [PMID: 27641692 PMCID: PMC5027524 DOI: 10.1038/srep32986] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/05/2016] [Accepted: 08/15/2016] [Indexed: 01/21/2023] Open
Abstract
Interoception is the sensing of physiological signals originating inside the body, such as hunger, pain and heart rate. People with greater sensitivity to interoceptive signals, as measured by, for example, tests of heart beat detection, perform better in laboratory studies of risky decision-making. However, there has been little field work to determine if interoceptive sensitivity contributes to success in real-world, high-stakes risk taking. Here, we report on a study in which we quantified heartbeat detection skills in a group of financial traders working on a London trading floor. We found that traders are better able to perceive their own heartbeats than matched controls from the non-trading population. Moreover, the interoceptive ability of traders predicted their relative profitability, and strikingly, how long they survived in the financial markets. Our results suggest that signals from the body - the gut feelings of financial lore - contribute to success in the markets.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Narayanan Kandasamy
- Institute of Metabolic Science and National Institute for Health Research Cambridge Biomedical Research Centre, University of Cambridge, Addenbrooke’s Hospital, Cambridge CB2 0QQ, UK
| | - Sarah N. Garfinkel
- Brighton and Sussex Medical School and Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, University of Sussex, Brighton BN1 9RR UK
| | - Lionel Page
- Queensland University of Technology, Business School, Brisbane, QLD 4001, Australia
| | - Ben Hardy
- University of Cambridge, Judge Business School, Cambridge CB2 1AG, United Kingdom
| | - Hugo D. Critchley
- Brighton and Sussex Medical School and Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, University of Sussex, Brighton BN1 9RR UK
| | - Mark Gurnell
- Institute of Metabolic Science and National Institute for Health Research Cambridge Biomedical Research Centre, University of Cambridge, Addenbrooke’s Hospital, Cambridge CB2 0QQ, UK
| | - John M. Coates
- University of Cambridge, Judge Business School, Cambridge CB2 1AG, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Shivkumar K, Ajijola OA, Anand I, Armour JA, Chen PS, Esler M, De Ferrari GM, Fishbein MC, Goldberger JJ, Harper RM, Joyner MJ, Khalsa SS, Kumar R, Lane R, Mahajan A, Po S, Schwartz PJ, Somers VK, Valderrabano M, Vaseghi M, Zipes DP. Clinical neurocardiology defining the value of neuroscience-based cardiovascular therapeutics. J Physiol 2016; 594:3911-54. [PMID: 27114333 PMCID: PMC4945719 DOI: 10.1113/jp271870] [Citation(s) in RCA: 236] [Impact Index Per Article: 26.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/06/2016] [Accepted: 04/08/2016] [Indexed: 12/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The autonomic nervous system regulates all aspects of normal cardiac function, and is recognized to play a critical role in the pathophysiology of many cardiovascular diseases. As such, the value of neuroscience-based cardiovascular therapeutics is increasingly evident. This White Paper reviews the current state of understanding of human cardiac neuroanatomy, neurophysiology, pathophysiology in specific disease conditions, autonomic testing, risk stratification, and neuromodulatory strategies to mitigate the progression of cardiovascular diseases.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kalyanam Shivkumar
- UCLA Cardiac Arrhythmia Center and Neurocardiology Research Center of Excellence, Los Angeles, CA, USA
| | - Olujimi A Ajijola
- UCLA Cardiac Arrhythmia Center and Neurocardiology Research Center of Excellence, Los Angeles, CA, USA
| | - Inder Anand
- Department of Cardiology, University of Minnesota Medical School, Minneapolis, MN, USA
| | - J Andrew Armour
- UCLA Cardiac Arrhythmia Center and Neurocardiology Research Center of Excellence, Los Angeles, CA, USA
| | - Peng-Sheng Chen
- Krannert Institute of Cardiology, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA
| | - Murray Esler
- Baker IDI Heart and Diabetes Institute, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
| | | | - Michael C Fishbein
- Department of Pathology and Laboratory Medicine, David Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, Los Angeles, California, USA
| | - Jeffrey J Goldberger
- Division of Cardiology, University of Miami Miller School of Medicine, Miami, FL, USA
| | - Ronald M Harper
- Department of Neurobiology and the Brain Research Institute, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
| | - Michael J Joyner
- Division of Cardiovascular Diseases, Mayo Clinic and Mayo Foundation, Rochester, MN, USA
| | | | - Rajesh Kumar
- Departments of Anesthesiology and Radiological Sciences, David Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, Los Angeles, CA, USA
| | - Richard Lane
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Arizona College of Medicine, Tucson, AZ, USA
| | - Aman Mahajan
- Department of Anesthesia, UCLA, Los Angeles, CA, USA
| | - Sunny Po
- Heart Rhythm Institute, University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center, Oklahoma City, OK, USA
- University of Tulsa Oxley College of Health Sciences, Tulsa, OK, USA
| | - Peter J Schwartz
- Center for Cardiac Arrhythmias of Genetic Origin, IRCCS Instituto Auxologico Italiano, c/o Centro Diagnostico e di Ricerrca San Carlo, Milan, Italy
| | - Virend K Somers
- Division of Cardiovascular Diseases, Mayo Clinic and Mayo Foundation, Rochester, MN, USA
| | - Miguel Valderrabano
- Methodist DeBakey Heart and Vascular Center and Methodist Hospital Research Institute, Houston Methodist Hospital, Houston, TX, USA
| | - Marmar Vaseghi
- UCLA Cardiac Arrhythmia Center and Neurocardiology Research Center of Excellence, Los Angeles, CA, USA
| | - Douglas P Zipes
- Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Azevedo RT, Aglioti SM, Lenggenhager B. Participants' above-chance recognition of own-heart sound combined with poor metacognitive awareness suggests implicit knowledge of own heart cardiodynamics. Sci Rep 2016; 6:26545. [PMID: 27211283 PMCID: PMC4876374 DOI: 10.1038/srep26545] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/13/2015] [Accepted: 04/25/2016] [Indexed: 01/04/2023] Open
Abstract
