1
|
Phaf RH. An Audience Facilitates Facial Feedback: A Social-Context Hypothesis Reconciling Original Study and Nonreplication. Psychol Rep 2024; 127:3170-3189. [PMID: 36735237 PMCID: PMC11529121 DOI: 10.1177/00332941231153975] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/04/2023]
Abstract
Nonreplications of previously undisputed phenomena tend to leave a theoretical vacuum. This theoretical perspective seeks to fill the gap left by the failure to replicate unobtrusive facial feedback. In the emblematic original study, participants who held a pen between the teeth (i.e., requiring activity of the zygomaticus major muscle) rated cartoons more positively than participants who held the pen between the lips. We argue that the same social mechanisms (e.g., the presence of an audience) modulate facial feedback to emotion as are involved in the feed-forward shaping of facial actions by emotions. Differing social contexts could thus help explain the contrast between original findings and failures to obtain unobtrusive facial feedback. An exploratory analysis that included results only from (unobtrusive) facial-feedback studies without explicit reference to emotion in the facial manipulation provided preliminary support for this hypothesis. Studies with a social context (e.g., due to experimenter presence) showed a medium-sized aggregate facial-feedback effect, whereas studies without a social context (e.g., when facial actions were only filmed), revealed a small effect. Video awareness strengthened facial feedback considerably within an engaging social context, but seemed to reduce it without a social context. We provisionally conclude that a (pro-)social interpretation of facial actions facilitates feedback to (primarily positive) emotion, and suggest further research explicitly manipulating this context.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- R. Hans Phaf
- Amsterdam Brain and Cognition Center, Universiteit van Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands; Brain and Cognition Group, Department of Psychology, Universiteit van Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Prince JS, Alvarez GA, Konkle T. Contrastive learning explains the emergence and function of visual category-selective regions. SCIENCE ADVANCES 2024; 10:eadl1776. [PMID: 39321304 PMCID: PMC11423896 DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.adl1776] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/31/2023] [Accepted: 08/21/2024] [Indexed: 09/27/2024]
Abstract
Modular and distributed coding theories of category selectivity along the human ventral visual stream have long existed in tension. Here, we present a reconciling framework-contrastive coding-based on a series of analyses relating category selectivity within biological and artificial neural networks. We discover that, in models trained with contrastive self-supervised objectives over a rich natural image diet, category-selective tuning naturally emerges for faces, bodies, scenes, and words. Further, lesions of these model units lead to selective, dissociable recognition deficits, highlighting their distinct functional roles in information processing. Finally, these pre-identified units can predict neural responses in all corresponding face-, scene-, body-, and word-selective regions of human visual cortex, under a highly constrained sparse positive encoding procedure. The success of this single model indicates that brain-like functional specialization can emerge without category-specific learning pressures, as the system learns to untangle rich image content. Contrastive coding, therefore, provides a unifying account of object category emergence and representation in the human brain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jacob S Prince
- Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA
| | - George A Alvarez
- Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA
| | - Talia Konkle
- Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA
- Center for Brain Science, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA
- Kempner Institute for Biological and Artificial Intelligence, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Sgadò P, Pross A, Lamanna J, Adiletta A. Face processing in animal models: implications for autism spectrum disorder. Front Neurosci 2024; 18:1462272. [PMID: 39184326 PMCID: PMC11341390 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2024.1462272] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/09/2024] [Accepted: 07/29/2024] [Indexed: 08/27/2024] Open
Abstract
Processing facial features is crucial to identify social partners (prey, predators, or conspecifics) and recognize and accurately interpret emotional expressions. Numerous studies in both human and non-human primates provided evidence promoting the notion of inherent mechanisms for detecting facial features. These mechanisms support a representation of faces independent of prior experiences and are vital for subsequent development in social and language domains. Moreover, deficits in processing faces are a reliable biomarker of autism spectrum disorder, appearing early and correlating with symptom severity. Face processing, however, is not only a prerogative of humans: other species also show remarkable face detection abilities. In this review, we present an overview of the current literature on face detection in vertebrate models that could be relevant to the study of autism.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paola Sgadò
- Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Rovereto, Italy
| | - Alessandra Pross
- Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Rovereto, Italy
| | - Jacopo Lamanna
- Center for Behavioral Neuroscience and Communication (BNC), Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan, Italy
- Faculty of Psychology, Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan, Italy
| | - Alice Adiletta
- Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Rovereto, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Turk-Browne NB, Aslin RN. Infant neuroscience: how to measure brain activity in the youngest minds. Trends Neurosci 2024; 47:338-354. [PMID: 38570212 DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2024.02.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/30/2023] [Revised: 01/08/2024] [Accepted: 02/09/2024] [Indexed: 04/05/2024]
Abstract
The functional properties of the infant brain are poorly understood. Recent advances in cognitive neuroscience are opening new avenues for measuring brain activity in human infants. These include novel uses of existing technologies such as electroencephalography (EEG) and magnetoencephalography (MEG), the availability of newer technologies including functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) and optically pumped magnetometry (OPM), and innovative applications of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in awake infants during cognitive tasks. In this review article we catalog these available non-invasive methods, discuss the challenges and opportunities encountered when applying them to human infants, and highlight the potential they may ultimately hold for advancing our understanding of the youngest minds.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicholas B Turk-Browne
- Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, CT 06520, USA; Wu Tsai Institute, Yale University, New Haven, CT 06510, USA.
| | - Richard N Aslin
- Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, CT 06520, USA; Child Study Center, Yale School of Medicine, New Haven, CT 06520, USA
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Jernigan CM, Freiwald WA, Sheehan MJ. Neural correlates of individual facial recognition in a social wasp. BIORXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR BIOLOGY 2024:2024.04.11.589095. [PMID: 38659842 PMCID: PMC11042187 DOI: 10.1101/2024.04.11.589095] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 04/26/2024]
Abstract
Individual recognition is critical for social behavior across species. Whether recognition is mediated by circuits specialized for social information processing has been a matter of debate. Here we examine the neurobiological underpinning of individual visual facial recognition in Polistes fuscatus paper wasps. Front-facing images of conspecific wasps broadly increase activity across many brain regions relative to other stimuli. Notably, we identify a localized subpopulation of neurons in the protocerebrum which show specialized selectivity for front-facing wasp images, which we term wasp cells. These wasp cells encode information regarding the facial patterns, with ensemble activity correlating with facial identity. Wasp cells are strikingly analogous to face cells in primates, indicating that specialized circuits are likely an adaptive feature of neural architecture to support visual recognition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christopher M. Jernigan
- Laboratory for Animal Social Evolution and Recognition, Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, Cornell University; Ithaca, NY, 14853, USA
| | - Winrich A. Freiwald
- Laboratory of Neural Systems, The Rockefeller University, New York, NY 10065, USA
| | - Michael J. Sheehan
- Laboratory for Animal Social Evolution and Recognition, Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, Cornell University; Ithaca, NY, 14853, USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Duque A, Picado G, Salgado G, Salgado A, Palacios B, Chaves C. Validation of the Edited Tromsø Infant Faces Database (E-TIF): A study on differences in the processing of children's emotional expressions. Behav Res Methods 2024; 56:2507-2518. [PMID: 37369938 PMCID: PMC10991014 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-023-02163-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 06/05/2023] [Indexed: 06/29/2023]
Abstract
Images of emotional facial expressions are often used in emotion research, which has promoted the development of different databases. However, most of these standardized sets of images do not include images from infants under 2 years of age, which is relevant for psychology research, especially for perinatal psychology. The present study aims to validate the edited version of the Tromsø Infant Faces Database (E-TIF) in a large sample of participants. The original set of 119 pictures was edited. The pictures were cropped to remove nonrelevant information, fitted in an oval window, and converted to grayscale. Four hundred and eighty participants (72.9% women) took part in the study, rating the images on five dimensions: depicted emotion, clarity, intensity, valence, and genuineness. Valence scores were useful for discriminating between positive, negative, and neutral facial expressions. Results revealed that women were more accurate at recognizing emotions in children. Regarding parental status, parents, in comparison with nonparents, rated neutral expressions as more intense and genuine. They also rated sad, angry, disgusted, and fearful faces as less negative, and happy expressions as less positive. The editing and validation of the E-TIF database offers a useful tool for basic and experimental research in psychology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Almudena Duque
- Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Pontificia de Salamanca, C/ Compañía 5, 37002, Salamanca, Spain
| | - Gonzalo Picado
- Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Pontificia de Salamanca, C/ Compañía 5, 37002, Salamanca, Spain
| | - Gloria Salgado
- Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Campus de Somosaguas s/n, 28223, Pozuelo de Alarcón, Spain
| | - Alfonso Salgado
- Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Pontificia de Salamanca, C/ Compañía 5, 37002, Salamanca, Spain
| | - Beatriz Palacios
- Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Pontificia de Salamanca, C/ Compañía 5, 37002, Salamanca, Spain
| | - Covadonga Chaves
- Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Campus de Somosaguas s/n, 28223, Pozuelo de Alarcón, Spain.
