1
|
Luo Y, Chen X. The Impact of Math-Gender Stereotypes on Students' Academic Performance: Evidence from China. J Intell 2024; 12:75. [PMID: 39195122 DOI: 10.3390/jintelligence12080075] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/16/2024] [Revised: 07/25/2024] [Accepted: 07/31/2024] [Indexed: 08/29/2024] Open
Abstract
This study investigates the impact of math-gender stereotypes on students' academic performance using data from the China Education Panel Survey (CEPS), which surveyed nationally representative middle schools in China. Our sample comprises over 2000 seventh-grade students, with an average age of 13 and a standard deviation of 0.711. Among these students, 52.4% are male, and 47.6% are female. Employing a fixed effects model and instrumental variable, our findings are as follows. First, over half of the male students believe that boys are better at math than girls, and they also perceive that their parents and society hold the same belief. In contrast, fewer than half of the female students hold this belief or perception. Intriguingly, among these students, female math performance surpasses that of males. Second, stereotypes hinder female math performance, especially among low-achieving ones, while benefiting high-achieving male students. Finally, perceptions of societal stereotypes have the greatest effect on math performance, followed by self-stereotypes and perceptions of parental stereotypes. Understanding the implications of these findings highlights the importance of addressing math-gender stereotypes to promote equal participation and success for both genders in STEM fields.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yilei Luo
- School of Economics, Renmin University of China, Beijing 100872, China
| | - Xinqi Chen
- School of Economics, Renmin University of China, Beijing 100872, China
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Fennern EB, Stein SL. Gender Inequity in the Clinical Setting. Clin Colon Rectal Surg 2023; 36:309-314. [PMID: 37564345 PMCID: PMC10411211 DOI: 10.1055/s-0043-1763518] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 08/12/2023]
Abstract
Women in surgery continue to face inequitable treatment from surgical leadership, their peers, hospital staff, and even from their patients. Despite this, women surgeons continue to produce equal, or improved, clinical outcomes for their patients, with their work being given less remuneration than that of their male peers. The cultural stereotypes and biases that drive these inequities are implicit and subtle; however, they have dramatic effects on the lives and careers of women surgeons.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Erin B. Fennern
- Department of Surgery, University Hospitals–Cleveland Medical Center and Case Western Reserve University School of Medicine, Cleveland, Ohio
| | - Sharon L. Stein
- Department of Surgery, University Hospitals–Cleveland Medical Center and Case Western Reserve University School of Medicine, Cleveland, Ohio
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Soares De Almeida A, Gillath O, Kahalon R, Shnabel N. Effects of attachment security priming on women's math performance. Front Psychol 2023; 14:1124308. [PMID: 37691797 PMCID: PMC10484519 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1124308] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/15/2022] [Accepted: 08/01/2023] [Indexed: 09/12/2023] Open
Abstract
Introduction Activating people's sense of attachment security can buffer against psychological threats. Here we tested whether security priming can also buffer the adverse effects of stereotype threat among women. Method Three studies (a pilot study (N = 79 women, 72 men), a laboratory study; N = 474 women, and an online study; N = 827 women) compared security priming to neutral and positive affect priming. Results The pilot study revealed that women exposed to attachment security primes (e.g., the word "love") had better math performance than women exposed to neutral primes (e.g., "boat"). Men's math performance did not differ across priming conditions. Study 1 revealed that women showed better math performance in the attachment security priming condition than in the neutral or positive (e.g., "luck") priming conditions. The effect was observed among women high on math identification. In Study 2, despite an effect of security priming on the manipulation check [higher State Adult Attachment Measure (SAAM) security score], security did not buffer stereotype threat effects. Discussion Our findings provide partial support to the idea that security priming (an interpersonal process) can buffer stereotype threat (an intergroup process). Theoretical and practical implications related to attachment security priming and stereotype threat are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Omri Gillath
- Department of Psychology, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, United States
| | - Rotem Kahalon
- The Azrieli Faculty of Medicine, Bar-Ilan University, Safed, Israel
| | - Nurit Shnabel
- The School of Psychological Sciences, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Sebastián-Tirado A, Félix-Esbrí S, Forn C, Sanchis-Segura C. Are gender-science stereotypes barriers for women in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics? Exploring when, how, and to whom in an experimentally-controlled setting. Front Psychol 2023; 14:1219012. [PMID: 37621930 PMCID: PMC10445161 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1219012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/08/2023] [Accepted: 07/25/2023] [Indexed: 08/26/2023] Open
Abstract
Based on Social Cognitive Career Theory principles, the present study sought to investigate whether stereotype threat experiences could act as a barrier and reduce the persistence of women in math-intensive activities. More specifically, we assessed whether the experimental activation of stereotypes about women's lower math capabilities affected the performance, persistence, and self-selected difficulty of engineering students in a math task which required sustained effort. We also evaluated the relationships between these effects and the participants' pre-testing gender-science stereotypes and math self-concept. A sample of 340 engineering students (175 and 165 self-identified as males and females, respectively) were recruited and randomly assigned to a control (Con) or stereotype threat (StA) condition to form four similarly sized groups. All participants rated their self-concept in four academic domains (math, chemistry, physics, and coding), completed the gender-science Implicit Association Test, and a "reading comprehension task" that served to promote the stereotype threat manipulation immediately before facing a modified version of the Math Effort Task (M-MET). We observed that, in the control condition, M-MET performance, self-selected difficulty, and persistence were similar in female and male participants, independent of their gender-science implicit stereotypes but correlated to their math self-concept scores. In contrast, the StA condition triggered opposite effects in female and male students, so they showed decreased/enhanced M-MET performance and self-selected difficulty, respectively. This experimental condition also resulted in enhanced persistence of the male students without affecting the number of trials completed by female students. These effects were correlated with the strength of the participants' gender-science implicit stereotypes but not with their math self-concept scores. In fact, as revealed by finer-grain analyses, stereotype threat only had a significant impact on individuals harboring stereotypical gender-science implicit associations. Therefore, it is concluded that: (1) stereotypes can promote differences between male and female engineering students in their performance, self-confidence, and persistence in math-related activities; (2) These effects seem to be more prominent in individuals implicitly perceiving science as a masculine domain. The relevance of these findings to explain women's enhanced abandonment rates of math-intensive studies is discussed.
