1
|
Walther K, Reindl C, Schwarz M, Gollwitzer S, Kasper BS, Lang JD, Stritzelberger J, Brandner S, Rössler K, Zhao Y, Dörfler A, Hamer HM. Object naming after epilepsy surgery in the dominant left temporal lobe: risk factors, time course and long-term outcome. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2024:jnnp-2024-334491. [PMID: 39667909 DOI: 10.1136/jnnp-2024-334491] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/19/2024] [Accepted: 11/26/2024] [Indexed: 12/14/2024]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Deterioration in naming function is a common sequelae after epilepsy surgery in the language-dominant temporal lobe but information on recovery and long-term outcome is scarce. We, therefore, assessed short-term and long-term outcome of object naming in patients undergoing surgery in the temporal lobe and determined factors affecting deterioration and recovery of naming function. METHOD Object naming (Boston naming test) before surgery, at early follow-up (FU, 6-12 months) and late FU (≥2 years) was assessed in people with epilepsy (PWE) undergoing resections in the language-dominant left and non-dominant right temporal lobe. RESULTS Sixty-six patients with left temporal lobe epilepsy (LTLE) and 87 control patients with right temporal lobe epilepsy (RLTE) were included. At early FU, 28 patients with LTLE (42%) and three patients with RTLE (3%) showed a significant naming decline. In patients with LTLE, risk for deterioration increased with lower verbal memory before surgery, older age at seizure onset and was particularly high with posterior temporal resections (≥40 mm from the temporal pole) and seizure onset >16 years. Of the patients with LTLE with early naming decline, 11 patients (39%) recovered fully in their naming abilities at late FU, averaging almost 10 years. Recovery was associated with the degree of postoperative naming decline at early FU. PWE with a decline of less than 10 items (<20%) had a good prognosis of recovery at late FU. Postoperative seizure control had no significant effect on recovery. CONCLUSIONS In our cohort, less than 50% of PWE showed significantly deteriorated naming function after resection of the dominant temporal lobe. If a decline occurred, it appeared to recover to a certain degree and remained as a permanent deficit in 26% of the patients. Long-term outcome of visual object naming can be predicted by the degree of early postoperative decline.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katrin Walther
- Department of Neurology, Universitätsklinikum Erlangen, Erlangen, Bayern, Germany
| | - Caroline Reindl
- Department of Neurology, Universitätsklinikum Erlangen, Erlangen, Bayern, Germany
| | - Michael Schwarz
- Department of Neurology, Universitätsklinikum Erlangen, Erlangen, Bayern, Germany
| | - Stephanie Gollwitzer
- Department of Neurology, Universitätsklinikum Erlangen, Erlangen, Bayern, Germany
| | - Burkhard S Kasper
- Department of Neurology, Universitätsklinikum Erlangen, Erlangen, Bayern, Germany
| | | | - Jenny Stritzelberger
- Department of Neurology, Universitätsklinikum Erlangen, Erlangen, Bayern, Germany
| | - Sebastian Brandner
- Department of Neurosurgery, Universitätsklinikum Erlangen, Erlangen, Bayern, Germany
- Department of Neurosurgery, Klinikum Fürth, Fürth, Bayern, Germany
| | - Karl Rössler
- Department of Neurosurgery, Universitätsklinikum Erlangen, Erlangen, Bayern, Germany
- Department of Neurosurgery, University Hospital Vienna, Wien, Vienna, Austria
| | - Yining Zhao
- Department of Neurosurgery, Universitätsklinikum Erlangen, Erlangen, Bayern, Germany
| | - Arnd Dörfler
- Department of Neuroradiology, Universitätsklinikum Erlangen, Erlangen, Bayern, Germany
| | - Hajo M Hamer
- Department of Neurology, Universitätsklinikum Erlangen, Erlangen, Bayern, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Reindl C, Walther K, Allgäuer AL, Lang JD, Welte TM, Stritzelberger J, Gollwitzer S, Schwarz M, Trollmann R, Madzar D, Knott M, Doerfler A, Seifert F, Rössler K, Brandner S, Rampp S, Schwab S, Hamer HM. Age of epilepsy onset as modulating factor for naming deficit after epilepsy surgery: a voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping study. Sci Rep 2023; 13:14395. [PMID: 37658152 PMCID: PMC10474263 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-40722-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/13/2023] [Accepted: 08/16/2023] [Indexed: 09/03/2023] Open
Abstract
Age at onset of epilepsy is an important predictor of deterioration in naming ability following epilepsy surgery. In 141 patients with left hemispheric epilepsy and language dominance who received epilepsy surgery at the Epilepsy Centre Erlangen, naming of objects (Boston naming test, BNT) was assessed preoperatively and 6 months postoperatively. Surgical lesions were plotted on postoperative MRI and normalized for statistical analysis using voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping (VBLSM). The correlation between lesion and presence of postoperative naming deterioration was examined varying the considered age range of epilepsy onsets. The VBLSM analysis showed that volumes of cortex areas in the left temporal lobe, which were associated with postoperative decline of naming, increased with each year of later epilepsy onset. In patients with later onset, an increasing left posterior temporobasal area was significantly associated with a postoperative deficit when included in the resection. For late epilepsy onset, the temporomesial expansion also included the left hippocampus. The results underline that early onset of epilepsy is a good prognostic factor for unchanged postoperative naming ability following epilepsy surgery. For later age of epilepsy onset, the extent of the area at risk of postoperative naming deficit at 6 months after surgery included an increasing left temporobasal area which finally also comprised the hippocampus.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Caroline Reindl
- Epilepsy Center Department of Neurology, University Hospital Erlangen, Schwabachanlage 6, 91054, Erlangen, Germany.