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Landmann E, Breil C, Huestegge L, Böckler A. The semantics of gaze in person perception: a novel qualitative-quantitative approach. Sci Rep 2024; 14:893. [PMID: 38195808 PMCID: PMC10776783 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-51331-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/15/2023] [Accepted: 12/31/2023] [Indexed: 01/11/2024] Open
Abstract
Interpreting gaze behavior is essential in evaluating interaction partners, yet the 'semantics of gaze' in dynamic interactions are still poorly understood. We aimed to comprehensively investigate effects of gaze behavior patterns in different conversation contexts, using a two-step, qualitative-quantitative procedure. Participants watched video clips of single persons listening to autobiographic narrations by another (invisible) person. The listener's gaze behavior was manipulated in terms of gaze direction, frequency and direction of gaze shifts, and blink frequency; emotional context was manipulated through the valence of the narration (neutral/negative). In Experiment 1 (qualitative-exploratory), participants freely described which states and traits they attributed to the listener in each condition, allowing us to identify relevant aspects of person perception and to construct distinct rating scales that were implemented in Experiment 2 (quantitative-confirmatory). Results revealed systematic and differential meanings ascribed to the listener's gaze behavior. For example, rapid blinking and fast gaze shifts were rated more negatively (e.g., restless and unnatural) than slower gaze behavior; downward gaze was evaluated more favorably (e.g., empathetic) than other gaze aversion types, especially in the emotionally negative context. Overall, our study contributes to a more systematic understanding of flexible gaze semantics in social interaction.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eva Landmann
- Department of Psychology, Julius-Maximilians-Universität Würzburg (JMU), 97070, Würzburg, Germany.
| | - Christina Breil
- Department of Psychology, Julius-Maximilians-Universität Würzburg (JMU), 97070, Würzburg, Germany
| | - Lynn Huestegge
- Department of Psychology, Julius-Maximilians-Universität Würzburg (JMU), 97070, Würzburg, Germany
| | - Anne Böckler
- Department of Psychology, Julius-Maximilians-Universität Würzburg (JMU), 97070, Würzburg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Tarchi P, Lanini MC, Frassineti L, Lanatà A. Real and Deepfake Face Recognition: An EEG Study on Cognitive and Emotive Implications. Brain Sci 2023; 13:1233. [PMID: 37759834 PMCID: PMC10526392 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci13091233] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/17/2023] [Revised: 08/09/2023] [Accepted: 08/18/2023] [Indexed: 09/29/2023] Open
Abstract
The human brain's role in face processing (FP) and decision making for social interactions depends on recognizing faces accurately. However, the prevalence of deepfakes, AI-generated images, poses challenges in discerning real from synthetic identities. This study investigated healthy individuals' cognitive and emotional engagement in a visual discrimination task involving real and deepfake human faces expressing positive, negative, or neutral emotions. Electroencephalographic (EEG) data were collected from 23 healthy participants using a 21-channel dry-EEG headset; power spectrum and event-related potential (ERP) analyses were performed. Results revealed statistically significant activations in specific brain areas depending on the authenticity and emotional content of the stimuli. Power spectrum analysis highlighted a right-hemisphere predominance in theta, alpha, high-beta, and gamma bands for real faces, while deepfakes mainly affected the frontal and occipital areas in the delta band. ERP analysis hinted at the possibility of discriminating between real and synthetic faces, as N250 (200-300 ms after stimulus onset) peak latency decreased when observing real faces in the right frontal (LF) and left temporo-occipital (LTO) areas, but also within emotions, as P100 (90-140 ms) peak amplitude was found higher in the right temporo-occipital (RTO) area for happy faces with respect to neutral and sad ones.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pietro Tarchi
- Department of Information Engineering, University of Florence, 50139 Florence, Italy; (P.T.); (M.C.L.); (L.F.)
| | - Maria Chiara Lanini
- Department of Information Engineering, University of Florence, 50139 Florence, Italy; (P.T.); (M.C.L.); (L.F.)
| | - Lorenzo Frassineti
- Department of Information Engineering, University of Florence, 50139 Florence, Italy; (P.T.); (M.C.L.); (L.F.)
- Department of Information Engineering, University of Pisa, 56122 Pisa, Italy
| | - Antonio Lanatà
- Department of Information Engineering, University of Florence, 50139 Florence, Italy; (P.T.); (M.C.L.); (L.F.)
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Ritchie KL, Flack TR, Maréchal L. Unfamiliar faces might as well be another species: Evidence from a face matching task with human and monkey faces. VISUAL COGNITION 2023. [DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2023.2184894] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 03/15/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Kay L. Ritchie
- School of Psychology, University of Lincoln, Lincoln, UK
| | - Tessa R. Flack
- School of Psychology, University of Lincoln, Lincoln, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Canas-Bajo T, Whitney D. Individual differences in classification images of Mooney faces. J Vis 2022; 22:3. [DOI: 10.1167/jov.22.13.3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Teresa Canas-Bajo
- Vision Science Graduate Group, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA, USA
| | - David Whitney
- Vision Science Graduate Group, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA, USA
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Luxembourger C, Fischer JP, Tazouti Y. Self-Recognition Before a Screen-Mirror Between 15 Months and 6 Years, The Contribution of Eye-Tracking and a New Protocol. JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 2022. [DOI: 10.1080/15248372.2022.2037607] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
|
12
|
Conte S, Baccolo E, Bulf H, Proietti V, Macchi Cassia V. Infants' visual exploration strategies for adult and child faces. INFANCY 2022; 27:492-514. [PMID: 35075767 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12458] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/31/2020] [Revised: 11/05/2021] [Accepted: 12/21/2021] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
By the end of the first year of life, infants' discrimination abilities tune to frequently experienced face groups. Little is known about the exploration strategies adopted to efficiently discriminate frequent, familiar face types. The present eye-tracking study examined the distribution of visual fixations produced by 10-month-old and 4-month-old singletons while learning adult (i.e., familiar) and child (i.e., unfamiliar) White faces. Infants were tested in an infant-controlled visual habituation task, in which post-habituation preference measured successful discrimination. Results confirmed earlier evidence that, without sibling experience, 10-month-olds discriminate only among adult faces. Analyses of gaze movements during habituation showed that infants' fixations were centered in the upper part of the stimuli. The mouth was sampled longer in adult faces than in child faces, while the child eyes were sampled longer and more frequently than the adult eyes. At 10 months, but not at 4 months, global measures of scanning behavior on the whole face also varied according to face age, as the spatiotemporal distribution of scan paths showed larger within- and between-participants similarity for adult faces than for child faces. Results are discussed with reference to the perceptual narrowing literature, and the influence of age-appropriate developmental tasks on infants' face processing abilities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stefania Conte
- Department of Psychology, University of South Carolina, Columbia, South Carolina, USA
| | - Elisa Baccolo
- Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milano, Italy
| | - Hermann Bulf
- Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milano, Italy
| | - Valentina Proietti
- Department of Psychology, Trinity Western University, Langley, British Columbia, Canada
| | | |
Collapse
|
13
|
Caruana N, Seymour K. Objects that induce face pareidolia are prioritized by the visual system. Br J Psychol 2021; 113:496-507. [PMID: 34923634 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12546] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/27/2021] [Accepted: 12/03/2021] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
The human visual system has evolved specialized neural mechanisms to rapidly detect faces. Its broad tuning for facial features is thought to underlie the illusory perception of faces in inanimate objects, a phenomenon called face pareidolia. Recent studies on face pareidolia suggest that the mechanisms underlying face processing, at least at the early stages of visual encoding, may treat objects that resemble faces as real faces; prioritizing their detection. In our study, we used breaking continuous flash suppression (b-CFS) to examine whether the human visual system prioritizes the detection of objects that induce face pareidolia over stimuli matched for object content. Similar to previous b-CFS results using real face stimuli, we found that participants detected the objects with pareidolia faces faster than object-matched control stimuli. Given that face pareidolia has been more frequently reported amongst individuals prone to hallucinations, we also explored whether this rapid prioritization is intact in individuals with schizophrenia, and found evidence suggesting that it was. Our findings suggest that face pareidolia engages a broadly tuned mechanism that facilitates rapid face detection. This may involve the proposed fast subcortical pathway that operates outside of visual awareness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nathan Caruana
- Department of Cognitive Science, Macquarie University, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia.,Perception in Action Research Centre, Macquarie University, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
| | - Kiley Seymour
- School of Psychology, Western Sydney University, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia.,The MARCS Institute for Brain, Behaviour and Development, Western Sydney University, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia.,Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, Tübingen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Scrivner C. The psychology of morbid curiosity: Development and initial validation of the morbid curiosity scale. PERSONALITY AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2021. [DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.111139] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
|
15
|
Labotka D, Gelman SA, Jipson JL. Parent-child conversations about animals on a visit to a (virtual) zoo. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2021. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2021.101123] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
16
|
Almasi RC, Behrmann M. Subcortical regions of the human visual system do not process faces holistically. Brain Cogn 2021; 151:105726. [PMID: 33933856 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2021.105726] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/06/2020] [Revised: 03/25/2021] [Accepted: 03/26/2021] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Face perception is considered to be evolutionarily adaptive and conserved across species. While subcortical visual brain areas are implicated in face perception based on existing evidence from phylogenetic and ontogenetic studies, whether these subcortical structures contribute to more complex visual computations such as the holistic processing (HP) of faces in humans is unknown. To address this issue, we used a well-established marker of HP, the composite face effect (CFE), with a group of adult human observers, and presented two sequential faces in a trial monocularly or interocularly using a Wheatstone stereoscope. HP refers to the finding that two identical top (or bottom) halves of a face are judged to be different when their task-irrelevant bottom (or top) halves belong to different faces. Because humans process faces holistically, they are unable to ignore the information from the irrelevant half of the composite face, and this is true to an even greater extent when the two halves of the faces are aligned compared with when they are misaligned ('Alignment effect'). The results revealed the HP effect and also uncovered the Alignment effect, a key marker of the CFE. The findings also indicated a monocular advantage, replicating the known subcortical contribution to face perception. There was, however, no statistically significant difference in the CFE when the images were presented in the monocular versus interocular conditions. These findings indicate that HP is not necessarily mediated by the subcortical visual pathway, and suggest that further investigation of cortical, rather than subcortical, structures might advance our understanding of HP and its role in face processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rebeka C Almasi
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience Institute, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA.
