1
|
Mather M. The emotion paradox in the aging body and brain. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2024; 1536:13-41. [PMID: 38676452 DOI: 10.1111/nyas.15138] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 04/29/2024]
Abstract
With age, parasympathetic activity decreases, while sympathetic activity increases. Thus, the typical older adult has low heart rate variability (HRV) and high noradrenaline levels. Younger adults with this physiological profile tend to be unhappy and stressed. Yet, with age, emotional experience tends to improve. Why does older adults' emotional well-being not suffer as their HRV decreases? To address this apparent paradox, I present the autonomic compensation model. In this model, failing organs, the initial phases of Alzheimer's pathology, and other age-related diseases trigger noradrenergic hyperactivity. To compensate, older brains increase autonomic regulatory activity in the pregenual prefrontal cortex (PFC). Age-related declines in nerve conduction reduce the ability of the pregenual PFC to reduce hyperactive noradrenergic activity and increase peripheral HRV. But these pregenual PFC autonomic compensation efforts have a significant impact in the brain, where they bias processing in favor of stimuli that tend to increase parasympathetic activity (e.g., stimuli that increase feelings of safety) and against stimuli that tend to increase sympathetic activity (e.g., threatening stimuli). In summary, the autonomic compensation model posits that age-related chronic sympathetic/noradrenergic hyperactivity stimulates regulatory attempts that have the side effect of enhancing emotional well-being.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mara Mather
- Leonard Davis School of Gerontology, Department of Psychology, and Department of Biomedical Engineering, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California, USA
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
de Chastelaine M, Horne ED, Hou M, Rugg MD. Relationships between age, fMRI correlates of familiarity and familiarity-based memory performance under single and dual task conditions. Neuropsychologia 2023; 189:108670. [PMID: 37633516 PMCID: PMC10591814 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108670] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/26/2023] [Revised: 08/22/2023] [Accepted: 08/23/2023] [Indexed: 08/28/2023]
Abstract
Using fMRI, we investigated the effects of age and divided attention on the neural correlates of familiarity and their relationship with memory performance. At study, word pairs were visually presented to young and older participants under the requirement to make a relational judgment on each pair. Participants were then scanned while undertaking an associative recognition test under single and dual (auditory tone detection) task conditions. The test items comprised studied, rearranged (words from different studied pairs) and new word pairs. fMRI familiarity effects were operationalized as greater activity elicited by studied pairs incorrectly identified as 'rearranged' than by correctly rejected new pairs. The reverse contrast was employed to identify 'novelty' effects. Behavioral familiarity estimates were equivalent across age groups and task conditions. Robust fMRI familiarity effects were identified in several regions, including medial and superior lateral parietal cortex, dorsal medial and left lateral prefrontal cortex, and bilateral caudate. fMRI novelty effects were identified in the anterior medial temporal lobe. Both familiarity and novelty effects were largely age-invariant and did not vary, or varied minimally, according to task condition. In addition, the familiarity effects correlated positively with a behavioral estimate of familiarity strength irrespective of age. These findings extend a previous report from our laboratory, and converge with prior behavioral reports, in demonstrating that the factors of age and divided attention have little impact on behavioral and neural estimates of familiarity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marianne de Chastelaine
- Center for Vital Longevity and School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, University of Texas, Dallas, TX, USA.
| | - Erin D Horne
- Center for Vital Longevity and School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, University of Texas, Dallas, TX, USA
| | - Mingzhu Hou
- Center for Vital Longevity and School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, University of Texas, Dallas, TX, USA
| | - Michael D Rugg
- Center for Vital Longevity and School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, University of Texas, Dallas, TX, USA
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
de Chastelaine M, Horne ED, Hou M, Rugg MD. Relationships between age, fMRI correlates of familiarity and familiarity-based memory performance under single and dual task conditions. BIORXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR BIOLOGY 2023:2023.05.26.542526. [PMID: 37398000 PMCID: PMC10312430 DOI: 10.1101/2023.05.26.542526] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 07/04/2023]
Abstract
Using fMRI, we investigated the effects of age and divided attention on the neural correlates of familiarity and their relationship with memory performance. At study, word pairs were visually presented to young and older participants under the requirement to make a relational judgment on each pair. Participants were then scanned while undertaking an associative recognition test under single and dual (auditory tone detection) task conditions. The test items comprised studied, rearranged (words from different studied pairs) and new word pairs. fMRI familiarity effects were operationalized as greater activity elicited by studied pairs incorrectly identified as 'rearranged' than by correctly rejected new pairs. The reverse contrast was employed to identify 'novelty' effects. Behavioral familiarity estimates were equivalent across age groups and task conditions. Robust fMRI familiarity effects were identified in several regions, including medial and superior lateral parietal cortex, dorsal medial and left lateral prefrontal cortex, and bilateral caudate. fMRI novelty effects were identified in the anterior medial temporal lobe. Both familiarity and novelty effects were age-invariant and did not vary according to task condition. In addition, the familiarity effects correlated positively with a behavioral estimate of familiarity strength irrespective of age. These findings extend a previous report from our laboratory, and converge with prior behavioral reports, in demonstrating that the factors of age and divided attention have minimal impact on behavioral and neural estimates of familiarity.
Collapse
|
4
|
Westlin C, Theriault JE, Katsumi Y, Nieto-Castanon A, Kucyi A, Ruf SF, Brown SM, Pavel M, Erdogmus D, Brooks DH, Quigley KS, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Barrett LF. Improving the study of brain-behavior relationships by revisiting basic assumptions. Trends Cogn Sci 2023; 27:246-257. [PMID: 36739181 PMCID: PMC10012342 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2022.12.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 15.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/07/2022] [Revised: 12/23/2022] [Accepted: 12/29/2022] [Indexed: 02/05/2023]
Abstract
Neuroimaging research has been at the forefront of concerns regarding the failure of experimental findings to replicate. In the study of brain-behavior relationships, past failures to find replicable and robust effects have been attributed to methodological shortcomings. Methodological rigor is important, but there are other overlooked possibilities: most published studies share three foundational assumptions, often implicitly, that may be faulty. In this paper, we consider the empirical evidence from human brain imaging and the study of non-human animals that calls each foundational assumption into question. We then consider the opportunities for a robust science of brain-behavior relationships that await if scientists ground their research efforts in revised assumptions supported by current empirical evidence.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Jordan E Theriault
- Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Yuta Katsumi
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Alfonso Nieto-Castanon
- Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, Boston University, Boston, MA, USA; Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA
| | - Aaron Kucyi
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Drexel University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Sebastian F Ruf
- Department of Civil and Environmental Engineering, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Sarah M Brown
- Department of Computer Science and Statistics, University of Rhode Island, Kingston, RI, USA
| | - Misha Pavel
- Khoury College of Computer Sciences, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA; Bouvé College of Health Sciences, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Deniz Erdogmus
- Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Dana H Brooks
- Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Karen S Quigley
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA
| | | | - Lisa Feldman Barrett
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA; A.A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA; Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Doucet GE, Kruse JA, Hamlin N, Oleson JJ, White SF. Changing role of the amygdala in affective and cognitive traits between early and late adulthood. Front Psychiatry 2023; 14:1033543. [PMID: 36824676 PMCID: PMC9941165 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1033543] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/31/2022] [Accepted: 01/18/2023] [Indexed: 02/10/2023] Open
Abstract
Introduction Healthy aging is typically associated with cognitive decline and lower negative affect. Previous studies have reported a significant and opposite role of the amygdala in relation to cognitive and affective processing in early adulthood. However, it remains unclear how aging impacts such relationships. Methods Seventy-seven healthy participants including 40 young (mean age = 26.1 years) and 37 older (mean age = 61.8 years) adults completed a functional MRI Affective Stroop (AS) paradigm, a cognitive battery, and the state-trait anxiety inventory. The AS fMRI paradigm included "task trials," where participants saw a positively, negatively or neutrally valenced distractor image, followed by a numerical display, followed by another distractor image. We extracted signal in both amygdalas during the AS Task and compared it across all conditions and age group. We further conducted moderation analyses to investigate the impact of aging on the relationship between amygdala activation and anxiety or cognitive variables, respectively. Results At the behavioral level, older participants showed lower trait anxiety than the younger adults (p = 0.002). While overall slower during the AS task, older adults achieved comparable accuracy during the AS task, relative to the younger adults. At the brain level, we revealed a significant interaction between age group and trial types in amygdala activation (F = 4.9, p = 0.03), with the older group showing stronger activation during the most complex trials compared to the passive view trials. We further found that age significantly modulated the relationship between anxiety and the left amygdala activation during negative stimuli, where the younger adults showed a positive association while the older adults showed a negative association. Age also significantly modulated the relationship between verbal fluency and left amygdala activation during incongruent versus view trials, with the younger adults showing a negative association and the older adults showing a positive association. Discussion The current study suggests that the role of the amygdala on both emotional processing and cognitive traits changes between early and late adulthood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gaelle E. Doucet
- Institute for Human Neuroscience, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Omaha, NE, United States
- Department of Pharmacology and Neuroscience, Creighton University School of Medicine, Omaha, NE, United States
| | - Jordanna A. Kruse
- Institute for Human Neuroscience, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Omaha, NE, United States
| | - Noah Hamlin
- Institute for Human Neuroscience, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Omaha, NE, United States
| | - Jacob J. Oleson
- Department of Biostatistics, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, United States
| | - Stuart F. White
- Institute for Human Neuroscience, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Omaha, NE, United States
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Zheng CJ, Van Drunen S, Egorova-Brumley N. Neural correlates of co-occurring pain and depression: an activation-likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analysis and systematic review. Transl Psychiatry 2022; 12:196. [PMID: 35545623 PMCID: PMC9095719 DOI: 10.1038/s41398-022-01949-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/26/2021] [Revised: 04/19/2022] [Accepted: 04/21/2022] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
The relationship between pain and depression is thought to be bidirectional and the underlying neurobiology 'shared' between the two conditions. However, these claims are often based on qualitative comparisons of brain regions implicated in pain or depression, while focused quantitative studies of the neurobiology of pain-depression comorbidity are lacking. Particularly, the direction of comorbidity, i.e., pain with depression vs. depression with pain, is rarely addressed. In this systematic review (PROSPERO registration CRD42020219876), we aimed to delineate brain correlates associated with primary pain with concomitant depression, primary depression with concurrent pain, and equal pain and depression comorbidity, using activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analysis. Neuroimaging studies published in English until the 28th of September 2021 were evaluated using PRISMA guidelines. A total of 70 studies were included, of which 26 reported stereotactic coordinates and were analysed with ALE. All studies were assessed for quality by two authors, using the National Institute of Health Quality Assessment Tool. Our results revealed paucity of studies that directly investigated the neurobiology of pain-depression comorbidity. The ALE analysis indicated that pain with concomitant depression was associated with the right amygdala, while depression with concomitant pain was related primarily to the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). We provide evidence that pain and depression have a cumulative negative effect on a specific set of brain regions, distinct for primary diagnosis of depression vs. pain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Natalia Egorova-Brumley
- The University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC, Australia.
