51
|
Effect of sugar and acid composition, aroma release, and assessment conditions on aroma enhancement by taste in model wines. Food Qual Prefer 2019. [DOI: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2018.07.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
|
52
|
Blankenship ML, Grigorova M, Katz DB, Maier JX. Retronasal Odor Perception Requires Taste Cortex, but Orthonasal Does Not. Curr Biol 2018; 29:62-69.e3. [PMID: 30581018 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.11.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 50] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/25/2018] [Revised: 10/31/2018] [Accepted: 11/02/2018] [Indexed: 12/16/2022]
Abstract
Smells can arise from a source external to the body and stimulate the olfactory epithelium upon inhalation through the nares (orthonasal olfaction). Alternatively, smells may arise from inside the mouth during consumption, stimulating the epithelium upon exhalation (retronasal olfaction). Both ortho- and retronasal olfaction produce highly salient percepts, but the two percepts have very different behavioral implications. Here, we use optogenetic manipulation in the context of a flavor preference learning paradigm to investigate differences in the neural circuits that process information in these two submodalities of olfaction. Our findings support a view in which retronasal, but not orthonasal, odors share processing circuitry commonly associated with taste. First, our behavioral results reveal that retronasal odors induce rapid preference learning and have a potentiating effect on orthonasal preference learning. Second, we demonstrate that inactivation of the insular gustatory cortex selectively impairs expression of retronasal preferences. Thus, orally sourced (retronasal) olfactory input is processed by a brain region responsible for taste processing, whereas externally sourced (orthonasal) olfactory input is not.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Maria Grigorova
- Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, Waltham, MA, USA
| | - Donald B Katz
- Program in Neuroscience, Brandeis University, Waltham, MA, USA; Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, Waltham, MA, USA; Volen National Center for Complex Systems, Brandeis University, Waltham, MA, USA.
| | - Joost X Maier
- Department of Neurobiology & Anatomy, Wake Forest School of Medicine, Winston-Salem, NC, USA
| |
Collapse
|
53
|
Bamji-Stocke S, Biggs BT, Samuelsen CL. Experience-dependent c-Fos expression in the primary chemosensory cortices of the rat. Brain Res 2018; 1701:189-195. [PMID: 30244018 PMCID: PMC6289795 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2018.09.019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/16/2018] [Revised: 08/30/2018] [Accepted: 09/18/2018] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
Eating a new food is a unique event that guides future food choices. A key element for these choices is the perception of flavor (odor-taste associations), a multisensory process dependent upon taste and smell. The two primary cortical areas for taste and smell, gustatory cortex and piriform cortex, are thought to be crucial regions for processing and responding to odor-taste mixtures. To determine how previous experience impacts the primary chemosensory cortices, we compared the expression of the immediate early gene, c-Fos, between rats presented with a taste, an odor, or an odor-taste mixture for the first-time with rats that had many days of prior experience. Compared to rats with prior experience, we found that first-time sampling of all three chemosensory stimuli led to significantly greater c-Fos expression in gustatory cortex. In piriform cortex, only the novel chemosensory stimuli containing odors showed greater c-Fos expression. These results indicate that prior experience with taste, odor, or odor-taste stimuli habituates responses in the primary chemosensory cortices and adds further evidence supporting gustatory cortex as a fundamental node for the integration of gustatory and olfactory signals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sanaya Bamji-Stocke
- Department of Anatomical Sciences and Neurobiology, University of Louisville, KY 40202, United States.