| | - Katrin Walther
- Epilepsy Center Department of Neurology, University Hospital Erlangen, Schwabachanlage 6, 91054, Erlangen, Germany
| | - Anna-Lena Allgäuer
- Epilepsy Center Department of Neurology, University Hospital Erlangen, Schwabachanlage 6, 91054, Erlangen, Germany
| | - Johannes D Lang
- Epilepsy Center Department of Neurology, University Hospital Erlangen, Schwabachanlage 6, 91054, Erlangen, Germany
| | - Tamara M Welte
- Epilepsy Center Department of Neurology, University Hospital Erlangen, Schwabachanlage 6, 91054, Erlangen, Germany
| | - Jenny Stritzelberger
- Epilepsy Center Department of Neurology, University Hospital Erlangen, Schwabachanlage 6, 91054, Erlangen, Germany
| | - Stephanie Gollwitzer
- Epilepsy Center Department of Neurology, University Hospital Erlangen, Schwabachanlage 6, 91054, Erlangen, Germany
| | - Michael Schwarz
- Epilepsy Center Department of Neurology, University Hospital Erlangen, Schwabachanlage 6, 91054, Erlangen, Germany
| | - Regina Trollmann
- Department of Neuropaediatrics, University Hospital Erlangen, Erlangen, Germany
| | - Dominik Madzar
- Department of Neurology, University Hospital Erlangen, Erlangen, Germany
| | - Michael Knott
- Department of Neuroradiology, University Hospital Erlangen, Erlangen, Germany
| | - Arnd Doerfler
- Department of Neuroradiology, University Hospital Erlangen, Erlangen, Germany
| | - Frank Seifert
- Department of Neurology, University Hospital Erlangen, Erlangen, Germany
| | - Karl Rössler
- Department of Neurosurgery, University Hospital Vienna (AKH), Vienna, Austria
| | - Sebastian Brandner
- Department of Neurosurgery, University Hospital Erlangen, Erlangen, Germany
| | - Stefan Rampp
- Department of Neurosurgery, University Hospital Erlangen, Erlangen, Germany
- Department of Neuroradiology, University Hospital Erlangen, Erlangen, Germany
- Department of Neurosurgery, University Hospital Halle (Saale), Halle, Germany
| | - Stefan Schwab
- Department of Neurology, University Hospital Erlangen, Erlangen, Germany
| | - Hajo M Hamer
- Epilepsy Center Department of Neurology, University Hospital Erlangen, Schwabachanlage 6, 91054, Erlangen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Roger E, Torlay L, Banjac S, Mosca C, Minotti L, Kahane P, Baciu M. Prediction of the clinical and naming status after anterior temporal lobe resection in patients with epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav 2021; 124:108357. [PMID: 34717247 DOI: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2021.108357] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/25/2021] [Revised: 09/15/2021] [Accepted: 09/25/2021] [Indexed: 01/20/2023]
Abstract
By assessing the cognitive capital, neuropsychological evaluation (NPE) plays a vital role in the perioperative workup of patients with refractory focal epilepsy. In this retrospective study, we used cutting-edge statistical approaches to examine a group of 47 patients with refractory temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE), who underwent standard anterior temporal lobectomy (ATL). Our objective was to determine whether NPE may represent a robust predictor of the postoperative status, two years after surgery. Specifically, based on pre- and postsurgical neuropsychological data, we estimated the sensitivity of cognitive indicators to predict and to disentangle phenotypes associated with more or less favorable outcomes. Engel (ENG) scores were used to assess clinical outcome, and picture naming (NAM) performance to estimate naming status. Two methods were applied: (a) machine learning (ML) to explore cognitive sensitivity to postoperative outcomes; and (b) graph theory (GT) to assess network properties reflecting favorable vs. less favorable phenotypes after surgery. Specific neuropsychological indices assessing language, memory, and executive functions can globally predict outcomes. Interestingly, preoperative cognitive networks associated with poor postsurgical outcome already exhibit an atypical, highly modular and less densely interconnected configuration. We provide statistical and clinical tools to anticipate the condition after surgery and achieve a more personalized clinical management. Our results also shed light on possible mechanisms put in place for cognitive adaptation after acute injury of central nervous system in relation with surgery.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elise Roger
- Univ. Grenoble Alpes, CNRS LPNC UMR 5105, 38000 Grenoble, France.