Mounting evidence suggests that interoceptive signals are fundamentally important for the experience of the self. Thus far, studies on interoception have mainly focused on the ability to monitor the timing of ongoing heartbeats and on how these influence emotional and self-related processes. However, cardiac afferent signalling is not confined to heartbeat timing and several other cardiac parameters characterize cardiodynamic functioning. Building on the fact that each heart has its own self-specific cardio-dynamics, which cannot be expressed uniquely by heart rate, we devised a novel task to test whether people could recognize the sound of their own heart even when perceived offline and thus not in synchrony with ongoing heartbeats. In a forced-choice paradigm, participants discriminated between sounds of their own heartbeat (previously recorded with a Doppler device) versus another person’s heart. Participants identified the sound of their own heart above chance, whereas their metacognition of performance – as calculated by contrasting performance against ratings of confidence - was considerably poorer. These results suggest an implicit access to fine-grained neural representations of elementary cardio-dynamic parameters beyond heartbeat timing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ruben T Azevedo
- IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Via Ardeatina 306, 00100 Rome, Italy.,Department of Psychology, University of Rome "La Sapienza", Via dei Marsi 78, 00185 Rome, Italy
| | - Salvatore Maria Aglioti
- IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Via Ardeatina 306, 00100 Rome, Italy.,Department of Psychology, University of Rome "La Sapienza", Via dei Marsi 78, 00185 Rome, Italy
| | | |
Collapse
|
29
|
Abstract
This study compared fear learning acquired through direct experience (Pavlovian conditioning) and fear learning acquired without direct experience via either observation or verbal instruction. We examined whether these three types of learning yielded differential responses to conditioned stimuli (CS+) that were presented unmasked (available to explicit awareness) or masked (not available to explicit awareness). In the Pavlovian group, the CS+ was paired with a mild shock, whereas the observational-learning group learned through observing the emotional expression of a confederate receiving shocks paired with the CS+. The instructed-learning group was told that the CS+ predicted a shock. The three groups demonstrated similar levels of learning as measured by the skin conductance response to unmasked stimuli. As in previous studies, participants also displayed a significant learning response to masked stimuli following Pavlovian conditioning. However, whereas the observational-learning group also showed this effect, the instructed-learning group did not.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andreas Olsson
- New York University, Meyer Hall-Psychology C&P, New York, NY 10003, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
30
|
Khalsa SS, Lapidus RC. Can Interoception Improve the Pragmatic Search for Biomarkers in Psychiatry? Front Psychiatry 2016; 7:121. [PMID: 27504098 PMCID: PMC4958623 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00121] [Citation(s) in RCA: 211] [Impact Index Per Article: 23.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/26/2016] [Accepted: 06/21/2016] [Indexed: 01/04/2023] Open
Abstract
Disrupted interoception is a prominent feature of the diagnostic classification of several psychiatric disorders. However, progress in understanding the interoceptive basis of these disorders has been incremental, and the application of interoception in clinical treatment is currently limited to panic disorder. To examine the degree to which the scientific community has recognized interoception as a construct of interest, we identified and individually screened all articles published in the English language on interoception and associated root terms in Pubmed, Psychinfo, and ISI Web of Knowledge. This search revealed that interoception is a multifaceted process that is being increasingly studied within the fields of psychiatry, psychology, neuroscience, and biomedical science. To illustrate the multifaceted nature of interoception, we provide a focused review of one of the most commonly studied interoceptive channels, the cardiovascular system, and give a detailed comparison of the most popular methods used to study cardiac interoception. We subsequently review evidence of interoceptive dysfunction in panic disorder, depression, somatic symptom disorders, anorexia nervosa, and bulimia nervosa. For each disorder, we suggest how interoceptive predictions constructed by the brain may erroneously bias individuals to express key symptoms and behaviors, and outline questions that are suitable for the development of neuroscience-based mental health interventions. We conclude that interoception represents a viable avenue for clinical and translational research in psychiatry, with a well-established conceptual framework, a neural basis, measurable biomarkers, interdisciplinary appeal, and transdiagnostic targets for understanding and improving mental health outcomes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sahib S Khalsa
- Laureate Institute for Brain Research (LIBR), Tulsa, OK, USA; Oxley College of Health Sciences, University of Tulsa, Tulsa, OK, USA
| | - Rachel C Lapidus
- Laureate Institute for Brain Research (LIBR), Tulsa, OK, USA; Department of Psychology, University of Tulsa, Tulsa, OK, USA
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Garfinkel SN, Critchley HD. Threat and the Body: How the Heart Supports Fear Processing. Trends Cogn Sci 2015; 20:34-46. [PMID: 26628111 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 161] [Impact Index Per Article: 16.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/31/2015] [Revised: 10/01/2015] [Accepted: 10/12/2015] [Indexed: 12/16/2022]
Abstract
Mental processes depend upon a dynamic integration of brain and body. Emotions encompass internal physiological changes which, through interoception (sensing bodily states), underpin emotional feelings, for example, cardiovascular arousal can intensify feelings of fear and anxiety. The brain is informed about how quickly and strongly the heart is beating by signals from arterial baroreceptors. These fire in bursts after each heartbeat, and are quiet between heartbeats. The processing of fear stimuli is selectively enhanced by these phasic signals, and these inhibit the processing of other types of stimuli including physical pain. Behavioural and neuroimaging studies detail this differential impact of heart signals on the processing of salient stimuli, and add to knowledge linking rhythmic activity in brain and body to perceptual consciousness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah N Garfinkel
- Division of Psychiatry, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, University of Sussex, Falmer, Brighton BN1 9RR, UK; Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, University of Sussex, Falmer Brighton, BN1 9RH, UK.