- The Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health, Parkville, VIC, Australia.
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Petro NM, Basyouni R, Neta M. Positivity effect in aging: evidence for the primacy of positive responses to emotional ambiguity. Neurobiol Aging 2021; 106:232-240. [PMID: 34311432 DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2021.06.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/15/2020] [Revised: 06/09/2021] [Accepted: 06/15/2021] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Older compared to younger adults show greater amygdala activity to positive emotions, and are more likely to interpret emotionally ambiguous stimuli (e.g., surprised faces) as positive. While some evidence suggests this positivity effect results from a top-down, effortful mechanism, others suggest it may emerge as the default or initial response. The amygdala is a key node in rapid, bottom-up processing and patterns of amygdala activity over time (e.g., habituation) can shed light on the mechanisms underlying the positivity effect. Younger and older adults passively viewed neutral and surprised faces in an MRI. Only in older adults, amygdala habituation was associated with the tendency to interpret surprised faces as positive or negative (valence bias), where a more positive bias was associated with greater habituation. Interestingly, although a positive bias in younger adults was associated with slower responses, consistent with an initial negativity hypothesis in younger adults, older adults showed faster categorizations of positivity. Together, we propose that there may be a switch to a primacy of positivity in aging.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nathan M Petro
- Department of Psychology, Center for Brain, Biology, and Behavior, University of Nebraska-Lincoln, Lincoln, NE, USA.
| | - Ruby Basyouni
- Department of Psychology, Center for Brain, Biology, and Behavior, University of Nebraska-Lincoln, Lincoln, NE, USA
| | - Maital Neta
- Department of Psychology, Center for Brain, Biology, and Behavior, University of Nebraska-Lincoln, Lincoln, NE, USA
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Maleki G, Mazaheri MA, Nejati V, Borhani K, Bosmans G. The Attachment-related picture set (ARPS): development and validation. CURRENT PSYCHOLOGY 2021. [DOI: 10.1007/s12144-021-01727-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
9
|
Imaging recollection, familiarity, and novelty in the frontoparietal control and default mode networks and the anterior-posterior medial temporal lobe: An integrated view and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2021; 126:491-508. [PMID: 33857579 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.04.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/08/2020] [Revised: 03/04/2021] [Accepted: 04/07/2021] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
A network-level model of recollection-based recognition (R), familiarity-based recognition (F), and novelty recognition (N) was constructed, and its validity was evaluated through meta-analyses to produce an integrated view of neuroimaging data. The model predicted the following: (a) the overall magnitude of the frontoparietal control network (FPCN) activity (which supports retrieval and decision effort) is in the order of F > R > N; (b) that of the posterior medial temporal network (MTL) activity (which plays a direct role in retrieval) is in the order of R > N > F; (c) that of the anterior MTL activity (which supports novelty-encoding) is in the order of N > R > F; (d) that of the default mode network (DMN) activity (which supports the subjective experience of remembering) is in the order of R > N > F. The meta-analyses results were consistent with these predictions. Subsystem analysis indicated a functional dissociation between the cingulo-opercular vs. frontoparietal components of the FPCN and between the core vs. medial temporal components of the DMN.
Collapse
|
10
|
MacCormack JK, Henry TR, Davis BM, Oosterwijk S, Lindquist KA. Aging bodies, aging emotions: Interoceptive differences in emotion representations and self-reports across adulthood. Emotion 2021; 21:227-246. [PMID: 31750705 PMCID: PMC7239717 DOI: 10.1037/emo0000699] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Bodily sensations are closely linked to emotional experiences. However, most research assessing the body-emotion link focuses on young adult samples. Inspired by prior work showing age-related declines in autonomic reactivity and interoception, we present 2 studies investigating age-related differences in the extent to which adults (18-75 years) associate interoceptive or internal bodily sensations with emotions. Study 1 (N = 150) used a property association task to assess age effects on adults' tendencies to associate interoceptive sensations, relative to behaviors or situations, with negative emotion categories (e.g., anger, sadness). Study 2 (N = 200) used the Day Reconstruction experience sampling method to assess the effect of age on adults' tendencies to report interoceptive sensations and emotional experiences in daily life. Consistent with prior literature suggesting that older adults have more muted physiological responses and interoceptive abilities than younger adults, we found that older adults' mental representations (Study 1) and self-reported experiences (Study 2) of emotion are less associated with interoceptive sensations than are those of younger adults. Across both studies, age effects were most prominent for high arousal emotions (e.g., anger, fear) and sensations (e.g., racing heart) that are often associated with peripheral psychophysiological concomitants in young adults. These findings are consistent with psychological constructionist models and a "maturational dualism" account of emotional aging, suggesting additional pathways by which emotions may differ across adulthood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
Collapse
|
11
|
Psychological mechanisms and functions of 5-HT and SSRIs in potential therapeutic change: Lessons from the serotonergic modulation of action selection, learning, affect, and social cognition. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2020; 119:138-167. [PMID: 32931805 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.09.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/17/2020] [Revised: 08/31/2020] [Accepted: 09/03/2020] [Indexed: 12/14/2022]
Abstract
Uncertainty regarding which psychological mechanisms are fundamental in mediating SSRI treatment outcomes and wide-ranging variability in their efficacy has raised more questions than it has solved. Since subjective mood states are an abstract scientific construct, only available through self-report in humans, and likely involving input from multiple top-down and bottom-up signals, it has been difficult to model at what level SSRIs interact with this process. Converging translational evidence indicates a role for serotonin in modulating context-dependent parameters of action selection, affect, and social cognition; and concurrently supporting learning mechanisms, which promote adaptability and behavioural flexibility. We examine the theoretical basis, ecological validity, and interaction of these constructs and how they may or may not exert a clinical benefit. Specifically, we bridge crucial gaps between disparate lines of research, particularly findings from animal models and human clinical trials, which often seem to present irreconcilable differences. In determining how SSRIs exert their effects, our approach examines the endogenous functions of 5-HT neurons, how 5-HT manipulations affect behaviour in different contexts, and how their therapeutic effects may be exerted in humans - which may illuminate issues of translational models, hierarchical mechanisms, idiographic variables, and social cognition.