| | - Laurent Torlay
- Univ. Grenoble Alpes, CNRS LPNC UMR 5105, 38000 Grenoble, France
| | - Sonja Banjac
- Univ. Grenoble Alpes, CNRS LPNC UMR 5105, 38000 Grenoble, France
| | - Chrystèle Mosca
- Univ. Grenoble Alpes, Grenoble Institute of Neuroscience, Synchronisation et modulation des réseaux neuronaux dans l'épilepsie' & Neurology Department, 38000 Grenoble, France
| | - Lorella Minotti
- Univ. Grenoble Alpes, Grenoble Institute of Neuroscience, Synchronisation et modulation des réseaux neuronaux dans l'épilepsie' & Neurology Department, 38000 Grenoble, France
| | - Philippe Kahane
- Univ. Grenoble Alpes, Grenoble Institute of Neuroscience, Synchronisation et modulation des réseaux neuronaux dans l'épilepsie' & Neurology Department, 38000 Grenoble, France
| | - Monica Baciu
- Univ. Grenoble Alpes, CNRS LPNC UMR 5105, 38000 Grenoble, France
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Drane DL, Pedersen NP. Knowledge of language function and underlying neural networks gained from focal seizures and epilepsy surgery. BRAIN AND LANGUAGE 2019; 189:20-33. [PMID: 30615986 PMCID: PMC7183240 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandl.2018.12.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/22/2016] [Revised: 09/05/2018] [Accepted: 12/19/2018] [Indexed: 05/09/2023]
Abstract
The effects of epilepsy and its treatments have contributed significantly to language models. The setting of epilepsy surgery, which allows for careful pre- and postsurgical evaluation of patients with cognitive testing and neuroimaging, has produced a wealth of language findings. Moreover, a new wave of surgical interventions, including stereotactic laser ablation and radio frequency ablation, have contributed new insights and corrections to language models as they can make extremely precise, focal lesions. This review covers the common language deficits observed in focal dyscognitive seizure syndromes. It also addresses the effects of surgical interventions on language, and highlights insights gained from unique epilepsy assessment methods (e.g., cortical stimulation mapping, Wada evaluation). Emergent findings are covered including a lack of involvement of the hippocampus in confrontation word retrieval, possible roles for key white matter tracts in language, and the often-overlooked basal temporal language area. The relationship between language and semantic memory networks is also explored, with brief consideration given to the prevailing models of semantic processing, including the amodal Hub and distributed, multi-modal processing models.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel L Drane
- Department of Neurology, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA; Department of Pediatrics, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA; Department of Neurology, University of Washington School of Medicine, Seattle, WA, USA.
| | - Nigel P Pedersen
- Department of Neurology, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Busch RM, Hogue O, Kattan MW, Hamberger M, Drane DL, Hermann B, Kim M, Ferguson L, Bingaman W, Gonzalez-Martinez J, Najm IM, Jehi L. Nomograms to predict naming decline after temporal lobe surgery in adults with epilepsy. Neurology 2018; 91:e2144-e2152. [PMID: 30404781 DOI: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000006629] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/22/2018] [Accepted: 08/17/2018] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE To develop and externally validate models to predict the probability of postoperative naming decline in adults following temporal lobe epilepsy surgery using easily accessible preoperative clinical predictors. METHODS In this retrospective, prediction model development study, multivariable models were developed in a cohort of 719 patients who underwent temporal lobe epilepsy surgery at Cleveland Clinic and externally validated in a cohort of 138 patients who underwent temporal lobe surgery at one of 3 epilepsy surgery centers in the United States (Columbia University Medical Center, Emory University School of Medicine, University of Washington School of Medicine). RESULTS The development cohort was 54% female with an average age at surgery of 36 years (SD 12). Twenty-six percent of this cohort experienced clinically relevant postoperative naming decline. The model included 5 variables: side of surgery, age at epilepsy onset, age at surgery, sex, and education. When applied to the external validation cohort, the model performed very well, with excellent calibration and a c statistic (reflecting discriminatory ability) of 0.81. A second model predicting moderate to severe postoperative naming decline included 3 variables: side of surgery, age at epilepsy onset, and preoperative naming score. This model generated a c statistic of 0.84 in the external validation cohort and showed good calibration. CONCLUSION Externally validated nomograms are provided in 2 easy-to-use formats (paper version and online calculator) clinicians can use to estimate the probability of naming decline in patients considering epilepsy surgery for treatment of pharmacoresistant temporal lobe epilepsy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robyn M Busch
- From the Epilepsy Center (R.M.B., L.F., W.B., J.G.-M., I.M.N., L.J.), Department of Psychiatry & Psychology (R.M.B., L.F.), Department of Neurology (R.M.B., I.M.N., L.J.), Neurological Institute, and Department of Quantitative Health Sciences (O.H., M.W.K.), Cleveland Clinic, OH; Department of Neurology (M.H.), Columbia University, New York, NY; Department of Neurology and Pediatrics (D.L.D.), Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA; Department of Neurology (B.H.), University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, Madison; and Department of Neurology (D.L.D., M.K.), University of Washington School of Medicine, Seattle.