| | - Bradley T Biggs
- Department of Anatomical Sciences and Neurobiology, University of Louisville, KY 40202, United States
| | - Chad L Samuelsen
- Department of Anatomical Sciences and Neurobiology, University of Louisville, KY 40202, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
54
|
Dose-Response Relationships for Vanilla Flavor and Sucrose in Skim Milk: Evidence of Synergy. BEVERAGES 2018. [DOI: 10.3390/beverages4040073] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Regarding cross-modality research, taste-aroma interaction is one of the most studied areas of research. Some studies have reported enhancement of sweetness by aroma, although it is unclear as to whether these effects actually occur: depending on the cognitive strategy employed by panelists, the effects may disappear, e.g., forcing panelists into an analytical strategy to control for dumping may not be able to reveal perceptual interactions. Previous studies have largely focused on solutions and model foods, and did not test stimuli or concentrations relevant to real food applications. This study addresses these gaps: 18 vanilla flavored sucrose milks, varying between 0–0.75% (w/w) two-fold vanilla, and 0–5% (w/w) sucrose, were rated by 108 panelists for liking and perceived sweetness, vanilla flavor, milk flavor, and thickness. Interactions between vanilla and sucrose were measured using deviations of real mixtures from additive models (via the isobole method), indicating vanilla aroma does enhance perceived sweetness. However, the sweetness enhancing effect of vanilla aroma was not as pronounced as that of sucrose on vanilla flavor. Measurable cross-modal interactions occur despite using an analytical cognitive strategy. More work is needed to investigate the influence of perceptual strategy on the degree of taste-aroma interactions in real foods.
Collapse
|
55
|
Abstract
Background This report produces a bibliographic study of psychophysical tests proposed clinical assessments of retronasal olfaction. Aims We review how these tests can be utilized and discuss their methodological properties. Study Design Systematic review. Methods We undertook a systematic literature review investigating the retronasal olfaction test methods. PubMed, the free online MEDLINE database on biomedical sciences, was searched for the period from 1984 to 2015 using the following relevant key phrases: “retronasal olfaction”, “orthonasal olfaction”, “olfaction disorders”, and “olfaction test”. For each of the selected titles cited in this study, the full manuscript was read and analyzed by each of the three authors of this paper independently before collaborative discussion for summation and analytical reporting. Two reviewers independently read the abstracts and full texts and categorised them into one of three subgroups as follow, suitable, not-suitable, and unsure. Then they cross-checked the results, and a third reviewer decided assigned the group “unsure” to either the suitable group or the not-suitable group. Fifty eight studies revealed as suitable for review by two authors whereas 13 found not suitable for review. The total amount of 60 uncertain (unsure) or differently categorized articles were further examined by the third author which resulted in 41 approvals and 19 rejections. Hence 99 approved articles passed the next step. Exclusion criteria were reviews, case reports, animal studies, and the articles of which methodology was a lack of olfaction tests. By this way excluded 69 papers, and finally, 30 original human research articles were taken as the data. Results The study found that the three most widely used and accepted retronasal olfaction test methods are the retronasal olfaction test, the candy smell test and odorant presentation containers. All of the three psychophysical retronasal olfaction tests were combined with orthonasal tests in clinical use to examine and understand the smell function of the patient completely. There were two limitations concerning testing: “the lack concentrations and doses of test materials” and “performing measurements within the supra-threshold zone”. Conclusion The appropriate test agents and optimal concentrations for the retronasal olfaction tests remain unclear and emerge as limitations of the retronasal olfaction test technique. The first step to overcoming these limitations will probably require identification of retronasal olfaction thresholds. Once these are determined, the concept of retronasal olfaction and its testing methods may be thoroughly reviewed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hüseyin Özay
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Dokuz Eylül University School of Medicine, İzmir, Turkey
