1
|
Vijayan K. K. V, De Paris K. Nonhuman primate models of pediatric viral diseases. Front Cell Infect Microbiol 2024; 14:1493885. [PMID: 39691699 PMCID: PMC11649651 DOI: 10.3389/fcimb.2024.1493885] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/09/2024] [Accepted: 11/08/2024] [Indexed: 12/19/2024] Open
Abstract
Infectious diseases are the leading cause of death in infants and children under 5 years of age. In utero exposure to viruses can lead to spontaneous abortion, preterm birth, congenital abnormalities or other developmental defects, often resulting in lifelong health sequalae. The underlying biological mechanisms are difficult to study in humans due to ethical concerns and limited sample access. Nonhuman primates (NHP) are closely related to humans, and pregnancy and immune ontogeny in infants are very similar to humans. Therefore, NHP are a highly relevant model for understanding fetal and postnatal virus-host interactions and to define immune mechanisms associated with increased morbidity and mortality in infants. We will discuss NHP models of viruses causing congenital infections, respiratory diseases in early life, and HIV. Cytomegalovirus (CMV) remains the most common cause of congenital defects worldwide. Measles is a vaccine-preventable disease, yet measles cases are resurging. Zika is an example of an emerging arbovirus with devastating consequences for the developing fetus and the surviving infant. Among the respiratory viruses, we will discuss influenza and Severe Acute Respiratory Syndrome Coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2). We will finish with HIV as an example of a lifelong infection without a cure or vaccine. The review will highlight (i) the impact of viral infections on fetal and infant immune development, (ii) how differences in infant and adult immune responses to infection alter disease outcome, and emphasize the invaluable contribution of pediatric NHP infection models to the design of effective treatment and prevention strategies, including vaccines, for human infants.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Vidya Vijayan K. K.
- Department of Microbiology and Immunology, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, United States
| | - Kristina De Paris
- Department of Microbiology and Immunology, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, United States
- Center for AIDS Research, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, United States
- Children’s Research Institute, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, United States
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Chan K, O T, Broude C, Waner S, Ceisler E, Fay A, Pacicco L, Waner M. Advantages of Early Surgical Management of Periorbital Infantile Hemangiomas. Facial Plast Surg Aesthet Med 2024; 26:722-729. [PMID: 38946543 DOI: 10.1089/fpsam.2023.0012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 07/02/2024] Open
Abstract
Background: Early evaluation and treatment of periorbital infantile hemangiomas (POIH) were associated with lower rates of ophthalmological complications. Objective: To evaluate age and characteristics associated with improved anisometropic astigmatism (anisoastigmatism) and eye symmetry measured by diopters and a 5-point scale, respectively, in patients with POIH treated with surgical excision. Methods: A retrospective study was performed on patients with POIH. Patient characteristics and eye symmetry were analyzed between patients with resolved and unresolved anisoastigmatism after surgery. Statistical analyses included the Mann-Whitney U tests, chi-square tests, and linear regression models. Results: In total, 54 patients were included (male: 20, female: 34). Upper medial eyelid was the most commonly affected site (resolved: 45%, unresolved: 43%), followed by upper lateral and upper central. Fifty-six percent (31/55) had postoperative resolution of anisoastigmatism, whereas 44% (24/55) did not. Earlier surgical evaluation (median: 4.5 vs. 6.0 months, p = 0.047) and excision (median: 5.0 vs. 12.0 months, p = 0.005) were associated with reversible anisoastigmatism. Good and suboptimal eye symmetry were not associated with earlier surgical excision (median: 6 vs. 6.5 months, p = 0.87). Follow-up ranged from 1 month to 12 years. Conclusion: Earlier surgical excision was associated with reversing anisoastigmatism but was not significant for improving eye symmetry.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kimberly Chan
- Vascular Birthmark Institute of New York, New York, New York, USA
- Department of Otolaryngology-Head and Neck Surgery, The Pennsylvania State University, College of Medicine, Hershey, Pennsylvania, USA
| | - Teresa O
- Vascular Birthmark Institute of New York, New York, New York, USA
- Department of Otolaryngology-Lenox Hill Hospital & Manhattan Eye, Ear and Throat Hospital, New York, New York, USA
| | - Caroline Broude
- Vascular Birthmark Institute of New York, New York, New York, USA
| | - Stefan Waner
- Department of Mathematics, Hofstra University, Hempstead, New York, USA
| | - Emily Ceisler
- Department of Ophthalmology, New York University Grossman School of Medicine, New York, New York, USA
| | - Aaron Fay
- Vascular Birthmark Institute of New York, New York, New York, USA
- Department of Ophthalmology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
| | - Lauren Pacicco
- Vascular Birthmark Institute of New York, New York, New York, USA
| | - Milton Waner
- Vascular Birthmark Institute of New York, New York, New York, USA
- Department of Otolaryngology-Lenox Hill Hospital & Manhattan Eye, Ear and Throat Hospital, New York, New York, USA
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Ma Z, Zhu S. Rare micropupil secondary to congenital cataract surgery favoring the development of the affected eye: a case report. BMC Ophthalmol 2024; 24:258. [PMID: 38877448 PMCID: PMC11179215 DOI: 10.1186/s12886-024-03507-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/22/2023] [Accepted: 06/03/2024] [Indexed: 06/16/2024] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Congenital microcoria has been extensively reported and usually leads to visual dysfunction or blindness. However, micropupil development secondary to cataract surgery has never been reported. Here, we describe a rare case of micropupil development in infancy that occurred secondary to combined cataract extraction and intraocular lens implantation for treatment of congenital cataract. When the patient reached adulthood, the affected eye not only gained good vision but also showed better ocular development and refractive status than the fellow eye. CASE PRESENTATION A 17-year-old boy presented to our outpatient clinic with decreased vision in his left eye related to congenital cataract surgery at 6 months of age. The affected eye had exhibited a pinhole pupil since the third month postoperatively. The condition had been managed with observation and regular monocular occlusion treatment. Upon presentation to our clinic, the best-corrected visual acuity (BCVA) in his fellow eye was 0.0 logMAR(20/20) with a refraction of - 5.75 diopters cylinder/-2.25 diopters sphere, and the BCVA in his affected eye was 0.5 logMAR(20/40) with a refraction of 0.00 diopters. Ophthalmic examination of the affected eye revealed a pinhole pupil (approximately 0.5 mm) with high light reflex sensitivity but no response to pupil-dilating drugs. The patient underwent pupilloplasty of the affected eye under corneal surface anesthesia. Postoperative examination revealed better ocular development in the affected eye than in the fellow eye (axial length: 24.21 vs. 27.02 mm, respectively) as well as better refractive status in the affected eye (BCVA of 0.0 logMAR(20/20) with a refraction of - 2.23 diopters cylinder/-3.00 diopters sphere vs. 0logMAR(20/20) with a refraction of -5.75 diopters cylinder/-2.25 diopters sphere). CONCLUSIONS We have reported a rare case of micropupil development secondary to congenital cataract surgery, which is an uncommon complication, especially in children. However, unlike congenital microcoria, the secondary pinhole pupil may have reduced imaging haze and halos, possibly favoring the development of the affected eye. This case provides further insight into the treatment of congenital cataract.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zicheng Ma
- Department of Ophthalmology, Beijing AnZhen Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, 100029, China
| | - Siquan Zhu
- Department of Ophthalmology, Beijing AnZhen Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, 100029, China.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Rodríguez Deliz CL, Lee GM, Bushnell BN, Majaj NJ, Movshon JA, Kiorpes L. Development of radial frequency pattern perception in macaque monkeys. J Vis 2024; 24:6. [PMID: 38843389 PMCID: PMC11160949 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.6.6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/20/2024] [Accepted: 04/30/2024] [Indexed: 06/09/2024] Open
Abstract
Infant primates see poorly, and most perceptual functions mature steadily beyond early infancy. Behavioral studies on human and macaque infants show that global form perception, as measured by the ability to integrate contour information into a coherent percept, improves dramatically throughout the first several years after birth. However, it is unknown when sensitivity to curvature and shape emerges in early life or how it develops. We studied the development of shape sensitivity in 18 macaques, aged 2 months to 10 years. Using radial frequency stimuli, circular targets whose radii are modulated sinusoidally, we tested monkeys' ability to radial frequency stimuli from circles as a function of the depth and frequency of sinusoidal modulation. We implemented a new four-choice oddity task and compared the resulting data with that from a traditional two-alternative forced choice task. We found that radial frequency pattern perception was measurable at the youngest age tested (2 months). Behavioral performance at all radial frequencies improved with age. Performance was better for higher radial frequencies, suggesting the developing visual system prioritizes processing of fine visual details that are ecologically relevant. By using two complementary methods, we were able to capture a comprehensive developmental trajectory for shape perception.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Gerick M Lee
- Center for Neural Science, New York University, NY, NY, USA
| | | | - Najib J Majaj
- Center for Neural Science, New York University, NY, NY, USA
| | | | - Lynne Kiorpes
- Center for Neural Science, New York University, NY, NY, USA
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Kobylkov D, Vallortigara G. Face detection mechanisms: Nature vs. nurture. Front Neurosci 2024; 18:1404174. [PMID: 38812973 PMCID: PMC11133589 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2024.1404174] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/20/2024] [Accepted: 05/02/2024] [Indexed: 05/31/2024] Open
Abstract
For many animals, faces are a vitally important visual stimulus. Hence, it is not surprising that face perception has become a very popular research topic in neuroscience, with ca. 2000 papers published every year. As a result, significant progress has been made in understanding the intricate mechanisms underlying this phenomenon. However, the ontogeny of face perception, in particular the role of innate predispositions, remains largely unexplored at the neural level. Several influential studies in monkeys have suggested that seeing faces is necessary for the development of the face-selective brain domains. At the same time, behavioural experiments with newborn human babies and newly-hatched domestic chicks demonstrate that a spontaneous preference towards faces emerges early in life without pre-existing experience. Moreover, we were recently able to record face-selective neural responses in the brain of young, face-naïve chicks, thus demonstrating the existence of an innate face detection mechanism. In this review, we discuss these seemingly contradictory results and propose potential experimental approaches to resolve some of the open questions.
Collapse
|
6
|
Birulés J, Goupil L, Josse J, Fort M. The Role of Talking Faces in Infant Language Learning: Mind the Gap between Screen-Based Settings and Real-Life Communicative Interactions. Brain Sci 2023; 13:1167. [PMID: 37626523 PMCID: PMC10452843 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci13081167] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/10/2023] [Revised: 07/28/2023] [Accepted: 08/01/2023] [Indexed: 08/27/2023] Open
Abstract
Over the last few decades, developmental (psycho) linguists have demonstrated that perceiving talking faces audio-visually is important for early language acquisition. Using mostly well-controlled and screen-based laboratory approaches, this line of research has shown that paying attention to talking faces is likely to be one of the powerful strategies infants use to learn their native(s) language(s). In this review, we combine evidence from these screen-based studies with another line of research that has studied how infants learn novel words and deploy their visual attention during naturalistic play. In our view, this is an important step toward developing an integrated account of how infants effectively extract audiovisual information from talkers' faces during early language learning. We identify three factors that have been understudied so far, despite the fact that they are likely to have an important impact on how infants deploy their attention (or not) toward talking faces during social interactions: social contingency, speaker characteristics, and task- dependencies. Last, we propose ideas to address these issues in future research, with the aim of reducing the existing knowledge gap between current experimental studies and the many ways infants can and do effectively rely upon the audiovisual information extracted from talking faces in their real-life language environment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joan Birulés
- Laboratoire de Psychologie et NeuroCognition, CNRS UMR 5105, Université Grenoble Alpes, 38058 Grenoble, France; (L.G.); (J.J.); (M.F.)
| | - Louise Goupil
- Laboratoire de Psychologie et NeuroCognition, CNRS UMR 5105, Université Grenoble Alpes, 38058 Grenoble, France; (L.G.); (J.J.); (M.F.)
| | - Jérémie Josse
- Laboratoire de Psychologie et NeuroCognition, CNRS UMR 5105, Université Grenoble Alpes, 38058 Grenoble, France; (L.G.); (J.J.); (M.F.)
| | - Mathilde Fort
- Laboratoire de Psychologie et NeuroCognition, CNRS UMR 5105, Université Grenoble Alpes, 38058 Grenoble, France; (L.G.); (J.J.); (M.F.)
- Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon, INSERM U1028-CNRS UMR 5292, Université Lyon 1, 69500 Bron, France
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
O TMJ, Ceisler E, Broude C, Chan K, Pacicco L, Fay A, Waner M. Distribution and Clinical Characteristics of Periorbital Infantile Hemangiomas. Facial Plast Surg Aesthet Med 2023; 25:172-178. [PMID: 36473202 DOI: 10.1089/fpsam.2022.0099] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Background: Periorbital infantile hemangiomas (POIHs) are associated with a high incidence of visual complications. Objective(s): To analyze the sites of predilection of POIHs and to determine whether certain sites require earlier intervention due to their higher rate of visual complications. Methods: A retrospective case series study was conducted on patients from two tertiary care centers for 25 years. The location of POIHs was determined from clinical photographs, medical records, and radiological studies. The presence or absence of anisometropic astigmatism (anisoastigmatism) and amblyopia was recorded. Data were analyzed using a chi-square test. Results: There were 486 patients, of which 302 patients had ophthalmology evaluations and 245 patients had refractive error data. At presentation, 10% of patients already had amblyopia and 44% had anisoastigmatism. Medial eyelid lesions had the highest risk of developing anisoastigmatism (anisoastigmatism correlates with eyelid position, p = 0.0001). Segmental and upper medial lesions had the highest risk of amblyopia at initial evaluation. Conclusion: The site of POIH is an important indicator for developing clinically significant anisoastigmatism and amblyopia, underlining the need for early ophthalmologic assessment and management.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Teresa Min-Jung O
- Vascular Birthmark Institute of New York, New York, New York, USA.,Department of Otolaryngology, Lenox Hill Hospital, New York, New York, USA
| | - Emily Ceisler
- Department of Ophthalmology, New York University Grossman School of Medicine, New York, New York, USA
| | - Caroline Broude
- Vascular Birthmark Institute of New York, New York, New York, USA.,Department of Otolaryngology, Lenox Hill Hospital, New York, New York, USA
| | - Kimberly Chan
- Vascular Birthmark Institute of New York, New York, New York, USA.,Department of Otolaryngology, Lenox Hill Hospital, New York, New York, USA.,College of Medicine, The Pennsylvania State University, Hershey, Pennsylvania, USA
| | - Lauren Pacicco
- Vascular Birthmark Institute of New York, New York, New York, USA.,Department of Otolaryngology, Lenox Hill Hospital, New York, New York, USA
| | - Aaron Fay
- Vascular Birthmark Institute of New York, New York, New York, USA.,Department of Otolaryngology, Lenox Hill Hospital, New York, New York, USA.,Department of Ophthalmology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
| | - Milton Waner
- Vascular Birthmark Institute of New York, New York, New York, USA.,Department of Otolaryngology, Lenox Hill Hospital, New York, New York, USA
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Lam M, Suh D. Screening, Diagnosis, and Treatment of Pediatric Ocular Diseases. CHILDREN (BASEL, SWITZERLAND) 2022; 9:children9121939. [PMID: 36553382 PMCID: PMC9777216 DOI: 10.3390/children9121939] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/09/2022] [Revised: 11/28/2022] [Accepted: 12/06/2022] [Indexed: 12/14/2022]
Abstract
Vision is an important aspect of a child's quality of life and intellectual, social, and emotional development. Disruptions to vision during infancy and early childhood can cause lifelong vision impairment or blindness. However, early identification and treatment of eye disease can prevent loss of sight and its consequent long-term effects. Therefore, screening guidelines exist to guide physicians in detecting the most common threats to sight in the different stages of infancy and childhood. This review describes common causes of pediatric vision impairment, the recommended screening guidelines for diagnosing them, and current treatment modalities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matthew Lam
- Creighton University School of Medicine Phoenix Regional Campus, Phoenix, AZ 85012, USA
| | - Donny Suh
- Gavin Herbert Eye Institute, University of California Irvine, Irvine, CA 92697, USA
- Correspondence:
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
The retinal pigmentation pathway in human albinism: Not so black and white. Prog Retin Eye Res 2022; 91:101091. [PMID: 35729001 DOI: 10.1016/j.preteyeres.2022.101091] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/31/2022] [Revised: 05/24/2022] [Accepted: 05/26/2022] [Indexed: 12/16/2022]
Abstract
Albinism is a pigment disorder affecting eye, skin and/or hair. Patients usually have decreased melanin in affected tissues and suffer from severe visual abnormalities, including foveal hypoplasia and chiasmal misrouting. Combining our data with those of the literature, we propose a single functional genetic retinal signalling pathway that includes all 22 currently known human albinism disease genes. We hypothesise that defects affecting the genesis or function of different intra-cellular organelles, including melanosomes, cause syndromic forms of albinism (Hermansky-Pudlak (HPS) and Chediak-Higashi syndrome (CHS)). We put forward that specific melanosome impairments cause different forms of oculocutaneous albinism (OCA1-8). Further, we incorporate GPR143 that has been implicated in ocular albinism (OA1), characterised by a phenotype limited to the eye. Finally, we include the SLC38A8-associated disorder FHONDA that causes an even more restricted "albinism-related" ocular phenotype with foveal hypoplasia and chiasmal misrouting but without pigmentation defects. We propose the following retinal pigmentation pathway, with increasingly specific genetic and cellular defects causing an increasingly specific ocular phenotype: (HPS1-11/CHS: syndromic forms of albinism)-(OCA1-8: OCA)-(GPR143: OA1)-(SLC38A8: FHONDA). Beyond disease genes involvement, we also evaluate a range of (candidate) regulatory and signalling mechanisms affecting the activity of the pathway in retinal development, retinal pigmentation and albinism. We further suggest that the proposed pigmentation pathway is also involved in other retinal disorders, such as age-related macular degeneration. The hypotheses put forward in this report provide a framework for further systematic studies in albinism and melanin pigmentation disorders.
Collapse
|
10
|
Landrum JT, Mendez V, Cao Y, Gomez R, Neuringer M. Analysis of macular carotenoids in the developing macaque retina: The timeline of macular pigment development. Methods Enzymol 2022; 674:215-253. [PMID: 36008008 DOI: 10.1016/bs.mie.2022.05.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/04/2023]
Abstract
In the mature retina, the components of the macular pigment, lutein (L), R,R-zeaxanthin (RRZ), R,S-zeaxanthin (RSZ, meso-zeaxanthin) are most concentrated in the central macula. L and RRZ are of dietary origin but RSZ is produced in situ from L. The relative proportions of L and Z isomers vary across the retina with eccentricity in the adult retina. Early reports have shown that during development, the proportions of L and Z isomers undergo changes as the total pigment levels increase. The methods described here demonstrate the unique utility of chiral phase HPLC to measure the amounts of L, RRZ, and RSZ, discriminating between the two zeaxanthin stereoisomers. In three concentric retinal sections of macaque retinas chiral phase HPLC has been employed to document the developmental changes in the distribution of each L, RSZ, and RRZ during the period just prior to full term gestation through 19 months after birth. The net rate of accumulation of carotenoids within the central retina during the first 20 months is quasi-linear and fit by a linear regression. During development, the rate of transport of L (0.12 (±0.033)ngmm-2mo-1 (SE)) into the central 2mm of the retina is double that of RRZ (0.062 (±0.02)ngmm-2mo-1 (SE)). The rate of accumulation of RSZ (0.06 (±0.01)ngmm-2mo-1 (SE)) is comparable to that of RRZ. In the peripheral retina, the rates of accumulation of L and RRZ are not correlated with increasing age, whereas accumulation of RSZ does correlate with age. The changing proportions of L to Z isomers in the central retina during development are explained by the rates for carotenoid accumulation within the central retina. At birth, the macular pigment in the central retina is dominated by L and RRZ, 0.35±0.11 and 0.21±0.054ngmm-2. In the central retina, RSZ was rarely detected in the youngest tissues analyzed. It can be estimated to represent 6% of the total macular pigment (0.033±0.11ngmm-2) at birth based on extrapolation from measurements in the peripheral retina and the ratio of L/(RRZ+RSZ) is ≈1.5. At maturity, the concentrations for L, RRZ, and RSZ in the central macaque retina are estimated to be 1.7, 1.8 and 1.08ngmm-2, with L/(RRZ+RSZ) being 0.6.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- John T Landrum
- Department of Chemistry and Biochemistry, Florida International University, Miami, FL, United States.
| | - Vanesa Mendez
- Department of Chemistry and Biochemistry, Florida International University, Miami, FL, United States
| | - Yisi Cao
- Department of Chemistry and Biochemistry, Florida International University, Miami, FL, United States
| | - Ramon Gomez
- Department of Statistics, Florida International University, Miami, FL, United States
| | - Martha Neuringer
- Division of Neuroscience, Oregon National Primate Research Center, Oregon Health and Science University, Beaverton, OR, United States
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Hsia NY, Wen LY, Chou CY, Lin CL, Wan L, Lin HJ. Increased Risk of Refractive Errors and Amblyopia among Children with Ptosis: A Nationwide Population-Based Study. J Clin Med 2022; 11:2334. [PMID: 35566461 PMCID: PMC9100637 DOI: 10.3390/jcm11092334] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/15/2022] [Revised: 04/19/2022] [Accepted: 04/20/2022] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Background: This study aimed to investigate the risk of refractive errors (astigmatism, myopia, and hyperopia) and amblyopia in children with ptosis and association between age at diagnosis of ptosis and subsequent risks of vision problems. Methods: Retrospective claims data from the Taiwan National Health Insurance Research Database (NHIRD) were analyzed. We identified 1799 children aged 0−18 years who were newly diagnosed with ptosis between 2000 and 2012 and 7187 individuals without the disease. Both cohorts were followed up until 2013 to estimate the incidence of refractive errors and amblyopia. Results: Children with ptosis had 5.93-fold, 3.46-fold, 7.60-fold, and 13.45-fold increases in the risk of developing astigmatism, myopia, hyperopia, and amblyopia, respectively, compared with the control cohort (astigmatism: adjusted hazard ratio, aHR = 5.93, 95% confidence interval, CI = 5.16−6.82; myopia: aHR = 3.46, 95% CI = 3.13−3.83; hyperopia: aHR = 7.60, 95% CI = 5.99−9.63; amblyopia: aHR = 13.45, 95% CI = 10.60−17.05). Children diagnosed with ptosis at an age older than 3 years old had a higher risk of myopia than patients diagnosed with ptosis before age 3. There was no significant difference of the risk of astigmatism, amblyopia, and hyperopia between age groups. Conclusions: Children with ptosis may exhibit a higher risk of astigmatism, myopia, hyperopia, and amblyopia than children without ptosis. The risk of myopia is higher in children with ptosis diagnosed at >3 years than those diagnosed at ≤3 years.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ning-Yi Hsia
- Department of Ophthalmology, China Medical University Hospital, Taichung 40402, Taiwan;
- School of Medicine, China Medical University, Taichung 40402, Taiwan
| | - Li-Yen Wen
- School of Chinese Medicine, China Medical University, Taichung 40402, Taiwan; (L.-Y.W.); (C.-Y.C.)
| | - Ching-Ying Chou
- School of Chinese Medicine, China Medical University, Taichung 40402, Taiwan; (L.-Y.W.); (C.-Y.C.)
| | - Cheng-Li Lin
- Management Office for Health Data, China Medical University Hospital, Taichung 40402, Taiwan;
| | - Lei Wan
- School of Chinese Medicine, China Medical University, Taichung 40402, Taiwan; (L.-Y.W.); (C.-Y.C.)