| | - Hugo D Critchley
- Division of Psychiatry, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, University of Sussex, Falmer, Brighton BN1 9RR, UK; Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, University of Sussex, Falmer Brighton, BN1 9RH, UK; Sussex Partnership National Health Service (NHS) Foundation Trust, Sussex, UK
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Hopkins LS, Schultz DH, Hannula DE, Helmstetter FJ. Eye Movements Index Implicit Memory Expression in Fear Conditioning. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0141949. [PMID: 26562298 PMCID: PMC4642991 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141949] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/17/2015] [Accepted: 10/15/2015] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
The role of contingency awareness in simple associative learning experiments with human participants is currently debated. Since prior work suggests that eye movements can index mnemonic processes that occur without awareness, we used eye tracking to better understand the role of awareness in learning aversive Pavlovian conditioning. A complex real-world scene containing four embedded household items was presented to participants while skin conductance, eye movements, and pupil size were recorded. One item embedded in the scene served as the conditional stimulus (CS). One exemplar of that item (e.g. a white pot) was paired with shock 100 percent of the time (CS+) while a second exemplar (e.g. a gray pot) was never paired with shock (CS-). The remaining items were paired with shock on half of the trials. Participants rated their expectation of receiving a shock during each trial, and these expectancy ratings were used to identify when (i.e. on what trial) each participant became aware of the programmed contingencies. Disproportionate viewing of the CS was found both before and after explicit contingency awareness, and patterns of viewing distinguished the CS+ from the CS-. These observations are consistent with "dual process" models of fear conditioning, as they indicate that learning can be expressed in patterns of viewing prior to explicit contingency awareness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lauren S. Hopkins
- Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee, Milwaukee, WI, 53211, United States of America
| | - Douglas H. Schultz
- Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee, Milwaukee, WI, 53211, United States of America
| | - Deborah E. Hannula
- Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee, Milwaukee, WI, 53211, United States of America
| | - Fred J. Helmstetter
- Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee, Milwaukee, WI, 53211, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Critchley HD, Garfinkel SN. Interactions between visceral afferent signaling and stimulus processing. Front Neurosci 2015; 9:286. [PMID: 26379481 PMCID: PMC4550795 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00286] [Citation(s) in RCA: 55] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/28/2015] [Accepted: 07/29/2015] [Indexed: 12/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Visceral afferent signals to the brain influence thoughts, feelings and behavior. Here we highlight the findings of a set of empirical investigations in humans concerning body-mind interaction that focus on how feedback from states of autonomic arousal shapes cognition and emotion. There is a longstanding debate regarding the contribution of the body to mental processes. Recent theoretical models broadly acknowledge the role of (autonomically-mediated) physiological arousal to emotional, social and motivational behaviors, yet the underlying mechanisms are only partially characterized. Neuroimaging is overcoming this shortfall; first, by demonstrating correlations between autonomic change and discrete patterns of evoked, and task-independent, neural activity; second, by mapping the central consequences of clinical perturbations in autonomic response and; third, by probing how dynamic fluctuations in peripheral autonomic state are integrated with perceptual, cognitive and emotional processes. Building on the notion that an important source of the brain's representation of physiological arousal is derived from afferent information from arterial baroreceptors, we have exploited the phasic nature of these signals to show their differential contribution to the processing of emotionally-salient stimuli. This recent work highlights the facilitation at neural and behavioral levels of fear and threat processing that contrasts with the more established observations of the inhibition of central pain processing during baroreceptors activation. The implications of this body-brain-mind axis are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hugo D Critchley
- Division of Medicine, Psychiatry, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, University of Sussex Brighton, UK ; Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, University of Sussex Brighton, UK ; Sussex Partnership NHS Foundation Trust Hove, UK
| | - Sarah N Garfinkel
- Division of Medicine, Psychiatry, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, University of Sussex Brighton, UK ; Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, University of Sussex Brighton, UK
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Kleckner IR, Wormwood JB, Simmons WK, Barrett LF, Quigley KS. Methodological recommendations for a heartbeat detection-based measure of interoceptive sensitivity. Psychophysiology 2015; 52:1432-40. [PMID: 26265009 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12503] [Citation(s) in RCA: 85] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/03/2014] [Accepted: 06/30/2015] [Indexed: 01/17/2023]
Abstract
Heartbeat detection tasks are often used to measure cardiac interoceptive sensitivity-the ability to detect sensations from one's heart. However, there is little work to guide decisions on the optimum number of trials to use, which should balance reliability and power against task duration and participant burden. Here, 174 participants completed 100 trials of a widely used heartbeat detection task where participants attempt to detect whether presented tones occurred synchronously or asynchronously with their heartbeats. First, we quantified measurement reliability of the participant's accuracy derived from differing numbers of trials of the task using a correlation metric; we found that at least 40-60 trials were required to yield sufficient reliability. Next, we quantified power by simulating how the number of trials influenced the ability to detect a correlation between cardiac interoceptive sensitivity and other variables that differ across participants, including a variable measured from our sample (body mass index) as well as simulated variables of varying effect sizes. Using these simulations, we quantified the trade-offs between sample size, effect size, and number of trials in the heartbeat detection task such that a researcher can easily determine any one of these variables at given values of the other two variables. We conclude that using fewer than 40 trials is typically insufficient due to poor reliability and low power in estimating an effect size, although the optimal number of trials can differ by study.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ian R Kleckner