| | - Marlene Behrmann
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience Institute, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Huang W, Chen C, Chen X, Chen X, Gan J, Zhu X, Xu J, Zhao L, Zhou J, Yang R, Lv J, Liu X, Li H. Association between global visual scanning and cognitive function in schizophrenia. Asian J Psychiatr 2021; 56:102559. [PMID: 33465748 DOI: 10.1016/j.ajp.2021.102559] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/26/2020] [Revised: 01/06/2021] [Accepted: 01/08/2021] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Abnormalities of global visual scanning have been well-recognized in schizophrenia patients. However, its relevance to various cognitive domains is still understudied. In the present study, we identified the association between global visual scanning and cognitive function in schizophrenia. METHODS Eye movement was captured and analyzed in 28 individuals with schizophrenia and 21 healthy controls in free viewing test. The MATRICS consensus cognitive battery (MCCB) was applied to evaluate cognitive function. RESULTS Consistent with previous reports, schizophrenia patients exhibited several eye movement abnormalities during global visual scanning and showed cognition deficits in MCCB test as compared with healthy controls. Fixation duration was significantly related to cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia. Moreover, there was a potential that eye movement measures during landscape scanning, rather than face scanning, were preferentially related to cognitive dysfunction. CONCLUSION This study indicated the association between free visual scanning and various cognitive functions in schizophrenia, shedding light on the possibility that some eye movement measures during visual exploration could be a biomarker for cognitive deficits in schizophrenia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wei Huang
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, No. 923 Hospital of Joint Logistic Support Force of Chinese People's Liberation Army (PLA), No. 52 Zhiwu Road, Nanning, 530000, China
| | - Chen Chen
- Department of Military Medical Psychology, Air Force Medical University of PLA (the Fourth Military Medical University), No. 169 West Changle Road, Xi'an, 710032, China
| | - Xianjun Chen
- Department of Physiology, Research Center of Neuroscience, Chongqing Medical University, Chongqing, 400016, China
| | - Xing Chen
- Department of Clinical Psychology, Nantong No. 4 People's Hospital, No. 37 Chenggang Road, Nantong, 226000, China
| | - Jingli Gan
- Department of Psychiatry, Mental Diseases Prevention and Treatment Institute of PLA, No. 988 Hospital of Joint Logistic Support Force of PLA, Jiaozuo, 454003, China
| | - Xiquan Zhu
- Department of Psychiatry, Mental Diseases Prevention and Treatment Institute of PLA, No. 988 Hospital of Joint Logistic Support Force of PLA, Jiaozuo, 454003, China
| | - Jian Xu
- Department of Clinical Psychology, Nantong No. 4 People's Hospital, No. 37 Chenggang Road, Nantong, 226000, China
| | - Liqiong Zhao
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, No. 923 Hospital of Joint Logistic Support Force of Chinese People's Liberation Army (PLA), No. 52 Zhiwu Road, Nanning, 530000, China
| | - Jie Zhou
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, No. 923 Hospital of Joint Logistic Support Force of Chinese People's Liberation Army (PLA), No. 52 Zhiwu Road, Nanning, 530000, China
| | - Rui Yang
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, No. 923 Hospital of Joint Logistic Support Force of Chinese People's Liberation Army (PLA), No. 52 Zhiwu Road, Nanning, 530000, China
| | - Jing Lv
- The Second Medical Center, Chinese PLA General Hospital, No.28 Fuxing Road, Beijing, 100853, China.
| | - Xufeng Liu
- Department of Military Medical Psychology, Air Force Medical University of PLA (the Fourth Military Medical University), No. 169 West Changle Road, Xi'an, 710032, China.
| | - Hongzheng Li
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, No. 923 Hospital of Joint Logistic Support Force of Chinese People's Liberation Army (PLA), No. 52 Zhiwu Road, Nanning, 530000, China.