- Department of Medical Laboratory Science and Biotechnology, Asia University, Taichung 41354, Taiwan
- Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, China Medical University Hospital, Taichung 40402, Taiwan
| | - Hui-Ju Lin
- Department of Ophthalmology, China Medical University Hospital, Taichung 40402, Taiwan;
- School of Chinese Medicine, China Medical University, Taichung 40402, Taiwan; (L.-Y.W.); (C.-Y.C.)
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Lopes MCB, Costa MF, Nakanami CR. Relationship between the Children’s Visual Function Questionnaire and psychophysical measures of visual acuity and chromaticity discrimination in older visually impaired children. BRITISH JOURNAL OF VISUAL IMPAIRMENT 2022. [DOI: 10.1177/02646196211068708] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
The Children’s Visual Function Questionnaire (CVFQ) is an instrument to measure the impact of visual impairment in children and their families. It can be used as a research tool to verify the effectiveness of treatment, therapy, and different methods for visual stimulation and rehabilitation. The aim of this study is to describe the relationship between General Health, General Vision Health, Personality, Family Impact, and Treatment – subscales of the CVFQ and psychophysical measures of visual acuity (VA) and chromaticity discrimination (CV). This prospective, cross-sectional study was conducted at the Sensory Psychophysiology Laboratory – University of São Paulo in partnership with the Ambulatory of Visual Stimulation in the Sector of Low Vision and Visual Rehabilitation – Federal University of São Paulo. The children who participated in this study were divided into two groups: study group (SG), composed of 11 children with a diagnosis of visual impairment and mean age of 55.36 months ( SD = 16.88), and control group (CG), composed of 8 children with normal vision and mean age of 64.87 months ( SD = 13.22). Both groups underwent the application of CVFQ, were tested for VA using the Teller Acuity Cards (TAC) test, and CV using the Cambridge Colour Test program for children (CCT Kids). The result of applying the CVFQ to groups of children over 3 years, comparing SG and CG, differences were observed in the following subscales: General Vision Health ( F = 10.00, p < .001); Competence ( F = 7.03, p = .030); Personality ( F = 6.48, p = .010); Total Quality of Life ( F = 11.39, p = .010). These differences show a worse score for the SG. In the VA test using TAC, the data collected in groups of children over 3 years, comparing SG and CG, differences were observed in VA RE ( F = 19.25, p < .001); VA LE ( F = 25.99, p < .001); VA BE ( F = 15.45, p < .001). These differences show worse scores for the SG. In the CV using the CCT Kids, the data collected in groups of children over 3 years, comparing SG and CG, no statistically significant differences for protan, tritan, and deutan groups were observed. For the SG, while for children over 3 years, correlations were found for General Vision Health, Competence, Family Impact, and General Quality of Life. We conclude that there are statistically significant differences when comparing the SG with the CG for the visual function of visual acuity and chromaticity discrimination, and we also demonstrated the sensitivity in the use of CVFQ in reflect VA and CV impairments.
Collapse
|
13
|
Casanova MI, Chen R, Garzel LM, Olstad KJ, Kim S, Harris RA, Li Y, Raveendran M, Liang Q, Wang J, Yiu G, Stout JT, Roberts JA, Rogers J, Moshiri A, Thomasy SM. Clinical presentation, treatment, and genetic and histopathological analysis of juvenile cataracts and secondary glaucoma in a rhesus macaque (Macaca mulatta). J Med Primatol 2021; 51:119-123. [PMID: 34897697 DOI: 10.1111/jmp.12560] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/12/2021] [Revised: 09/25/2021] [Accepted: 10/20/2021] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
This report describes the clinical and histological findings, genetic study, and treatment in a 1.3-year-old rhesus macaque with bilateral cataracts and unilateral secondary glaucoma. Intravitreal injection of gentamicin decreased the intraocular pressure from 56 to <2 mm Hg. A putative genetic cause of the cataracts was not identified.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M Isabel Casanova
- Department of Surgical and Radiological Sciences, School of Veterinary Medicine, University of California-Davis, Davis, California, USA
| | - Rui Chen
- Human Genome Sequencing Center and Department of Molecular and Human Genetics, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas, USA.,Department of Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas, USA
| | - Laura M Garzel
- California National Primate Research Center, Davis, California, USA
| | | | - Soohyun Kim
- Department of Surgical and Radiological Sciences, School of Veterinary Medicine, University of California-Davis, Davis, California, USA
| | - Ronald Alan Harris
- Human Genome Sequencing Center and Department of Molecular and Human Genetics, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas, USA
| | - Yumei Li
- Human Genome Sequencing Center and Department of Molecular and Human Genetics, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas, USA
| | - Muthuswamy Raveendran
- Human Genome Sequencing Center and Department of Molecular and Human Genetics, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas, USA
| | - Qingnan Liang
- Human Genome Sequencing Center and Department of Molecular and Human Genetics, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas, USA.,Department of Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas, USA
| | - Jun Wang
- Human Genome Sequencing Center and Department of Molecular and Human Genetics, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas, USA
| | - Glenn Yiu
- Department of Ophthalmology & Vision Science, School of Medicine, University of California Davis, Sacramento, California, USA
| | - John Timothy Stout
- Department of Ophthalmology, Cullen Eye Institute, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas, USA
| | - Jeffrey A Roberts
- California National Primate Research Center, Davis, California, USA.,Medicine and Epidemiology Department, School of Veterinary Medicine, University of California-Davis, Davis, California, USA
| | - Jeffrey Rogers
- Human Genome Sequencing Center and Department of Molecular and Human Genetics, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas, USA
| | - Ala Moshiri
- Department of Ophthalmology & Vision Science, School of Medicine, University of California Davis, Sacramento, California, USA
| | - Sara M Thomasy
- Department of Surgical and Radiological Sciences, School of Veterinary Medicine, University of California-Davis, Davis, California, USA.,California National Primate Research Center, Davis, California, USA.,Department of Ophthalmology & Vision Science, School of Medicine, University of California Davis, Sacramento, California, USA
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Nishio N, Hayashi K, Ishikawa AW, Yoshimura Y. The role of early visual experience in the development of spatial-frequency preference in the primary visual cortex. J Physiol 2021; 599:4131-4152. [PMID: 34275157 DOI: 10.1113/jp281463] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/02/2021] [Accepted: 07/15/2021] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
KEY POINTS The mature functioning of the primary visual cortex depends on postnatal visual experience, while the orientation/direction preference is established just after eye-opening, independently of visual experience. In this study, we find that visual experience is required for the normal development of spatial-frequency (SF) preference in mouse primary visual cortex. We show that age- and experience-dependent shifts in optimal SFs towards higher frequencies occurred similarly in excitatory neurons and parvalbumin-positive interneurons. We also show that some excitatory and parvalbumin-positive neurons preferentially responded to visual stimuli consisting of very high SFs and posterior directions, and that the preference was established at earlier developmental stages than the SF preference in the standard frequency range. These results suggest that early visual experience is required for the development of SF representation and shed light on the experience-dependent developmental mechanisms underlying visual cortical functions. ABSTRACT Early visual experience is crucial for the maturation of visual cortical functions. It has been demonstrated that the orientation and direction preferences in individual neurons of the primary visual cortex are well established immediately after eye-opening. The postnatal development of spatial frequency (SF) tuning and its dependence on visual experience, however, has not been thoroughly quantified. In this study, macroscopic imaging with flavoprotein autofluorescence revealed that the optimal SFs shift towards higher frequency values during normal development in mouse primary visual cortex. This developmental shift was impaired by binocular deprivation during the sensitive period, postnatal 3 weeks (PW3) to PW6. Furthermore, two-photon Ca2+ imaging revealed that the developmental shift of the optimal SFs, depending on visual experience, concurrently occurs in excitatory neurons and parvalbumin-positive inhibitory interneurons (PV neurons). In addition, some excitatory and PV neurons exhibited a preference for visual stimuli consisting of particularly high SFs and posterior directions at relatively early developmental stages; this preference was not affected by binocular deprivation. Thus, there may be two distinct developmental mechanisms for the establishment of SF preference depending on the frequency values. After PW3, SF tuning for neurons tuned to standard frequency ranges was sharper in excitatory neurons and slightly broader in PV neurons, leading to considerably attenuated SF tuning in PV neurons compared to excitatory neurons by PW5. Our findings suggest that early visual experience is far more important than orientation/direction selectivity for the development of the neural representation of the diverse SFs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nana Nishio
- Division of Visual Information Processing, National Institute for Physiological Sciences, National Institutes of Natural Sciences, Okazaki, Japan
| | - Kenji Hayashi
- Division of Visual Information Processing, National Institute for Physiological Sciences, National Institutes of Natural Sciences, Okazaki, Japan.,Department of Physiological Sciences, The Graduate University for Advanced Studies (SOKENDAI), Okazaki, Japan
| | - Ayako Wendy Ishikawa
- Division of Visual Information Processing, National Institute for Physiological Sciences, National Institutes of Natural Sciences, Okazaki, Japan.,Department of Physiological Sciences, The Graduate University for Advanced Studies (SOKENDAI), Okazaki, Japan
| | - Yumiko Yoshimura
- Division of Visual Information Processing, National Institute for Physiological Sciences, National Institutes of Natural Sciences, Okazaki, Japan.,Department of Physiological Sciences, The Graduate University for Advanced Studies (SOKENDAI), Okazaki, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Retinotopic organization of visual cortex in human infants. Neuron 2021; 109:2616-2626.e6. [PMID: 34228960 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.06.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/01/2020] [Revised: 05/07/2021] [Accepted: 06/04/2021] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Vision develops rapidly during infancy, yet how visual cortex is organized during this period is unclear. In particular, it is unknown whether functional maps that organize the mature adult visual cortex are present in the infant striate and extrastriate cortex. Here, we test the functional maturity of infant visual cortex by performing retinotopic mapping with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Infants aged 5-23 months had retinotopic maps, with alternating preferences for vertical and horizontal meridians indicating the boundaries of visual areas V1 to V4 and an orthogonal gradient of preferences from high to low spatial frequencies. The presence of multiple visual maps throughout visual cortex in infants indicates a greater maturity of extrastriate cortex than previously appreciated. The areas showed subtle age-related fine-tuning, suggesting that early maturation undergoes continued refinement. This early maturation of area boundaries and tuning may scaffold subsequent developmental changes.