| | - Olivia Hogue
- From the Epilepsy Center (R.M.B., L.F., W.B., J.G.-M., I.M.N., L.J.), Department of Psychiatry & Psychology (R.M.B., L.F.), Department of Neurology (R.M.B., I.M.N., L.J.), Neurological Institute, and Department of Quantitative Health Sciences (O.H., M.W.K.), Cleveland Clinic, OH; Department of Neurology (M.H.), Columbia University, New York, NY; Department of Neurology and Pediatrics (D.L.D.), Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA; Department of Neurology (B.H.), University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, Madison; and Department of Neurology (D.L.D., M.K.), University of Washington School of Medicine, Seattle
| | - Michael W Kattan
- From the Epilepsy Center (R.M.B., L.F., W.B., J.G.-M., I.M.N., L.J.), Department of Psychiatry & Psychology (R.M.B., L.F.), Department of Neurology (R.M.B., I.M.N., L.J.), Neurological Institute, and Department of Quantitative Health Sciences (O.H., M.W.K.), Cleveland Clinic, OH; Department of Neurology (M.H.), Columbia University, New York, NY; Department of Neurology and Pediatrics (D.L.D.), Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA; Department of Neurology (B.H.), University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, Madison; and Department of Neurology (D.L.D., M.K.), University of Washington School of Medicine, Seattle
| | - Marla Hamberger
- From the Epilepsy Center (R.M.B., L.F., W.B., J.G.-M., I.M.N., L.J.), Department of Psychiatry & Psychology (R.M.B., L.F.), Department of Neurology (R.M.B., I.M.N., L.J.), Neurological Institute, and Department of Quantitative Health Sciences (O.H., M.W.K.), Cleveland Clinic, OH; Department of Neurology (M.H.), Columbia University, New York, NY; Department of Neurology and Pediatrics (D.L.D.), Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA; Department of Neurology (B.H.), University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, Madison; and Department of Neurology (D.L.D., M.K.), University of Washington School of Medicine, Seattle
| | - Daniel L Drane
- From the Epilepsy Center (R.M.B., L.F., W.B., J.G.-M., I.M.N., L.J.), Department of Psychiatry & Psychology (R.M.B., L.F.), Department of Neurology (R.M.B., I.M.N., L.J.), Neurological Institute, and Department of Quantitative Health Sciences (O.H., M.W.K.), Cleveland Clinic, OH; Department of Neurology (M.H.), Columbia University, New York, NY; Department of Neurology and Pediatrics (D.L.D.), Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA; Department of Neurology (B.H.), University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, Madison; and Department of Neurology (D.L.D., M.K.), University of Washington School of Medicine, Seattle
| | - Bruce Hermann
- From the Epilepsy Center (R.M.B., L.F., W.B., J.G.-M., I.M.N., L.J.), Department of Psychiatry & Psychology (R.M.B., L.F.), Department of Neurology (R.M.B., I.M.N., L.J.), Neurological Institute, and Department of Quantitative Health Sciences (O.H., M.W.K.), Cleveland Clinic, OH; Department of Neurology (M.H.), Columbia University, New York, NY; Department of Neurology and Pediatrics (D.L.D.), Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA; Department of Neurology (B.H.), University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, Madison; and Department of Neurology (D.L.D., M.K.), University of Washington School of Medicine, Seattle
| | - Michelle Kim
- From the Epilepsy Center (R.M.B., L.F., W.B., J.G.-M., I.M.N., L.J.), Department of Psychiatry & Psychology (R.M.B., L.F.), Department of Neurology (R.M.B., I.M.N., L.J.), Neurological Institute, and Department of Quantitative Health Sciences (O.H., M.W.K.), Cleveland Clinic, OH; Department of Neurology (M.H.), Columbia University, New York, NY; Department of Neurology and Pediatrics (D.L.D.), Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA; Department of Neurology (B.H.), University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, Madison; and Department of Neurology (D.L.D., M.K.), University of Washington School of Medicine, Seattle
| | - Lisa Ferguson
- From the Epilepsy Center (R.M.B., L.F., W.B., J.G.-M., I.M.N., L.J.), Department of Psychiatry & Psychology (R.M.B., L.F.), Department of Neurology (R.M.B., I.M.N., L.J.), Neurological Institute, and Department of Quantitative Health Sciences (O.H., M.W.K.), Cleveland Clinic, OH; Department of Neurology (M.H.), Columbia University, New York, NY; Department of Neurology and Pediatrics (D.L.D.), Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA; Department of Neurology (B.H.), University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, Madison; and Department of Neurology (D.L.D., M.K.), University of Washington School of Medicine, Seattle
| | - William Bingaman
- From the Epilepsy Center (R.M.B., L.F., W.B., J.G.-M., I.M.N., L.J.), Department of Psychiatry & Psychology (R.M.B., L.F.), Department of Neurology (R.M.B., I.M.N., L.J.), Neurological Institute, and Department of Quantitative Health Sciences (O.H., M.W.K.), Cleveland Clinic, OH; Department of Neurology (M.H.), Columbia University, New York, NY; Department of Neurology and Pediatrics (D.L.D.), Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA; Department of Neurology (B.H.), University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, Madison; and Department of Neurology (D.L.D., M.K.), University of Washington School of Medicine, Seattle
| | - Jorge Gonzalez-Martinez
- From the Epilepsy Center (R.M.B., L.F., W.B., J.G.-M., I.M.N., L.J.), Department of Psychiatry & Psychology (R.M.B., L.F.), Department of Neurology (R.M.B., I.M.N., L.J.), Neurological Institute, and Department of Quantitative Health Sciences (O.H., M.W.K.), Cleveland Clinic, OH; Department of Neurology (M.H.), Columbia University, New York, NY; Department of Neurology and Pediatrics (D.L.D.), Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA; Department of Neurology (B.H.), University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, Madison; and Department of Neurology (D.L.D., M.K.), University of Washington School of Medicine, Seattle
| | - Imad M Najm
- From the Epilepsy Center (R.M.B., L.F., W.B., J.G.-M., I.M.N., L.J.), Department of Psychiatry & Psychology (R.M.B., L.F.), Department of Neurology (R.M.B., I.M.N., L.J.), Neurological Institute, and Department of Quantitative Health Sciences (O.H., M.W.K.), Cleveland Clinic, OH; Department of Neurology (M.H.), Columbia University, New York, NY; Department of Neurology and Pediatrics (D.L.D.), Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA; Department of Neurology (B.H.), University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, Madison; and Department of Neurology (D.L.D., M.K.), University of Washington School of Medicine, Seattle