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Quinones Sanchez JF, Liu X, Zhou C, Hildebrandt A. Nature and nurture shape structural connectivity in the face processing brain network. Neuroimage 2021; 229:117736. [PMID: 33486123 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117736] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/31/2020] [Revised: 12/23/2020] [Accepted: 01/05/2021] [Indexed: 01/12/2023] Open
Abstract
Face processing is a key ability facilitating social cognition. Only a few studies explored how nature and nurture shape face processing ontogeny at the behavioral and neural level. Also, very little is known about the contributions of nature and nurture to the establishment of white matter fibers supporting this specific human ability. The main purpose of this study was to assess genetic and environmental influences on white matter bundles connecting atlas-defined and functionally-defined face-responsive areas in the brain. Diffusion weighted images from 408 twins (monozygotic = 264, dizygotic = 144) were obtained from the WU-Minn Human Connectome Project. Fractional anisotropy - a widely used measure of fiber quality - of seven white matter tracts in the face network and ten global white matter tracts was analyzed by means of Structural Equation Modeling for twin data. Results revealed small and moderate genetic effects on face network fiber quality in addition to their shared variance with global brain white matter integrity. Furthermore, a theoretically expected common latent factor accounted for limited genetic and larger environmental variance in multiple face network fibers. The findings suggest that both genetic and environmental factors explain individual differences in fiber quality within the face network, as compared with much larger genetic effects on global brain white matter quality. In addition to heritability, individual-specific environmental influences on the face processing brain network are large, a finding that suggests to connect nature and nurture views on this remarkably specific human ability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Xinyang Liu
- Department of Psychology, Carl von Ossietzky Universität Oldenburg, Germany; Department of Physics, Centre for Nonlinear Studies and Beijing-Hong Kong-Singapore Joint Centre for Nonlinear and Complex Systems (Hong Kong), Institute of Computational and Theoretical Studies, Hong Kong Baptist University, Hong Kong
| | - Changsong Zhou
- Department of Physics, Centre for Nonlinear Studies and Beijing-Hong Kong-Singapore Joint Centre for Nonlinear and Complex Systems (Hong Kong), Institute of Computational and Theoretical Studies, Hong Kong Baptist University, Hong Kong; Department of Physics, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China
| | - Andrea Hildebrandt
- Department of Psychology, Carl von Ossietzky Universität Oldenburg, Germany; Research Center Neurosensory Science, Carl von Ossietzky Universität Oldenburg, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Malsert J, Tran K, Tran TAT, Ha-Vinh T, Gentaz E, Leuchter RHV. Cross-Cultural and Environmental Influences on Facial Emotional Discrimination Sensitivity in 9-Year-Old Children from Swiss and Vietnamese Schools. SWISS JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY 2020. [DOI: 10.1024/1421-0185/a000240] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Abstract. The Other Race Effect (ORE), i.e., recognition facilitation for own-race faces, is a well-established phenomenon with broad evidence in adults and infants. Nevertheless, the ORE in older children is poorly understood, and even less so for emotional face processing. This research samples 87 9-year-old children from Vietnamese and Swiss schools. In two separate studies, we evaluated the children’s abilities to perceive the disappearance of emotions in Asian and Caucasian faces in an offset task. The first study evaluated an “emotional ORE” in Vietnamese-Asian, Swiss-Caucasian, and Swiss-Multicultural children. Offset times showed an emotional ORE in Vietnamese-Asian children living in an ethnically homogeneous environment, whereas mixed ethnicities in Swiss children seem to have balanced performance between face types. The second study compared socioemotionally trained versus untrained Vietnamese-Asian children. Vietnamese children showed a strong emotional ORE and tend to increase their sensitivity to emotion offset after training. Moreover, an effect of emotion consistent with previous observation in adults could suggest a cultural sensitivity to disapproval signs. Taken together, the results suggest that 9-year-old children can present an emotional ORE, but that a heterogeneous environment or an emotional training could strengthen face-processing abilities without reducing skills on their own-group.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jennifer Malsert
- SensoriMotor, Affective, and Social Development Lab, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
- Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, Campus Biotech, Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Khanh Tran
- Eurasia Foundation and Association for Special Education in Vietnam, Ho Chi Minh City, Vietnam
| | - Tu Anh Thi Tran
- University of Education, Hue University, Thua Thien Hue, Vietnam
| | - Tho Ha-Vinh
- Eurasia Foundation and Association for Special Education in Vietnam, Ho Chi Minh City, Vietnam
| | - Edouard Gentaz
- SensoriMotor, Affective, and Social Development Lab, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
- Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, Campus Biotech, Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Russia Ha-Vinh Leuchter
- Division of Development and Growth, Department of Pediatrics, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Malsert J, Palama A, Gentaz E. Emotional facial perception development in 7, 9 and 11 year-old children: The emergence of a silent eye-tracked emotional other-race effect. PLoS One 2020; 15:e0233008. [PMID: 32392271 PMCID: PMC7213684 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0233008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/03/2019] [Accepted: 04/27/2020] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
The present study examined emotional facial perception (happy and angry) in 7, 9 and 11-year-old children from Caucasian and multicultural environments with an offset task for two ethnic groups of faces (Asian and Caucasian). In this task, participants were required to respond to a dynamic facial expression video when they believed that the first emotion presented had disappeared. Moreover, using an eye-tracker, we evaluated the ocular behavior pattern used to process these different faces. The analyses of reaction times do not show an emotional other-race effect (i.e., a facility in discriminating own-race faces over to other-race ones) in Caucasian children for Caucasian vs. Asian faces through offset times, but an effect of emotional face appeared in the oldest children. Furthermore, an eye-tracked ocular emotion and race-effect relative to processing strategies is observed and evolves between age 7 and 11. This study strengthens the interest in advancing an eye-tracking study in developmental and emotional processing studies, showing that even a "silent" effect should be detected and shrewdly analyzed through an objective means.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jennifer Malsert
- SensoriMotor, Affective and Social Development Laboratory, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
- Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, Campus Biotech, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
- University of Teacher Education, Special Needs Education Unit, State of Vaud (HEP Vaud), Lausanne, Switzerland
| | - Amaya Palama
- SensoriMotor, Affective and Social Development Laboratory, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
- Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, Campus Biotech, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Edouard Gentaz
- SensoriMotor, Affective and Social Development Laboratory, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
- Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, Campus Biotech, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
- University of Grenoble Alpes, LPNC, CNRS, Grenoble, France
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
|
22
|
Stevens EM, Johnson AL, Leshner G, Sun F, Kim S, Leavens ELS, Tackett AP, Hébert ET, Wagener TL. People in E-Cigarette Ads Attract More Attention: An Eye-Tracking Study. TOB REGUL SCI 2020; 6:105-117. [PMID: 33816715 PMCID: PMC8018674 DOI: 10.18001/trs.6.2.3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Minimally regulated electronic cigarettes (e-cigarette) advertising may be one potential factor driving the increasing prevalence of young adult e-cigarette use. Using eye-tracking, the current study examined which e-cigarette advertising features were the most appealing to young adults as a first step to examine how e-cigarette advertising may be regulated. METHODS Using a within-subjects design, 30 young adults (M age = 20.0 years) viewed e-cigarette ads in a laboratory. Ad features or areas of interest (AOIs) included: 1) brand logo, 2) product descriptor, and 3) people. During ad viewing, eye-tracking measured participants' dwell time and time to first fixation for each AOI as well as each ad brand. Harm perceptions pre- and post-viewing were measured. RESULTS Participants spent the longest dwell time on people (M = 2701 ms), then product descriptors (M = 924 ms), then brand logos (M = 672 ms; ps < .001). They also fixated fastest on AOIs in that order. Participant sex significantly impacted dwell time of ad brand, and harm perceptions decreased after viewing the ads (ps < .05). CONCLUSIONS This study provides initial evidence about which e-cigarette ad features may appeal most to young adults and may be useful when designing evidence-based policy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elise M Stevens
- Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health, Dana-Farber Cancer Institute, Harvard University, Boston, MA
| | - Amanda L Johnson
- Oklahoma Tobacco Research Center, Stephenson Cancer Center, University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center, Oklahoma City, OK
| | - Glenn Leshner
- Edward L. and Thelma Gaylord Chair in journalism, Gaylord College of Journalism and Mass Communication, University of Oklahoma, Norman, OK
| | - FuWei Sun
- Department of Journalism, Fu Hsing Kang College, National Defense University, Taiwan
| | - Seunghyun Kim
- Department of Marketing and Advertising, College of Business, University of Arkansas at Little Rock, Little Rock, AK
| | - Eleanor L S Leavens
- Department of Population Health, University of Kansas Medical Center, Kansas City, KS
| | - Alayna P Tackett
- Oklahoma Tobacco Research Center, Stephenson Cancer Center, University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center, Oklahoma City, OK
| | - Emily T Hébert
- Oklahoma Tobacco Research Center, Stephenson Cancer Center, University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center, Oklahoma City, OK
| | - Theodore L Wagener
- Center for Tobacco Research, The Ohio State University Comprehensive Cancer Center, Department of Internal Medicine, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Prunty JE, Jackson KC, Keemink JR, Kelly DJ. Caucasian Infants' Attentional Orienting to Own- and Other-Race Faces. Brain Sci 2020; 10:E53. [PMID: 31963429 PMCID: PMC7016870 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci10010053] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/08/2019] [Revised: 01/09/2020] [Accepted: 01/14/2020] [Indexed: 01/23/2023] Open
Abstract
Infants show preferential attention toward faces and detect faces embedded within complex naturalistic scenes. Newborn infants are insensitive to race, but rapidly develop differential processing of own- and other-race faces. In the present study, we investigated the development of attentional orienting toward own- and other-race faces embedded within naturalistic scenes. Infants aged six-, nine- and twelve-months did not show differences in the speed of orienting to own- and other race faces, but other-race faces held infants' visual attention for longer. We also found a clear developmental progression in attentional capture and holding, with older infants orienting to faces faster and fixating them for longer. Results are interpreted within the context of the two-process model of face processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jonathan E. Prunty
- School of Psychology, Keynes College, University of Kent, Canterbury, Kent CT2 7NP, UK; (J.E.P.); (J.R.K.)
| | - Kelsey C. Jackson
- Seattle Children’s Innovative Technologies Lab, Seattle Children’s Hospital, Seattle, WA 98105, USA;
| | - Jolie. R. Keemink
- School of Psychology, Keynes College, University of Kent, Canterbury, Kent CT2 7NP, UK; (J.E.P.); (J.R.K.)
| | - David J. Kelly
- School of Psychology, Keynes College, University of Kent, Canterbury, Kent CT2 7NP, UK; (J.E.P.); (J.R.K.)