Collapse
|
12
|
Ye R, Touroutoglou A, Brickhouse M, Katz S, Growdon JH, Johnson KA, Dickerson BC, Gomperts SN. Topography of cortical thinning in the Lewy body diseases. NEUROIMAGE-CLINICAL 2020; 26:102196. [PMID: 32059167 PMCID: PMC7016450 DOI: 10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102196] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/17/2019] [Revised: 01/21/2020] [Accepted: 01/22/2020] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Objective Regional cortical thinning in dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) and Parkinson disease dementia (PDD) may underlie some aspect of their clinical impairments; cortical atrophy likely reflects extensive Lewy body pathology with alpha-synuclein deposits, as well as associated Alzheimer's disease co-pathologies, when present. Here we investigated the topographic distribution of cortical thinning in these Lewy body diseases compared to cognitively normal PD and healthy non-PD control subjects, explored the association of regional thinning with clinical features and evaluated the impact of amyloid deposition. Methods Twenty-one participants with dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB), 16 with Parkinson disease (PD) - associated cognitive impairment (PD-MCI and PDD), and 24 cognitively normal participants with PD underwent MRI, PiB PET, and clinical evaluation. Cortical thickness across the brain and in regions of interest (ROIs) was compared across diagnostic groups and across subgroups stratified by amyloid status, and was related to clinical and cognitive measures. Results DLB and PD-impaired groups shared a similar distribution of cortical thinning that included regions characteristic of AD, as well as the fusiform, precentral, and paracentral gyri. Elevated PiB retention in DLB and PD-impaired but not in PD-normal participants was associated with more extensive and severe cortical thinning, in an overlapping topography that selectively affected the medial temporal lobe in DLB participants. In DLB, greater thinning in AD signature and fusiform regions was associated with greater cognitive impairment. Conclusions The pattern of cortical thinning is similar in DLB and PD-associated cognitive impairment, overlapping with and extending beyond AD signature regions to involve fusiform, precentral, and paracentral regions. Cortical thinning in AD signature and fusiform regions in these diseases reflects cognitive impairment and is markedly accentuated by amyloid co-pathology. Further work will be required to determine whether the distinct topography of cortical thinning in DLB and PD-associated cognitive impairment might have value as a diagnostic and/ or outcome biomarker in clinical trials.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rong Ye
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA
| | - Alexandra Touroutoglou
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA; Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA
| | - Michael Brickhouse
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA; Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA
| | - Samantha Katz
- Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA, USA
| | - John H Growdon
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA
| | - Keith A Johnson
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA; Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA; Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Bradford C Dickerson
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA; Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA
| | - Stephen N Gomperts
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Gu S, Wang F, Cao C, Wu E, Tang YY, Huang JH. An Integrative Way for Studying Neural Basis of Basic Emotions With fMRI. Front Neurosci 2019; 13:628. [PMID: 31275107 PMCID: PMC6593191 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00628] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/04/2018] [Accepted: 05/31/2019] [Indexed: 01/18/2023] Open
Abstract
How emotions are represented in the nervous system is a crucial unsolved problem in the affective neuroscience. Many studies are striving to find the localization of basic emotions in the brain but failed. Thus, many psychologists suspect the specific neural loci for basic emotions, but instead, some proposed that there are specific neural structures for the core affects, such as arousal and hedonic value. The reason for this widespread difference might be that basic emotions used previously can be further divided into more “basic” emotions. Here we review brain imaging data and neuropsychological data, and try to address this question with an integrative model. In this model, we argue that basic emotions are not contrary to the dimensional studies of emotions (core affects). We propose that basic emotion should locate on the axis in the dimensions of emotion, and only represent one typical core affect (arousal or valence). Therefore, we propose four basic emotions: joy-on positive axis of hedonic dimension, sadness-on negative axis of hedonic dimension, fear, and anger-on the top of vertical dimensions. This new model about basic emotions and construction model of emotions is promising to improve and reformulate neurobiological models of basic emotions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Simeng Gu
- Institute of Brain and Psychological Science, Sichuan Normal University, Chengdu, China.,Department of Psychology, Jiangsu University, Zhenjiang, China
| | - Fushun Wang
- Institute of Brain and Psychological Science, Sichuan Normal University, Chengdu, China.,Department of Pharmacology, Nanjing University of Chinese Medicine, Nanjing, China.,Department of Neurosurgery, Baylor Scott & White Health, Temple, TX, United States
| | - Caiyun Cao
- Department of Pharmacology, Nanjing University of Chinese Medicine, Nanjing, China
| | - Erxi Wu
- Department of Neurosurgery, Baylor Scott & White Health, Temple, TX, United States.,Department of Surgery, Texas A&M University College of Medicine, Temple, TX, United States.,Department of Pharmaceutical Sciences, Texas A&M University College of Pharmacy, College Station, TX, United States.,LIVESTRONG Cancer Institutes, Dell Medical School, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, United States
| | - Yi-Yuan Tang
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX, United States
| | - Jason H Huang
- Department of Neurosurgery, Baylor Scott & White Health, Temple, TX, United States.,Department of Surgery, Texas A&M University College of Medicine, Temple, TX, United States.,Department of Pharmaceutical Sciences, Texas A&M University College of Pharmacy, College Station, TX, United States.,LIVESTRONG Cancer Institutes, Dell Medical School, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, United States
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Touroutoglou A, Zhang J, Andreano JM, Dickerson BC, Barrett LF. Dissociable Effects of Aging on Salience Subnetwork Connectivity Mediate Age-Related Changes in Executive Function and Affect. Front Aging Neurosci 2018; 10:410. [PMID: 30618717 PMCID: PMC6304391 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00410] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/21/2018] [Accepted: 11/28/2018] [Indexed: 12/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Aging is associated with both changes in affective experience and attention. An intrinsic brain network subserving these functions, the salience network, has not shown clear evidence of a corresponding age-related change. We propose a solution to this discrepancy: that aging differentially affects the connectivity of two dissociated subsystems of the salience network identified in our prior research (Touroutoglou et al., 2012). We examined the age-related changes in intrinsic connectivity between a dorsal and a ventral salience subsystem in a sample of 111 participants ranging in age from 18 years to 81 years old. We predicted that connectivity within the ventral subsystem is relatively preserved with age, while connectivity in the dorsal subsystem declines. Our findings showed that the connectivity within the ventral subsystem was not only preserved but it actually increased with age, whereas the connectivity within the dorsal subsystem decreased with age. Furthermore, age-related increase in arousal experience was partially mediated by age-related increases in ventral salience subsystem, whereas age-related decline in executive function was fully mediated by age-related decreases in dorsal salience subsystem connectivity. These findings explain previously conflicting results on age-related changes in the salience network, and suggest a mechanism for relatively preserved affective function in the elderly.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alexandra Touroutoglou
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
| | - Jiahe Zhang
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, United States
| | - Joseph M. Andreano
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
- Psychiatric Neuroimaging Division, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
| | - Bradford C. Dickerson
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
- Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
| | - Lisa Feldman Barrett
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, United States
- Psychiatric Neuroimaging Division, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Zhang Y, Mao Z, Pan L, Ling Z, Liu X, Zhang J, Yu X. Dysregulation of Pain- and Emotion-Related Networks in Trigeminal Neuralgia. Front Hum Neurosci 2018; 12:107. [PMID: 29662445 PMCID: PMC5890150 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00107] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/30/2017] [Accepted: 03/06/2018] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Classical trigeminal neuralgia (TN) is a severe neuropathic facial pain disorder associated with increased risks of anxiety and depression. Converging evidence suggests that chronic pain pathophysiology involves dysfunctional pain-related and emotion-related networks. However, whether these systems are also among the culprit networks for TN remains unclear. Here, we aimed to assess TN-related anatomical and functional brain anomalies in pain-related and emotion-related networks. We investigated differences in gray matter (GM) volume and the related resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) between 29 classical TN patients and 34 matched healthy controls. Relationships between brain measurement alterations, clinical pain and emotional states were identified. A longitudinal observation was further conducted to determine whether alterations in the brain could renormalize following pain relief. Reduced GM volumes in the bilateral amygdala, periaqueductal gray (PAG) and right insula were found in TN patients compared with healthy control subjects. Whole-brain rsFC analyses with the four above-mentioned anatomical regions as seeds identified three significantly altered functional circuits, including amygdala-DLPFC, amygdala-mPFC and amygdala-thalamus/putamen circuitry. The amygdala-DLPFC and amygdala-mPFC circuits were associated with clinical pain duration and emotional state ratings, respectively. Further longitudinal analysis found that rsFC strength abnormalities in two fronto-limbic circuits (left amygdala/left DLPFC and right amygdala/right PFC) were resolved after pain relief. Together, structural and functional deficits in pain-related and emotion-related networks were associated with TN patients, as demonstrated by our multimodal results. Pain relief had protective effects on brain functional connectivity within fronto-limbic circuits. Our study provides novel insights into the pathophysiology of TN, which may ultimately facilitate advances in TN intervention.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yanyang Zhang
- Department of Neurosurgery, Chinese PLA General Hospital, Beijing, China
| | - Zhiqi Mao
- Department of Neurosurgery, Chinese PLA General Hospital, Beijing, China
| | - Longsheng Pan
- Department of Neurosurgery, Chinese PLA General Hospital, Beijing, China
| | - Zhipei Ling
- Department of Neurosurgery, Chinese PLA General Hospital, Beijing, China
| | - Xinyun Liu
- Department of Radiology, Chinese PLA General Hospital, Beijing, China
| | - Jun Zhang