Collapse
|
5
|
Saemi E, Moteshareie E, Jalilinasab S, Afrash S, Deshayes M. Gender stereotypes and motor performance: How explicit and implicit stereotypes influence girls standing long jump and anxiety. PSYCHOLOGY OF SPORT AND EXERCISE 2023; 64:102334. [PMID: 37665817 DOI: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2022.102334] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/07/2022] [Revised: 11/09/2022] [Accepted: 11/09/2022] [Indexed: 09/06/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Inducing a negative stereotype toward women usually leads to a decrease in women's motor performance. Given that most studies have focused on explicit stereotype induction among adults, the main aim of this study was to investigate the effects of explicit and implicit gender stereotypes on standing long jump performance in children. The second aim was to investigate the effects of these same manipulations on children's state anxiety. DESIGN A mixed model design with within-between-subject was used with standing long jump performance and state anxiety as dependent variables. METHOD Two hundred and four children (Mage = 10.95 years, SDage = 0.85) participated in this study and were randomly assigned, after baseline measurement, into four different groups (i.e., explicit/implicit vs. explicit vs. implicit vs. control). Specifically, participants performed 8 trials of standing long jump (4 trials during the baseline phase and 4 trials during the experimental phase). Children also completed the competitive state Anxiety Inventory at baseline as well as immediately after the trials. RESULTS For motor performance, children in the explicit/implicit group and in the implicit group were negatively affected by the stereotype manipulation during all trials whereas participants in the explicit group were only negatively impacted during the last two trials. However, regarding state anxiety, children were negatively affected after both explicit only and implicit only manipulations and more significantly after explicit/implicit manipulation. CONCLUSIONS The present research showed that the explicit and implicit manipulations influenced motor performance differently, but that these two manipulations increased state anxiety in the same way. Moreover, the combination of the explicit and implicit inductions leads to a greater significant negative influence on state anxiety but not motor performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Esmaeel Saemi
- Department of Motor Behavior and Sport Psychology, Faculty of Sport Sciences, Shahid Chamran University of Ahvaz, Ahvaz, Iran.
| | - Ebrahim Moteshareie
- Department of Cognitive and Behavioral Science and Technology in Sport, Faculty of Sport Sciences and Health, Shahid Beheshti University, Tehran, Iran
| | - Sara Jalilinasab
- Department of Motor Behavior and Sport Psychology, Faculty of Sport Sciences, Shahid Chamran University of Ahvaz, Ahvaz, Iran
| | - Sana Afrash
- Department of Motor Behavior and Sport Psychology, Faculty of Sport Sciences, Shahid Chamran University of Ahvaz, Ahvaz, Iran
| | | |
Collapse
|
6
|
Santos J, Bittencourt I, Reis M, Chalco G, Isotani S. Two billion registered students affected by stereotyped educational environments: an analysis of gender-based color bias. HUMANITIES & SOCIAL SCIENCES COMMUNICATIONS 2022; 9:249. [PMID: 35967484 PMCID: PMC9362687 DOI: 10.1057/s41599-022-01220-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/22/2021] [Accepted: 05/31/2022] [Indexed: 06/15/2023]
Abstract
According to the literature, educational technologies present several learning benefits to promote online education. However, there are several associated challenges, and some studies illustrate the limitations in elaborating educational technologies, called Design limitations. This aspect is responsible for unleashing various issues in the learning process, such as gender inequality, creating adverse effects on cognitive, motivational, and behavioral mediators, which opposes the fifth UN's Sustainable Development Goal. Therefore, many studies notice the harmful effects of stereotypes in educational technologies. These effects can be included in the design, like colors or other stereotyped elements, or how the activity is conducted. Based on this, the present study aimed to verify the predominance of color bias in educational technologies available on the WEB. This study developed a computational solution to calculate male and female color bias in the available educational technology web pages. The results suggest the prevalence of the development of educational technologies with a male color bias, with an imbalance among genders, without adequate customization for age groups. Furthermore, some environments, such as Computer Science, present a higher color bias for men when compared to women. Despite both scales being independent, results indicated interesting evidence of a substantial prevalence of colors associated with the male scale. According to the literature, this may be associated with dropout and lack of interest in female students, especially in sciences, technology, engineering, and mathematics domains.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jário Santos
- Institute of Mathematics and Computer Science, University of São Paulo (ICMC-USP), CEP: 13566-590 São Carlos, SP Brazil
| | - Ig Bittencourt
- Institute of Computer Science, University of Alagoas (UFAL), Maceió, Brazil, CEP: 57072-970 Maceió, AL Brazil
| | - Marcelo Reis
- Institute of Computer Science, University of Alagoas (UFAL), Maceió, Brazil, CEP: 57072-970 Maceió, AL Brazil
| | - Geiser Chalco
- Computer Science Center (C3), Federal University of Rio Grande (FURG), CEP: 96203-900 Rio Grande, RS Brazil
| | - Seiji Isotani
- Institute of Mathematics and Computer Science, University of São Paulo (ICMC-USP), CEP: 13566-590 São Carlos, SP Brazil
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Stoevenbelt AH, Flore PC, Schwabe I, Wicherts JM. The uniformity of stereotype threat: Analyzing the moderating effects of premeasured performance. INTELLIGENCE 2022. [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2022.101655] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
|
8
|
Pownall M, Conner M, Hutter RRC. ‘Baby brain’ in pregnancy: A review of social psychological explanations and future research directions. SOCIAL AND PERSONALITY PSYCHOLOGY COMPASS 2022. [DOI: 10.1111/spc3.12696] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | - Mark Conner
- School of Psychology University of Leeds Leeds UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
König S, Stang-Rabrig J, Hannover B, Zander L, McElvany N. Stereotype threat in learning situations? An investigation among language minority students. EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY OF EDUCATION 2022. [DOI: 10.1007/s10212-022-00618-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
AbstractStereotype threat (ST) is a potential explanation for inequalities in language competencies observed between students from different language backgrounds. Language competencies are an important prerequisite for educational success, wherefore the significance for investigation arises. While ST effects on achievement are empirically well documented, little is known about whether ST also impairs learning. Thus, we investigated vocabulary learning in language minority elementary school students, also searching for potential moderators. In a pre-post design, 240 fourth-grade students in Germany who were on average 10 years old (MAge = 9.92, SD = 0.64; 49.8% female) were randomly assigned to one of four experimental conditions: implicit ST, explicit ST without threat removal before posttest, explicit ST with threat removal before posttest, and a control group. Results showed that learning difficult vocabulary from reading two narrative texts was unaffected by ST. Neither students’ identification with their culture of residence and culture of origin nor stereotyped domain of reading were moderators. The findings are discussed with regard to content and methodological aspects such that a motivation effect might have undermined a possible ST effect. Implications for future research include examining the question at what age children become susceptible to ST and whether students have internalized negative stereotypes about their own group, which could increase the likelihood of ST effects occurring.