Collapse
|
16
|
The impact of strabismus on psychosocial heath and quality of life: a systematic review. Surv Ophthalmol 2021; 66:1051-1064. [PMID: 33773997 DOI: 10.1016/j.survophthal.2021.03.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/13/2020] [Revised: 03/09/2021] [Accepted: 03/15/2021] [Indexed: 02/07/2023]
Abstract
Strabismus can hinder an individual's ability to perform daily functions and negatively affect their well-being. I examine the impact strabismus has on psychosocial health and quality of life in children and adults and evaluate the challenges confronted by parents of children with strabismus. Numerous misconceptions exist regarding the impact strabismus has on overall health. Negative attitudes persist toward those affected, resulting in difficulties with self-image. Individuals with strabismus are at increased risk for both visual system and psychiatric disorders. Misinformation regarding available treatment options for children and adults with strabismus continues to exist, resulting in decreased access to care. Improved education of health care providers can increase appropriate referrals and initiation of treatment. Treatment of strabismus is not merely cosmetic and has the potential to improve psychosocial health and quality of life for children and adults with and without diplopia.
Collapse
|
17
|
Raper J, Chahroudi A. Clinical and Preclinical Evidence for Adverse Neurodevelopment after Postnatal Zika Virus Infection. Trop Med Infect Dis 2021; 6:tropicalmed6010010. [PMID: 33445671 PMCID: PMC7838975 DOI: 10.3390/tropicalmed6010010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/26/2020] [Accepted: 01/04/2021] [Indexed: 02/07/2023] Open
Abstract
Although the Zika virus (ZIKV) typically causes mild or no symptoms in adults, during the 2015−2016 outbreak, ZIKV infection in pregnancy resulted in a spectrum of diseases in infants, including birth defects and neurodevelopmental disorders identified in childhood. While intense clinical and basic science research has focused on the neurodevelopmental outcomes of prenatal ZIKV infection, less is known about the consequences of infection during early life. Considering the neurotropism of ZIKV and the rapidly-developing postnatal brain, it is important to understand how infection during infancy may disrupt neurodevelopment. This paper reviews the current knowledge regarding early postnatal ZIKV infection. Emerging clinical evidence supports the hypothesis that ZIKV infection during infancy can result in negative neurologic consequences. However, clinical data regarding postnatal ZIKV infection in children are limited; as such, animal models play an important role in understanding the potential complications of ZIKV infection related to the vulnerable developing brain. Preclinical data provide insight into the potential behavioral, cognitive, and motor domains that clinical studies should examine in pediatric populations exposed to ZIKV during infancy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jessica Raper
- Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30329, USA;
- Department of Pediatrics, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA 30322, USA
| | - Ann Chahroudi
- Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30329, USA;
- Department of Pediatrics, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA 30322, USA
- Center for Childhood Infections and Vaccines of Children’s Healthcare of Atlanta and Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30322, USA
- Correspondence:
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Levi DM. Amblyopia. HANDBOOK OF CLINICAL NEUROLOGY 2021; 178:13-30. [PMID: 33832673 DOI: 10.1016/b978-0-12-821377-3.00002-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/24/2022]
Abstract
Amblyopia is a neurodevelopmental abnormality that results in physiological alterations in the visual pathways and impaired vision in one eye, less commonly in both. It reflects a broad range of neural, perceptual, oculomotor, and clinical abnormalities that can occur when normal visual development is disrupted early in life. Aside from refractive error, amblyopia is the most common cause of vision loss in infants and young children. It causes a constellation of perceptual deficits in the vision of the amblyopic eye, including a loss of visual acuity, position acuity, and contrast sensitivity, particularly at high spatial frequencies, as well as increased internal noise and prolonged manual and saccadic reaction times. There are also perceptual deficits in the strong eye, such as certain types of motion perception, reflecting altered neural responses and functional connectivity in visual cortex (Ho et al., 2005). Treatment in young children consists of correction of any refractive error and patching of the strong eye. Compliance with patching is challenging and a substantial proportion of amblyopic children fail to achieve normal acuity or stereopsis even after extended periods of treatment. There are a number of promising experimental treatments that may improve compliance and outcomes, such as the playing of action video games with the strong eye patched. Although there may be a sensitive period for optimal effects of treatment, there is evidence that amblyopic adults may still show some benefit of treatment. However, there is as yet no consensus on the treatment of adults with amblyopia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dennis M Levi
- School of Optometry & Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California Berkeley, Berkeley, CA, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Abstract
Recent work has transformed our ideas about the neural mechanisms, behavioral consequences and effective therapies for amblyopia. Since the 1700's, the clinical treatment for amblyopia has consisted of patching or penalizing the strong eye, to force the "lazy" amblyopic eye, to work. This treatment has generally been limited to infants and young children during a sensitive period of development. Over the last 20 years we have learned much about the nature and neural mechanisms underlying the loss of spatial and binocular vision in amblyopia, and that a degree of neural plasticity persists well beyond the sensitive period. Importantly, the last decade has seen a resurgence of research into new approaches to the treatment of amblyopia both in children and adults, which emphasize that monocular therapies may not be the most effective for the fundamentally binocular disorder that is amblyopia. These approaches include perceptual learning, video game play and binocular methods aimed at reducing inhibition of the amblyopic eye by the strong fellow eye, and enhancing binocular fusion and stereopsis. This review focuses on the what we've learned over the past 20 years or so, and will highlight both the successes of these new treatment approaches in labs around the world, and their failures in clinical trials. Reconciling these results raises important new questions that may help to focus future directions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dennis M Levi
- University of California, Berkeley, School of Optometry & Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, Berkeley, CA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
She Z, Hung LF, Arumugam B, Beach KM, Smith EL. Effects of low intensity ambient lighting on refractive development in infant rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Vision Res 2020; 176:48-59. [PMID: 32777589 PMCID: PMC7487012 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2020.07.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/01/2020] [Revised: 07/02/2020] [Accepted: 07/09/2020] [Indexed: 02/06/2023]
Abstract
Studies in chickens suggest low intensity ambient lighting causes myopia. The purpose of this experiment was to examine the effects of low intensity ambient lighting (dim light) on normal refractive development in macaque monkeys. Seven infant rhesus monkeys were reared under dim light (room illumination level: ~55 lx) from 24 to ~310 days of age with otherwise unrestricted vision. Refractive error, corneal power, ocular axial dimensions, and choroidal thickness were measured in anesthetized animals at the onset of the experiment and periodically throughout the dim-light-rearing period, and were compared with those of normal-light-reared monkeys. We found that dim light did not produce myopia; instead, dim-light monkeys were hyperopic relative to normal-light monkeys (median refractive errors at ~155 days, OD: +3.13 D vs. +2.31 D; OS: +3.31D vs. +2.44 D; at ~310 days, OD: +2.75D vs. +1.78D, OS: +3.00D vs. +1.75D). In addition, dim-light rearing caused sustained thickening in the choroid, but it did not alter corneal power development, nor did it change the axial nature of the refractive errors. These results showed that, for rhesus monkeys and possibly other primates, low ambient lighting by itself is not necessarily myopiagenic, but might compromise the efficiency of emmetropization.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zhihui She
- College of Optometry, University of Houston, Houston, TX, United States
| | - Li-Fang Hung
- College of Optometry, University of Houston, Houston, TX, United States; Brien Holden Vision Institute, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| | - Baskar Arumugam
- College of Optometry, University of Houston, Houston, TX, United States
| | - Krista M Beach
- College of Optometry, University of Houston, Houston, TX, United States
| | - Earl L Smith
- College of Optometry, University of Houston, Houston, TX, United States; Brien Holden Vision Institute, Sydney, NSW, Australia.
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Ramachandran S, Das VE. Fixation Preference for Visual and Auditory Targets in Monkeys with Strabismus. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2020; 61:24. [PMID: 32931572 PMCID: PMC7500111 DOI: 10.1167/iovs.61.11.24] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Purpose During binocular viewing, many strabismic subjects choose the eye of fixation depending on the retinotopic location of a visual target. Here, we compare eye choice behavior when orienting to visual and non-visual (auditory) targets. Methods Eye movements were measured in two head-fixed exotropic strabismic monkeys in a saccadic task involving either a visual or an auditory stimulus (no visual target information or feedback) during monocular or binocular viewing. The stimulus was one of 21 visual or auditory targets arranged 10° apart in a 7 × 3 array at a distance of 57 cm in an otherwise dark room. Fixation preference was calculated by recording the incidence of using a specific eye to acquire the target at any location. Results Spatial patterns of fixation preference were observed in both monkeys for both visual and auditory stimuli; targets to the far right were acquired by the right eye, and targets to the far left were acquired by the left eye. For visual targets, the border for a change in fixation preference occurred in between the visual axes of the fixating and deviated eyes (variable in the two animals). In contrast, the border for fixation change remained near the cranio-center during the auditory task. During monocular viewing, fixation switching was observed only at the extremities during visual tasks; during the auditory task, fixation preference was similar to that observed during binocular viewing. Conclusions Fixation preference persists for invisible auditory targets. Our data suggest that visual suppression could modify underlying eye choice behavior that functions independently from vision.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Vallabh E Das
- College of Optometry, University of Houston, Houston, Texas, United States
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Karsolia A, Burns E, Pullela M, Das VE. Longitudinal Development of Ocular Misalignment in Nonhuman Primate Models for Strabismus. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2020; 61:8. [PMID: 32282917 PMCID: PMC7401961 DOI: 10.1167/iovs.61.4.8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Purpose To investigate the longitudinal change in horizontal and vertical ocular alignment in normal and prism-reared infant monkeys during the critical developmental period. Methods Ocular alignment was measured using Hirschberg photographic methods in 6 infant monkeys reared under prism-viewing from day 1 after birth to 4 months, and 2 monkeys reared with normal visual experience. Photographs were acquired twice a week for the first 6 months of life and analyzed to identify pupil center and the first Purkinje image from which eye positions and strabismus angle were calculated. Results At 3 weeks after birth, prism monkeys presented with significant horizontal ocular misalignment. A gradual change in alignment was seen in all prism-reared monkeys stabilizing at approximately 11 weeks, at which time 5 monkeys were exotropic (mean, 16° XT; range, 13°–24°) and 1 monkey was esotropic (5° ET). A reduction in ocular misalignment was observed after exposure to normal visual environment at 16 weeks, but at 34 weeks of age, that is, 18 weeks after removal of prisms, prism-reared monkeys displayed a mean horizontal strabismus of 7° XT (range, 2° ET to 20° XT), which was still significantly different from normal monkeys. Conclusions Prism-rearing disrupts binocular fusion mechanisms, and horizontal and vertical strabismus is seen to develop as early as 3 weeks of age in monkey models, equivalent to approximately 3 months in humans. The time course of change in alignment overlaps with disruption in various visual sensory functions, suggesting a causal temporal link between sensory and motor mechanisms for alignment.