- Department of Neurosurgery, Chinese PLA General Hospital, Beijing, China
| | - Xinguang Yu
- Department of Neurosurgery, Chinese PLA General Hospital, Beijing, China
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Boshyan J, Feldman Barrett L, Betz N, Adams RB, Kveraga K. Line-Drawn Scenes Provide Sufficient Information for Discrimination of Threat and Mere Negativity. Iperception 2018; 9:2041669518755806. [PMID: 29774139 PMCID: PMC5950935 DOI: 10.1177/2041669518755806] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/05/2017] [Accepted: 01/02/2018] [Indexed: 12/05/2022] Open
Abstract
Previous work using color photographic scenes has shown that human observers are keenly sensitive to different types of threatening and negative stimuli and reliably classify them by the presence, and spatial and temporal directions of threat. To test whether such distinctions can be extracted from impoverished visual information, we used 500 line drawings made by hand-tracing the original set of photographic scenes. Sixty participants rated the scenes on spatial and temporal dimensions of threat. Based on these ratings, trend analysis revealed five scene categories that were comparable to those identified for the matching color photographic scenes. Another 61 participants were randomly assigned to rate the valence or arousal evoked by the line drawings. The line drawings perceived to be the most negative were also perceived to be the most arousing, replicating the finding for color photographic scenes. We demonstrate here that humans are very sensitive to the spatial and temporal directions of threat even when they must extract this information from simple line drawings, and rate the line drawings very similarly to matched color photographs. The set of 500 hand-traced line-drawing scenes has been made freely available to the research community: http://www.kveragalab.org/threat.html.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jasmine Boshyan
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA; Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Lisa Feldman Barrett
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA; Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA; Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA
| | - Nicole Betz
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Reginald B Adams
- Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA
| | - Kestutis Kveraga
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA; Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Szajer J, Jacobson A, Green E, Murphy C. Reduced brain response to a sweet taste in Hispanic young adults. Brain Res 2017; 1674:101-110. [PMID: 28851601 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2017.08.024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/15/2017] [Revised: 08/17/2017] [Accepted: 08/22/2017] [Indexed: 12/26/2022]
Abstract
Hispanics have an increased risk for metabolic disorders, which evidence suggests may be due to interactions between lifespan biological, genetic, and lifestyle factors. Studies show the diet of many U.S. Hispanic groups have high sugar consumption, which has been shown to influence future preference for and consumption of high-sugar foods, and is associated with increased risk for insulin-related disorders and obesity. Taste is a primary determinant of food preference and selection. Differences in neural response to taste have been associated with obesity. Understanding brain response to sweet taste stimuli in healthy Hispanic adults is an important first step in characterizing the potential neural mechanisms for this behavior. We used fMRI to examine brain activation during the hedonic evaluation of sucrose as a function of ethnicity in Hispanic and non-Hispanic young adults. Taste stimuli were administered orally while subjects were scanned at 3T. Data were analyzed with AFNI via 3dROIstats and 3dMEMA, a mixed effects multi-level analysis of whole brain activation. The Hispanic group had significantly lower ROI activation in the left amygdala and significantly lower whole brain activation in regions critical for reward processing, and hedonic evaluation (e.g. frontal, orbitofrontal, and anterior cingulate cortices) than the non-Hispanic group. Differences in processing of sweet tastes have important clinical and public health implications, especially considering increased risk of metabolic syndrome and cognitive decline in Hispanic populations. Future research to better understanding relationships between health risk and brain function in Hispanic populations is warranted to better conceptualize and develop interventions for these populations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jacquelyn Szajer
- San Diego State University/UC San Diego Joint Doctoral Program, San Diego, CA, USA
| | | | - Erin Green
- San Diego State University/UC San Diego Joint Doctoral Program, San Diego, CA, USA
| | - Claire Murphy
- San Diego State University/UC San Diego Joint Doctoral Program, San Diego, CA, USA; San Diego State University, San Diego, CA, USA; University of California, San Diego, CA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
de Chastelaine M, Mattson JT, Wang TH, Donley BE, Rugg MD. Independent contributions of fMRI familiarity and novelty effects to recognition memory and their stability across the adult lifespan. Neuroimage 2017; 156:340-351. [PMID: 28528847 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.05.039] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/21/2017] [Revised: 05/16/2017] [Accepted: 05/17/2017] [Indexed: 12/12/2022] Open
Abstract
The impact of age on the neural correlates of familiarity-driven recognition memory has received relatively little attention. Here, the relationships between age, the neural correlates of familiarity, and memory performance were investigated using an associative recognition test in young, middle-aged and older participants. Test items comprised studied, rearranged (items studied on different trials) and new word pairs. fMRI 'familiarity effects' were operationalized as greater activity for studied test pairs incorrectly identified as 'rearranged' than for correctly rejected new pairs. The reverse contrast was employed to identify 'novelty' effects. Estimates of familiarity strength were slightly but significantly lower for the older relative to the younger group. With the exception of one region in dorsal medial prefrontal cortex, fMRI familiarity effects (which were identified in medial and lateral parietal cortex, dorsal medial and left lateral prefrontal cortex, and bilateral caudate among other regions) did not differ significantly with age. Age-invariant 'novelty effects' were identified in the anterior hippocampus and the perirhinal cortex. When entered into the same regression model, familiarity and novelty effects independently predicted familiarity strength across participants, suggesting that the two classes of memory effect reflect functionally distinct mnemonic processes. It is concluded that the neural correlates of familiarity-based memory judgments, and their relationship with familiarity strength, are largely stable across much of the healthy adult lifespan.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marianne de Chastelaine
- Center for Vital Longevity and School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, The University of Texas at Dallas, Dallas, TX 75235, USA.
| | | | - Tracy H Wang
- Center for Vital Longevity and School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, The University of Texas at Dallas, Dallas, TX 75235, USA
| | - Brian E Donley
- Center for Vital Longevity and School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, The University of Texas at Dallas, Dallas, TX 75235, USA
| | - Michael D Rugg
- Center for Vital Longevity and School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, The University of Texas at Dallas, Dallas, TX 75235, USA
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Xia C, Touroutoglou A, Quigley KS, Barrett LF, Dickerson BC. Salience Network Connectivity Modulates Skin Conductance Responses in Predicting Arousal Experience. J Cogn Neurosci 2017; 29:827-836. [PMID: 27991182 PMCID: PMC5690982 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_01087] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/17/2022]
Abstract
Individual differences in arousal experience have been linked to differences in resting-state salience network connectivity strength. In this study, we investigated how adding task-related skin conductance responses (SCR), a measure of sympathetic autonomic nervous system activity, can predict additional variance in arousal experience. Thirty-nine young adults rated their subjective experience of arousal to emotionally evocative images while SCRs were measured. They also underwent a separate resting-state fMRI scan. Greater SCR reactivity (an increased number of task-related SCRs) to emotional images and stronger intrinsic salience network connectivity independently predicted more intense experiences of arousal. Salience network connectivity further moderated the effect of SCR reactivity: In individuals with weak salience network connectivity, SCR reactivity more significantly predicted arousal experience, whereas in those with strong salience network connectivity, SCR reactivity played little role in predicting arousal experience. This interaction illustrates the degeneracy in neural mechanisms driving individual differences in arousal experience and highlights the intricate interplay between connectivity in central visceromotor neural circuitry and peripherally expressed autonomic responses in shaping arousal experience.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chenjie Xia
- Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, MA
| | | | - Karen S. Quigley
- Edith Nourse Rogers Memorial VA Hospital, Bedford, MA
- Northeastern University, Boston, MA
| | - Lisa Feldman Barrett
- Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, MA
- Northeastern University, Boston, MA
| | | |
Collapse
|
20
|
Kleckner IR, Zhang J, Touroutoglou A, Chanes L, Xia C, Simmons WK, Quigley KS, Dickerson BC, Barrett LF. Evidence for a Large-Scale Brain System Supporting Allostasis and Interoception in Humans. Nat Hum Behav 2017; 1:0069. [PMID: 28983518 PMCID: PMC5624222 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-017-0069] [Citation(s) in RCA: 313] [Impact Index Per Article: 44.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/27/2016] [Accepted: 02/13/2017] [Indexed: 12/23/2022]
Abstract
Large-scale intrinsic brain systems have been identified for exteroceptive senses (e.g., sight, hearing, touch). We introduce an analogous system for representing sensations from within the body, called interoception, and demonstrate its relation to regulating peripheral systems in the body, called allostasis. Employing the recently introduced Embodied Predictive Interoception Coding (EPIC) model, we used tract-tracing studies of macaque monkeys, followed by two intrinsic functional magnetic resonance imaging samples (N = 280 and N = 270) to evaluate the existence of an intrinsic allostatic/interoceptive system in the human brain. Another sample (N = 41) allowed us to evaluate the convergent validity of the hypothesized allostatic/interoceptive system by showing that individuals with stronger connectivity between system hubs performed better on an implicit index of interoceptive ability related to autonomic fluctuations. Implications include insights for the brain's functional architecture, dissolving the artificial boundary between mind and body, and unifying mental and physical illness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ian R. Kleckner
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA
| | - Jiahe Zhang
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA
| | - Alexandra Touroutoglou
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging
- Psychiatric Neuroimaging Division, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, MA
| | - Lorena Chanes
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging
- Psychiatric Neuroimaging Division, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, MA
| | - Chenjie Xia
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging
- Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, MA
| | - W. Kyle Simmons
- Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, OK