Collapse
|
10
|
Collective self-doubt: does subjective SES predict behavioral self-handicapping tendency in college students? SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OF EDUCATION 2021. [DOI: 10.1007/s11218-021-09678-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
11
|
Pownall M, Conner M, Hutter RRC. The effects of activating a “baby brain” stereotype on pregnant women’s cognitive functioning. JOURNAL OF APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2021. [DOI: 10.1111/jasp.12802] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | - Mark Conner
- School of Psychology University of Leeds Leeds UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
The relative importance of personal beliefs, meta-stereotypes and societal stereotypes of age for the wellbeing of older people. AGEING & SOCIETY 2020. [DOI: 10.1017/s0144686x20000537] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
AbstractNegative images of old age can harm older individuals’ cognitive and physical functioning and health. Yet, older people may be confronted with age stereotypes that are inconsistent with their own personal beliefs. We examine the implications for older people's wellbeing of three distinct elements of age stereotypes: their personal beliefs about their age group, their perception about how others generally perceive older people (i.e. their meta-stereotypes) and the societal age stereotypes that are empirically widely shared in society. Using measures from the Stereotype Content Model and survey data of older people from the United Kingdom (UK) (Study 1, N = 171), we found only partial overlap between older people's personal beliefs and their meta-stereotypes. Personal beliefs were unrelated to wellbeing, but positive meta-stereotypes of older people's competence were linked to higher wellbeing. These findings were largely replicated with a sample of baby-boomers from Switzerland (Study 2, N = 400) controlling for socio-demographics. Study 3 used representative survey data (N = 10,803) across 29 European countries, to test and confirm that the link between positive competence meta-stereotypes and wellbeing can be generalised to different cultures, and that positive warmth meta-stereotypes were an additional predictor. At the country level, societal age stereotypes about competence were positively related to the wellbeing of older people, but only in countries that provide greater opportunities for competence attainment.
Collapse
|
13
|
Smith RW, Baranik LE, Duffy RD. Psychological ownership within psychology of working theory: A three-wave study of gender and sexual minority employees. JOURNAL OF VOCATIONAL BEHAVIOR 2020. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jvb.2019.103374] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
14
|
Sanchez CA, Weber K. Using relevant animations to counter stereotype threat when learning science. JOURNAL OF APPLIED RESEARCH IN MEMORY AND COGNITION 2019. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2019.08.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
15
|
Grabow H, Kühl M. You Don't Bend It Like Beckham if You're Female and Reminded of It: Stereotype Threat Among Female Football Players. Front Psychol 2019; 10:1963. [PMID: 31555169 PMCID: PMC6724661 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01963] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/22/2018] [Accepted: 08/09/2019] [Indexed: 11/27/2022] Open
Abstract
Originally, the stereotype threat effect – poorer performance due to a fear of fulfilling a negative stereotype about one’s group – was demonstrated for cognitive tasks (e.g. Steele and Aronson, 1995, or Steele, 1997). Drawing on the widespread stereotype of women being unable to play football we experimentally tested (N = 80) whether a respective threat affected female football players’ goal scoring precision, i.e. a complex and demanding motor task. Those participants who were reminded of the stereotype scored significantly less hits than those not reminded. Additionally, deviations from the instruction during task execution (e.g. shooting from another distance than demanded or using the wrong foot) were recorded. Stereotype threat did not affect this comparatively more cognitive task of following instructions correctly. In order to explore underlying mechanisms of the observed stereotype effect, several potential mediators, e.g. measures of cognitive interference, or collective identification, were tested. None emerged as an unquestionable link between threat and motor performance. We discuss, however, why collective identification – in comparison to cognitive demand – appears to be the more promising explanatory concept.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hilmar Grabow
- Social and Political Psychology, Institute of Psychology, Kiel University, Kiel, Germany
| | - Melanie Kühl
- Social and Political Psychology, Institute of Psychology, Kiel University, Kiel, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Double jeopardy – Double remedy? The effectiveness of self-affirmation for improving doubly disadvantaged students' mathematical performance. J Sch Psychol 2019; 75:58-73. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jsp.2019.07.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/27/2018] [Revised: 06/22/2019] [Accepted: 07/08/2019] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
|
17
|
Academic disparities and health: How gender-based disparities in schools relate to boys' and girls' health. Soc Sci Med 2019; 228:126-134. [PMID: 30909156 DOI: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2019.02.036] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/22/2017] [Revised: 01/23/2019] [Accepted: 02/23/2019] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
RATIONALE Recent research reveals that, although girls encounter some barriers in school (e.g., in science and math), on balance, boys perform worse academically. Moreover, other research has identified a correlation between exposure to a context characterized by large disparities in performance or resources and a range of negative outcomes, including negative health and well-being, among members of lower status groups. OBJECTIVE Building on these literatures, the present research tests the relationship between gender disparities in academic performance within a school and students' health outcomes. Specifically, we investigated whether boys had worse health when they attended schools where there was a greater disparity between boys' and girls' academic performance. METHOD We tested this hypothesis in two different samples with different health outcomes. In a sample of healthy eighth graders (Study 1; 159 girls and 81 boys), we assessed two indices of metabolic syndrome, and in a sample of children with asthma (Study 2; 122 girls and 153 boys), we assessed immune function (Th1 and Th2 cytokine production) and self-reported symptoms. Participants in both samples also reported the name of the school that they attended so that we could access publicly available information about the percentage of girls and the percentage of boys in each school who met expectations for their grade level on standardized tests. RESULTS In both samples, the greater the gap in a school between the percentage of girls and the percentage of boys who met expectations for their grade level on standardized tests, the worse boys' health. This pattern did not emerge among girls. CONCLUSION Results thus highlight the negative health correlates of academic disparities among members of lower-performing groups.