Collapse
|
23
|
Abstract
Collinear facilitation, the mechanism for grouping contour elements, is a process involving lateral interactions that improve the detectability of a target by the presence of collinear flankers. It was shown that the development of collinear facilitation is experience dependent and that it may be impaired when the visual input is distorted in one meridian (meridional amblyopia). In oblique astigmatism, the blurring is on the opposite oblique meridian in both eyes, resulting in two conflicting images, which may affect the development of binocular vision. We hypothesized that the collinear facilitation of adults with oblique astigmatism is reminiscent of the abnormal development of the lateral facilitation of meridional amblyopia. We explored the perception of binocular vision and collinear facilitation in cases of both distorted and non-distorted vision. Fully corrected participants that tested for the target contrast detection of Gabor patches and two collinear flankers, presented for 80 ms, were positioned at different orientations (0° (180°), 45°, 90°, and 135°) and for different eyes (monocular, binocular). The results show a significant anisotropy for monocular collinear facilitation between the blured and the clear meridians, being lower in the blurriest meridian than in the clearest meridian, resembling the meridional amblyopia results. Collinear facilitation results in poor binocular summation between the monocular channels. Our results indicate that the perceptual behavior was similar to that of meridional amblyopic subjects having an anisotropy of collinear facilitation between cardinal meridians in oblique astigmatic subjects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gad Serero
- School of Optometry and Vision Science, Mina and Everard Goodman, Faculty of Life Sciences, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel
| | - Maria Lev
- School of Optometry and Vision Science, Mina and Everard Goodman, Faculty of Life Sciences, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel
| | - Uri Polat
- School of Optometry and Vision Science, Mina and Everard Goodman, Faculty of Life Sciences, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel.
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Raper J, Kovacs-Balint Z, Mavigner M, Gumber S, Burke MW, Habib J, Mattingly C, Fair D, Earl E, Feczko E, Styner M, Jean SM, Cohen JK, Suthar MS, Sanchez MM, Alvarado MC, Chahroudi A. Long-term alterations in brain and behavior after postnatal Zika virus infection in infant macaques. Nat Commun 2020; 11:2534. [PMID: 32439858 PMCID: PMC7242369 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-020-16320-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/11/2019] [Accepted: 04/21/2020] [Indexed: 12/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Zika virus (ZIKV) infection has a profound impact on the fetal nervous system. The postnatal period is also a time of rapid brain growth, and it is important to understand the potential neurobehavioral consequences of ZIKV infection during infancy. Here we show that postnatal ZIKV infection in a rhesus macaque model resulted in long-term behavioral, motor, and cognitive changes, including increased emotional reactivity, decreased social contact, loss of balance, and deficits in visual recognition memory at one year of age. Structural and functional MRI showed that ZIKV-infected infant rhesus macaques had persistent enlargement of lateral ventricles, smaller volumes and altered functional connectivity between brain areas important for socioemotional behavior, cognitive, and motor function (e.g. amygdala, hippocampus, cerebellum). Neuropathological changes corresponded with neuroimaging results and were consistent with the behavioral and memory deficits. Overall, this study demonstrates that postnatal ZIKV infection in this model may have long-lasting neurodevelopmental consequences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jessica Raper
- Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
- Department of Pediatrics, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA
| | | | - Maud Mavigner
- Department of Pediatrics, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA
- Center for Childhood Infections and Vaccines of Children's Healthcare of Atlanta and Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
| | - Sanjeev Gumber
- Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
| | - Mark W Burke
- Department of Physiology and Biophysics, Howard University, Washington, DC, USA
| | - Jakob Habib
- Department of Pediatrics, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA
| | - Cameron Mattingly
- Department of Pediatrics, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA
| | - Damien Fair
- Oregon Health and Science University, Portland, OR, USA
| | - Eric Earl
- Oregon Health and Science University, Portland, OR, USA
| | - Eric Feczko
- Oregon Health and Science University, Portland, OR, USA
| | - Martin Styner
- Department of Psychiatry, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
| | - Sherrie M Jean
- Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
| | - Joyce K Cohen
- Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
- Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA
| | - Mehul S Suthar
- Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
- Center for Childhood Infections and Vaccines of Children's Healthcare of Atlanta and Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
- Emory Vaccine Center, Atlanta, GA, 30329, USA
| | - Mar M Sanchez
- Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
- Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA
| | - Maria C Alvarado
- Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
| | - Ann Chahroudi
- Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA.
- Department of Pediatrics, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA.
- Center for Childhood Infections and Vaccines of Children's Healthcare of Atlanta and Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Yamagiwa Y, Kurata M, Satoh H. Histological Features of Postnatal Development of the Eye in White Rabbits. Toxicol Pathol 2020; 49:419-437. [PMID: 32323615 DOI: 10.1177/0192623320915460] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/19/2022]
Abstract
Rabbits are frequently used in studies assessing the toxicity of ophthalmic drugs; however, the postnatal histological changes that occur in the rabbit eye have not been fully described. To characterize postnatal ocular development in white rabbits, a histological investigation of the eyes and eyelids was sequentially performed between postnatal days (PNDs) 1 and 42. The eyes opened during PNDs10 to 12. Significant changes prior to eyelid opening included the proliferation of uveal and optic nerve cells, regression of the lenticular vasculature, and thinning of the retina with a decreasing number of retinal cells. After eyelid opening, several significant changes occurred in the anterior segment, including thickening of the cornea and the development of lacrimation-related tissues in the eyelid and conjunctiva. Additionally, the differentiation of retinal layer-derived cells and optic nerve thickening occurred. The lens size continued to increase throughout the postnatal period. The histological structure of the eyes and eyelids was nearly mature by PNDs28 to 42. This study characterizes the postnatal changes in the histological features of the eyes in juvenile white rabbits, providing fundamental knowledge on the appropriate design of histological studies of the eyes in juvenile rabbits, particularly ophthalmic drug evaluations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yoshinori Yamagiwa
- Central Research Laboratories, Research and Development Division, 38332Senju Pharmaceutical Co, Ltd, Hyogo, Japan.,Veterinary Pharmacology and Toxicology Laboratory, Graduate School of Veterinary Sciences, 12836Iwate University, Iwate, Japan
| | - Masaaki Kurata
- Central Research Laboratories, Research and Development Division, 38332Senju Pharmaceutical Co, Ltd, Hyogo, Japan
| | - Hiroshi Satoh
- Veterinary Pharmacology and Toxicology Laboratory, Graduate School of Veterinary Sciences, 12836Iwate University, Iwate, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Understanding the development of amblyopia using macaque monkey models. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2019; 116:26217-26223. [PMID: 31871163 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1902285116] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Amblyopia is a sensory developmental disorder affecting as many as 4% of children around the world. While clinically identified as a reduction in visual acuity and disrupted binocular function, amblyopia affects many low- and high-level perceptual abilities. Research with nonhuman primate models has provided much needed insight into the natural history of amblyopia, its origins and sensitive periods, and the brain mechanisms that underly this disorder. Amblyopia results from abnormal binocular visual experience and impacts the structure and function of the visual pathways beginning at the level of the primary visual cortex (V1). However, there are multiple instances of abnormalities in areas beyond V1 that are not simply inherited from earlier stages of processing. The full constellation of deficits must be taken into consideration in order to understand the broad impact of amblyopia on visual and visual-motor function. The data generated from studies of animal models of the most common forms of amblyopia have provided indispensable insight into the disorder, which has significantly impacted clinical practice. It is expected that this translational impact will continue as ongoing research into the neural correlates of amblyopia provides guidance for novel therapeutic approaches.
Collapse
|
27
|
Irving EL, Lillakas L. Difference between vertical and horizontal saccades across the human lifespan. Exp Eye Res 2019; 183:38-45. [DOI: 10.1016/j.exer.2018.08.020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/11/2018] [Revised: 08/22/2018] [Accepted: 08/24/2018] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
28
|
Abstract
We show how a multi-resolution network can model the development of acuity and coarse-to-fine processing in the mammalian visual cortex. The network adapts to input statistics in an unsupervised manner, and learns a coarse-to-fine representation by using cumulative inhibition of nodes within a network layer. We show that a system of such layers can represent input by hierarchically composing larger parts from smaller components. It can also model aspects of top-down processes, such as image regeneration.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Trond A Tjøstheim
- Lund University Cognitive Science, Lund University, Box 117, 221 00, Lund, Sweden
| | - Christian Balkenius
- Lund University Cognitive Science, Lund University, Box 117, 221 00, Lund, Sweden.
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Graham KL, Byosiere SE, Feng LC, Sanders M, Bennett PC, Caruso K, McCowan CI, White A. A forced-choice preferential looking task for the assessment of vision in dogs: pilot study. J Small Anim Pract 2018; 60:340-347. [PMID: 30474235 DOI: 10.1111/jsap.12965] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/04/2018] [Revised: 08/24/2018] [Accepted: 10/17/2018] [Indexed: 02/01/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES To describe preliminary use of a forced-choice preferential looking task for the clinical assessment of vision in dogs. MATERIALS AND METHODS The vision of 18 pet dogs was investigated in two separate studies using a forced-choice preferential looking task: multiple observers watched eye, head and body movements on video recordings to identify cues suggesting when a dog had seen the feature of interest. Human observer reliability was determined using eight dogs and computer-generated stimuli. Visual acuity was assessed using computer-generated grating stimuli: in real-time, an observer watched each dog's eye movement patterns and behaviour to decide whether each grating was seen. Stimuli were presented in a step-wise manner and were controlled by the observer. Acuity was estimated as the highest spatial frequency the dog was determined to have seen. RESULTS Median estimated visual acuity was better at 1 m compared to that at 3 m. Average test time was longer at a 3-m distance than at 1 m. Inter- and intra-observer reliability was better from 1 m than from 3 m. CLINICAL SIGNIFICANCE Preliminary use of a forced-choice preferential looking task for measurement of visual acuity in dogs has potential use as a clinical tool for the assessment of vision in dogs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- K L Graham
- Clinical Ophthalmology and Eye Health, Sydney Medical School, University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales 2000, Australia
| | - S-E Byosiere
- School of Psychology and Public Health, La Trobe University, Melbourne, Victoria 3083, Australia
| | - L C Feng
- School of Psychology and Public Health, La Trobe University, Melbourne, Victoria 3083, Australia
| | - M Sanders
- Animal Eye Care, Melbourne, Victoria 3145, Australia
| | - P C Bennett
- School of Psychology and Public Health, La Trobe University, Melbourne, Victoria 3083, Australia
| | - K Caruso
- Eye Clinic for Animals, Sydney, New South Wales 2065, Australia
| | - C I McCowan
- University of Melbourne Veterinary Hospital, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Victoria 3030, Australia.,Department of Economic Development, Jobs, Transport and Resources, Melbourne, Victoria 3000, Australia
| | - A White
- Clinical Ophthalmology and Eye Health, Sydney Medical School, University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales 2000, Australia.,Centre for Vision Research, Westmead Institute for Medical Research, Westmead, New South Wales 2145, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Experience-Dependent Development of Feature-Selective Synchronization in the Primary Visual Cortex. J Neurosci 2018; 38:7852-7869. [PMID: 30064994 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.0027-18.2018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/05/2018] [Revised: 07/24/2018] [Accepted: 07/24/2018] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Early visual experience is essential for the maturation of visual functions in which the primary visual cortex plays crucial roles. The extraction of visual features based on response selectivity of individual neurons, a fundamental process in the cortex, is basically established by eye opening in rodents, suggesting that visual experience is required for the development of neural functions other than feature extraction. Here, we show that synchronized firing, which is important for visual information processing, occurs selectively in adjacent neurons sharing similar orientation or spatial frequency preferences in layers 2-4 (upper layer) of rat visual cortex. This feature-selective spike synchrony was rudimentary when the eyes opened and became prominent during the first few weeks after eye opening only in the presence of pattern vision. In contrast, synchronization in layers 5-6 (lower layer) was almost independent of orientation similarity and more weakly dependent on spatial frequency similarity compared with upper layer synchrony. Lower layer synchronization was strengthened during development after eye opening independently of visual experience as a whole. However, the feature selectivity of synchronization was regulated by visual inputs, whereas the inputs without contours were sufficient for this regulation. Therefore, we speculate that feature-selective synchronization in the upper layer may convey detailed information on visual objects to the higher-order cortex, whereas weakly feature-selective synchronization in the lower layer may covey rather rough visual information to the subcortical areas or higher-order cortex. A major role of visual experience may be to establish the specific neural circuits underlying highly feature-selective synchronization.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The neuronal mechanisms underlying experience-dependent improvement of visual functions still remain unresolved. In this study, we investigated whether early visual experience contributes to the development of synchronized neural firing in the primary visual cortex, which plays important roles in visual information processing. We found that synchronized firing depends more remarkably on the similarity of preferred visual stimuli in the upper than lower layer neurons. Pattern vision during development was required for the establishment of spike synchrony in the upper but not the lower layer. These findings provide a new view regarding the role of sensory experience in the functional development of the cortex and the differences in the modes of information processing in the upper and lower cortical layers.