| | - Lara Jehi
- From the Epilepsy Center (R.M.B., L.F., W.B., J.G.-M., I.M.N., L.J.), Department of Psychiatry & Psychology (R.M.B., L.F.), Department of Neurology (R.M.B., I.M.N., L.J.), Neurological Institute, and Department of Quantitative Health Sciences (O.H., M.W.K.), Cleveland Clinic, OH; Department of Neurology (M.H.), Columbia University, New York, NY; Department of Neurology and Pediatrics (D.L.D.), Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA; Department of Neurology (B.H.), University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, Madison; and Department of Neurology (D.L.D., M.K.), University of Washington School of Medicine, Seattle
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Busch RM, Floden DP, Prayson B, Chapin JS, Kim KH, Ferguson L, Bingaman W, Najm IM. Estimating risk of word-finding problems in adults undergoing epilepsy surgery. Neurology 2016; 87:2363-2369. [PMID: 27815406 DOI: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000003378] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/16/2016] [Accepted: 08/22/2016] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE This retrospective, observational study examined the frequency and magnitude of change in naming ability as a function of side/site of epilepsy surgery and identified predictive factors to assist clinicians in identifying patients at low, moderate, or high risk of postoperative naming decline. METHODS A total of 875 adults with pharmacoresistant epilepsy (454 left/421 right; 763 temporal/87 frontal/25 posterior quadrant) met inclusion criteria and completed the Boston Naming Test before and after surgery. Clinically meaningful change in naming ability was assessed using reliable change indices for epilepsy. Demographic, cognitive, and seizure variables were examined to determine factors most predictive of naming decline and to develop a decision tree to assist with clinical decision-making. RESULTS Naming decline was rare in right-sided resections and did not exceed the level expected by chance (5% overall; 90% confidence interval [CI] ± 2%). Naming decline occurred in 41% (CI ± 5%) of patients after left temporal resection (TLR) compared to 10%-12% (CI ± 10%-19%) in other left-sided surgical groups. A sizable proportion of left TLR patients (17%; CI ± 4%) showed substantial declines in naming (>11 points). Decline following left TLR was related to later age at seizure onset, older age at surgery, and higher preoperative naming ability. These factors correctly predicted naming decline in 68% of patients and were associated with degree of decline following left TLR. A decision tree is provided to assist clinicians in identifying patients at low, moderate, or high risk for postoperative naming declines. CONCLUSIONS In addition to discussions regarding risk for memory decline following left TLR, patients should be counseled about potential decline in word-finding ability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robyn M Busch
- From the Epilepsy Center (R.M.B., W.B., I.M.N., L.F.), Department of Psychiatry & Psychology (R.M.B., D.P.F., L.F.), and Center for Neurological Restoration (D.P.F.), Neurological Institute, Cleveland Clinic, OH; Wellesley College (B.P.), MA; Behavioral Health Services (J.S.C.), Aurora Health Care, Grafton, WI; and Department of Psychology in Education (K.H.K.), University of Pittsburgh, PA.
| | - Darlene P Floden
- From the Epilepsy Center (R.M.B., W.B., I.M.N., L.F.), Department of Psychiatry & Psychology (R.M.B., D.P.F., L.F.), and Center for Neurological Restoration (D.P.F.), Neurological Institute, Cleveland Clinic, OH; Wellesley College (B.P.), MA; Behavioral Health Services (J.S.C.), Aurora Health Care, Grafton, WI; and Department of Psychology in Education (K.H.K.), University of Pittsburgh, PA
| | - Brigid Prayson
- From the Epilepsy Center (R.M.B., W.B., I.M.N., L.F.), Department of Psychiatry & Psychology (R.M.B., D.P.F., L.F.), and Center for Neurological Restoration (D.P.F.), Neurological Institute, Cleveland Clinic, OH; Wellesley College (B.P.), MA; Behavioral Health Services (J.S.C.), Aurora Health Care, Grafton, WI; and Department of Psychology in Education (K.H.K.), University of Pittsburgh, PA
| | - Jessica S Chapin
- From the Epilepsy Center (R.M.B., W.B., I.M.N., L.F.), Department of Psychiatry & Psychology (R.M.B., D.P.F., L.F.), and Center for Neurological Restoration (D.P.F.), Neurological Institute, Cleveland Clinic, OH; Wellesley College (B.P.), MA; Behavioral Health Services (J.S.C.), Aurora Health Care, Grafton, WI; and Department of Psychology in Education (K.H.K.), University of Pittsburgh, PA
| | - Kevin H Kim
- From the Epilepsy Center (R.M.B., W.B., I.M.N., L.F.), Department of Psychiatry & Psychology (R.M.B., D.P.F., L.F.), and Center for Neurological Restoration (D.P.F.), Neurological Institute, Cleveland Clinic, OH; Wellesley College (B.P.), MA; Behavioral Health Services (J.S.C.), Aurora Health Care, Grafton, WI; and Department of Psychology in Education (K.H.K.), University of Pittsburgh, PA
| | - Lisa Ferguson
- From the Epilepsy Center (R.M.B., W.B., I.M.N., L.F.), Department of Psychiatry & Psychology (R.M.B., D.P.F., L.F.), and Center for Neurological Restoration (D.P.F.), Neurological Institute, Cleveland Clinic, OH; Wellesley College (B.P.), MA; Behavioral Health Services (J.S.C.), Aurora Health Care, Grafton, WI; and Department of Psychology in Education (K.H.K.), University of Pittsburgh, PA
| | - William Bingaman
- From the Epilepsy Center (R.M.B., W.B., I.M.N., L.F.), Department of Psychiatry & Psychology (R.M.B., D.P.F., L.F.), and Center for Neurological Restoration (D.P.F.), Neurological Institute, Cleveland Clinic, OH; Wellesley College (B.P.), MA; Behavioral Health Services (J.S.C.), Aurora Health Care, Grafton, WI; and Department of Psychology in Education (K.H.K.), University of Pittsburgh, PA
| | - Imad M Najm
- From the Epilepsy Center (R.M.B., W.B., I.M.N., L.F.), Department of Psychiatry & Psychology (R.M.B., D.P.F., L.F.), and Center for Neurological Restoration (D.P.F.), Neurological Institute, Cleveland Clinic, OH; Wellesley College (B.P.), MA; Behavioral Health Services (J.S.C.), Aurora Health Care, Grafton, WI; and Department of Psychology in Education (K.H.K.), University of Pittsburgh, PA