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
| | | | - W Kyle Simmons
- Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, Oklahoma, USA.,Faculty of Community Medicine, The University of Tulsa, Tulsa, Oklahoma, USA
| | - Lisa Feldman Barrett
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA.,Department of Psychiatry and the Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Harvard Medical School, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
| | - Karen S Quigley
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA.,Edith Nourse Rogers Memorial VA Hospital, Bedford, Massachusetts, USA
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
Moustafa AA, Gluck MA, Herzallah MM, Myers CE. The influence of trial order on learning from reward vs. punishment in a probabilistic categorization task: experimental and computational analyses. Front Behav Neurosci 2015; 9:153. [PMID: 26257616 PMCID: PMC4513240 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00153] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/05/2015] [Accepted: 05/26/2015] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Previous research has shown that trial ordering affects cognitive performance, but this has not been tested using category-learning tasks that differentiate learning from reward and punishment. Here, we tested two groups of healthy young adults using a probabilistic category learning task of reward and punishment in which there are two types of trials (reward, punishment) and three possible outcomes: (1) positive feedback for correct responses in reward trials; (2) negative feedback for incorrect responses in punishment trials; and (3) no feedback for incorrect answers in reward trials and correct answers in punishment trials. Hence, trials without feedback are ambiguous, and may represent either successful avoidance of punishment or failure to obtain reward. In Experiment 1, the first group of subjects received an intermixed task in which reward and punishment trials were presented in the same block, as a standard baseline task. In Experiment 2, a second group completed the separated task, in which reward and punishment trials were presented in separate blocks. Additionally, in order to understand the mechanisms underlying performance in the experimental conditions, we fit individual data using a Q-learning model. Results from Experiment 1 show that subjects who completed the intermixed task paradoxically valued the no-feedback outcome as a reinforcer when it occurred on reinforcement-based trials, and as a punisher when it occurred on punishment-based trials. This is supported by patterns of empirical responding, where subjects showed more win-stay behavior following an explicit reward than following an omission of punishment, and more lose-shift behavior following an explicit punisher than following an omission of reward. In Experiment 2, results showed similar performance whether subjects received reward-based or punishment-based trials first. However, when the Q-learning model was applied to these data, there were differences between subjects in the reward-first and punishment-first conditions on the relative weighting of neutral feedback. Specifically, early training on reward-based trials led to omission of reward being treated as similar to punishment, but prior training on punishment-based trials led to omission of reward being treated more neutrally. This suggests that early training on one type of trials, specifically reward-based trials, can create a bias in how neutral feedback is processed, relative to those receiving early punishment-based training or training that mixes positive and negative outcomes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ahmed A Moustafa
- School of Social Sciences and Psychology and Marcs Institute for Brain and Behaviour, University of Western Sydney Sydney, NSW, Australia ; Department of Veterans Affairs, New Jersey Health Care System East Orange, NJ, USA
| | - Mark A Gluck
- Center for Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience, Rutgers University Newark, NJ, USA
| | - Mohammad M Herzallah
- Center for Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience, Rutgers University Newark, NJ, USA ; Al-Quds Cognitive Neuroscience Lab, Palestinian Neuroscience Initiative, Faculty of Medicine, Al-Quds University Jerusalem, Palestine
| | - Catherine E Myers
- Department of Veterans Affairs, New Jersey Health Care System East Orange, NJ, USA ; Department of Pharmacology, Physiology and Neuroscience, Rutgers-New Jersey Medical School Newark, NJ, USA ; Department of Psychology, Rutgers University-Newark Newark, NJ, USA
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Schwartz CE, Ayandeh A, Rodgers JD, Duberstein P, Weinstock-Guttman B, Benedict RHB. A new perspective on proxy report: Investigating implicit processes of understanding through patient-proxy congruence. Qual Life Res 2015; 24:2637-49. [PMID: 26038218 DOI: 10.1007/s11136-015-1017-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 05/08/2015] [Indexed: 12/17/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Utilizing proxy report is a common solution to gathering quality-of-life information from people who are not capable of reliably answering questionnaires, such as people with dementia. Proxy report could, however, also provide information about patients' implicit processes of understanding, which we define as automatic, schema-driven cognitive processes that allow one to have a better understanding of oneself and of one's body, make oneself known and knowable to members of the social network, and allow one to react proactively in response to cues. We investigated whether implicit processes of understanding explain some of the association between reserve and healthy lifestyle behaviors. METHODS We operationalized three implicit processes of understanding: (a) psychosocial understanding; (b) insight into physical disability; and (c) somatic awareness. This secondary analysis involved a cohort of multiple sclerosis patients and their caregiver informants (n = 118 pairs). Measures included a neurologist-administered Expanded Disability Status Scale, patient- and informant-completed survey measures, and a heartbeat perception test (interoception). Patient-other congruence assessed implicit processes of understanding: psychosocial understanding (neurocognitive and personality); physical-disability insight; and somatic awareness (interoception). RESULTS Effect sizes (ES) for the inter-correlations between the three implicit processes were small. Psychosocial understanding was associated with higher past reserve-building activities (small ES). Psychosocial understanding explained variance in healthy lifestyle behaviors over and above the variance explained by current reserve-building activities (∆R (2) = 0.04; model R Adjusted (2) = 0.18). CONCLUSIONS Proxy versus patient report can provide information about underlying interpretational processes related to insight. These processes are distinct from reserve, predict health outcomes, and can inform lifestyle-changing interventions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carolyn E Schwartz
- DeltaQuest Foundation, Inc., 31 Mitchell Road, Concord, MA, 01742, USA.