Collapse
|
18
|
Fresson M, Dardenne B, Meulemans T. Impact of Diagnosis Threat on Neuropsychological Assessment of People with Acquired Brain Injury: Evidence of Mediation by Negative Emotions. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2019; 34:222-235. [PMID: 29579136 DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acy024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/03/2017] [Accepted: 03/09/2018] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Objective Some studies have shown that diagnosis threat (DT) could negatively impact the cognitive performance of undergraduate students who had sustained a mild traumatic brain injury. This study was designed to examine DT in people with acquired brain injury (ABI). As a second goal, we investigated the effect of stereotype lift as a way to overcome DT's harmful impact. The purpose of this study was also to examine the mechanisms mediating stereotype effects. Method People with ABI and control participants were assigned to one of three conditions: DT, cognitive-neutral (in which the cognitive status of participants with ABI and the cognitive characteristics of the tasks were deemphasized), and stereotype lift (in which a downward comparison was made with another neurological group). Participants then completed neuropsychological tasks. Negative emotions, intrusive thoughts, task expectancy, and self-efficacy were assessed for mediation analyses. Results Instructions impacted the performance of people with ABI, but not control participants. Compared to the cognitive-neutral condition, participants with ABI in the DT condition performed worse on memory and executive tasks (but not on attention tasks). These effects were mediated by negative emotions. There was no increase in performance in the stereotype lift condition compared to the DT condition. Conclusions This study showed that DT can aggravate the cognitive difficulties of people with ABI during neuropsychological assessment. The mediating role of negative emotions and the selective impact of DT on tasks that rely heavily on executive functioning are discussed in the light of the stereotype threat model.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Megan Fresson
- University of Liège (Belgium), Psychology and Neurosciences of Cognition Unit, Place des Orateurs 1, Liège, Belgium
| | - Benoit Dardenne
- University of Liège (Belgium), Psychology and Neurosciences of Cognition Unit, Place des Orateurs 2, Liège, Belgium
| | - Thierry Meulemans
- University of Liège (Belgium), Psychology and Neurosciences of Cognition Unit, Place des Orateurs 1, Liège, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Flore PC, Mulder J, Wicherts JM. The influence of gender stereotype threat on mathematics test scores of Dutch high school students: a registered report. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2019. [DOI: 10.1080/23743603.2018.1559647] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Paulette C. Flore
- Department of Methodology and Statistics, Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands
| | - Joris Mulder
- Department of Methodology and Statistics, Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands
| | - Jelte M. Wicherts
- Department of Methodology and Statistics, Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Effects of meta-stereotype on aggressive behavior among migrant children and the mediating effect of frustration. ACTA PSYCHOLOGICA SINICA 2019. [DOI: 10.3724/sp.j.1041.2019.00484] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
21
|
We are in this together: How the presence of similarly stereotyped allies buffer against identity threat. JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2018. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2018.09.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/30/2023]
|
22
|
Pennington CR, Litchfield D, McLatchie N, Heim D. Stereotype threat may not impact women's inhibitory control or mathematical performance: Providing support for the null hypothesis. EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2018. [DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.2540] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Neil McLatchie
- Department of Psychology Lancaster University Lancaster UK
| | - Derek Heim
- Department of Psychology Edge Hill University Ormskirk UK
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Forbes CE, Amey R, Magerman AB, Duran K, Liu M. Stereotype-based stressors facilitate emotional memory neural network connectivity and encoding of negative information to degrade math self-perceptions among women. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2018; 13:719-740. [PMID: 29939344 PMCID: PMC6121152 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsy043] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/01/2017] [Accepted: 06/18/2018] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
Stress engendered by stereotype threatening situations may facilitate encoding of negative, stereotype confirming feedback received during a performance among women in science, technology, engineering and mathematics (STEM). It is unclear, however, whether this process is comprised of the same neurophysiological mechanisms evident in any emotional memory encoding context, or if this encoding bias directly undermines positive self-perceptions in the stigmatized domain. A total of 160 men and women completed a math test that provided veridical positive and negative feedback, a memory test for feedback, and math self-enhancing and valuing measures in a stereotype threatening or neutral context while continuous electroencephalography activity and startle probe responses to positive and negative feedback was recorded. Indexing amygdala activity to feedback via startle responses and emotional memory network connectivity elicited during accurate recognition of positive and negative feedback via graph analyses, only stereotype threatened women encoded negative feedback better when they exhibited increased amygdala activity and emotional memory network connectivity in response to said feedback. Emotional memory biases, in turn, predicted decreases in women’s self-enhancing, math valuing and performance. Findings provide an emotional memory encoding-based mechanism for well-established findings indicating that women have more negative math self-perceptions compared with men regardless of actual performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chad E Forbes