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Abel TJ, Rhone AE, Nourski KV, Ando TK, Oya H, Kovach CK, Kawasaki H, Howard MA, Tranel D. Beta modulation reflects name retrieval in the human anterior temporal lobe: an intracranial recording study. J Neurophysiol 2016; 115:3052-61. [PMID: 27075536 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00012.2016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/05/2016] [Accepted: 03/18/2016] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Naming people, places, and things is a fundamental human ability that is often impaired in patients with language-dominant anterior temporal lobe (ATL) dysfunction or ATL resection as part of epilepsy treatment. Convergent lines of evidence point to the importance of the ATL in name retrieval. The physiologic mechanisms that mediate name retrieval in the ATL, however, are not well understood. The purpose of this study was to characterize the electrophysiologic responses of the human ATL during overt cued naming of famous people and objects. Eight neurosurgical patients with suspected temporal lobe epilepsy who underwent implantation of intracranial electrodes for seizure focus localization were the subjects of this study. Specialized coverage of the ATL was achieved in each subject. The subjects named pictures of U.S. presidents and images of common hand-held tools. Event-related band power was measured for each ATL recording site. Both the left and right ATL demonstrated robust and focal increases in beta-band (14-30 Hz) power during person and tool naming. The onset of this response typically occurred at 400 ms but sometimes as early as 200 ms. Visual naming of famous people and tools is associated with robust and localized modulation of the beta band in both the left and right ATL. Measurement of visual naming responses may provide the groundwork for future mapping modalities to localize eloquent cortex in the ATL.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Taylor J Abel
- Department of Neurosurgery, University of Iowa Hospitals and Clinics, Iowa City, Iowa; and Department of Neurology, University of Iowa Hospitals and Clinics, Iowa City, Iowa
| | - Ariane E Rhone
- Department of Neurosurgery, University of Iowa Hospitals and Clinics, Iowa City, Iowa; and
| | - Kirill V Nourski
- Department of Neurosurgery, University of Iowa Hospitals and Clinics, Iowa City, Iowa; and
| | - Timothy K Ando
- Department of Neurosurgery, University of Iowa Hospitals and Clinics, Iowa City, Iowa; and
| | - Hiroyuki Oya
- Department of Neurosurgery, University of Iowa Hospitals and Clinics, Iowa City, Iowa; and
| | - Christopher K Kovach
- Department of Neurosurgery, University of Iowa Hospitals and Clinics, Iowa City, Iowa; and
| | - Hiroto Kawasaki
- Department of Neurosurgery, University of Iowa Hospitals and Clinics, Iowa City, Iowa; and
| | - Matthew A Howard
- Department of Neurosurgery, University of Iowa Hospitals and Clinics, Iowa City, Iowa; and
| | - Daniel Tranel
- Department of Neurology, University of Iowa Hospitals and Clinics, Iowa City, Iowa
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Abel TJ, Rhone AE, Nourski KV, Howard MA, Tranel D. Investigating the Anterior Temporal Lobe With Direct Intracranial Recordings. Neurosurgery 2015; 62 Suppl 1:185-9. [PMID: 26181942 DOI: 10.1227/neu.0000000000000789] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Taylor J Abel
- Departments of *Neurosurgery and ‡Neurology and Psychology, University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Miró J, Ripollés P, López-Barroso D, Vilà-Balló A, Juncadella M, de Diego-Balaguer R, Marco-Pallares J, Rodríguez-Fornells A, Falip M. Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm. BMC Neurol 2014; 14:98. [PMID: 24885511 PMCID: PMC4017227 DOI: 10.1186/1471-2377-14-98] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/17/2013] [Accepted: 03/17/2014] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Mesial temporal lobe epilepsy (MTLE) is the most common type of focal epilepsy in adults and can be successfully cured by surgery. One of the main complications of this surgery however is a decline in language abilities. The magnitude of this decline is related to the degree of language lateralization to the left hemisphere. Most fMRI paradigms used to determine language dominance in epileptic populations have used active language tasks. Sometimes, these paradigms are too complex and may result in patient underperformance. Only a few studies have used purely passive tasks, such as listening to standard speech. Methods In the present study we characterized language lateralization in patients with MTLE using a rapid and passive semantic language task. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to study 23 patients [12 with Left (LMTLE), 11 with Right mesial temporal lobe epilepsy (RMTLE)] and 19 healthy right-handed controls using a 6 minute long semantic task in which subjects passively listened to groups of sentences (SEN) and pseudo sentences (PSEN). A lateralization index (LI) was computed using a priori regions of interest of the temporal lobe. Results The LI for the significant contrasts produced activations for all participants in both temporal lobes. 81.8% of RMTLE patients and 79% of healthy individuals had a bilateral language representation for this particular task. However, 50% of LMTLE patients presented an atypical right hemispheric dominance in the LI. More importantly, the degree of right lateralization in LMTLE patients was correlated with the age of epilepsy onset. Conclusions The simple, rapid, non-collaboration dependent, passive task described in this study, produces a robust activation in the temporal lobe in both patients and controls and is capable of illustrating a pattern of atypical language organization for LMTLE patients. Furthermore, we observed that the atypical right-lateralization patterns in LMTLE patients was associated to earlier age at epilepsy onset. These results are in line with the idea that early onset of epileptic activity is associated to larger neuroplastic changes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Júlia Miró