Collapse
|
31
|
Abstract
The primary visual cortex (V1) is the first cortical area that processes visual information. Normal development of V1 depends on binocular vision during the critical period, and age-related losses of vision are linked with neurobiological changes in V1. Animal studies have provided important details about the neurobiological mechanisms in V1 that support normal vision or are changed by visual diseases. There is very little information, however, about those neurobiological mechanisms in human V1. That lack of information has hampered the translation of biologically inspired treatments from preclinical models to effective clinical treatments. We have studied human V1 to characterize the expression of neurobiological mechanisms that regulate visual perception and neuroplasticity. We have identified five stages of development for human V1 that start in infancy and continue across the life span. Here, we describe these stages, compare them with visual and anatomical milestones, and discuss implications for translating treatments for visual disorders that depend on neuroplasticity of V1 function.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Caitlin R Siu
- McMaster Integrative Neuroscience Discovery and Study (MiNDS) Program, McMaster University, Hamilton, ON, Canada
| | - Kathryn M Murphy
- McMaster Integrative Neuroscience Discovery and Study (MiNDS) Program, McMaster University, Hamilton, ON, Canada.,Department of Psychology, Neuroscience & Behaviour, McMaster University, Hamilton, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Preston KL, Freedman HL. The Teller Acuity Card Procedure: An Acuity Screening Technique for Pre-Verbal Children. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2018. [DOI: 10.1080/0065955x.1988.11981765] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/17/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Karen L. Preston
- Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington
| | - Howard L. Freedman
- Bear Creek Professional Center, 17130 Avondale Way N.E., Redmond, Washington
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Van Grootel TJ, Meeson A, Munk MHJ, Kourtzi Z, Movshon JA, Logothetis NK, Kiorpes L. Development of visual cortical function in infant macaques: A BOLD fMRI study. PLoS One 2017; 12:e0187942. [PMID: 29145469 PMCID: PMC5690606 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0187942] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/23/2017] [Accepted: 10/28/2017] [Indexed: 12/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Functional brain development is not well understood. In the visual system, neurophysiological studies in nonhuman primates show quite mature neuronal properties near birth although visual function is itself quite immature and continues to develop over many months or years after birth. Our goal was to assess the relative development of two main visual processing streams, dorsal and ventral, using BOLD fMRI in an attempt to understand the global mechanisms that support the maturation of visual behavior. Seven infant macaque monkeys (Macaca mulatta) were repeatedly scanned, while anesthetized, over an age range of 102 to 1431 days. Large rotating checkerboard stimuli induced BOLD activation in visual cortices at early ages. Additionally we used static and dynamic Glass pattern stimuli to probe BOLD responses in primary visual cortex and two extrastriate areas: V4 and MT-V5. The resulting activations were analyzed with standard GLM and multivoxel pattern analysis (MVPA) approaches. We analyzed three contrasts: Glass pattern present/absent, static/dynamic Glass pattern presentation, and structured/random Glass pattern form. For both GLM and MVPA approaches, robust coherent BOLD activation appeared relatively late in comparison to the maturation of known neuronal properties and the development of behavioral sensitivity to Glass patterns. Robust differential activity to Glass pattern present/absent and dynamic/static stimulus presentation appeared first in V1, followed by V4 and MT-V5 at older ages; there was no reliable distinction between the two extrastriate areas. A similar pattern of results was obtained with the two analysis methods, although MVPA analysis showed reliable differential responses emerging at later ages than GLM. Although BOLD responses to large visual stimuli are detectable, our results with more refined stimuli indicate that global BOLD activity changes as behavioral performance matures. This reflects an hierarchical development of the visual pathways. Since fMRI BOLD reflects neural activity on a population level, our results indicate that, although individual neurons might be adult-like, a longer maturation process takes place on a population level.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tom J Van Grootel
- Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, Tübingen, Germany.,Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, United States of America
| | - Alan Meeson
- Behavioural and Brain Sciences, School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom
| | | | - Zoe Kourtzi
- Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, Tübingen, Germany.,Behavioural and Brain Sciences, School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom.,Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
| | - J Anthony Movshon
- Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, United States of America
| | | | - Lynne Kiorpes
- Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, Tübingen, Germany.,Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Zhang X, Chen W, Li S, Zhou W. Developmental Fine-tuning of Human Olfactory Discriminability. Chem Senses 2017; 42:655-662. [PMID: 28981823 DOI: 10.1093/chemse/bjx047] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Unlike vision or audition, human olfaction is generally considered evolutionarily ancient and well-functioning at birth, yet there have been few empirical data on the development of olfactory acuity. The current study has assessed olfactory discriminability in children aged 3 to 6 years with 16 pairs of single-compound odorants that differ in various degrees in structure and smell. We report a significant improvement over age in young children's overall olfactory discriminability. Critically, such improvement is modulated by the degree of structural similarity between odorants independent of odor familiarity. Our findings indicate that odor representations in the olfactory system are fine-tuned during early childhood (3-6 years of age) to allow refined discrimination. Moreover, they suggest the need to take molecular similarity into consideration in the evaluation of olfactory discrimination in pediatric populations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xiaomeng Zhang
- Institute of Psychology, CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China
| | - Wei Chen
- Institute of Psychology, CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China.,CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China.,Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China
| | - Su Li
- Institute of Psychology, CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China.,Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China
| | - Wen Zhou
- Institute of Psychology, CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China.,CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China.,Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
Van Cruchten S, Vrolyk V, Perron Lepage MF, Baudon M, Voute H, Schoofs S, Haruna J, Benoit-Biancamano MO, Ruot B, Allegaert K. Pre- and Postnatal Development of the Eye: A Species Comparison. Birth Defects Res 2017; 109:1540-1567. [PMID: 28941218 DOI: 10.1002/bdr2.1100] [Citation(s) in RCA: 49] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/23/2017] [Accepted: 07/07/2017] [Indexed: 12/26/2022]
Abstract
In this review paper, literature data on pre- and postnatal eye development are compared between humans and nonclinical species that are commonly used for human safety assessment, namely, mouse, rat, rabbit, dog, minipig, and nonhuman primates. Some new data on rat and minipig ocular development are also included. This compiled information can be helpful for species selection in juvenile toxicity studies or assist in the interpretation of (non)clinical data during pediatric drug development. Despite some differences in developmental windows and anatomical peculiarities, such as the lack of a fovea centralis in nonprimate species or the presence of a nictitating membrane in some nonclinical species, the functioning and development of the eye is strikingly similar between humans and other mammals. As such, all commonly used nonclinical species appear to be relatively good models for human eye development, although some practical constraints such as size may be a limiting factor. Birth Defects Research 109:1540-1567, 2017. © 2017 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Steven Van Cruchten
- Applied Veterinary Morphology, Department of Veterinary Sciences, University of Antwerp, Wilrijk, Belgium
| | - Vanessa Vrolyk
- Département de pathologie et microbiologie, Faculté de Médecine Vétérinaire, Université de Montréal, Saint-Hyacinthe, Canada
| | | | - Marie Baudon
- Charles River, Safety Assessment, Saint-Germain-Nuelles, Lyon, France
| | - Hélène Voute
- Charles River, Safety Assessment, Saint-Germain-Nuelles, Lyon, France
| | | | | | - Marie-Odile Benoit-Biancamano
- Département de pathologie et microbiologie, Faculté de Médecine Vétérinaire, Université de Montréal, Saint-Hyacinthe, Canada
| | - Benoît Ruot
- Charles River, Safety Assessment, Saint-Germain-Nuelles, Lyon, France
| | - Karel Allegaert
- Intensive Care and Department of Surgery, Erasmus MC-Sophia Children's Hospital, Rotterdam, The Netherlands.,Department of development and regeneration, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Patel NB, Hung LF, Harwerth RS. Postnatal maturation of the fovea in Macaca mulatta using optical coherence tomography. Exp Eye Res 2017; 164:8-21. [PMID: 28778401 DOI: 10.1016/j.exer.2017.07.018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/10/2017] [Revised: 06/30/2017] [Accepted: 07/31/2017] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
Changes in the foveal anatomy during infancy are an important component in early development of spatial vision. The present longitudinal study in rhesus monkeys was undertaken to characterize the postnatal maturation of the fovea. Starting at four weeks after birth, the retinas of the left eyes of sixteen infant monkeys were imaged using spectral domain optical coherence tomography (SD OCT). Retinal scans were repeated every 30 days during the first year of life and every 60 days thereafter. Volume scans through the fovea were registered, scaled using a three surface schematic eye, and analyzed to measure foveal pit parameters. The individual layers of the retina were manually segmented and thicknesses were measured over a transverse distance of 1250 microns from the center of the foveal pit. Based on infrared scanning laser ophthalmoscope (IR SLO) images acquired with the SD OCT system, there were significant changes in the extent of the retina scanned as the eyes matured. Using a three-surface schematic eye, the length of each scan could be computed and was validated using image registration (R2 = 0.88, slope = 1.003, p < 0.05). Over the first 18 months of life, the mean retinal thickness at the pit center had increased by 21.4% with a corresponding 20.3% decrease in pit depth. The major changes occurred within the first 120 days, but did not stabilize until a year after birth. In Macaca mulatta infants, the primary anatomical maturation of the fovea occurs within the first few months of life, as determined by longitudinal data from SD OCT measurements. The timelines for maturation of the fovea correspond well with the normal development of the lateral geniculate nucleus, cortical neurophysiology, and spatial resolution in monkeys.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nimesh B Patel
- University of Houston College of Optometry, United States.
| | - Li-Fang Hung
- University of Houston College of Optometry, United States
| | | |
Collapse
|
37
|
The Puzzle of Visual Development: Behavior and Neural Limits. J Neurosci 2017; 36:11384-11393. [PMID: 27911740 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.2937-16.2016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 48] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/21/2016] [Revised: 10/11/2016] [Accepted: 10/12/2016] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
The development of visual function takes place over many months or years in primate infants. Visual sensitivity is very poor near birth and improves over different times courses for different visual functions. The neural mechanisms that underlie these processes are not well understood despite many decades of research. The puzzle arises because research into the factors that limit visual function in infants has found surprisingly mature neural organization and adult-like receptive field properties in very young infants. The high degree of visual plasticity that has been documented during the sensitive period in young children and animals leaves the brain vulnerable to abnormal visual experience. Abnormal visual experience during the sensitive period can lead to amblyopia, a developmental disorder of vision affecting ∼3% of children. This review provides a historical perspective on research into visual development and the disorder amblyopia. The mismatch between the status of the primary visual cortex and visual behavior, both during visual development and in amblyopia, is discussed, and several potential resolutions are considered. It seems likely that extrastriate visual areas further along the visual pathways may set important limits on visual function and show greater vulnerability to abnormal visual experience. Analyses based on multiunit, population activity may provide useful representations of the information being fed forward from primary visual cortex to extrastriate processing areas and to the motor output.