- Social Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19716, USA
| | - Rachel Amey
- Social Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19716, USA
| | - Adam B Magerman
- Social Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19716, USA
| | - Kelly Duran
- Social Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19716, USA
| | - Mengting Liu
- Social Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19716, USA
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Hermann JM, Vollmeyer R. Das mathematische Selbstkonzept als Moderator des Stereotype-Threat- und Stereotype-Lift-Effekts. ZEITSCHRIFT FUR PADAGOGISCHE PSYCHOLOGIE 2017. [DOI: 10.1024/1010-0652/a000209] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Zusammenfassung. Ziel dieser Studie war es, zu untersuchen, ob Schüler/-innen durch die Aktivierung von Stereotypen in ihrer Mathematikleistung beeinflusst werden und inwiefern diese Prozesse durch das mathematische Selbstkonzept moderiert werden. Bei Mädchen gehen wir davon aus, dass bei schweren Aufgaben eine Stereotyp-Aktivierung nur bei hohem mathematischem Selbstkonzept zu einer schlechteren Leistung im Vergleich mit der weiblichen Kontrollgruppe führt. Für Jungen mit hohem mathematischem Selbstkonzept wird in der Stereotyp-Threat-Bedingung hingegen ein Leistungsanstieg im Vergleich mit der Kontrollgruppe erwartet. An der Studie nahmen 97 Schüler/-innen der 10. Jahrgangsstufe teil (Alter: M = 15.37, SD = .63). Erwartungskonform zeigte sich bei schweren Mathematikaufgaben eine signifikante Interaktion zwischen Geschlecht, Selbstkonzept und Versuchsbedingung. Mädchen mit hohem mathematischem Selbstkonzept schnitten in der Stereotype-Threat-Bedingung signifikant schlechter ab als Mädchen der Kontrollgruppe. Ein signifikanter Lift-Effekt bei Jungen mit hohem mathematischem Selbstkonzept ergab sich nicht. Die Befunde werden im Hinblick auf Erkenntnisse der Selbstkonzeptforschung und Interventionsstrategien in der Schule diskutiert.
Collapse
|
25
|
Zhang B, Wang M, Li J, Yu G, Bi YL. The Effects of Concealing Academic Achievement Information on Adolescents’ Self-Concept. PSYCHOLOGICAL RECORD 2017. [DOI: 10.1007/bf03395744] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
26
|
Hakim C, Kurman J, Eshel Y. Stereotype Threat and Stereotype Reactance: The Effect of Direct and Indirect Stereotype Manipulations on Performance of Palestinian Citizens of Israel on Achievement Tests. JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY 2017. [DOI: 10.1177/0022022117698040] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
The role of stereotype threat in performance of Palestinian citizens of Israel (PCI) on the psychometric exam (parallel to the SAT in the United States) was investigated. Two experiments, conducted in field settings, examined the role of stereotype threat in performance of PCI students in psychometric-like tests. Experiment 1, conducted on 135 Arab high school students, indicated that activation of the stereotype pertaining to the lower academic ability of PCI lowered participants’ performance relative to a control group. Experiment 2, which included 115 Arab students who participated in preparatory courses for the psychometric tests, showed that a direct manipulation of the stereotype led to stereotype reactance and increased performance, whereas an indirect manipulation activated the stereotype and resulted in lowered performance. These findings suggest that activating a stereotype threat or a stereotype reactance is contingent on the manipulation employed. Practical implications of these results are discussed.
Collapse
|
27
|
Huber ME, Brown AJ, Sternad D. Girls can play ball: Stereotype threat reduces variability in a motor skill. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2016; 169:79-87. [PMID: 27249638 PMCID: PMC4987161 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.05.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/28/2015] [Revised: 04/01/2016] [Accepted: 05/17/2016] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
The majority of research on stereotype threat shows what is expected: threat debilitates performance. However, facilitation is also possible, although seldom reported. This study investigated how stereotype threat influences novice females when performing the sensorimotor task of bouncing a ball to a target. We tested the predictions of two prevailing accounts for debilitation and facilitation due to sterotype threat effects: working memory and mere effort. Experimental results showed that variability in performance decreased more in stigmatized females than in control females, consistent with the prediction of the mere effort account, but inconsistent with the working memory account. These findings suggest that stereotype threat effects may be predicated upon the correctness of the dominant motor behavior, rather than on a novice-expert distinction or task difficulty. Further, a comprehensive understanding should incorporate the fact that stereotype threat can facilitate, as well as debilitate, performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Meghan E Huber
- Department of Bioengineering, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA.