- School of Community Medicine, The University of Tulsa, Tulsa, OK
| | - Karen S. Quigley
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA
- Edith Nourse Rogers Memorial VA Hospital, Bedford, MA
| | - Bradford C. Dickerson
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging
- Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, MA
| | - Lisa Feldman Barrett
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging
- Psychiatric Neuroimaging Division, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, MA
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Satpute AB, Hanington L, Barrett LF. Novel response patterns during repeated presentation of affective and neutral stimuli. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2016; 11:1919-1932. [PMID: 27928070 PMCID: PMC5141956 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsw104] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/03/2015] [Revised: 02/18/2016] [Accepted: 08/03/2016] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Repeated stimulus presentations are commonly used in social and affective neuroimaging tasks, but much remains to be known about how the brain processes such repetitions. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, we found three groups of brain regions with distinct response patterns during repeated presentations of natural scene images. One group consisted of several limbic, paralimbic, frontoparietal and medial prefrontal areas and showed a habituation-like response across pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral image categories. A second group of occipital and adjacent posterior cortical regions showed a pattern of diminishing responses with repeated presentations of affective images but not for neutral images, and also plateaued to activation levels above baseline for all image categories. A third group involved bilateral frontopolar areas and the precuneus and exhibited a novel, non-monotonic response pattern. Activity was low on the first presentation, peaked upon the second presentation (first repetition) and subsequently diminished. These findings indicate that the transition from novel to increasingly familiar, and also arousing to less arousing, involves a broad array of neural mechanisms alluding to both passive learning and active inference strategies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ajay B Satpute
- Department of Psychology, Pomona College, Claremont, CA 91711, USA
| | - Lydia Hanington
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA 02115, USA
| | - Lisa F Barrett
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA 02115, USA
- Department of Psychiatry Harvard Medical School and Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA 02115, USA
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
|
23
|
Abstract
Although aging is associated with clear declines in physical and cognitive processes, emotional functioning fares relatively well. Consistent with this behavioral profile, two core emotional brain regions, the amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex, show little structural and functional decline in aging, compared with other regions. However, emotional processes depend on interacting systems of neurotransmitters and brain regions that go beyond these structures. This review examines how age-related brain changes influence processes such as attending to and remembering emotional stimuli, regulating emotion, and recognizing emotional expressions, as well as empathy, risk taking, impulsivity, behavior change, and attentional focus.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mara Mather
- Davis School of Gerontology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California 90089;
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Kiverstein J, Miller M. The embodied brain: towards a radical embodied cognitive neuroscience. Front Hum Neurosci 2015; 9:237. [PMID: 25999836 PMCID: PMC4422034 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00237] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/27/2014] [Accepted: 04/13/2015] [Indexed: 12/28/2022] Open
Abstract
In this programmatic paper we explain why a radical embodied cognitive neuroscience is needed. We argue for such a claim based on problems that have arisen in cognitive neuroscience for the project of localizing function to specific brain structures. The problems come from research concerned with functional and structural connectivity that strongly suggests that the function a brain region serves is dynamic, and changes over time. We argue that in order to determine the function of a specific brain area, neuroscientists need to zoom out and look at the larger organism-environment system. We therefore argue that instead of looking to cognitive psychology for an analysis of psychological functions, cognitive neuroscience should look to an ecological dynamical psychology. A second aim of our paper is to develop an account of embodied cognition based on the inseparability of cognitive and emotional processing in the brain. We argue that emotions are best understood in terms of action readiness (Frijda, 1986, 2007) in the context of the organism's ongoing skillful engagement with the environment (Rietveld, 2008; Bruineberg and Rietveld, 2014; Kiverstein and Rietveld, 2015, forthcoming). States of action readiness involve the whole living body of the organism, and are elicited by possibilities for action in the environment that matter to the organism. Since emotion and cognition are inseparable processes in the brain it follows that what is true of emotion is also true of cognition. Cognitive processes are likewise processes taking place in the whole living body of an organism as it engages with relevant possibilities for action.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julian Kiverstein
- Institute of Logic, Language and Computation, University of Amsterdam Amsterdam, Netherlands
| | - Mark Miller
- School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences, University of Edinburgh Edinburgh, UK
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Harshaw C. Interoceptive dysfunction: toward an integrated framework for understanding somatic and affective disturbance in depression. Psychol Bull 2015; 141:311-363. [PMID: 25365763 PMCID: PMC4346391 DOI: 10.1037/a0038101] [Citation(s) in RCA: 181] [Impact Index Per Article: 20.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/05/2023]
Abstract
Depression is characterized by disturbed sleep and eating, a variety of other nonspecific somatic symptoms, and significant somatic comorbidities. Why there is such close association between cognitive and somatic dysfunction in depression is nonetheless poorly understood. An explosion of research in the area of interoception-the perception and interpretation of bodily signals-over the last decade nonetheless holds promise for illuminating what have until now been obscure links between the social, cognitive-affective, and somatic features of depression. This article reviews rapidly accumulating evidence that both somatic signaling and interoception are frequently altered in depression. This includes comparative studies showing vagus-mediated effects on depression-like behaviors in rodent models as well as studies in humans indicating both dysfunction in the neural substrates for interoception (e.g., vagus, insula, anterior cingulate cortex) and reduced sensitivity to bodily stimuli in depression. An integrative framework for organizing and interpreting this evidence is put forward which incorporates (a) multiple potential pathways to interoceptive dysfunction; (b) interaction with individual, gender, and cultural differences in interoception; and (c) a developmental psychobiological systems perspective, emphasizing likely differential susceptibility to somatic and interoceptive dysfunction across the lifespan. Combined with current theory and evidence, it is suggested that core symptoms of depression (e.g., anhedonia, social deficits) may be products of disturbed interoceptive-exteroceptive integration. More research is nonetheless needed to fully elucidate the relationship between mind, body, and social context in depression.
Collapse
|
26
|
Dolcos S, Katsumi Y, Dixon RA. The role of arousal in the spontaneous regulation of emotions in healthy aging: a fMRI investigation. Front Psychol 2014; 5:681. [PMID: 25120498 PMCID: PMC4112914 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00681] [Citation(s) in RCA: 53] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/05/2014] [Accepted: 06/12/2014] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
Abstract
Despite ample support for enhanced affective well-being and emotional stability in healthy aging, the role of potentially important dimensions, such as the emotional arousal, has not been systematically investigated in neuroimaging studies. In addition, the few behavioral studies that examined effects of arousal have produced inconsistent findings. The present study manipulated the arousal of pictorial stimuli to test the hypothesis that preserved emotional functioning in aging is modulated by the level of arousal, and to identify the associated neural correlates. Young and older healthy participants were presented with negative and neutral pictures, which they rated for emotional content, while fMRI data were recorded. There were three main novel findings regarding the neural mechanisms underlying the processing of negative pictures with different levels of arousal in young and older adults. First, the common engagement of the right amygdala in young and older adults was driven by high arousing negative stimuli. Second, complementing an age-related reduction in the subjective ratings for low arousing negative pictures, there were opposing patterns of activity in the rostral/ventral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and the amygdala, which showed increased vs. decreased responses, respectively, to low arousing negative pictures. Third, increased spontaneous activity in the ventral ACC/ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) in older adults was linked to reduced ratings for low arousing negative pictures. Overall, these findings advance our understanding of the neural correlates underlying processing of negative emotions with different levels of arousal in the context of enhanced emotional functioning in healthy aging. Notably, the results support the idea that older adults have emotion regulation networks chronically activated, in the absence of explicit induction of the goal to regulate emotions, and that this effect is specific to low arousing negative emotions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sanda Dolcos
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign Champaign, IL, USA
| | - Yuta Katsumi
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign Champaign, IL, USA
| | - Roger A Dixon
- Department of Psychology, University of Alberta Edmonton, AB, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Kring AM, Siegel EH, Barrett LF. Unseen Affective Faces Influence Person Perception Judgments in Schizophrenia. Clin Psychol Sci 2014; 2:443-454. [PMID: 25664225 DOI: 10.1177/2167702614536161] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
To demonstrate the influence of unconscious affective processing on consciously processed information among people with and without schizophrenia, we used a continuous flash suppression (CFS) paradigm to examine whether early and rapid processing of affective information influences first impressions of structurally neutral faces. People with and without schizophrenia rated visible neutral faces as more or less trustworthy, warm, and competent when paired with unseen smiling or scowling faces compared to when paired with unseen neutral faces. Yet, people with schizophrenia also exhibited a deficit in explicit affect perception. These findings indicate that early processing of affective information is intact in schizophrenia but the integration of this information with semantic contexts is problematic. Furthermore, people with schizophrenia who were more influenced by smiling faces presented outside awareness reported experiencing more anticipatory pleasure, suggesting that the ability to rapidly process affective information is important for anticipation of future pleasurable events.