Collapse
|
38
|
Lochy A, de Heering A, Rossion B. The non-linear development of the right hemispheric specialization for human face perception. Neuropsychologia 2017; 126:10-19. [PMID: 28655606 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.06.029] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/01/2017] [Revised: 06/23/2017] [Accepted: 06/23/2017] [Indexed: 12/23/2022]
Abstract
The developmental origins of human adults' right hemispheric specialization for face perception remain unclear. On the one hand, infant studies have shown a right hemispheric advantage for face perception. On the other hand, it has been proposed that the adult right hemispheric lateralization for face perception slowly emerges during childhood due to reading acquisition, which increases left lateralized posterior responses to competing written material (e.g., visual letters and words). Since methodological approaches used in infant and children typically differ when their face capabilities are explored, resolving this issue has been difficult. Here we tested 5-year-old preschoolers varying in their level of visual letter knowledge with the same fast periodic visual stimulation (FPVS) paradigm leading to strongly right lateralized electrophysiological occipito-temporal face-selective responses in 4- to 6-month-old infants (de Heering and Rossion, 2015). Children's face-selective response was quantitatively larger and differed in scalp topography from infants', but did not differ across hemispheres. There was a small positive correlation between preschoolers' letter knowledge and a non-normalized index of right hemispheric specialization for faces. These observations show that previous discrepant results in the literature reflect a genuine nonlinear development of the neural processes underlying face perception and are not merely due to methodological differences across age groups. We discuss several factors that could contribute to the adult right hemispheric lateralization for faces, such as myelination of the corpus callosum and reading acquisition. Our findings point to the value of FPVS coupled with electroencephalography to assess specialized face perception processes throughout development with the same methodology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aliette Lochy
- Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Institute of Neuroscience, University of Louvain, Belgium.
| | - Adélaïde de Heering
- UNESCOG, Center for Cognition & Neurosciences, Universite Libre de Bruxelles, Belgium
| | - Bruno Rossion
- Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Institute of Neuroscience, University of Louvain, Belgium; Neurology Unit, Centre Hospitalier Regional Universitaire (CHRU) de Nancy, F-54000 Nancy, France
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
Kelly KR, Felius J, Ramachandran S, John BA, Jost RM, Birch EE. Congenitally Impaired Disparity Vergence in Children With Infantile Esotropia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2017; 57:2545-51. [PMID: 27159445 PMCID: PMC4868091 DOI: 10.1167/iovs.15-18606] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Purpose We examined whether congenital impairment of disparity vergence in infantile esotropia (ET) exists in children with short duration ET (≤3 months) compared with long-duration ET and healthy controls. A short duration of misalignment would allow for a substantial amount of balanced binocular input during the critical period of binocular disparity development. Methods A total of 19 children aged 5 to 12 years and treated for infantile ET with a short (≤3 months; n = 10) or long (≥5 months; n = 9) duration of constant misalignment before alignment were enrolled. A total of 22 healthy control children were enrolled as a comparison group. Eye movements during disparity vergence and accommodative vergence were recorded using an EyeLink 1000 binocular eye tracker. Mean response gain was compared between and within groups to determine the effect of duration of misalignment and viewing condition. Results Compared with controls, children with short (P = 0.002) and long (P < 0.001) duration infantile ET had reduced response gains for disparity vergence, but not for accommodative vergence (P = 0.19). Conclusions Regardless of duration of misalignment, children with infantile ET had reduced disparity vergence, consistent with a congenital impairment of disparity vergence in infantile ET. Although early correction of misalignment increases the likelihood that some level of binocular disparity sensitivity will be present, normal levels may never be achieved.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Krista R Kelly
- Retina Foundation of the Southwest, Dallas, Texas, United States
| | - Joost Felius
- Retina Foundation of the Southwest, Dallas, Texas, United States 2Department of Ophthalmology, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, Dallas, Texas, United States
| | | | - Blesson A John
- Retina Foundation of the Southwest, Dallas, Texas, United States
| | - Reed M Jost
- Retina Foundation of the Southwest, Dallas, Texas, United States
| | - Eileen E Birch
- Retina Foundation of the Southwest, Dallas, Texas, United States 2Department of Ophthalmology, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, Dallas, Texas, United States
| |
Collapse
|
40
|
Norcia AM, Pei F, Kohler PJ. Evidence for long-range spatiotemporal interactions in infant and adult visual cortex. J Vis 2017. [PMID: 28622700 PMCID: PMC5477630 DOI: 10.1167/17.6.12] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
The development of spatiotemporal interactions giving rise to classical receptive field properties has been well studied in animal models, but little is known about the development of putative nonclassical mechanisms in any species. Here we used visual evoked potentials to study the developmental status of spatiotemporal interactions for stimuli that were biased to engage long-range spatiotemporal integration mechanisms. We compared responses to widely spaced stimuli presented either in temporal succession or at the same time. The former configuration elicits a percept of apparent motion in adults but the latter does not. Component flash responses were summed to make a linear prediction (no spatiotemporal interaction) for comparison with the measured evoked responses to sequential or simultaneous flash conditions. In adults, linear summation of the separate flash responses measured with 40% contrast stimuli predicted sequential flash responses twice as large as those measured, indicating that the response measured under apparent motion conditions is subadditive. Simultaneous-flash responses at the same spatial separation were also subadditive, but substantially less so. The subadditivity in both cases could be modeled as a simple multiplicative gain term across all electrodes and time points. In infants aged 3-8 months, responses to the stimuli used in adults were similar to their linear predictions at 40%, but the responses measured at 80% contrast resembled the subadditive responses of the adults for both sequential and simultaneous flash conditions. We interpret the developmental data as indicating that adult-like long-range spatiotemporal interactions can be demonstrated by 3-8 months, once stimulus contrast is high enough.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anthony M Norcia
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
| | - Francesca Pei
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USADepartment of Psychiatry, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
| | - Peter J Kohler
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
41
|
Pullela M, Degler BA, Coats DK, Das VE. Longitudinal Evaluation of Eye Misalignment and Eye Movements Following Surgical Correction of Strabismus in Monkeys. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2017; 57:6040-6047. [PMID: 27820877 PMCID: PMC5102570 DOI: 10.1167/iovs.16-20481] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Purpose Strabismus correction surgery is well documented in both the literature and practice with varying levels of success and permanence. Our goal was to characterize longitudinal changes in eye alignment and eye movements following strabismus correction surgery in a monkey model for developmental strabismus. Methods We studied two juvenile rhesus monkeys with exotropia previously induced via an optical prism-rearing paradigm in infancy. Eye misalignment was corrected via a resection–recession surgery of the horizontal rectus muscles of one eye. Binocular search coils were used to collect eye movement data during smooth-pursuit, saccades, and fixation tasks before surgical treatment, immediately after surgery, and through 6 months after treatment. Results Both animals showed an immediate ∼70% reduction in misalignment as a consequence of surgery that regressed to a 20%–40% improvement by 6 months after treatment. Significant changes were observed in saccade and smooth-pursuit gain of the nonviewing eye after surgery, which also reverted to presurgical values by 6 months. A temporary improvement in fixation stability of the nonviewing eye was observed after surgery; naso-temporal (N/T) asymmetry of monocular smooth-pursuit remained unchanged. Conclusions Surgical realignment is followed by plastic changes that often lead to reversal of surgery effects. Immediate improvement in misalignment and changes in eye movement gains are likely a result of contractility changes at the level of the extraocular muscle, whereas longer-term effects are likely a combination of neural and muscle adaptation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mythri Pullela
- College of Optometry, University of Houston, Houston, Texas, United States
| | - Brittany A Degler
- College of Optometry, University of Houston, Houston, Texas, United States
| | - David K Coats
- Department of Ophthalmology, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas, United States
| | - Vallabh E Das
- College of Optometry, University of Houston, Houston, Texas, United States
| |
Collapse
|
42
|
Joshi AC, Agaoglu MN, Das VE. Comparison of Naso-temporal Asymmetry During Monocular Smooth Pursuit, Optokinetic Nystagmus, and Ocular Following Response in Strabismic Monkeys. Strabismus 2017; 25:47-55. [PMID: 28463578 DOI: 10.1080/09273972.2017.1317821] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/29/2022]
Abstract
PURPOSE Under monocular viewing conditions, humans and monkeys with infantile strabismus exhibit asymmetric naso-temporal (N-T) responses to motion stimuli. The goal of this study was to compare and contrast these N-T asymmetries during 3 visually mediated eye tracking tasks-optokinetic nystagmus (OKN), smooth pursuit (SP) response, and ocular following responses (OFR). METHODS Two adult strabismic monkeys were tested under monocular viewing conditions during OKN, SP, or OFR stimulation. OKN stimulus was unidirectional motion of a 30°x30° random dot pattern at 20°, 40°, or 80°/s for 1 minute. OFR stimulus was brief (200 ms) unidirectional motion of a 38°x28°whitenoise at 20°, 40°, or 80°/s. SP stimulus consisted of foveal step-ramp target motion at 10°, 20°, or 40°/s. RESULTS Mean nasalward steady state gain (0.87±0.16) was larger than temporalward gain (0.67±0.19) during monocular OKN (P<0.001). In monocular OFR, the asymmetry is manifested as a difference in OFR velocity gain (nasalward: 0.33±0.19, temporalward: 0.22±0.12; P=0.007). During monocular SP, mean nasal gain (0.97±0.2) was larger than temporal gain (0.66±0.14; P<0.001) and the mean nasalward acceleration during pursuit initiation (156±61°/s2) was larger than temporalward acceleration (118±77°/s2; P=0.04). Comparison of N-T asymmetry ratio across the 3 conditions using ANOVA showed no significant difference. CONCLUSIONS N-T asymmetries are identified in all 3 visual tracking paradigms in both monkeys with either eye viewing. Our data are consistent with the current hypothesis for the mechanism for N-T asymmetry that invokes an imbalance in cortical drive to brainstem circuits.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anand C Joshi
- a College of Optometry , University of Houston , Houston , TX
| | | | - Vallabh E Das
- a College of Optometry , University of Houston , Houston , TX
| |
Collapse
|
43
|
Meier K, Giaschi D. Effect of spatial and temporal stimulus parameters on the maturation of global motion perception. Vision Res 2017; 135:1-9. [PMID: 28414023 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2017.04.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/06/2017] [Revised: 03/31/2017] [Accepted: 04/11/2017] [Indexed: 02/03/2023]
Abstract
There are discrepancies with respect to the age at which adult-like performance is reached on tasks assessing global motion perception. This is in part because performance in children depends on stimulus parameters. We recently showed that five-year-olds demonstrated adult-like performance over a range of speeds when the speed ratio was comprised of longer spatial and temporal displacements; but displayed immature performance when the speed ratio was comprised of shorter displacements. The goal of the current study was to assess the effect of these global motion stimulus parameters across a broader age range in order to estimate the age at which mature performance is reached. Motion coherence thresholds were assessed in 182 children and adults aged 7-30years. Dot displacement (Δx) was 1, 5, or 30min of arc; frame duration (Δt) was 17 or 50ms. This created a total of six conditions. Consistent with our previous results, coherence thresholds in the youngest children assessed were adult-like at the two conditions with the largest Δx. Maturity was reached around age 12 for the medium Δx, and by age 16 for the smallest Δx. Performance did not appear to be affected by Δt. This late maturation may reflect a long developmental period for cortical networks underlying global motion perception. These findings resolve many of the discrepancies across previous studies, and should be considered when using global motion tasks to assess children with atypical development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kimberly Meier
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, 2136 West Mall, Vancouver, B.C. V6T 1Z4, Canada.