- Cognition and Brain Plasticity Group [Bellvitge Biomedical Research Institute]- IDIBELL, L'Hospitalet de Llobregat, Barcelona 08907, Spain.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Abel TJ, Rhone AE, Nourski KV, Granner MA, Oya H, Griffiths TD, Tranel DT, Kawasaki H, Howard MA. Mapping the temporal pole with a specialized electrode array: technique and preliminary results. Physiol Meas 2014; 35:323-37. [PMID: 24480831 DOI: 10.1088/0967-3334/35/3/323] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022]
Abstract
Temporopolar cortex plays a crucial role in the pathogenesis of temporal lobe epilepsy and subserves important cognitive functions. Because of its shape and position in the middle cranial fossa, complete electrode coverage of the temporal pole (TP) is difficult to achieve using existing devices. We designed a novel TP electrode array that conforms to the surface of temporopolar cortex and achieves dense electrode coverage of this important brain region. A multi-pronged electrode array was designed that can be placed over the surface of the TP using a straightforward insertion technique. Twelve patients with medically intractable epilepsy were implanted with the TP electrode array for purposes of seizure localization. Select patients underwent cognitive mapping by electrocorticographic (ECoG) recording from the TP during a naming task. Use of the array resulted in excellent TP electrode coverage in all patients. High quality ECoG data were consistently obtained for purposes of delineating seizure activity and functional mapping. During a naming task, significant increases in ECoG power were observed within localized subregions of the TP. One patient developed a transient neurological deficit thought to be related to the mass effect of multiple intracranial recording arrays, including the TP array. This deficit resolved following removal of all electrodes. The TP electrode array overcomes limitations of existing devices and enables clinicians and researchers to obtain optimal multi-site recordings from this important brain region.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Taylor J Abel
- Department of Neurosurgery, University of Iowa Hospitals and Clinics, 200 Hawkins Drive, Iowa City, IA 52242, USA
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Drane DL, Ojemann JG, Phatak V, Loring DW, Gross RE, Hebb AO, Silbergeld DL, Miller JW, Voets NL, Saindane AM, Barsalou L, Meador KJ, Ojemann GA, Tranel D. Famous face identification in temporal lobe epilepsy: support for a multimodal integration model of semantic memory. Cortex 2013; 49:1648-67. [PMID: 23040175 PMCID: PMC3679345 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.08.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 80] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/06/2012] [Revised: 04/26/2012] [Accepted: 08/22/2012] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
Abstract
This study aims to demonstrate that the left and right anterior temporal lobes (ATLs) perform critical but unique roles in famous face identification, with damage to either leading to differing deficit patterns reflecting decreased access to lexical or semantic concepts but not their degradation. Famous face identification was studied in 22 presurgical and 14 postsurgical temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) patients and 20 healthy comparison subjects using free recall and multiple choice (MC) paradigms. Right TLE patients exhibited presurgical deficits in famous face recognition, and postsurgical deficits in both famous face recognition and familiarity judgments. However, they did not exhibit any problems with naming before or after surgery. In contrast, left TLE patients demonstrated both pre- and postsurgical deficits in famous face naming but no significant deficits in recognition or familiarity. Double dissociations in performance between groups were alleviated by altering task demands. Postsurgical right TLE patients provided with MC options correctly identified greater than 70% of famous faces they initially rated as unfamiliar. Left TLE patients accurately chose the name for nearly all famous faces they recognized (based on their verbal description) but initially failed to name, although they tended to rapidly lose access to this name. We believe alterations in task demands activate alternative routes to semantic and lexical networks, demonstrating that unique pathways to such stored information exist, and suggesting a different role for each ATL in identifying visually presented famous faces. The right ATL appears to play a fundamental role in accessing semantic information from a visual route, with the left ATL serving to link semantic information to the language system to produce a specific name. These findings challenge several assumptions underlying amodal models of semantic memory, and provide support for the integrated multimodal theories of semantic memory and a distributed representation of concepts.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel L Drane
- Department of Neurology, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA 30322, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
Bell B, Lin JJ, Seidenberg M, Hermann B. The neurobiology of cognitive disorders in temporal lobe epilepsy. Nat Rev Neurol 2011; 7:154-64. [PMID: 21304484 DOI: 10.1038/nrneurol.2011.3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 328] [Impact Index Per Article: 23.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/21/2022]
Abstract
Cognitive impairment, particularly memory disruption, is a major complicating feature of epilepsy. This Review will begin with a focus on the problem of memory impairment in temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE). We present a brief overview of anatomical substrates of memory disorders in TLE, followed by a discussion of how our understanding of these disorders has been improved by studying the outcomes of anterior temporal lobectomy. The clinical efforts made to predict which patients are at greatest risk of experiencing adverse cognitive outcomes following epilepsy surgery are also considered. Finally, we examine the vastly changing view of TLE, including findings demonstrating that anatomical abnormalities extend far outside the temporal lobe, and that cognitive impairments extend beyond memory function. Linkage between these distributed cognitive and anatomical abnormalities point to a new understanding of the anatomical architecture of cognitive impairment in epilepsy. Clarifying the origin of these cognitive and anatomical abnormalities, their progression over time and, most importantly, methods for protecting cognitive and brain health in epilepsy, present a challenge to neurologists.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brian Bell