Collapse
|
28
|
Touroutoglou A, Bickart KC, Barrett LF, Dickerson BC. Amygdala task-evoked activity and task-free connectivity independently contribute to feelings of arousal. Hum Brain Mapp 2014; 35:5316-27. [PMID: 24862171 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.22552] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/11/2013] [Revised: 04/17/2014] [Accepted: 05/06/2014] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
Individual differences in the intensity of feelings of arousal while viewing emotional pictures have been associated with the magnitude of task-evoked blood-oxygen dependent (BOLD) response in the amygdala. Recently, we reported that individual differences in feelings of arousal are associated with task-free (resting state) connectivity within the salience network. There has not yet been an investigation of whether these two types of functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) measures are redundant or independent in their relationships to behavior. Here we tested the hypothesis that a combination of task-evoked amygdala activation and task-free amygdala connectivity within the salience network relate to individual differences in feelings of arousal while viewing of negatively potent images. In 25 young adults, results revealed that greater task-evoked amygdala activation and stronger task-free amygdala connectivity within the salience network each contributed independently to feelings of arousal, predicting a total of 45% of its variance. Individuals who had both increased task-evoked amygdala activation and stronger task-free amygdala connectivity within the salience network had the most heightened levels of arousal. Task-evoked amygdala activation and task-free amygdala connectivity within the salience network were not related to each other, suggesting that resting-state and task-evoked dynamic brain imaging measures may provide independent and complementary information about affective experience, and likely other kinds of behaviors as well.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alexandra Touroutoglou
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Massachusetts; Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Massachusetts; Psychiatric Neuroimaging Division, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Massachusetts
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
29
|
Kveraga K, Boshyan J, Adams RB, Mote J, Betz N, Ward N, Hadjikhani N, Bar M, Barrett LF. If it bleeds, it leads: separating threat from mere negativity. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2014; 10:28-35. [PMID: 24493851 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsu007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Most theories of emotion hold that negative stimuli are threatening and aversive. Yet in everyday experiences some negative sights (e.g. car wrecks) attract curiosity, whereas others repel (e.g. a weapon pointed in our face). To examine the diversity in negative stimuli, we employed four classes of visual images (Direct Threat, Indirect Threat, Merely Negative and Neutral) in a set of behavioral and functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Participants reliably discriminated between the images, evaluating Direct Threat stimuli most quickly, and Merely Negative images most slowly. Threat images evoked greater and earlier blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) activations in the amygdala and periaqueductal gray, structures implicated in representing and responding to the motivational salience of stimuli. Conversely, the Merely Negative images evoked larger BOLD signal in the parahippocampal, retrosplenial, and medial prefrontal cortices, regions which have been implicated in contextual association processing. Ventrolateral as well as medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortices were activated by both threatening and Merely Negative images. In conclusion, negative visual stimuli can repel or attract scrutiny depending on their current threat potential, which is assessed by dynamic shifts in large-scale brain network activity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kestutis Kveraga
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA, Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, 415 South Street, Waltham MA 02453-2728, USA, Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, 16802-3103 USA, Department of Psychology, 3210 Tolman Hall, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, 94720 CA, USA, Department of Psychology, 125 Nightingale Hall, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, 02129 USA, and Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Building 901, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, 5290002 Israel Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA, Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, 415 South Street, Waltham MA 02453-2728, USA, Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, 16802-3103 USA, Department of Psychology, 3210 Tolman Hall, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, 94720 CA, USA, Department of Psychology, 125 Nightingale Hall, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, 02129 USA, and Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Building 901, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, 5290002 Israel
| | - Jasmine Boshyan
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA, Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, 415 South Street, Waltham MA 02453-2728, USA, Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, 16802-3103 USA, Department of Psychology, 3210 Tolman Hall, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, 94720 CA, USA, Department of Psychology, 125 Nightingale Hall, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, 02129 USA, and Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Building 901, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, 5290002 Israel Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA, Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, 415 South Street, Waltham MA 02453-2728, USA, Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, 16802-3103 USA, Department of Psychology, 3210 Tolman Hall, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, 94720 CA, USA, Department of Psychology, 125 Nightingale Hall, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, 02129 USA, and Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Building 901, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, 5290002 Israel
| | - Reginald B Adams
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA, Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, 415 South Street, Waltham MA 02453-2728, USA, Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, 16802-3103 USA, Department of Psychology, 3210 Tolman Hall, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, 94720 CA, USA, Department of Psychology, 125 Nightingale Hall, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, 02129 USA, and Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Building 901, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, 5290002 Israel
| | - Jasmine Mote
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA, Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, 415 South Street, Waltham MA 02453-2728, USA, Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, 16802-3103 USA, Department of Psychology, 3210 Tolman Hall, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, 94720 CA, USA, Department of Psychology, 125 Nightingale Hall, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, 02129 USA, and Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Building 901, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, 5290002 Israel
| | - Nicole Betz
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA, Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, 415 South Street, Waltham MA 02453-2728, USA, Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, 16802-3103 USA, Department of Psychology, 3210 Tolman Hall, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, 94720 CA, USA, Department of Psychology, 125 Nightingale Hall, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, 02129 USA, and Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Building 901, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, 5290002 Israel
| | - Noreen Ward
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA, Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, 415 South Street, Waltham MA 02453-2728, USA, Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, 16802-3103 USA, Department of Psychology, 3210 Tolman Hall, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, 94720 CA, USA, Department of Psychology, 125 Nightingale Hall, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, 02129 USA, and Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Building 901, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, 5290002 Israel
| | - Nouchine Hadjikhani
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA, Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, 415 South Street, Waltham MA 02453-2728, USA, Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, 16802-3103 USA, Department of Psychology, 3210 Tolman Hall, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, 94720 CA, USA, Department of Psychology, 125 Nightingale Hall, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, 02129 USA, and Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Building 901, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, 5290002 Israel Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA, Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, 415 South Street, Waltham MA 02453-2728, USA, Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, 16802-3103 USA, Department of Psychology, 3210 Tolman Hall, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, 94720 CA, USA, Department of Psychology, 125 Nightingale Hall, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, 02129 USA, and Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Building 901, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, 5290002 Israel
| | - Moshe Bar
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA, Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, 415 South Street, Waltham MA 02453-2728, USA, Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, 16802-3103 USA, Department of Psychology, 3210 Tolman Hall, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, 94720 CA, USA, Department of Psychology, 125 Nightingale Hall, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, 02129 USA, and Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Building 901, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, 5290002 Israel Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA, Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, 415 South Street, Waltham MA 02453-2728, USA, Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, 16802-3103 USA, Department of Psychology, 3210 Tolman Hall, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, 94720 CA, USA, Department of Psychology, 125 Nightingale Hall, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, 02129 USA, and Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Building 901, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, 5290002 Israel Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA, Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, 415 South Street, Waltham MA 02453-2728, USA, Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, 16802-3103 USA
| | - Lisa F Barrett
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA, Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, 415 South Street, Waltham MA 02453-2728, USA, Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, 16802-3103 USA, Department of Psychology, 3210 Tolman Hall, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, 94720 CA, USA, Department of Psychology, 125 Nightingale Hall, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, 02129 USA, and Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Building 901, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, 5290002 Israel Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA, Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, 415 South Street, Waltham MA 02453-2728, USA, Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, 16802-3103 USA, Department of Psychology, 3210 Tolman Hall, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, 94720 CA, USA, Department of Psychology, 125 Nightingale Hall, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, 02129 USA, and Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Building 901, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, 5290002 Israel Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA, Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, 02115 USA, Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, 415 South Street, Waltham MA 02453-2728, USA, Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, 16802-3103 USA
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Hashmi JA, Baliki MN, Huang L, Baria AT, Torbey S, Hermann KM, Schnitzer TJ, Apkarian AV. Shape shifting pain: chronification of back pain shifts brain representation from nociceptive to emotional circuits. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2013; 136:2751-68. [PMID: 23983029 DOI: 10.1093/brain/awt211] [Citation(s) in RCA: 513] [Impact Index Per Article: 46.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/16/2023]
Abstract
Chronic pain conditions are associated with abnormalities in brain structure and function. Moreover, some studies indicate that brain activity related to the subjective perception of chronic pain may be distinct from activity for acute pain. However, the latter are based on observations from cross-sectional studies. How brain activity reorganizes with transition from acute to chronic pain has remained unexplored. Here we study this transition by examining brain activity for rating fluctuations of back pain magnitude. First we compared back pain-related brain activity between subjects who have had the condition for ∼2 months with no prior history of back pain for 1 year (early, acute/subacute back pain group, n = 94), to subjects who have lived with back pain for >10 years (chronic back pain group, n = 59). In a subset of subacute back pain patients, we followed brain activity for back pain longitudinally over a 1-year period, and compared brain activity between those who recover (recovered acute/sub-acute back pain group, n = 19) and those in which the back pain persists (persistent acute/sub-acute back pain group, n = 20; based on a 20% decrease in intensity of back pain in 1 year). We report results in relation to meta-analytic probabilistic maps related to the terms pain, emotion, and reward (each map is based on >200 brain imaging studies, derived from neurosynth.org). We observed that brain activity for back pain in the early, acute/subacute back pain group is limited to regions involved in acute pain, whereas in the chronic back pain group, activity is confined to emotion-related circuitry. Reward circuitry was equally represented in both groups. In the recovered acute/subacute back pain group, brain activity diminished in time, whereas in the persistent acute/subacute back pain group, activity diminished in acute pain regions, increased in emotion-related circuitry, and remained unchanged in reward circuitry. The results demonstrate that brain representation for a constant percept, back pain, can undergo large-scale shifts in brain activity with the transition to chronic pain. These observations challenge long-standing theoretical concepts regarding brain and mind relationships, as well as provide important novel insights regarding definitions and mechanisms of chronic pain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Javeria A Hashmi