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Scrivner C, Choe KW, Henry J, Lyu M, Maestripieri D, Berman MG. Violence reduces attention to faces and draws attention to points of contact. Sci Rep 2019; 9:17779. [PMID: 31780726 PMCID: PMC6883035 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-54327-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/03/2019] [Accepted: 11/09/2019] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Although violence is a frequently researched topic, little is known about how different social features influence information gathering from violent interactions. Regions of an interaction that provide contextual information should receive more attention. We predicted the most informative features of a violent social interaction would be faces, points of contact, and objects being held. To test this, we tracked the eyes of 90 participants as they viewed images of social interactions that varied with respect to violence. When viewing violent interactions, participants attended significantly less to faces and significantly more to points of contact. Moreover, first-fixation analysis suggests that some of these biases are present from the beginning of scene-viewing. These findings are the first to demonstrate the visual relevance of faces and contact points in gathering information from violent social interactions. These results also question the attentional dominance of faces in active social scenes, highlighting the importance of using a variety of stimuli and contexts in social cognition research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Coltan Scrivner
- Department of Comparative Human Development, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA. .,Institute for Mind and Biology, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA.
| | - Kyoung Whan Choe
- Department of Psychology, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA.,Mansueto Institute for Urban Innovation, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Joseph Henry
- Institute for Mind and Biology, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA.,Department of Psychology, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Muxuan Lyu
- Department of Psychology, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Dario Maestripieri
- Department of Comparative Human Development, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA.,Institute for Mind and Biology, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Marc G Berman
- Department of Psychology, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA.,Grossman Institute for Neuroscience, Quantitative Biology, and Human Behavior, Chicago, IL, USA
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
|
26
|
White H, Jubran R, Heck A, Chroust A, Bhatt RS. Sex-specific scanning in infancy: Developmental changes in the use of face/head and body information. J Exp Child Psychol 2019; 182:126-143. [PMID: 30825728 PMCID: PMC6414250 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2019.01.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/13/2018] [Revised: 12/07/2018] [Accepted: 01/07/2019] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
The current investigation sought to differentiate between contrasting perspectives of body knowledge development by determining whether infants' adult-like scanning of male and female bodies is dependent on relevant information from the face/head alone, the body alone, or a combination of both sources. Scanning patterns of 3.5-, 6.5-, and 9-month-olds (N = 80) in response to images that contained information relevant to sex classification in either the face/head or the body were examined. The results indicate that sex-specific scanning in the presence of only one source of relevant information (i.e., face/head or body) is present only at 9 months. Thus, although sex-specific scanning of bodies emerges as early as 3.5 months, information from both faces/heads and bodies is required until sometime between 6.5 and 9 months of age. These findings constrain theories of the development of social perception by documenting the complex interplay between body and face/head processing early in life.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hannah White
- University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY 40506, USA
| | | | - Alison Heck
- University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY 40506, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
27
|
Kelly DJ, Duarte S, Meary D, Bindemann M, Pascalis O. Infants rapidly detect human faces in complex naturalistic visual scenes. Dev Sci 2019; 22:e12829. [PMID: 30896078 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12829] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/26/2018] [Revised: 03/13/2019] [Accepted: 03/16/2019] [Indexed: 01/23/2023]
Abstract
Infants respond preferentially to faces and face-like stimuli from birth, but past research has typically presented faces in isolation or amongst an artificial array of competing objects. In the current study infants aged 3- to 12-months viewed a series of complex visual scenes; half of the scenes contained a person, the other half did not. Infants rapidly detected and oriented to faces in scenes even when they were not visually salient. Although a clear developmental improvement was observed in face detection and interest, all infants displayed sensitivity to the presence of a person in a scene, by displaying eye movements that differed quantifiably across a range of measures when viewing scenes that either did or did not contain a person. We argue that infant's face detection capabilities are ostensibly "better" with naturalistic stimuli and artificial array presentations used in previous studies have underestimated performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David J Kelly
- School of Psychology, Keynes College, University of Kent, Canterbury, Kent, UK
| | - Sofia Duarte
- School of Psychology, Keynes College, University of Kent, Canterbury, Kent, UK
| | | | - Markus Bindemann
- School of Psychology, Keynes College, University of Kent, Canterbury, Kent, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
28
|
van den Boomen C, Munsters NM, Kemner C. Emotion processing in the infant brain: The importance of local information. Neuropsychologia 2019; 126:62-68. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.09.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/01/2017] [Revised: 08/30/2017] [Accepted: 09/04/2017] [Indexed: 12/22/2022]
|
29
|
Rigato S, Banissy MJ, Romanska A, Thomas R, van Velzen J, Bremner AJ. Cortical signatures of vicarious tactile experience in four-month-old infants. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2019; 35:75-80. [PMID: 28942240 PMCID: PMC6968956 DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.09.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/29/2017] [Revised: 06/30/2017] [Accepted: 09/11/2017] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
The human brain recruits similar brain regions when a state is experienced (e.g., touch, pain, actions) and when that state is passively observed in other individuals. In adults, seeing other people being touched activates similar brain areas as when we experience touch ourselves. Here we show that already by four months of age, cortical responses to tactile stimulation are modulated by visual information specifying another person being touched. We recorded somatosensory evoked potentials (SEPs) in 4-month-old infants while they were presented with brief vibrotactile stimuli to the hands. At the same time that the tactile stimuli were presented the infants observed another person's hand being touched by a soft paintbrush or approached by the paintbrush which then touched the surface next to their hand. A prominent positive peak in SEPs contralateral to the site of tactile stimulation around 130 ms after the tactile stimulus onset was of a significantly larger amplitude for the "Surface" trials than for the "Hand" trials. These findings indicate that, even at four months of age, somatosensory cortex is not only involved in the personal experience of touch but can also be vicariously recruited by seeing other people being touched.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Silvia Rigato
- Centre for Brain Science, Department of Psychology, University of Essex, Colchester, CO4 3SQ, UK
| | - Michael J Banissy
- Sensorimotor Development Research Unit, Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London, London, SE14 6NW, UK
| | - Aleksandra Romanska
- Sensorimotor Development Research Unit, Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London, London, SE14 6NW, UK
| | - Rhiannon Thomas
- Sensorimotor Development Research Unit, Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London, London, SE14 6NW, UK
| | - José van Velzen
- Sensorimotor Development Research Unit, Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London, London, SE14 6NW, UK
| | - Andrew J Bremner
- Sensorimotor Development Research Unit, Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London, London, SE14 6NW, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Tamir DI, Hughes BL. Social Rewards: From Basic Social Building Blocks to Complex Social Behavior. PERSPECTIVES ON PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2018; 13:700-717. [DOI: 10.1177/1745691618776263] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/10/2023]
Abstract
Humans are social creatures, engaging almost constantly in social behaviors that serve ultimate social goals, such as forming strong bonds with one another. However, most social behaviors provide only incremental progress toward an ultimate goal. Instead, the drive to engage in any individual social act may derive from its proximal value rather than its ultimate goal. Thus, this proximal value forms the foundation on which the complexities of human sociality are built. We describe two complementary approaches for using proximal social rewards to understand social behaviors and their ultimate goals: (a) decontextualizing social rewards—paring down complex social interactions can help identify which basic building blocks remain valuable even in minimalistic contexts—and (b) recontextualizing social rewards—reintroducing motivational and contextual factors into the study of social experience can help identify how proximal rewards serve their ultimate function. We discuss how this dual-approach framework can inform future research by bridging basic social building blocks and real-world social goals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Diana I. Tamir
- Department of Psychology and Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University
| | - Brent L. Hughes
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Costa M, Gomez A, Barat E, Lio G, Duhamel JR, Sirigu A. Implicit preference for human trustworthy faces in macaque monkeys. Nat Commun 2018; 9:4529. [PMID: 30375399 PMCID: PMC6207650 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-018-06987-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/26/2018] [Accepted: 09/29/2018] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
It has been shown that human judgements of trustworthiness are based on subtle processing of specific facial features. However, it is not known if this ability is a specifically human function, or whether it is shared among primates. Here we report that macaque monkeys (Macaca Mulatta and Macaca Fascicularis), like humans, display a preferential attention to trustworthiness-associated facial cues in computer-generated human faces. Monkeys looked significantly longer at faces categorized a priori as trustworthy compared to untrustworthy. In addition, spatial sequential analysis of monkeys’ initial saccades revealed an upward shift with attention moving to the eye region for trustworthy faces while no change was observed for the untrustworthy ones. Finally, we found significant correlations between facial width-to-height ratio– a morphometric feature that predicts trustworthiness’ judgments in humans – and looking time in both species. These findings suggest the presence of common mechanisms among primates for first impression of trustworthiness. Humans infer the trustworthiness of others based on subtle facial features such as the facial width-to-height ratio, but it is not known whether other primates are sensitive to these cues. Here, the authors show that macaque monkeys prefer to look at human faces which appear trustworthy to humans.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Manuela Costa
- Institut des Sciences Cognitives Marc Jeannerod, CNRS, UCBL, Lyon 1, 67, boulevard Pinel, 69675, Bron, Cedex, France
| | - Alice Gomez
- Institut des Sciences Cognitives Marc Jeannerod, CNRS, UCBL, Lyon 1, 67, boulevard Pinel, 69675, Bron, Cedex, France
| | - Elodie Barat
- Institut des Sciences Cognitives Marc Jeannerod, CNRS, UCBL, Lyon 1, 67, boulevard Pinel, 69675, Bron, Cedex, France
| | - Guillaume Lio
- Institut des Sciences Cognitives Marc Jeannerod, CNRS, UCBL, Lyon 1, 67, boulevard Pinel, 69675, Bron, Cedex, France
| | - Jean-René Duhamel
- Institut des Sciences Cognitives Marc Jeannerod, CNRS, UCBL, Lyon 1, 67, boulevard Pinel, 69675, Bron, Cedex, France
| | - Angela Sirigu
- Institut des Sciences Cognitives Marc Jeannerod, CNRS, UCBL, Lyon 1, 67, boulevard Pinel, 69675, Bron, Cedex, France.