| | - Adam J Brown
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Dagmar Sternad
- Department of Biology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA; Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA; Department of Physics, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Not Feeling Good in STEM: Effects of Stereotype Activation and Anticipated Affect on Women’s Career Aspirations. SEX ROLES 2016. [DOI: 10.1007/s11199-016-0665-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
29
|
Davies LC, Conner M, Sedikides C, Hutter RRC. Math Question Type and Stereotype Threat: Evidence from Educational Settings. SOCIAL COGNITION 2016. [DOI: 10.1521/soco.2016.34.3.196] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
|
30
|
Doyle RA, Voyer D. Stereotype manipulation effects on math and spatial test performance: A meta-analysis. LEARNING AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2016. [DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2015.12.018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 42] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
|
31
|
|
32
|
Pennington CR, Heim D, Levy AR, Larkin DT. Twenty Years of Stereotype Threat Research: A Review of Psychological Mediators. PLoS One 2016; 11:e0146487. [PMID: 26752551 PMCID: PMC4713435 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0146487] [Citation(s) in RCA: 88] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/23/2015] [Accepted: 12/17/2015] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
This systematic literature review appraises critically the mediating variables of stereotype threat. A bibliographic search was conducted across electronic databases between 1995 and 2015. The search identified 45 experiments from 38 articles and 17 unique proposed mediators that were categorized into affective/subjective (n = 6), cognitive (n = 7) and motivational mechanisms (n = 4). Empirical support was accrued for mediators such as anxiety, negative thinking, and mind-wandering, which are suggested to co-opt working memory resources under stereotype threat. Other research points to the assertion that stereotype threatened individuals may be motivated to disconfirm negative stereotypes, which can have a paradoxical effect of hampering performance. However, stereotype threat appears to affect diverse social groups in different ways, with no one mediator providing unequivocal empirical support. Underpinned by the multi-threat framework, the discussion postulates that different forms of stereotype threat may be mediated by distinct mechanisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Charlotte R. Pennington
- Department of Psychology, Edge Hill University, Ormskirk, Lancashire, England, United Kingdom
| | - Derek Heim
- Department of Psychology, Edge Hill University, Ormskirk, Lancashire, England, United Kingdom
| | - Andrew R. Levy
- Department of Psychology, Edge Hill University, Ormskirk, Lancashire, England, United Kingdom
| | - Derek T. Larkin
- Department of Psychology, Edge Hill University, Ormskirk, Lancashire, England, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Affiliation(s)
- Steven J. Spencer
- Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada, N2L 3G1;
| | - Christine Logel
- Renison University College, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada, N2L 3G1;
| | - Paul G. Davies
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Kelowna, British Columbia, Canada, V1V 1V7;
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Hermann JM, Vollmeyer R. Stereotype Threat in der Grundschule. ZEITSCHRIFT FUR ENTWICKLUNGSPSYCHOLOGIE UND PADAGOGISCHE PSYCHOLOGIE 2016. [DOI: 10.1026/0049-8637/a000143] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Zusammenfassung. Das Ziel der Studie war es zu untersuchen, ob bereits Grundschulkinder von Stereotype-Threat- beziehungsweise Lift-Effekten in ihrer Mathematikleistung beeinflusst werden. Dazu wurde auf eine implizite Manipulation zurückgegriffen, um Geschlechtsstereotype zu aktivieren, bevor ein Mathematiktest absolviert wurde. Bei Mädchen sollte die Aktivierung zu einer schlechteren Leistung führen, während für Jungen ein Leistungsvorsprung erwartet wurde. An der Untersuchung nahmen 120 Viertklässler teil (66 männlich, 54 weiblich, Alter M = 9.24, SD = 0.61). Hypothesenkonform ergab sich eine signifikante Interaktion zwischen Geschlecht und Stereotypaktivierung bei schwierigen Aufgaben. Während Mädchen in der Stereotypgruppe schlechter abschnitten als Mädchen in der Kontrollgruppe, konnte bei den Jungen kein Leistungsunterschied beobachtet werden. Die Ergebnisse bestätigen, dass Geschlechtsstereotype implizit bereits in der Grundschule die Mathematikleistung von Mädchen beeinträchtigen können.
Collapse
|
35
|
Forbes CE, Duran KA, Leitner JB, Magerman A. Stereotype Threatening Contexts Enhance Encoding of Negative Feedback to Engender Underperformance and Anxiety. SOCIAL COGNITION 2015. [DOI: 10.1521/soco.2015.33.6.605] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
|
36
|
Bécares L, Priest N. Understanding the Influence of Race/Ethnicity, Gender, and Class on Inequalities in Academic and Non-Academic Outcomes among Eighth-Grade Students: Findings from an Intersectionality Approach. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0141363. [PMID: 26505623 PMCID: PMC4624767 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141363] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/10/2015] [Accepted: 10/06/2015] [Indexed: 12/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Socioeconomic, racial/ethnic, and gender inequalities in academic achievement have been widely reported in the US, but how these three axes of inequality intersect to determine academic and non-academic outcomes among school-aged children is not well understood. Using data from the US Early Childhood Longitudinal Study—Kindergarten (ECLS-K; N = 10,115), we apply an intersectionality approach to examine inequalities across eighth-grade outcomes at the intersection of six racial/ethnic and gender groups (Latino girls and boys, Black girls and boys, and White girls and boys) and four classes of socioeconomic advantage/disadvantage. Results of mixture models show large inequalities in socioemotional outcomes (internalizing behavior, locus of control, and self-concept) across classes of advantage/disadvantage. Within classes of advantage/disadvantage, racial/ethnic and gender inequalities are predominantly found in the most advantaged class, where Black boys and girls, and Latina girls, underperform White boys in academic assessments, but not in socioemotional outcomes. In these latter outcomes, Black boys and girls perform better than White boys. Latino boys show small differences as compared to White boys, mainly in science assessments. The contrasting outcomes between racial/ethnic and gender minorities in self-assessment and socioemotional outcomes, as compared to standardized assessments, highlight the detrimental effect that intersecting racial/ethnic and gender discrimination have in patterning academic outcomes that predict success in adult life. Interventions to eliminate achievement gaps cannot fully succeed as long as social stratification caused by gender and racial discrimination is not addressed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laia Bécares
- Centre on Dynamics of Ethnicity, Department of Social Statistics, University of Manchester, Manchester, United Kingdom
- * E-mail:
| | - Naomi Priest
- Australian National University, Acton, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
Leveling up on stereotype threat: The role of avatar customization and avatar embodiment. COMPUTERS IN HUMAN BEHAVIOR 2015. [DOI: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.04.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
|
38
|
Phillips NL, Adams G, Salter PS. Beyond Adaptation: Decolonizing Approaches to Coping With Oppression. JOURNAL OF SOCIAL AND POLITICAL PSYCHOLOGY 2015. [DOI: 10.5964/jspp.v3i1.310] [Citation(s) in RCA: 51] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
How should one respond to racial oppression? Conventional prescriptions of mainstream social psychological science emphasize the idea ofcopingwith oppression—whether via emotional management strategies that emphasize denial or disengagement; problem-focused strategies that emphasize compensation, self-efficacy, or skills training; or collective strategies that emphasize emotional support—in ways that promote adaptation to, rather than transformation of, oppressive social structures. Following a brief review of the literature on coping with racism and oppression, we present an alternative model rooted in perspectives of liberation psychology (Martín-Baró, 1994). This decolonial approach emphasizes critical consciousness (rather than cultivated ignorance) of racial oppression, a focus on de-ideologization (rather than legitimation) of status quo realities, and illumination of models of identification conducive to collective action. Whereas the standard approach to coping with oppression may ultimately both reinforce and reproduce systems of domination, we propose a decolonial approach to racism perception as a more effective strategy for enduring prosperity and well-being.