- Department of Physiology, Feinberg School of Medicine, Northwestern University, Chicago, Illinois 60611, USA
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
31
|
Andreano JM, Dickerson BC, Barrett LF. Sex differences in the persistence of the amygdala response to negative material. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2013; 9:1388-94. [PMID: 24036962 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nst127] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/15/2023] Open
Abstract
Previous studies have indicated that men and women have different amygdala responses to novel (vs familiar) and valenced (positive vs negative) material. It is not known, however, whether these affective sex differences are related. In this study, we tested whether women have more persistent amygdala responses to familiar, negative material than men do. During fMRI, male and female participants viewed evocative images that varied in novelty and valence. Women and men showed equivalent responses to novel negative material, but women showed a sustained amygdala response to familiar negative material relative to men, indicating that women's amygdala responses were more persistent over multiple repetitions of negative material. Individuals with more persistent amygdala responses also reported greater levels of negative effect. These findings have implications for sex differences in the incidence of affective disorders.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joseph M Andreano
- Department of Psychiatry, Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital/Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02129 and Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA 02115, USA
| | - Bradford C Dickerson
- Department of Psychiatry, Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital/Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02129 and Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA 02115, USA
| | - Lisa Feldman Barrett
- Department of Psychiatry, Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital/Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02129 and Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA 02115, USA Department of Psychiatry, Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital/Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02129 and Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA 02115, USA
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Moriguchi Y, Touroutoglou A, Dickerson BC, Barrett LF. Sex differences in the neural correlates of affective experience. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2013; 9:591-600. [PMID: 23596188 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nst030] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Abstract
People believe that women are more emotionally intense than men, but the scientific evidence is equivocal. In this study, we tested the novel hypothesis that men and women differ in the neural correlates of affective experience, rather than in the intensity of neural activity, with women being more internally (interoceptively) focused and men being more externally (visually) focused. Adult men (n = 17) and women (n = 17) completed a functional magnetic resonance imaging study while viewing affectively potent images and rating their moment-to-moment feelings of subjective arousal. We found that men and women do not differ overall in their intensity of moment-to-moment affective experiences when viewing evocative images, but instead, as predicted, women showed a greater association between the momentary arousal ratings and neural responses in the anterior insula cortex, which represents bodily sensations, whereas men showed stronger correlations between their momentary arousal ratings and neural responses in the visual cortex. Men also showed enhanced functional connectivity between the dorsal anterior insula cortex and the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, which constitutes the circuitry involved with regulating shifts of attention to the world. These results demonstrate that the same affective experience is realized differently in different people, such that women's feelings are relatively more self-focused, whereas men's feelings are relatively more world-focused.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yoshiya Moriguchi
- Department of Psychophysiology at National Institute of Mental Health, and Integrative Brain Imaging Center, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, 4-1-1 Ogawahigashi, Kodaira, Tokyo 187-0031, Japan.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
33
|
Sakaki M, Nga L, Mather M. Amygdala functional connectivity with medial prefrontal cortex at rest predicts the positivity effect in older adults' memory. J Cogn Neurosci 2013; 25:1206-24. [PMID: 23530897 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_00392] [Citation(s) in RCA: 52] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/28/2022]
Abstract
As people get older, they tend to remember more positive than negative information. This age-by-valence interaction has been called "positivity effect." The current study addressed the hypotheses that baseline functional connectivity at rest is predictive of older adults' brain activity when learning emotional information and their positivity effect in memory. Using fMRI, we examined the relationship among resting-state functional connectivity, subsequent brain activity when learning emotional faces, and individual differences in the positivity effect (the relative tendency to remember faces expressing positive vs. negative emotions). Consistent with our hypothesis, older adults with a stronger positivity effect had increased functional coupling between amygdala and medial PFC (MPFC) during rest. In contrast, younger adults did not show the association between resting connectivity and memory positivity. A similar age-by-memory positivity interaction was also found when learning emotional faces. That is, memory positivity in older adults was associated with (a) enhanced MPFC activity when learning emotional faces and (b) increased negative functional coupling between amygdala and MPFC when learning negative faces. In contrast, memory positivity in younger adults was related to neither enhanced MPFC activity to emotional faces, nor MPFC-amygdala connectivity to negative faces. Furthermore, stronger MPFC-amygdala connectivity during rest was predictive of subsequent greater MPFC activity when learning emotional faces. Thus, emotion-memory interaction in older adults depends not only on the task-related brain activity but also on the baseline functional connectivity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michiko Sakaki
- University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90089, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
34
|
Barrett LF, Satpute AB. Large-scale brain networks in affective and social neuroscience: towards an integrative functional architecture of the brain. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2013; 23:361-72. [PMID: 23352202 DOI: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.12.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 336] [Impact Index Per Article: 30.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/03/2012] [Revised: 12/26/2012] [Accepted: 12/28/2012] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
Understanding how a human brain creates a human mind ultimately depends on mapping psychological categories and concepts to physical measurements of neural response. Although it has long been assumed that emotional, social, and cognitive phenomena are realized in the operations of separate brain regions or brain networks, we demonstrate that it is possible to understand the body of neuroimaging evidence using a framework that relies on domain general, distributed structure-function mappings. We review current research in affective and social neuroscience and argue that the emerging science of large-scale intrinsic brain networks provides a coherent framework for a domain-general functional architecture of the human brain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lisa Feldman Barrett
- Northeastern University, Massachusetts General Hospital/Harvard Medical School, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
35
|
Foster TC, Defazio RA, Bizon JL. Characterizing cognitive aging of spatial and contextual memory in animal models. Front Aging Neurosci 2012; 4:12. [PMID: 22988436 PMCID: PMC3439636 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2012.00012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 73] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/06/2012] [Accepted: 05/15/2012] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Episodic memory, especially memory for contextual or spatial information, is particularly vulnerable to age-related decline in humans and animal models of aging. The continuing improvement of virtual environment technology for testing humans signifies that widely used procedures employed in the animal literature for examining spatial memory could be developed for examining age-related cognitive decline in humans. The current review examines cross species considerations for implementing these tasks and translating findings across different levels of analysis. The specificity of brain systems as well as gaps in linking human and animal laboratory models is discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas C Foster
- Department of Neuroscience, Evelyn F. and William L. McKnight Brain Institute, University of Florida Gainesville, FL, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
36
|
Abstract
In our response, we clarify important theoretical differences between basic emotion and psychological construction approaches. We evaluate the empirical status of the basic emotion approach, addressing whether it requires brain localization, whether localization can be observed with better analytic tools, and whether evidence for basic emotions exists in other types of measures. We then revisit the issue of whether the key hypotheses of psychological construction are supported by our meta-analytic findings. We close by elaborating on commentator suggestions for future research.
Collapse
|
37
|
Ebner NC, Johnson MK, Fischer H. Neural mechanisms of reading facial emotions in young and older adults. Front Psychol 2012; 3:223. [PMID: 22798953 PMCID: PMC3394436 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00223] [Citation(s) in RCA: 65] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/02/2012] [Accepted: 06/15/2012] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The ability to read and appropriately respond to emotions in others is central for successful social interaction. Young and older adults are better at identifying positive than negative facial expressions and also expressions of young than older faces. Little, however, is known about the neural processes associated with reading different emotions, particularly in faces of different ages, in samples of young and older adults. During fMRI, young and older participants identified expressions in happy, neutral, and angry young and older faces. The results suggest a functional dissociation of ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC) in reading facial emotions that is largely comparable in young and older adults: Both age groups showed greater vmPFC activity to happy compared to angry or neutral faces, which was positively correlated with expression identification for happy compared to angry faces. In contrast, both age groups showed greater activity in dmPFC to neutral or angry than happy faces which was negatively correlated with expression identification for neutral compared to happy faces. A similar region of dmPFC showed greater activity for older than young faces, but no brain-behavior correlations. Greater vmPFC activity in the present study may reflect greater affective processing involved in reading happy compared to neutral or angry faces. Greater dmPFC activity may reflect more cognitive control involved in decoding and/or regulating negative emotions associated with neutral or angry than happy, and older than young, faces.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Natalie C. Ebner
- Department of Psychology, University of FloridaGainesville, FL, USA
| | | | - Håkan Fischer
- Department of Psychology, Stockholm UniversityStockholm, Sweden
- Aging Research Center, Karolinska InstituteStockholm, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
38
|
Neural network development in late adolescents during observation of risk-taking action. PLoS One 2012; 7:e39527. [PMID: 22768085 PMCID: PMC3387168 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0039527] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/15/2011] [Accepted: 05/23/2012] [Indexed: 12/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Emotional maturity and social awareness are important for adolescents, particularly college students beginning to face the challenges and risks of the adult world. However, there has been relatively little research into personality maturation and psychological development during late adolescence and the neural changes underlying this development. We investigated the correlation between psychological properties (neuroticism, extraversion, anxiety, and depression) and age among late adolescents (n = 25, from 18 years and 1 month to 22 years and 8 months). The results revealed that late adolescents became less neurotic, less anxious, less depressive and more extraverted as they aged. Participants then observed video clips depicting hand movements with and without a risk of harm (risk-taking or safe actions) during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). The results revealed that risk-taking actions elicited significantly stronger activation in the bilateral inferior parietal lobule, temporal visual regions (superior/middle temporal areas), and parieto-occipital visual areas (cuneus, middle occipital gyri, precuneus). We found positive correlations of age and extraversion with neural activation in the insula, middle temporal gyrus, lingual gyrus, and precuneus. We also found a negative correlation of age and anxiety with activation in the angular gyrus, precentral gyrus, and red nucleus/substantia nigra. Moreover, we found that insula activation mediated the relationship between age and extraversion. Overall, our results indicate that late adolescents become less anxious and more extraverted with age, a process involving functional neural changes in brain networks related to social cognition and emotional processing. The possible neural mechanisms of psychological and social maturation during late adolescence are discussed.