| | - Aslı Çakır
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Dokuz Eylül University School of Medicine, İzmir, Turkey
| | - Mustafa Cenk Ecevit
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Dokuz Eylül University School of Medicine, İzmir, Turkey
| |
Collapse
|
56
|
Hudson SD, Sims CA, Odabasi AZ, Colquhoun TA, Snyder DJ, Stamps JJ, Dotson SC, Puentes L, Bartoshuk LM. Flavor Alterations Associated with Miracle Fruit and Gymnema sylvestre. Chem Senses 2018; 43:481-488. [PMID: 29905783 PMCID: PMC6108391 DOI: 10.1093/chemse/bjy032] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Taste and flavor (retronasal olfaction) interact in the brain. The rules of that interaction are not well understood. This study uses 2 taste modifiers that alter sweet to examine the effects on flavors. Subjects used the Global Sensory Intensity Scale to assess the aroma, sweetness, sourness, and flavor of 10 foods. As previous work had shown, miracle fruit added sweetness to acids, which secondarily reduced sourness (mixture suppression) and Gymnema sylvestre reduced sweetness in sweet foods as well as the sweetness induced by miracle fruit. In this study, multiple regression showed that both sweet and sour contribute to flavor. Gymnema sylvestre reduced the perceived sweet of predominantly sweet foods (chocolate and maple syrup) as expected; reducing the sweet, reduced the flavor. The effects of miracle fruit were complicated by its dual action: intensification of sweet and reduction of sour. Predominantly sour foods (vinegar, lemon, mustard, pickle) were sweetened by miracle fruit but any flavor enhancement associated with the added sweet appears to have been countered by the flavor reduction associated with reduced sourness. Moderately sour foods that are also sweet (tomatoes, strawberries) were sweetened by miracle fruit and thus flavor was enhanced; flavor loss through sour reduction was apparently not sufficient to counter the flavor enhancement due to increased sweet so the net result was that tomato and strawberry flavors were enhanced. The flavors of control foods (not predominantly sweet or sour [sausage, peanuts]) showed only small changes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sonia D Hudson
- Center for Food Safety and Applied Nutrition, Food and Drug Administration, College Park, MD, USA
| | - Charles A Sims
- Department of Food Science and Human Nutrition, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
| | - Asli Z Odabasi
- Department of Food Science and Human Nutrition, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
| | - Thomas A Colquhoun
- Department of Environmental Horticulture, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
| | - Derek J Snyder
- Department of Food Science and Human Nutrition, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
| | - Jennifer J Stamps
- Department of Food Science and Human Nutrition, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
| | - Shawn C Dotson
- BioSciences and Sweetness Research, Coca-Cola, Atlanta, GA, USA
| | | | - Linda M Bartoshuk
- Department of Food Science and Human Nutrition, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
| |
Collapse
|
57
|
Kokkinidou S, Peterson D, Bloch T, Bronston A. The Important Role of Carbohydrates in the Flavor, Function, and Formulation of Oral Nutritional Supplements. Nutrients 2018; 10:E742. [PMID: 29890616 PMCID: PMC6024762 DOI: 10.3390/nu10060742] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/25/2018] [Revised: 05/31/2018] [Accepted: 06/04/2018] [Indexed: 12/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Patients who are malnourished or at-risk for malnutrition often benefit from the consumption of oral nutritional supplements (ONS). ONS supply a range of micro- and macro-nutrients, and they can be used to supplement a diet or provide total nutrition. Since ONS are specially formulated products, all ONS ingredients—including carbohydrates—are added ingredients. This may seem to be at odds with the growing public health discourse on the need to reduce “added sugars” in the diet. However, carbohydrate is an essential nutrient for human health and is a critical ingredient in ONS. Helping to educate patients on the value of “added sugars” in ONS may be useful to improve compliance with nutritional recommendations when ONS are indicated. This perspective paper reviews the important roles of “added sugars” in ONS, in terms of flavor, function, and product formulation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Devin Peterson
- Flavor Research and Education Center, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210, USA.
| | - Tama Bloch
- Abbott Nutrition, Columbus, OH 43219, USA.