- Departments of Medicine and Orthopaedic Surgery, Tufts University Medical School, Boston, MA, USA.
| | - Armon Ayandeh
- DeltaQuest Foundation, Inc., 31 Mitchell Road, Concord, MA, 01742, USA
| | - Jonathan D Rodgers
- Department of Neurology, School of Medicine and Biomedical Sciences, University at Buffalo, State University of New York, Buffalo, NY, USA
| | - Paul Duberstein
- Department of Psychiatry and Rochester Health Care Decision Making Group, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, NY, USA
| | - Bianca Weinstock-Guttman
- Department of Neurology, School of Medicine and Biomedical Sciences, University at Buffalo, State University of New York, Buffalo, NY, USA
| | - Ralph H B Benedict
- Department of Neurology, School of Medicine and Biomedical Sciences, University at Buffalo, State University of New York, Buffalo, NY, USA
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
Owens AP, David AS, Low DA, Mathias CJ, Sierra-Siegert M. Abnormal cardiovascular sympathetic and parasympathetic responses to physical and emotional stimuli in depersonalization disorder. Front Neurosci 2015; 9:89. [PMID: 25859177 PMCID: PMC4374468 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00089] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/31/2014] [Accepted: 03/03/2015] [Indexed: 12/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Depersonalization disorder (DPD) is characterized by a subjective sense of unreality, disembodiment, emotional numbing and reduced psychogenic (sudomotor) sympathoexcitation. Aims Three related experiments utilized escalating physical and emotional challenges in 14 DPD participants and 16 controls aimed to elucidate (i) whether the cardiovascular sympathetic (SNS) and parasympathetic (PNS) nervous systems are implicated in DPD pathophysiology and (ii) if possible, to determine whether the blunted sympathoexcitation in DPD is peripherally or centrally mediated. Method Participants completed the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI), Dissociative Experience Scale (DES), and Cambridge Depersonalization Scale (CDS). Study I recorded heart rate (HR) and blood pressure (BP) during 5 min supine baseline, 3 min sustained handgrip (HG), 3 min cold pressor (CP) and 5 min 60° head-up tilt (HUT). In study II, HR, BP, and heart rate variability (HRV) were recorded during 5 min simultaneous 60° HUT and continuous presentation of unpleasant images (5 s per image). Study III examined HR and BP orienting responses (ORs) to simultaneous 60° HUT and pseudorandom presentation of unpleasant, neutral and pleasant images (5 s per image 3 min 25 s). OR data was grouped by image valence post hoc. Results DPD BAI (p = 0.0004), DES (p = 0.0002), and CDS (p ≤ 0.0001) scores were higher than controls. The DPD group produced diminished diastolic BP (DBP) (p = 0.045) increases to HG. Other indices were comparable between groups. DPD participants produced diminished systolic BP (SBP) (p = 0.003) and DBP (p = 0.002) increases, but greater (p = 0.004) HR increases to CP. In study II, DPD high frequency HRV (HF-HRV)—indicating parasympathetic vagal activity–was reduced (p = 0.029). In study III, DPD DBP was higher throughout the 5 s duration of HUT/pseudorandom unpleasant image presentation (1 s, p = 0.002, 2 s p = 0.033, 3 s p = 0.001, 4 s p = 0.009, 5 s p = 0.029). Conclusions Study I's BP pressor data supports previous findings of suppressed sympathoexcitation in DPD. The greater HR increases to CP, decreased HF-HRV in study II, and increased DBP during unpleasant ORs in study III implicates the SNS and PNS in DPD pathophysiology. These studies suggest the cardiovascular autonomic dysregulation in DPD is likely to be centrally-mediated.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrew P Owens
- Autonomic and Neurovascular Medicine Unit, Institute of Neurology, Imperial College London London, UK ; Autonomic Unit, Institute of Neurology, University College London London, UK
| | - Anthony S David
- Section of Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London London, UK
| | - David A Low
- Autonomic and Neurovascular Medicine Unit, Institute of Neurology, Imperial College London London, UK ; School of Sport and Exercise Sciences, Liverpool John Moores University Liverpool, UK
| | - Christopher J Mathias
- Autonomic and Neurovascular Medicine Unit, Institute of Neurology, Imperial College London London, UK ; Autonomic Unit, Institute of Neurology, University College London London, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
38
|
Delaying in vivo exposure to a tarantula with very brief exposure to phobic stimuli. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2015; 46:182-8. [PMID: 25460265 DOI: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.10.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/24/2014] [Revised: 07/13/2014] [Accepted: 10/10/2014] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES Research has documented the very brief exposure (VBE) effect: the reduction of phobic fear by continuous presentation of masked phobic pictures. In prior studies, phobic participants approached a live tarantula immediately after the masked stimuli were presented. This study tested the hypothesis that VBE would reduce phobic avoidance of the tarantula 24 h later. METHOD 86 spider-phobic participants were identified with a fear questionnaire and Behavioral Avoidance Test (BAT) with a live tarantula indicative of a DSM-IV diagnosis of Specific Phobia. One week later, they were randomly assigned in double-blind fashion to presentation of a continuous series of 25 trials of masked images of either spiders or flowers (33-ms each), i.e., to VBE or control exposure. The participants gave subjective distress ratings just before and after these exposures. Then they engaged in the BAT again either immediately thereafter or 24 h later to measure changes in avoidance of the tarantula. RESULTS Masked images of spiders reduced avoidance of the tarantula both immediately after exposure and 24 h later without causing subjective distress. The effect sizes at these two time points did not significantly differ from each other. LIMITATIONS We did not manipulate awareness of the spider images by presenting them unmasked to a third group. Conclusions about the effect of awareness of the stimuli cannot be drawn. CONCLUSIONS VBE induces a process of fear reduction before phobic individuals engage in in vivo exposure, which is more distressing. Thus, VBE may help phobic-resistant individuals start treatment more gradually.