Collapse
|
39
|
Forbes CE, Leitner JB, Duran-Jordan K, Magerman AB, Schmader T, Allen JJB. Spontaneous default mode network phase-locking moderates performance perceptions under stereotype threat. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2015; 10:994-1002. [PMID: 25398433 PMCID: PMC4483567 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsu145] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/01/2014] [Revised: 10/14/2014] [Accepted: 11/12/2014] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
This study assessed whether individual differences in self-oriented neural processing were associated with performance perceptions of minority students under stereotype threat. Resting electroencephalographic activity recorded in white and minority participants was used to predict later estimates of task errors and self-doubt on a presumed measure of intelligence. We assessed spontaneous phase-locking between dipole sources in left lateral parietal cortex (LPC), precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex (P/PCC), and medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC); three regions of the default mode network (DMN) that are integral for self-oriented processing. Results revealed that minorities with greater LPC-P/PCC phase-locking in the theta band reported more accurate error estimations. All individuals experienced less self-doubt to the extent they exhibited greater LPC-MPFC phase-locking in the alpha band but this effect was driven by minorities. Minorities also reported more self-doubt to the extent they overestimated errors. Findings reveal novel neural moderators of stereotype threat effects on subjective experience. Spontaneous synchronization between DMN regions may play a role in anticipatory coping mechanisms that buffer individuals from stereotype threat.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chad E Forbes
- Social Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, USA, Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada, and Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA
| | - Jordan B Leitner
- Social Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, USA, Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada, and Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA
| | - Kelly Duran-Jordan
- Social Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, USA, Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada, and Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA
| | - Adam B Magerman
- Social Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, USA, Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada, and Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA
| | - Toni Schmader
- Social Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, USA, Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada, and Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA
| | - John J B Allen
- Social Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, USA, Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada, and Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA
| |
Collapse
|
40
|
Huber ME, Seitchik AE, Brown AJ, Sternad D, Harkins SG. The effect of stereotype threat on performance of a rhythmic motor skill. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 2015; 41:525-41. [PMID: 25706769 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0000039] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Many studies using cognitive tasks have found that stereotype threat, or concern about confirming a negative stereotype about one's group, debilitates performance. The few studies that documented similar effects on sensorimotor performance have used only relatively coarse measures to quantify performance. This study tested the effect of stereotype threat on a rhythmic ball bouncing task, where previous analyses of the task dynamics afforded more detailed quantification of the effect of threat on motor control. In this task, novices hit the ball with positive racket acceleration, indicative of unstable performance. With practice, they learn to stabilize error by changing their ball-racket impact from positive to negative acceleration. Results showed that for novices, stereotype threat potentiated hitting the ball with positive racket acceleration, leading to poorer performance of stigmatized females. However, when the threat manipulation was delivered after having acquired some skill, reflected by negative racket acceleration, the stigmatized females performed better. These findings are consistent with the mere effort account that argues that stereotype threat potentiates the most likely response on the given task. The study also demonstrates the value of identifying the control mechanisms through which stereotype threat has its effects on outcome measures.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Adam J Brown
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University
| | - Dagmar Sternad
- Departments of Biology, Electrical and Computer Engineering, and Physics, Northeastern University
| | | |
Collapse
|
41
|
Belmi P, Barragan RC, Neale MA, Cohen GL. Threats to Social Identity Can Trigger Social Deviance. PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN 2015; 41:467-84. [DOI: 10.1177/0146167215569493] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
We hypothesized that threats to people’s social (i.e., group) identity can trigger deviant attitudes and behaviors. A correlational study and five experiments showed that experiencing or recalling situations associated with the devaluation of a social identity caused participants to endorse or engage in deviant actions, including stealing, cheating, and lying. The effect was driven by the tendency to construe social identity threats not as isolated incidents but as symbolic of the continuing devaluation and disrespectful treatment of one’s group. Supplementing sociological approaches to deviance and delinquency, the results suggest the relevance and utility of a social-psychological account.