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Hadders-Algra M. Early human motor development: From variation to the ability to vary and adapt. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2018; 90:411-427. [PMID: 29752957 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.05.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 137] [Impact Index Per Article: 19.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/22/2018] [Revised: 05/01/2018] [Accepted: 05/04/2018] [Indexed: 12/17/2022]
Abstract
This review summarizes early human motor development. From early fetal age motor behavior is based on spontaneous neural activity: activity of networks in the brainstem and spinal cord that is modulated by supraspinal activity. The supraspinal activity, first primarily brought about by the cortical subplate, later by the cortical plate, induces movement variation. Initially, movement variation especially serves exploration; its associated afferent information is primarily used to sculpt the developing nervous system, and less to adapt motor behavior. In the next phase, beginning at function-specific ages, movement variation starts to serve adaptation. In sucking and swallowing, this phase emerges shortly before term age. In speech, gross and fine motor development, it emerges from 3 to 4 months post-term onwards, i.e., when developmental focus in the primary sensory and motor cortices has shifted to the permanent cortical circuitries. With increasing age and increasing trial-and-error exploration, the infant improves its ability to use adaptive and efficicient forms of upright gross motor behavior, manual activities and vocalizations belonging to the native language.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mijna Hadders-Algra
- University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Dept. Pediatrics - Section Developmental Neurology, Groningen, The Netherlands.
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Proietti V, Rigoldi M, Croci E, Macchi Cassia V. Sibling experience modulates perceptual narrowing toward adult faces in the first year of life. Dev Psychobiol 2018. [PMID: 29528119 DOI: 10.1002/dev.21616] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
During the first year of life face discrimination abilities narrow toward adult human faces of the most frequently encountered ethnic group/s. Earlier studies showed that perceptual learning under laboratory-training protocols can modulate this narrowing process. Here we investigated whether natural experience acquired in everyday settings with an older sibling's face can shape the trajectory of perceptual narrowing towards adult faces. Using an infant-controlled habituation procedure we measured discrimination of adult (Experiment 1) and child faces (Experiment 2) in 3- and 9- month-old infants with and without a child sibling. Discrimination of adult faces was observed for infants at both ages, although accompanied by posthabituation preferences in opposite directions, whereas at both ages the discrimination of child faces critically depended on sibling experience. These results provide the first evidence that natural experience acquired with siblings affects the tuning properties of infant face representation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Valentina Proietti
- Department of Psychology, Nipissing University, North Bay, Ontario, Canada.,NeuroMi, Milan Center for Neuroscience, Milan, Italy
| | - Marta Rigoldi
- Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milan, Italy
| | - Emanuela Croci
- Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milan, Italy
| | - Viola Macchi Cassia
- NeuroMi, Milan Center for Neuroscience, Milan, Italy.,Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milan, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Reynolds GD, Roth KC. The Development of Attentional Biases for Faces in Infancy: A Developmental Systems Perspective. Front Psychol 2018; 9:222. [PMID: 29541043 PMCID: PMC5835799 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00222] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/30/2017] [Accepted: 02/09/2018] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
We present an integrative review of research and theory on major factors involved in the early development of attentional biases to faces. Research utilizing behavioral, eye-tracking, and neuroscience measures with infant participants as well as comparative research with animal subjects are reviewed. We begin with coverage of research demonstrating the presence of an attentional bias for faces shortly after birth, such as newborn infants' visual preference for face-like over non-face stimuli. The role of experience and the process of perceptual narrowing in face processing are examined as infants begin to demonstrate enhanced behavioral and neural responsiveness to mother over stranger, female over male, own- over other-race, and native over non-native faces. Next, we cover research on developmental change in infants' neural responsiveness to faces in multimodal contexts, such as audiovisual speech. We also explore the potential influence of arousal and attention on early perceptual preferences for faces. Lastly, the potential influence of the development of attention systems in the brain on social-cognitive processing is discussed. In conclusion, we interpret the findings under the framework of Developmental Systems Theory, emphasizing the combined and distributed influence of several factors, both internal (e.g., arousal, neural development) and external (e.g., early social experience) to the developing child, in the emergence of attentional biases that lead to enhanced responsiveness and processing of faces commonly encountered in the native environment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Greg D. Reynolds
- Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Psychology, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN, United States
| | | |
Collapse
|
35
|
Taylor DJ, Smith ND, Binns AM, Crabb DP. The effect of non-neovascular age-related macular degeneration on face recognition performance. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol 2018; 256:815-821. [PMID: 29484559 PMCID: PMC5856898 DOI: 10.1007/s00417-017-3879-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/10/2017] [Revised: 12/06/2017] [Accepted: 12/14/2017] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Purpose There is a well-established research base surrounding face recognition in patients with age-related macular degeneration (AMD). However, much of this existing research does not differentiate between results obtained for ‘wet’ AMD and ‘dry’ AMD. Here, we test the hypothesis that face recognition performance is worse in patients with dry AMD compared with visually healthy peers. Methods Patients (>60 years of age, logMAR binocular visual acuity 0.7 or better) with dry AMD of varying severity and visually healthy age-related peers (controls) completed a modified version of the Cambridge Face Memory Test (CFMT). Percentage of correctly identified faces was used as an outcome measure for performance for each participant. A 90% normative reference limit was generated from the distribution of CFMT scores recorded in the visually healthy controls. Scores for AMD participants were then specifically compared to this limit, and comparisons between average scores in the AMD severity groups were investigated. Results Thirty patients (median [interquartile range] age of 76 [70, 79] years) and 34 controls (median age of 70 [64, 75] years) were examined. Four, seventeen and nine patients were classified as having early, intermediate and late AMD (geographic atrophy) respectively. Five (17%) patients recorded a face recognition performance worse than the 90% limit (Fisher’s exact test, p = 0.46) set by controls; four of these had geographic atrophy. Patients with geographic atrophy identified fewer faces on average (±SD) (61% ± 22%) than those with early and intermediate AMD (75 ± 11%) and controls (74% ± 11%). Conclusions People with dry AMD may not suffer from problems with face recognition until the disease is in its later stages; those with late AMD (geographic atrophy) are likely to have difficulty recognising faces. The results from this study should influence the management and expectations of patients with dry AMD in both community practice and hospital clinics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Deanna J Taylor
- Division of Optometry and Visual Science, School of Health Sciences, City, University of London, Northampton Square, London, EC1V 0HB, UK
| | - Nicholas D Smith
- Division of Optometry and Visual Science, School of Health Sciences, City, University of London, Northampton Square, London, EC1V 0HB, UK
| | - Alison M Binns
- Division of Optometry and Visual Science, School of Health Sciences, City, University of London, Northampton Square, London, EC1V 0HB, UK
| | - David P Crabb
- Division of Optometry and Visual Science, School of Health Sciences, City, University of London, Northampton Square, London, EC1V 0HB, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
White H, Hock A, Jubran R, Heck A, Bhatt RS. Visual scanning of male and female bodies in infancy. J Exp Child Psychol 2018; 166:79-95. [PMID: 28888194 PMCID: PMC5724933 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.08.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/18/2017] [Revised: 08/10/2017] [Accepted: 08/11/2017] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
This study addressed the development of attention to information that is socially relevant to adults by examining infants' (N=64) scanning patterns of male and female bodies. Infants exhibited systematic attention to regions associated with sex-related scanning by adults, with 3.5- and 6.5-month-olds looking longer at the torsos of females than of males and looking longer at the legs of males than of females. However, this pattern of looking was not found when infants were tested on headless bodies in Experiment 2, suggesting that infants' differential gaze pattern in Experiment 1 was not due to low-level stimulus features, such as clothing, and also indicating that facial/head information is necessary for infants to exhibit sex-specific scanning. We discuss implications for models of face and body knowledge development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hannah White
- University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY 40506, USA
| | - Alyson Hock
- University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY 40506, USA
| | | | - Alison Heck
- University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY 40506, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
37
|
Caeiro C, Guo K, Mills D. Dogs and humans respond to emotionally competent stimuli by producing different facial actions. Sci Rep 2017; 7:15525. [PMID: 29138393 PMCID: PMC5686192 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-017-15091-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/25/2017] [Accepted: 10/20/2017] [Indexed: 01/17/2023] Open
Abstract
The commonality of facial expressions of emotion has been studied in different species since Darwin, with most of the research focusing on closely related primate species. However, it is unclear to what extent there exists common facial expression in species more phylogenetically distant, but sharing a need for common interspecific emotional understanding. Here we used the objective, anatomically-based tools, FACS and DogFACS (Facial Action Coding Systems), to quantify and compare human and domestic dog facial expressions in response to emotionally-competent stimuli associated with different categories of emotional arousal. We sought to answer two questions: Firstly, do dogs display specific discriminatory facial movements in response to different categories of emotional stimuli? Secondly, do dogs display similar facial movements to humans when reacting in emotionally comparable contexts? We found that dogs displayed distinctive facial actions depending on the category of stimuli. However, dogs produced different facial movements to humans in comparable states of emotional arousal. These results refute the commonality of emotional expression across mammals, since dogs do not display human-like facial expressions. Given the unique interspecific relationship between dogs and humans, two highly social but evolutionarily distant species sharing a common environment, these findings give new insight into the origin of emotion expression.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cátia Caeiro
- School of Psychology, University of Lincoln, Lincoln, UK. .,School of Life Sciences, University of Lincoln, Lincoln, UK.