- Department of Neurology, University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, 600 North Highland Avenue, Madison, WI 53792, USA
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
13
|
Simmons WK, Reddish M, Bellgowan PSF, Martin A. The selectivity and functional connectivity of the anterior temporal lobes. Cereb Cortex 2010; 20:813-25. [PMID: 19620621 PMCID: PMC2837089 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhp149] [Citation(s) in RCA: 159] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
One influential account asserts that the anterior temporal lobe (ATL) is a domain-general hub for semantic memory. Other evidence indicates it is part of a domain-specific social cognition system. Arbitrating these accounts using functional magnetic resonance imaging has previously been difficult because of magnetic susceptibility artifacts in the region. The present study used parameters optimized for imaging the ATL, and had subjects encode facts about unfamiliar people, buildings, and hammers. Using both conjunction and region of interest analyses, person-selective responses were observed in both the left and right ATL. Neither building-selective, hammer-selective nor domain-general responses were observed in the ATLs, although they were observed in other brain regions. These findings were supported by "resting-state" functional connectivity analyses using independent datasets from the same subjects. Person-selective ATL clusters were functionally connected with the brain's wider social cognition network. Rather than serving as a domain-general semantic hub, the ATLs work in unison with the social cognition system to support learning facts about others.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- W Kyle Simmons
- Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD 20892-1366, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
14
|
Tranel D, Bechara A. Sex-related functional asymmetry of the amygdala: preliminary evidence using a case-matched lesion approach. Neurocase 2009; 15:217-34. [PMID: 19308794 PMCID: PMC2829120 DOI: 10.1080/13554790902775492] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
We have reported previously that there appears to be an intriguing sex-related functional asymmetry of the prefrontal cortices, especially the ventromedial sector, in regard to social conduct, emotional processing, and decision-making, whereby the right-sided sector is important in men but not women and the left-sided sector is important in women but not men. The amygdala is another structure that has been widely implicated in emotion processing and social decision-making, and the question arises as to whether the amygdala, in a manner akin to what has been observed for the prefrontal cortex, might have sex-related functional asymmetry in regard to social and emotional functions. A preliminary test of this question was carried out in the current study, where we used a case-matched lesion approach and contrasted a pair of men cases and a pair of women cases, where in each pair one patient had left amygdala damage and the other had right amygdala damage. We investigated the domains of social conduct, emotional processing and personality, and decision-making. The results provide support for the notion that there is sex-related functional asymmetry of the amygdala in regard to these functions - in the male pair, the patient with right-sided amygdala damage was impaired in these functions, and the patient with left-sided amygdala damage was not, whereas in the female pair, the opposite pattern obtained, with the left-sided woman being impaired and the right-sided woman being unimpaired. These data provide preliminary support for the notion that sex-related functional asymmetry of the amygdala may entail functions such as social conduct, emotional processing, and decision-making, a finding that in turn could reflect (as either a cause or effect) differences in the manner in which men and women apprehend, process, and execute emotion-related information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel Tranel
- Department of Neurology, Division of Behavioral Neurology and Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Iowa College of Medicine, 200 Hawkins Drive, Iowa City, IA 52242, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
Category-specific recognition and naming deficits following resection of a right anterior temporal lobe tumor in a patient with atypical language lateralization. Cortex 2008; 45:630-40. [PMID: 18632095 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.04.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/15/2007] [Revised: 01/23/2008] [Accepted: 04/07/2008] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
We present a patient with right-hemispheric speech lateralization who exhibited severe recognition and naming deficits for unique objects (famous faces and landmarks) and grossly normal recognition and naming performances for nonunique objects (animals and man-made objects) following an anterior right temporal lobe (TL) resection of a ganglioglioma. While recognition deficits have been reported for famous faces following right temporal pole lesions, and for landmarks and geographic regions following right TL damage in general, this is the first reported case of both recognition and naming deficits for these objects resulting from a single lesion. These results are consistent with research suggesting that the neuroanatomic substrates for the recognition and naming of unique objects lie in the anterior TL regions. Left temporal pole lesions have been associated with naming deficits for unique objects while right temporal pole lesions have been associated with recognition deficits for unique objects. However, these findings suggest that the substrates of naming can be located in homotopic regions of the right hemisphere when language lateralization is atypical. As various object categories appear to have different neuroanatomical representations in the TLs, we discuss the possible benefits of sampling a wider array of objects during cortical stimulation mapping of language.
Collapse
|