Collapse
|
42
|
Does stereotype threat influence performance of girls in stereotyped domains? A meta-analysis. J Sch Psychol 2014; 53:25-44. [PMID: 25636259 DOI: 10.1016/j.jsp.2014.10.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 215] [Impact Index Per Article: 21.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/26/2013] [Revised: 10/24/2014] [Accepted: 10/25/2014] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
Abstract
Although the effect of stereotype threat concerning women and mathematics has been subject to various systematic reviews, none of them have been performed on the sub-population of children and adolescents. In this meta-analysis we estimated the effects of stereotype threat on performance of girls on math, science and spatial skills (MSSS) tests. Moreover, we studied publication bias and four moderators: test difficulty, presence of boys, gender equality within countries, and the type of control group that was used in the studies. We selected study samples when the study included girls, samples had a mean age below 18years, the design was (quasi-)experimental, the stereotype threat manipulation was administered between-subjects, and the dependent variable was a MSSS test related to a gender stereotype favoring boys. To analyze the 47 effect sizes, we used random effects and mixed effects models. The estimated mean effect size equaled -0.22 and significantly differed from 0. None of the moderator variables was significant; however, there were several signs for the presence of publication bias. We conclude that publication bias might seriously distort the literature on the effects of stereotype threat among schoolgirls. We propose a large replication study to provide a less biased effect size estimate.
Collapse
|
43
|
Forbes CE, Leitner JB. Stereotype threat engenders neural attentional bias toward negative feedback to undermine performance. Biol Psychol 2014; 102:98-107. [DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.07.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/02/2014] [Revised: 05/16/2014] [Accepted: 07/07/2014] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
44
|
Vanderploeg RD, Belanger HG, Kaufmann PM. Nocebo Effects and Mild Traumatic Brain Injury: Legal Implications. PSYCHOLOGICAL INJURY & LAW 2014. [DOI: 10.1007/s12207-014-9201-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
45
|
Vermeulen L, Núñez Castellar E, Van Looy J. Challenging the other: exploring the role of opponent gender in digital game competition for female players. CYBERPSYCHOLOGY BEHAVIOR AND SOCIAL NETWORKING 2014; 17:303-9. [PMID: 24724802 DOI: 10.1089/cyber.2013.0331] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022]
Abstract
Abstract The present study investigated the effect of opponent gender on the game experience of female players. Concretely, it looked into skill perception and player emotions of women in same gender and cross-gender game competition. We set up a 2×2×2 (male vs. female opponent×low vs. high competitive women×lost vs. won game) experimental design in which women were instructed to play against a proclaimed male and female competitor. Unknowingly, however, participants played against an AI, which was configured to produce a winning and a losing condition for each opponent by manipulating difficulty. Results indicated that opponent gender only had an effect on perceived stress, which was higher with male opponents. Moreover, players evaluated their own gaming skills as lower and the skills of presumed male opponents as higher when they thought they were playing against men. Importantly, our results also showed that the above described pattern for self-perceived skills and perceived opponent skills was modulated by trait competitiveness with a larger effect size for low competitive women. Overall, this study illustrates that gender dynamics affect the play experience of women in cross-gender gaming competition. Implications and suggestions for future research are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lotte Vermeulen
- Department of Communication Sciences, iMinds-MICT-Ghent University , Ghent, Belgium
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
46
|
Abstract
We investigated effects of the media’s portrayal of boys as “scholastic failures” on secondary school students. The negative portrayal induced stereotype threat (boys underperformed in reading), stereotype reactance (boys displayed stronger learning goals towards mathematics but not reading), and stereotype lift (girls performed better in reading but not in mathematics). Apparently, boys were motivated to disconfirm their group’s negative depiction, however, while they could successfully apply compensatory strategies when describing their learning goals, this motivation did not enable them to perform better. Overall the media portrayal thus contributes to the maintenance of gender stereotypes, by impairing boys’ and strengthening girls’ performance in female connoted domains and by prompting boys to align their learning goals to the gender connotation of the domain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Martin Latsch
- Department of Educational Science and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, Germany
| | - Bettina Hannover
- Department of Educational Science and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
47
|
Jansen M, Schroeders U, Lüdtke O. Academic self-concept in science: Multidimensionality, relations to achievement measures, and gender differences. LEARNING AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2014. [DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.12.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 79] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
48
|
Influence of negative stereotypes and beliefs on neuropsychological test performance in a traumatic brain injury population. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2014; 20:157-67. [PMID: 24352047 DOI: 10.1017/s1355617713001264] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
The impact of stereotype threat and self-efficacy beliefs on neuropsychological test performance in a clinical traumatic brain injury (TBI) population was investigated. A total of 42 individuals with mild-to-moderate TBI and 42 (age-, gender-, educationally matched) healthy adults were recruited. The study consisted of a 2 (Type of injury: control, TBI) × 2 (Threat Condition: reduced threat, heightened threat) between-participants design. The purpose of the reduced threat condition was to reduce negative stereotyped beliefs regarding cognitive effects of TBI and to emphasize personal control over cognition. The heightened threat condition consisted of an opposing view. Main effects included greater anxiety, motivation, and dejection but reduced memory self-efficacy for head-injured-groups, compared to control groups. On neuropsychological testing, the TBI-heightened-threat-group displayed lower scores on Initial Encoding (initial recall) and trended toward displaying lower scores on Attention (working memory) compared to the TBI-reduced-threat-group. No effect was found for Delayed Recall measures. Memory self-efficacy mediated the relation between threat condition and neuropsychological performance, indicating a potential mechanism for the threat effect. The findings highlight the impact of stereotype threat and self-referent beliefs on neuropsychological test performance in a clinical TBI population.
Collapse
|
49
|
Koch SC, Konigorski S, Sieverding M. Sexist Behavior Undermines Women’s Performance in a Job Application Situation. SEX ROLES 2014. [DOI: 10.1007/s11199-014-0342-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
50
|
Kronberger N, Horwath I. The Ironic Costs of Performing Well: Grades Differentially Predict Male and Female Dropout From Engineering. BASIC AND APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2013. [DOI: 10.1080/01973533.2013.840629] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
|