| | - Kun Guo
- School of Psychology, University of Lincoln, Lincoln, UK
| | - Daniel Mills
- School of Life Sciences, University of Lincoln, Lincoln, UK
| |
Collapse
|
38
|
Damon F, Mottier H, Méary D, Pascalis O. A Review of Attractiveness Preferences in Infancy: From Faces to Objects. ADAPTIVE HUMAN BEHAVIOR AND PHYSIOLOGY 2017. [DOI: 10.1007/s40750-017-0071-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
|
39
|
Abstract
Here we aim to lay the theoretical foundations of human-robot relationship drawing upon insights from disciplines that govern relevant human behaviors: ecology and ethology. We show how the paradox of the so called “uncanny valley hypothesis” can be solved by applying the “niche” concept to social robots, and relying on the natural behavior of humans. Instead of striving to build human-like social robots, engineers should construct robots that are able to maximize their performance in their niche (being optimal for some specific functions), and if they are endowed with appropriate form of social competence then humans will eventually interact with them independent of their embodiment. This new discipline, which we call ethorobotics, could change social robotics, giving a boost to new technical approaches and applications.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ádám Miklósi
- Department of Ethology, Eötvös Loránd UniversityBudapest, Hungary.,Magyar Tudományos Akadémia - Eötvös Loránd University Comparative Ethology Research GroupBudapest, Hungary
| | - Péter Korondi
- Department of Mechatronics, Optics and Information Engineering, Budapest University of Technology and EconomicsBudapest, Hungary
| | - Vicente Matellán
- Departamento Ingeniería Mecánica, Informática y Aeroespacial, Universidad de LeónLeón, Spain
| | - Márta Gácsi
- Magyar Tudományos Akadémia - Eötvös Loránd University Comparative Ethology Research GroupBudapest, Hungary
| |
Collapse
|
40
|
Hyde DC, Flom R, Porter CL. Behavioral and Neural Foundations of Multisensory Face-Voice Perception in Infancy. Dev Neuropsychol 2017; 41:273-292. [PMID: 28059567 DOI: 10.1080/87565641.2016.1255744] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
In this article, we describe behavioral and neurophysiological evidence for infants' multimodal face-voice perception. We argue that the behavioral development of face-voice perception, like multimodal perception more broadly, is consistent with the intersensory redundancy hypothesis (IRH). Furthermore, we highlight that several recently observed features of the neural responses in infants converge with the behavioral predictions of the intersensory redundancy hypothesis. Finally, we discuss the potential benefits of combining brain and behavioral measures to study multisensory processing, as well as some applications of this work for atypical development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel C Hyde
- a Department of Psychology , University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign , Champaign , Illinois
| | - Ross Flom
- b Department of Psychology , Brigham Young University , Provo , Utah
| | - Chris L Porter
- c School of Family Life , Brigham Young University , Provo , Utah
| |
Collapse
|
41
|
A comparison between preterm and full‐term infants’ preference for faces. JORNAL DE PEDIATRIA (VERSÃO EM PORTUGUÊS) 2017. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jpedp.2016.07.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
|
42
|
Simpson EA, Jakobsen KV, Damon F, Suomi SJ, Ferrari PF, Paukner A. Face Detection and the Development of Own-Species Bias in Infant Macaques. Child Dev 2017; 88:103-113. [PMID: 27223687 PMCID: PMC5123966 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12565] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
In visually complex environments, numerous items compete for attention. Infants may exhibit attentional efficiency-privileged detection, attention capture, and holding-for face-like stimuli. However, it remains unknown when these biases develop and what role, if any, experience plays in this emerging skill. Here, nursery-reared infant macaques' (Macaca mulatta; n = 10) attention to faces in 10-item arrays of nonfaces was measured using eye tracking. With limited face experience, 3-week-old monkeys were more likely to detect faces and looked longer at faces compared to nonfaces, suggesting a robust face detection system. By 3 months, after peer exposure, infants looked faster to conspecific faces but not heterospecific faces, suggesting an own-species bias in face attention capture, consistent with perceptual attunement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elizabeth A. Simpson
- Social Cognition Laboratory, Department of Psychology, University of Miami, Coral Gables, Florida, 33124 USA
| | - Krisztina V. Jakobsen
- Cognitive Development Laboratory, Department of Psychology, James Madison University, Harrisonburg, Virginia, 22807 USA
| | - Fabrice Damon
- Laboratoire de Psychologie et NeuroCognition, Université Grenoble Alpes, Grenoble, France
| | - Stephen J. Suomi
- Laboratory of Comparative Ethology, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, National Institutes of Health, Department of Health and Human Services, Poolesville, Maryland, 20837 USA
| | - Pier F. Ferrari
- Dipartimento di Neuroscienze, Università di Parma, Parma, 4300 Italy
| | - Annika Paukner
- Laboratory of Comparative Ethology, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, National Institutes of Health, Department of Health and Human Services, Poolesville, Maryland, 20837 USA
| |
Collapse
|
43
|
Pereira SA, Pereira Junior A, Costa MFD, Monteiro MDV, Almeida VAD, Fonseca Filho GGD, Arrais N, Simion F. A comparison between preterm and full-term infants' preference for faces. J Pediatr (Rio J) 2017; 93:35-39. [PMID: 27435358 DOI: 10.1016/j.jped.2016.04.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 42] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/02/2016] [Revised: 04/28/2016] [Accepted: 04/28/2016] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Visual preference for faces at birth is the product of a multimodal sensory experience experienced by the fetus even during the gestational period. The ability to recognize faces allows an ecologically advantageous interaction with the social environment. However, perinatal events such as premature birth, may adversely affect the adequate development of this capacity. In this study, we evaluated the preference for facial stimuli in preterm infants within the first few hours after birth. METHODS This is a cross-sectional observational study of 59 newborns, 28 preterm and 31 full-term infants. The babies were assessed in the first hours of life, with two white boards in the shape of a head and neck: one with the drawing of a face similar to the human face (natural face), and one with the drawing of misaligned eyes, mouth and nose (distorted face). After the newborn fixated the eyes on the presented stimulus, it was slowly moved along the visual field. The recognition of the stimulus was considered present when the baby had eye or head movements toward the stimulus. RESULTS The preterm infants, in addition to showing a lower occurrence of orientation movements for both stimuli, on average (1.8±1.1 to natural faces and 2.0±1.2 for distorted ones) also showed no preference for any of them (p=0.35). Full-term newborns showed a different behavior, in which they showed a preference for natural faces (p=0.002) and a higher number of orientations for the stimulus, for both natural (3.2±0.8) and distorted faces (2.5±0.9). CONCLUSION Preterm newborns recognize facial stimuli and disclose no preference for natural faces, different from full-term newborns.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Silvana A Pereira
- Universidade de São Paulo (USP), Instituto de Psicologia, Departamento de Psicologia Experimental, São Paulo, SP, Brazil; Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte (UFRN), Natal, RN, Brazil; Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte (UFRN), Faculdade de Ciências da Saúde do Trairí (FACISA), Natal, RN, Brazil.