| | - Deborah Giaschi
- Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, University of British Columbia, Rm E300E, 4480 Oak Street, Vancouver, B.C. V6H 3V4, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
44
|
Damon F, Méary D, Quinn PC, Lee K, Simpson EA, Paukner A, Suomi SJ, Pascalis O. Preference for facial averageness: Evidence for a common mechanism in human and macaque infants. Sci Rep 2017; 7:46303. [PMID: 28406237 PMCID: PMC5390246 DOI: 10.1038/srep46303] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/15/2016] [Accepted: 03/14/2017] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Human adults and infants show a preference for average faces, which could stem from a general processing mechanism and may be shared among primates. However, little is known about preference for facial averageness in monkeys. We used a comparative developmental approach and eye-tracking methodology to assess visual attention in human and macaque infants to faces naturally varying in their distance from a prototypical face. In Experiment 1, we examined the preference for faces relatively close to or far from the prototype in 12-month-old human infants with human adult female faces. Infants preferred faces closer to the average than faces farther from it. In Experiment 2, we measured the looking time of 3-month-old rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) viewing macaque faces varying in their distance from the prototype. Like human infants, macaque infants looked longer to faces closer to the average. In Experiments 3 and 4, both species were presented with unfamiliar categories of faces (i.e., macaque infants tested with adult macaque faces; human infants and adults tested with infant macaque faces) and showed no prototype preferences, suggesting that the prototypicality effect is experience-dependent. Overall, the findings suggest a common processing mechanism across species, leading to averageness preferences in primates.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Fabrice Damon
- Univ. Grenoble-Alpes, LPNC, France
- CNRS, LPNC,UMR 5105, France
| | - David Méary
- Univ. Grenoble-Alpes, LPNC, France
- CNRS, LPNC,UMR 5105, France
| | | | | | | | - Annika Paukner
- Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, USA
| | - Stephen J. Suomi
- Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
45
|
Upadhyaya S, Meng H, Das VE. Electrical stimulation of superior colliculus affects strabismus angle in monkey models for strabismus. J Neurophysiol 2016; 117:1281-1292. [PMID: 28031397 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00437.2016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/02/2016] [Revised: 12/20/2016] [Accepted: 12/27/2016] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Disruption of binocular vision during the critical period for development leads to eye misalignment in humans and in monkey models. We have previously suggested that disruption within a vergence circuit could be the neural basis for strabismus. Electrical stimulation in the rostral superior colliculus (rSC) leads to vergence eye movements in normal monkeys. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to investigate the effect of SC stimulation on eye misalignment in strabismic monkeys. Electrical stimulation was delivered to 51 sites in the intermediate and deep layers of the SC (400 Hz, 0.5-s duration, 10-40 μA) in 3 adult optical prism-reared strabismic monkeys. Scleral search coils were used to measure movements of both eyes during a fixation task. Staircase saccades with horizontal and vertical components were elicited by stimulation as predicted from the SC topographic map. Electrical stimulation also resulted in significant changes in horizontal strabismus angle, i.e., a shift toward exotropia/esotropia depending on stimulation site. Electrically evoked saccade vector amplitude in the two eyes was not significantly different (P > 0.05; paired t-test) but saccade direction differed. However, saccade disconjugacy accounted for only ~50% of the change in horizontal misalignment while disconjugate postsaccadic movements accounted for the other ~50% of the change in misalignment due to electrical stimulation. In summary, our data suggest that electrical stimulation of the SC of strabismic monkeys produces a change in horizontal eye alignment that is due to a combination of disconjugate saccadic eye movements and disconjugate postsaccadic movements.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Electrical stimulation of the superior colliculus in strabismic monkeys results in a change in eye misalignment. These data support the notion of developmental disruption of vergence circuits leading to maintenance of eye misalignment in strabismus.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Hui Meng
- College of Optometry, University of Houston, Houston, Texas
| | - Vallabh E Das
- College of Optometry, University of Houston, Houston, Texas
| |
Collapse
|
46
|
Abstract
Disrupting binocular vision in infancy leads to strabismus and oftentimes to a variety of associated visual sensory deficits and oculomotor abnormalities. Investigation of this disorder has been aided by the development of various animal models, each of which has advantages and disadvantages. In comparison to studies of binocular visual responses in cortical structures, investigations of neural oculomotor structures that mediate the misalignment and abnormalities of eye movements have been more recent, and these studies have shown that different brain areas are intimately involved in driving several aspects of the strabismic condition, including horizontal misalignment, dissociated deviations, A and V patterns of strabismus, disconjugate eye movements, nystagmus, and fixation switch. The responses of cells in visual and oculomotor areas that potentially drive the sensory deficits and also eye alignment and eye movement abnormalities follow a general theme of disrupted calibration, lower sensitivity, and poorer specificity compared with the normally developed visual oculomotor system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Vallabh E Das
- College of Optometry, University of Houston, Houston, Texas 77204;
| |
Collapse
|
47
|
Carlson SE, Colombo J. Docosahexaenoic Acid and Arachidonic Acid Nutrition in Early Development. Adv Pediatr 2016; 63:453-71. [PMID: 27426911 PMCID: PMC5207030 DOI: 10.1016/j.yapd.2016.04.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 82] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/15/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Susan E Carlson
- Department of Dietetics and Nutrition, University of Kansas Medical Center, MS 4013, 3901 Rainbow Boulevard, Kansas City, KS 66160, USA.
| | - John Colombo
- Department of Neurology, University of Kansas Medical Center, 3901 Rainbow Boulevard, Kansas City, KS 66160, USA; Department of Psychology, Life Span Institute, 1000 Sunnyside Avenue, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS 66045, USA
| |
Collapse
|
48
|
Pirdankar OH, Das VE. Influence of Target Parameters on Fixation Stability in Normal and Strabismic Monkeys. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2016; 57:1087-95. [PMID: 26968739 PMCID: PMC4790473 DOI: 10.1167/iovs.15-17896] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
PURPOSE The purpose of this study was to assess the effect of fixation target parameters on fixation instability in strabismic monkeys. METHODS One normal and three exotropic monkeys were presented with four differently shaped fixation targets, with three diameters, during monocular or binocular viewing. Fixation targets were white on a black background or vice versa. Binocular eye movements were recorded using the magnetic search coil technique and fixation stability quantified by calculating the bivariate contour ellipse area (BCEA). RESULTS Fixation instability was greater in all the strabismic monkeys compared with the normal monkey. During monocular viewing, strabismic monkeys showed significantly greater instability in the covered eye compared to the fixating eye. Multifactorial ANOVA suggested statistically significant target parameter influences, although effect sizes were small. Thus, a disk-shaped target resulted in greater instability than other target shapes in the viewing eyes of the normal monkey and two of three strabismic monkeys. A similar target-shape effect was also observed in the covered eye. Least instability was elicited with a 0.5° target in the normal monkey and a 1.0° target in the strabismic monkeys, both in the viewing and the covered eye. Target/background polarity effects were idiosyncratic. In strabismic monkeys, stability of the fixating eye during binocular viewing was not different from the stability of the same eye during monocular viewing. CONCLUSIONS Abnormal drifts and nystagmus contribute to increased fixation instability in strabismic monkeys. Target parameters (shape and size) that influence fixation stability in a normal animal also affected fixation stability in our sample of strabismic monkeys.
Collapse
|
49
|
Adaptive Neuromorphic Circuit for Stereoscopic Disparity Using Ocular Dominance Map. NEUROSCIENCE JOURNAL 2016; 2016:8751874. [PMID: 27243029 PMCID: PMC4868909 DOI: 10.1155/2016/8751874] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/20/2015] [Revised: 02/27/2016] [Accepted: 03/13/2016] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
Stereopsis or depth perception is a critical aspect of information processing in the brain and is computed from the positional shift or disparity between the images seen by the two eyes. Various algorithms and their hardware implementation that compute disparity in real time have been proposed; however, most of them compute disparity through complex mathematical calculations that are difficult to realize in hardware and are biologically unrealistic. The brain presumably uses simpler methods to extract depth information from the environment and hence newer methodologies that could perform stereopsis with brain like elegance need to be explored. This paper proposes an innovative aVLSI design that leverages the columnar organization of ocular dominance in the brain and uses time-staggered Winner Take All (ts-WTA) to adaptively create disparity tuned cells. Physiological findings support the presence of disparity cells in the visual cortex and show that these cells surface as a result of binocular stimulation received after birth. Therefore, creating in hardware cells that can learn different disparities with experience not only is novel but also is biologically more realistic. These disparity cells, when allowed to interact diffusively on a larger scale, can be used to adaptively create stable topological disparity maps in silicon.
Collapse
|
50
|
Parr LA, Murphy L, Feczko E, Brooks J, Collantes M, Heitz TR. Experience-dependent changes in the development of face preferences in infant rhesus monkeys. Dev Psychobiol 2016; 58:1002-1018. [DOI: 10.1002/dev.21434] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/27/2015] [Accepted: 05/12/2016] [Indexed: 01/18/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Lisa A. Parr
- Yerkes National Primate Research Center; Atlanta Georgia
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science; Emory University; Atlanta Georgia
- Center for Translational Social Neuroscience; Emory University; Atlanta Georgia
| | - Lauren Murphy
- Yerkes National Primate Research Center; Atlanta Georgia
- Department of Psychology; Emory University; Atlanta Georgia
| | - Eric Feczko
- Yerkes National Primate Research Center; Atlanta Georgia
- Center for Translational Social Neuroscience; Emory University; Atlanta Georgia
| | - Jenna Brooks
- Yerkes National Primate Research Center; Atlanta Georgia
| | | | | |
Collapse
|