Collapse
|
39
|
Sueyoshi T, Sugimoto F, Katayama J, Fukushima H. Neural correlates of error processing reflect individual differences in interoceptive sensitivity. Int J Psychophysiol 2014; 94:278-86. [DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.10.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/08/2014] [Revised: 09/25/2014] [Accepted: 10/01/2014] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
|
40
|
Mallorquí-Bagué N, Garfinkel SN, Engels M, Eccles JA, Pailhez G, Bulbena A, Critchley HD. Neuroimaging and psychophysiological investigation of the link between anxiety, enhanced affective reactivity and interoception in people with joint hypermobility. Front Psychol 2014; 5:1162. [PMID: 25352818 PMCID: PMC4196473 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01162] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/20/2014] [Accepted: 09/24/2014] [Indexed: 12/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Objective: Anxiety is associated with increased physiological reactivity and also increased “interoceptive” sensitivity to such changes in internal bodily arousal. Joint hypermobility, an expression of a common variation in the connective tissue protein collagen, is increasingly recognized as a risk factor to anxiety and related disorders. This study explored the link between anxiety, interoceptive sensitivity and hypermobility in a sub-clinical population using neuroimaging and psychophysiological evaluation. Methods: Thirty-six healthy volunteers undertook interoceptive sensitivity tests, a clinical examination for hypermobility and completed validated questionnaire measures of state anxiety and body awareness tendency. Nineteen participants also performed an emotional processing paradigm during functional neuroimaging. Results: We confirmed a significant relationship between state anxiety score and joint hypermobility. Interoceptive sensitivity mediated the relationship between state anxiety and hypermobility. Hypermobile, compared to non-hypermobile, participants displayed heightened neural reactivity to sad and angry scenes within brain regions implicated in anxious feeling states, notably insular cortex. Conclusions: Our findings highlight the dependence of anxiety state on bodily context, and increase our understanding of the mechanisms through which vulnerability to anxiety disorders arises in people bearing a common variant of collagen.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Núria Mallorquí-Bagué
- Psychiatry, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, University of Sussex Falmer, UK ; Department of Psychiatry and Forensic Medicine, School of Medicine, Universitat Autonoma de Barcelona Barcelona, Spain ; Department of Psychiatry, Psychology and Psychosomatics, Hospital Universitari Quirón Dexeus Barcelona, Spain
| | - Sarah N Garfinkel
- Psychiatry, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, University of Sussex Falmer, UK ; Mood and Anxiety Research in Sussex (MARS), Sussex Partnership NHS Foundation Trust Sussex, UK ; Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, University of Sussex Falmer, UK
| | - Miriam Engels
- Psychiatry, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, University of Sussex Falmer, UK ; Clinical Psychological Science, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University Maastricht, Netherlands
| | - Jessica A Eccles
- Psychiatry, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, University of Sussex Falmer, UK ; Mood and Anxiety Research in Sussex (MARS), Sussex Partnership NHS Foundation Trust Sussex, UK
| | - Guillem Pailhez
- Department of Psychiatry and Forensic Medicine, School of Medicine, Universitat Autonoma de Barcelona Barcelona, Spain ; Anxiety Unit, Institute of Neuropsychiatry and Addictions, Hospital del Mar, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona Barcelona, Spain ; Hospital del Mar Medical Research Institute Barcelona, Spain
| | - Antonio Bulbena
- Department of Psychiatry and Forensic Medicine, School of Medicine, Universitat Autonoma de Barcelona Barcelona, Spain ; Anxiety Unit, Institute of Neuropsychiatry and Addictions, Hospital del Mar, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona Barcelona, Spain ; Hospital del Mar Medical Research Institute Barcelona, Spain
| | - Hugo D Critchley
- Psychiatry, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, University of Sussex Falmer, UK ; Mood and Anxiety Research in Sussex (MARS), Sussex Partnership NHS Foundation Trust Sussex, UK ; Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, University of Sussex Falmer, UK
| |
Collapse
|
41
|