Collapse
|
39
|
Lindquist KA, Wager TD, Kober H, Bliss-Moreau E, Barrett LF. The brain basis of emotion: a meta-analytic review. Behav Brain Sci 2012; 35:121-43. [PMID: 22617651 PMCID: PMC4329228 DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x11000446] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1138] [Impact Index Per Article: 94.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
Researchers have wondered how the brain creates emotions since the early days of psychological science. With a surge of studies in affective neuroscience in recent decades, scientists are poised to answer this question. In this target article, we present a meta-analytic summary of the neuroimaging literature on human emotion. We compare the locationist approach (i.e., the hypothesis that discrete emotion categories consistently and specifically correspond to distinct brain regions) with the psychological constructionist approach (i.e., the hypothesis that discrete emotion categories are constructed of more general brain networks not specific to those categories) to better understand the brain basis of emotion. We review both locationist and psychological constructionist hypotheses of brain-emotion correspondence and report meta-analytic findings bearing on these hypotheses. Overall, we found little evidence that discrete emotion categories can be consistently and specifically localized to distinct brain regions. Instead, we found evidence that is consistent with a psychological constructionist approach to the mind: A set of interacting brain regions commonly involved in basic psychological operations of both an emotional and non-emotional nature are active during emotion experience and perception across a range of discrete emotion categories.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kristen A. Lindquist
- Department of Neurology, Harvard Medical School/Massachusetts General Hospital/ /Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Charlestown, MA 02129 Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138 http://www.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/~lindqukr/
| | - Tor D. Wager
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado, Boulder, CO 80309 http://www.psych.colorado.edu/~tor/
| | - Hedy Kober
- Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT 06519 http://medicine.yale.edu/psychiatry/people/hedy_kober.profile
| | - Eliza Bliss-Moreau
- California National Primate Research Center, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of California, Davis, CA 95616
| | - Lisa Feldman Barrett
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA 02115 Departments of Radiology and Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School/Massachusetts General Hospital/Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Charlestown, MA 02129 http://www.affective-science.org/
| |
Collapse
|
40
|
Effects of aging on neural connectivity underlying selective memory for emotional scenes. Neurobiol Aging 2012; 34:451-67. [PMID: 22542836 DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.03.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/23/2011] [Revised: 03/17/2012] [Accepted: 03/20/2012] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
Older adults show age-related reductions in memory for neutral items within complex visual scenes, but just like young adults, older adults exhibit a memory advantage for emotional items within scenes compared with the background scene information. The present study examined young and older adults' encoding-stage effective connectivity for selective memory of emotional items versus memory for both the emotional item and its background. In a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study, participants viewed scenes containing either positive or negative items within neutral backgrounds. Outside the scanner, participants completed a memory test for items and backgrounds. Irrespective of scene content being emotionally positive or negative, older adults had stronger positive connections among frontal regions and from frontal regions to medial temporal lobe structures than did young adults, especially when items and backgrounds were subsequently remembered. These results suggest there are differences between young and older adults' connectivity accompanying the encoding of emotional scenes. Older adults may require more frontal connectivity to encode all elements of a scene rather than just encoding the emotional item.
Collapse
|
41
|
Dissociable large-scale networks anchored in the right anterior insula subserve affective experience and attention. Neuroimage 2012; 60:1947-58. [PMID: 22361166 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 212] [Impact Index Per Article: 17.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/12/2011] [Revised: 01/25/2012] [Accepted: 02/04/2012] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Meta-analytic summaries of neuroimaging studies point to at least two major functional-anatomic subdivisions within the anterior insula that contribute to the detection and processing of salient information: a dorsal region that is routinely active during attention tasks and a ventral region that is routinely active during affective experience. In two independent samples of cognitively normal human adults, we used intrinsic functional connectivity magnetic resonance imaging to demonstrate that the right dorsal and right ventral anterior insula are nodes in separable large-scale functional networks. Furthermore, stronger intrinsic connectivity within the right dorsal anterior insula network was associated with better performance on a task involving attention and processing speed whereas stronger connectivity within the right ventral anterior insula network was associated with more intense affective experience. These results support the hypothesis that the identification and manipulation of salient information is subserved by at least two brain networks anchored in the right anterior insula that exhibit distinct large-scale topography and dissociable behavioral correlates.
Collapse
|
42
|
Wilson-Mendenhall CD, Barrett LF, Simmons WK, Barsalou LW. Grounding emotion in situated conceptualization. Neuropsychologia 2011; 49:1105-1127. [PMID: 21192959 PMCID: PMC3078178 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.032] [Citation(s) in RCA: 172] [Impact Index Per Article: 13.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/10/2010] [Revised: 12/03/2010] [Accepted: 12/20/2010] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
According to the Conceptual Act Theory of Emotion, the situated conceptualization used to construe a situation determines the emotion experienced. A neuroimaging experiment tested two core hypotheses of this theory: (1) different situated conceptualizations produce different forms of the same emotion in different situations, (2) the composition of a situated conceptualization emerges from shared multimodal circuitry distributed across the brain that produces emotional states generally. To test these hypotheses, the situation in which participants experienced an emotion was manipulated. On each trial, participants immersed themselves in a physical danger or social evaluation situation and then experienced fear or anger. According to Hypothesis 1, the brain activations for the same emotion should differ as a function of the preceding situation (after removing activations that arose while constructing the situation). According to Hypothesis 2, the critical activations should reflect conceptual processing relevant to the emotion in the current situation, drawn from shared multimodal circuitry underlying emotion. The results supported these predictions and demonstrated the compositional process that produces situated conceptualizations dynamically.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - W Kyle Simmons
- The Laureate Institute for Brain Research, United States
| | | |
Collapse
|
43
|
Weierich MR, Kensinger EA, Munnell AH, Sass SA, Dickerson BC, Wright CI, Barrett LF. Older and wiser? An affective science perspective on age-related challenges in financial decision making. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2011; 6:195-206. [PMID: 20587596 PMCID: PMC3073391 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsq056] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/09/2009] [Accepted: 05/24/2010] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Financial planning decisionss are fundamentally affective in nature; they are decisions related to money, longevity and quality of life. Over the next several decades people will be increasingly responsible for managing their own assets and investments, and they will be subject to the affective influences on active, personal decision-making. Many of these crucial decisions are made and revised across the lifespan, including when to buy or sell a home, how to save for childrens' education, how to manage healthcare costs, when to retire, how much to save for retirement and how to allocate retirement funds. As average life expectancy increases, many retirees will be faced with inadequate savings to live comfortably until the end of their lives. In the current article, we examine the problems of and potential solutions to inadequate financial planning through the lens of affective science, with an emphasis on how brain-based changes in affective processing with age might contribute to the challenge of financial planning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mariann R Weierich
- North Building Room 627B, Hunter College CUNY, 695 Park Avenue, New York, NY 10065, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
44
|
Suvak MK, Barrett LF. Considering PTSD from the perspective of brain processes: a psychological construction approach. J Trauma Stress 2011; 24:3-24. [PMID: 21298725 PMCID: PMC3141586 DOI: 10.1002/jts.20618] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
Abstract
Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a complex psychiatric disorder that involves symptoms from various domains that appear to be produced by the combination of several mechanisms. The authors contend that existing neural accounts fail to provide a viable model that explains the emergence and maintenance of PTSD and the associated heterogeneity in the expression of this disorder (cf. Garfinkel & Liberzon, 2009). They introduce a psychological construction approach as a novel framework to probe the brain basis of PTSD, where distributed networks within the human brain are thought to correspond to the basic psychological ingredients of the mind. The authors posit that it is the combination of these ingredients that produces the heterogeneous symptom clusters in PTSD. Their goal is show that a constructionist approach has significant heuristic value in understanding the emergence and maintenance of PTSD symptoms, and leads to different and perhaps more useful conjectures about the origins and maintenance of the syndrome than the traditional hyperreactive fear account.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael K Suvak
- Women's Health Sciences Division, VA National Center for PTSD, VA Boston Healthcare System, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|