| | - Ashley Bronston
- Nutrition Consultant, Abbott Nutrition, Columbus, OH 43219, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
58
|
Involuntary polymodal imagery involving olfaction, audition, touch, taste, and vision. Conscious Cogn 2018; 62:9-20. [PMID: 29709666 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2018.04.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/14/2017] [Revised: 03/09/2018] [Accepted: 04/13/2018] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Percepts and urges often enter consciousness involuntarily. The Reflexive Imagery Task (RIT) reveals how high-level cognitions, too, can enter consciousness involuntarily. In the task, the eliciting stimuli are visual (e.g., picture of a cat), and the involuntary imagery is verbal (e.g., the subvocalization "cat"). The generalizability of the RIT effect has been questioned because verbal imagery is an easily elicited form of imagery. Do such effects arise for other kinds of imagery? It is known that imagery is more elicitable in some senses (e.g., vision) than in other senses (e.g., olfaction). We found such differences in an RIT in which food items were presented as orthographic stimuli or as drawings. Although subjects were instructed to suppress mental imagery, involuntary imagery still arose: Olfactory (effect in ∼40% of trials), taste (∼54%), touch (∼60%), and visual/auditory (∼79%). Of theoretical import, effects were comparable when the eliciting stimuli were orthographs or visual objects.
Collapse
|
59
|
Suess B, Brockhoff A, Meyerhof W, Hofmann T. The Odorant ( R)-Citronellal Attenuates Caffeine Bitterness by Inhibiting the Bitter Receptors TAS2R43 and TAS2R46. JOURNAL OF AGRICULTURAL AND FOOD CHEMISTRY 2018; 66:2301-2311. [PMID: 27569025 DOI: 10.1021/acs.jafc.6b03554] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/06/2023]
Abstract
Sensory studies showed the volatile fraction of lemon grass and its main constituent, the odor-active citronellal, to significantly decrease the perceived bitterness of a black tea infusion as well as caffeine solutions. Seven citronellal-related derivatives were synthesized and shown to inhibit the perceived bitterness of caffeine in a structure-dependent manner. The aldehyde function at carbon 1, the ( R)-configuration of the methyl-branched carbon 3, and a hydrophobic carbon chain were found to favor the bitter inhibitory activity of citronellal; for example, even low concentrations of 25 ppm were observed to reduce bitterness perception of caffeine solution (6 mmol/L) by 32%, whereas ( R)-citronellic acid (100 pm) showed a reduction of only 21% and ( R)-citronellol (100 pm) was completely inactive. Cell-based functional experiments, conducted with the human bitter taste receptors TAS2R7, TAS2R10, TAS2R14, TAS2R43, and TAS2R46 reported to be sensitive to caffeine, revealed ( R)-citronellal to completely block caffeine-induced calcium signals in TAS2R43-expressing cells, and, to a lesser extent, in TAS2R46-expressing cells. Stimulation of TAS2R43-expressing cells with structurally different bitter agonists identified ( R)-citronellal as a general allosteric inhibitor of TAS2R43. Further structure/activity studies indicated 3-methyl-branched aliphatic aldehydes with a carbon chain of ≥4 C atoms as best TAS2R43 antagonists. Whereas odor-taste interactions have been mainly interpreted in the literature to be caused by a central neuronal integration of odors and tastes, rather than by peripheral events at the level of reception, the findings of this study open up a new dimension regarding the interaction of the two chemical senses.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Barbara Suess
- Chair of Food Chemistry and Molecular Sensory Science , Technische Universität München , Lise-Meitner-Straße 34 , D-85354 Freising-Weihenstephan , Germany
| | - Anne Brockhoff
- Department of Molecular Genetics , German Institute of Human Nutrition (DIFE) Potsdam-Rehbrücke , Arthur-Scheunert-Allee 114-116 , 14558 Nuthetal , Germany
| | - Wolfgang Meyerhof
- Department of Molecular Genetics , German Institute of Human Nutrition (DIFE) Potsdam-Rehbrücke , Arthur-Scheunert-Allee 114-116 , 14558 Nuthetal , Germany
| | - Thomas Hofmann
- Chair of Food Chemistry and Molecular Sensory Science , Technische Universität München , Lise-Meitner-Straße 34 , D-85354 Freising-Weihenstephan , Germany
| |
Collapse
|
60
|
Proserpio C, Laureati M, Invitti C, Pagliarini E. Reduced taste responsiveness and increased food neophobia characterize obese adults. Food Qual Prefer 2018. [DOI: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2017.08.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/25/2022]
|
61
|