Mazzola V, Marano G, Biganzoli EM, Boracchi P, Lanciano T, Arciero G, Bondolfi G. The In-Out dispositional affective style questionnaire (IN-OUT DASQ): an exploratory factorial analysis. Front Psychol 2014; 5:1005. [PMID: 25309478 PMCID: PMC4161060 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/11/2013] [Accepted: 08/23/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The issue of individual differences has always been an important area of research in psychology and, more recently, neuroimaging. A major source of interindividual variability stems from differences in basic affective dispositions. In order to make a contribution to this field of research, we have developed a new type of assessment - the In-Out dispositional affective style questionnaire (IN-OUT DASQ) - to measure the proneness between two different ways of feeling situated: a predominantly body-bound one in the case of the inward tendency and an externally anchored one in the case of the outward tendency (Arciero and Bondolfi, 2009). The IN-OUT DASQ contains two scales of seven items each, Self-centric engagement (SCE) and Other-centric engagement (OCE), as a disposition index for inwardness and outwardness respectively. The exploratory factor analysis in sample 1 (n = 292) confirmed a two-factor solution. Confirmatory factor analysis in sample 2 (n = 300) showed the good fit of this two-factor model. Next, we examined construct validity also investigating the correlations between the IN-OUT DASQ, the Big Five Questionnaire and the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule in sample 3 (n = 153). The SCE and OCE scales had robust internal consistency and reliability, though the capacity to discriminate higher inward and outward participants was stronger in SCE. Although further validation research is required, the present study suggests the IN-OUT DASQ has the potential to be a measurement tool for detecting individual differences in social behavior and social affective neuroscience.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Viridiana Mazzola
- Swiss Center for Affective Sciences - University of Geneva Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Giuseppe Marano
- Department of Clinical Sciences and Community Healt, University of Milan Milan, Italy
| | - Elia M Biganzoli
- Department of Clinical Sciences and Community Healt, University of Milan Milan, Italy ; Unit of Medical Statistics and Biometry, Fondazione Istituto Di Ricovero e Cura a Carattere Scientifico Istituto Nazionale dei Tumori Milan, Italy
| | - Patrizia Boracchi
- Department of Clinical Sciences and Community Healt, University of Milan Milan, Italy
| | - Tiziana Lanciano
- Institute of Post-Rationalist Psychology Rome, Italy ; Department of Education, Psychology, Communication Science, University of Bari Bari, Italy
| | - Giampiero Arciero
- Institute of Post-Rationalist Psychology Rome, Italy ; Department of Psychiatry, Hôpitaux Universitaires de Genève Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Guido Bondolfi
- Department of Psychiatry, Hôpitaux Universitaires de Genève Geneva, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
42
|
Stern ER. Neural Circuitry of Interoception: New Insights into Anxiety and Obsessive-Compulsive Disorders. CURRENT TREATMENT OPTIONS IN PSYCHIATRY 2014; 1:235-247. [PMID: 33344105 PMCID: PMC7747958 DOI: 10.1007/s40501-014-0019-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/02/2023]
Abstract
Over the past century, much research has investigated how the brain processes signals from the body (interoception) and how this processing may be disturbed in patients with psychiatric disorders. In this paper, I discuss the literature examining the relationship between interoceptive awareness and emotional and cognitive processes, and review the evidence suggesting that anxiety and obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) are characterized by abnormal interoception. A network of cortical and subcortical brain regions centered on the insula has repeatedly been implicated in interoception and is abnormal in anxiety and OCD. The investigation of interoception provides a framework for understanding behavioral and neural mechanisms of anxiety and OCD, although additional research is needed to directly link insula functioning to aberrant interoception in these disorders. Future work targeting interoception may be useful for the development of novel treatments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emily R. Stern
- Department of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, One Gustave Levy Place( Box 1230, New York, NY 11105, USA
| |
Collapse
|
43
|
Heartfelt imitation: High interoceptive awareness is linked to greater automatic imitation. Neuropsychologia 2014; 60:21-8. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.05.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 63] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/20/2014] [Revised: 04/23/2014] [Accepted: 05/16/2014] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
|
44
|
Sokol-Hessner P, Hartley CA, Hamilton JR, Phelps EA. Interoceptive ability predicts aversion to losses. Cogn Emot 2014; 29:695-701. [PMID: 24916358 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2014.925426] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
Emotions have been proposed to inform risky decision-making through the influence of affective physiological responses on subjective value. The ability to perceive internal body states, or "interoception" may influence this relationship. Here, we examined whether interoception predicts participants' degree of loss aversion, which has been previously linked to choice-related arousal responses. Participants performed both a heartbeat-detection task indexing interoception and a risky monetary decision-making task, from which loss aversion, risk attitudes and choice consistency were parametrically measured. Interoceptive ability correlated selectively with loss aversion and was unrelated to the other value parameters. This finding suggests that specific and separable component processes underlying valuation are shaped not only by our physiological responses, as shown in previous findings, but also by our interoceptive access to such signals.