| | | | - Marcelo F da Costa
- Universidade de São Paulo (USP), Instituto de Psicologia, Departamento de Psicologia Experimental, São Paulo, SP, Brazil
| | | | - Valéria A de Almeida
- Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte (UFRN), Faculdade de Ciências da Saúde do Trairí (FACISA), Natal, RN, Brazil; Hospital Universitário Ana Bezerra (HUAB), Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte (UFRN), Santa Cruz, RN, Brazil
| | - Gentil G da Fonseca Filho
- Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte (UFRN), Faculdade de Ciências da Saúde do Trairí (FACISA), Natal, RN, Brazil
| | - Nívia Arrais
- Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte (UFRN), Natal, RN, Brazil; Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte (UFRN), Maternidade Escola Januário Cicco, Natal, RN, Brazil
| | - Francesca Simion
- Centro Neuroscienze Cognitive, Universita di Padova, Dipartimento di Psicologia dello Sviluppo e della Socializzazione, Padova, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
44
|
Galati A, Hock A, Bhatt RS. Perceptual learning and face processing in infancy. Dev Psychobiol 2016; 58:829-840. [PMID: 27753459 PMCID: PMC6326576 DOI: 10.1002/dev.21420] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/17/2015] [Accepted: 04/01/2016] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Configural information (spacing between features) contributes to face-processing expertise in adulthood. We examined whether infants can be "trained" to process this information. In Experiment 1, 3.5-month-olds failed to discriminate changes in the spacing between facial features. However, in Experiments 2 and 3, infants processed the same information after being primed with faces in which the spacing was repeatedly altered. Experiment 4 found that priming was not effective with inverted faces or with faces depicting changes in features but not relations among features, indicating that the priming exhibited in Experiments 2 and 3 was specific to upright faces depicting spacing changes. Thus, even young infants who do not readily process facial configural information can be induced to do so through priming. These findings suggest that learning to encode critical structural information contributes to the development of face processing expertise.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ashley Galati
- Kent State University at Tuscarawas, New Philadelphia, Ohio
| | - Alyson Hock
- Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky
| | - Ramesh S Bhatt
- Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky.
| |
Collapse
|
45
|
Fausey CM, Jayaraman S, Smith LB. From faces to hands: Changing visual input in the first two years. Cognition 2016; 152:101-107. [PMID: 27043744 PMCID: PMC4856551 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.03.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 130] [Impact Index Per Article: 14.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/12/2015] [Revised: 03/07/2016] [Accepted: 03/08/2016] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Human development takes place in a social context. Two pervasive sources of social information are faces and hands. Here, we provide the first report of the visual frequency of faces and hands in the everyday scenes available to infants. These scenes were collected by having infants wear head cameras during unconstrained everyday activities. Our corpus of 143hours of infant-perspective scenes, collected from 34 infants aged 1month to 2years, was sampled for analysis at 1/5Hz. The major finding from this corpus is that the faces and hands of social partners are not equally available throughout the first two years of life. Instead, there is an earlier period of dense face input and a later period of dense hand input. At all ages, hands in these scenes were primarily in contact with objects and the spatio-temporal co-occurrence of hands and faces was greater than expected by chance. The orderliness of the shift from faces to hands suggests a principled transition in the contents of visual experiences and is discussed in terms of the role of developmental gates on the timing and statistics of visual experiences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Caitlin M Fausey
- Department of Psychology, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR 97403, United States.
| | - Swapnaa Jayaraman
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405, United States
| | - Linda B Smith
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405, United States
| |
Collapse
|
46
|
Abstract
Researchers studying a range of psychological phenomena (e.g., theory of mind, emotion, stereotyping and prejudice, interpersonal attraction, etc.) sometimes employ photographs of people as stimuli. In this paper, we introduce the Chicago Face Database, a free resource consisting of 158 high-resolution, standardized photographs of Black and White males and females between the ages of 18 and 40 years and extensive data about these targets. In Study 1, we report pre-testing of these faces, which includes both subjective norming data and objective physical measurements of the images included in the database. In Study 2 we surveyed psychology researchers to assess the suitability of these targets for research purposes and explored factors that were associated with researchers' judgments of suitability. Instructions are outlined for those interested in obtaining access to the stimulus set and accompanying ratings and measures.
Collapse
|
47
|
|
48
|
Droit-Volet S, Fayolle S, Gil S. Emotion and Time Perception in Children and Adults: The Effect of Task Difficulty. TIMING & TIME PERCEPTION 2016. [DOI: 10.1163/22134468-03002055] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/22/2022]
Abstract
In the present study, adults and children aged five and eight years were given a temporal bisection task involving emotional stimuli (angry and neutral faces) and three levels of discrimination difficulty that differed as a function of the ratio used between the short and the long standard duration (very easy, easy, and difficult). In addition, their cognitive capacities in terms of working memory and attention inhibition were assessed by neuropsychological tests. In the very easy temporal task (ratio of 1:4), the results showed that the psychophysical functions were shifted toward the left in all participants for the angry faces compared to the neutral faces, with a significant lowering of the Bisection Point, suggesting that the stimulus duration was judged to last longer for the emotional stimuli. In addition, the results did not show any relationship between the magnitude of this lengthening effect and individual cognitive capacities as assessed by the neuropsychological tests. The individual differences in working memory capacities only explained differences in sensitivity to time. However, when the difficulty of the temporal task increased, the children’s performance decreased and it was no longer possible to test for the emotional effect. Unlike the children, the adults were still able to discriminate time in the emotional task. However, the emotional effect was no longer observed. In conclusion, our study on temporal task difficulty shows the influence of available cognitive resources on the emergence of an emotional effect on time perception.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - S. Gil
- Université de Poitiers (CeRCA)France
| |
Collapse
|
49
|
Bhatt RS, Hock A, White H, Jubran R, Galati A. The Development of Body Structure Knowledge in Infancy. CHILD DEVELOPMENT PERSPECTIVES 2016; 10:45-52. [PMID: 28663770 PMCID: PMC5486992 DOI: 10.1111/cdep.12162] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Although we know much about the development of face processing, we know considerably less about the development of body knowledge-despite bodies also being significant sources of social information. One set of studies indicated that body structure knowledge is poor during the 1st year of life and spawned a model that posits that, unlike the development of face knowledge, which benefits from innate propensities and dedicated learning mechanisms, the development of body knowledge relies on general learning mechanisms and develops slowly. In this article, we review studies on infants' knowledge about the structure of bodies and their processing of gender and emotion that paint a different picture. Although questions remain, a general social cognition system likely engenders similar trajectories of development of knowledge about faces and bodies, and may equip developing infants with the capacity to obtain socially critical information from many sources.
Collapse
|
50
|
Ploeger A, van der Hoort B. Evolutionary Psychology as a Metatheory for the Social Sciences: How to Gather Interdisciplinary Evidence for a Psychological Adaptation. REVIEW OF GENERAL PSYCHOLOGY 2015. [DOI: 10.1037/gpr0000052] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Evolutionary psychology has been proposed as a new metatheory for the social sciences ( Buss, 1995 ). Evolutionary psychology is an approach that emphasizes the evolutionary background of psychological phenomena (e.g., cognition, motivation, perception), with the expectation that knowledge about this background enhances our understanding of the working of the present human mind. This proposal has met with both enthusiasm and criticism. An important criticism is that it is hard, if possible at all, to find empirical evidence for a hypothesized psychological adaptation. This criticism has been addressed with the proposal to build a nomological network of evidence around a hypothesized psychological adaptation ( Schmitt & Pilcher, 2004 ). In this article, we show that it is possible to use this nomological network of evidence to support the hypothesis that face recognition is an adaptation. We reviewed the literature on face recognition from different disciplines (psychology, medicine, neuroscience, genetics, primatology, and anthropology) and conclude that there is an extensive network of evidence for the proposed hypothesis. We argue that building a nomological network of evidence is a promising way to address several criticisms of evolutionary psychology, and that such a network can serve as a metatheoretical framework for the social sciences.
Collapse
|