Collapse
|
45
|
Núñez JP, de Vicente F. Unconscious Learning. Conditioning to Subliminal Visual Stimuli. SPANISH JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY 2014; 7:13-28. [PMID: 15139245 DOI: 10.1017/s1138741600004716] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
The role of consciousness in Pavlovian conditioning was examined in two experiments in which visually masked neutral words were used as the conditioned stimuli (CS) and an electric shock as the unconditioned stimulus (US). The inter-stimulus interval (ISI) was established individually. A detection threshold was used in Experiment 1 and an identification threshold in Experiment 2. The primary dependent variable was the skin conductance response (SCR). Results showed that the conditioned response (CR) was acquired by 58% of participants who perceived stimuli above the identification threshold, 50% of participants who perceived stimuli below the detection threshold, and 11% of participants who perceived stimuli below the identification threshold, but above the detection threshold. These results suggest that consciousness of the CS-US contingency is not a necessary condition for acquiring a CR of the autonomous nervous system (ANS).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Juan P Núñez
- Departamento de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales y Humanas, Universidad Pontificia de Comillas de Madrid, C/Universidad de Comillas no 3, 28049 Madrid, Spain.
| | | |
Collapse
|
46
|
Werner NS, Mannhart T, Reyes Del Paso GA, Duschek S. Attention interference for emotional stimuli in cardiac interoceptive awareness. Psychophysiology 2014; 51:573-8. [DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12200] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/08/2013] [Accepted: 01/16/2014] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Natalie S. Werner
- Department of Psychology; Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich; Munich Germany
| | - Tanja Mannhart
- Department of Psychology; Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich; Munich Germany
| | | | - Stefan Duschek
- UMIT-University for Health Sciences, Medical Informatics and Technology Hall; Tirol Austria
| |
Collapse
|
47
|
Abstract
While emotion is a central component of human health and well-being, traditional approaches to understanding its biological function have been wanting. A dynamic systems model, however, broadly redefines and recasts emotion as a primary sensory system-perhaps the first sensory system to have emerged, serving the ancient autopoietic function of "self-regulation." Drawing upon molecular biology and revelations from the field of epigenetics, the model suggests that human emotional perceptions provide an ongoing stream of "self-relevant" sensory information concerning optimally adaptive states between the organism and its immediate environment, along with coupled behavioral corrections that honor a universal self-regulatory logic, one still encoded within cellular signaling and immune functions. Exemplified by the fundamental molecular circuitry of sensorimotor control in the E coli bacterium, the model suggests that the hedonic (affective) categories emerge directly from positive and negative feedback processes, their good/bad binary appraisals relating to dual self-regulatory behavioral regimes-evolutionary purposes, through which organisms actively participate in natural selection, and through which humans can interpret optimal or deficit states of balanced being and becoming. The self-regulatory sensory paradigm transcends anthropomorphism, unites divergent theoretical perspectives and isolated bodies of literature, while challenging time-honored assumptions. While suppressive regulatory strategies abound, it suggests that emotions are better understood as regulating us, providing a service crucial to all semantic language, learning systems, evaluative decision-making, and fundamental to optimal physical, mental, and social health.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katherine T Peil
- College of Professional Studies, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts; Harvard Divinity School, Cambridge, Massachusetts, United States
| |
Collapse
|
48
|
Interoceptive awareness moderates neural activity during decision-making. Biol Psychol 2013; 94:498-506. [DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.09.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 49] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/31/2013] [Revised: 08/16/2013] [Accepted: 09/05/2013] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
|
49
|
Siegel P, Han E, Cohen D, Anderson J. A dissociation between detection and identification of phobic stimuli: Unconscious perception? Cogn Emot 2013; 27:1153-67. [DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2013.774264] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
|
50
|
Nentjes L, Meijer E, Bernstein D, Arntz A, Medendorp W. Brief communication: investigating the relationship between psychopathy and interoceptive awareness. J Pers Disord 2013; 27:617-24. [PMID: 23786270 DOI: 10.1521/pedi_2013_27_105] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Psychopathy is a disorder that is characterized by marked emotional deficiencies. Because previous studies suggest that an individual's sensitivity to bodily signals--or "interoceptive awareness"--is associated with various components of emotional functioning, the authors expected this capacity to be reduced in psychopathic individuals. Therefore, the current study examined the relationship between psychopathy and interoceptive awareness by assessing heartbeat detection abilities in a group of 75 male personality disordered offenders, varying in their degree of psychopathy, as assessed with the Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R; Hare, 2003). Regression analyses revealed that PCL-R Facet 4, which reflects antisocial behavior, was predictive of reduced interoceptive awareness. These findings suggest that the expression of psychopathic behavior might be influenced by an attenuated sensitivity to one's own bodily signals.
Collapse
|