Rosbrook K, Erythropel HC, DeWinter TM, Falinski M, O’Malley S, Krishnan-Sarin S, Anastas PT, Zimmerman JB, Green BG. The effect of sucralose on flavor sweetness in electronic cigarettes varies between delivery devices. PLoS One 2017; 12:e0185334. [PMID: 28968411 PMCID: PMC5624589 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0185334] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/19/2017] [Accepted: 09/11/2017] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
The appeal of sweet electronic cigarette flavors makes it important to identify the chemical compounds that contribute to their sweetness. While volatile chemicals that produce sweet aromas have been identified in e-liquids, there are no published reports of sugars or artificial sweeteners in commercial e-liquids. However, the sweetener sucralose is marketed as an e-liquid additive to commercial flavors. The primary aims of the study were to determine if sucralose is delivered in sufficient concentration in the inhaled aerosol to enhance flavor sweetness, and whether the amount delivered depends on the e-liquid delivery system. Thirty-two adult smokers rated flavor intensity, sweetness, harshness and liking/disliking for 4 commercial flavors with and without sucralose (1%) using 2 e-cigarette delivery systems (cartridge and tank). Participants alternately vaped normally or with the nose pinched closed to block perception of volatile flavor components via olfaction. LC/MS was used to measure the concentration of sucralose in the e-liquid aerosols using a device that mimicked vaping. Sweetness and flavor intensity were perceived much more strongly when olfaction was permitted. The contribution of sucralose to sweetness was significant only for the cartridge system, and the chemical analysis showed that the concentration of sucralose in the aerosol was higher when the cartridge was used. Together these findings indicate that future regulation of sweet flavor additives should focus first on the volatile constituents of e-liquids with the recognition that artificial sweeteners may also contribute to flavor sweetness depending upon e-cigarette design.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kathryn Rosbrook
- The John B. Pierce Laboratory, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America
| | - Hanno C. Erythropel
- Center for Green Chemistry & Green Engineering at Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America
- Department of Chemical and Environmental Engineering, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America
| | - Tamara M. DeWinter
- Center for Green Chemistry & Green Engineering at Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America
- Department of Forestry and Environmental Studies, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America
| | - Mark Falinski
- Center for Green Chemistry & Green Engineering at Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America
- Department of Chemical and Environmental Engineering, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America
| | - Stephanie O’Malley
- Department of Psychiatry, Yale School of Medicine, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America
| | - Suchitra Krishnan-Sarin
- Department of Psychiatry, Yale School of Medicine, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America
| | - Paul T. Anastas
- Center for Green Chemistry & Green Engineering at Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America
- Department of Forestry and Environmental Studies, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America
| | - Julie B. Zimmerman
- Center for Green Chemistry & Green Engineering at Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America
- Department of Chemical and Environmental Engineering, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America
- Department of Forestry and Environmental Studies, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America
| | - Barry G. Green
- The John B. Pierce Laboratory, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America
- Department of Surgery (Otolaryngology), Yale School of Medicine, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
62
|
Pilot Experiment: The Effect of Added Flavorants on the Taste and Pleasantness of Mixtures of Glycerol and Propylene Glycol. CHEMOSENS PERCEPT 2017; 11:1-9. [PMID: 29755639 DOI: 10.1007/s12078-017-9231-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
Introduction The US Food and Drug Administration banned most "sweet" flavorants for use in cigarettes due to the concern that sweet flavors appeal to young, beginning smokers. However, many of the same flavors, including fruity and confection-associated aromas (e.g. vanilla) are still used in e-cigarettes. Sweet flavors may have a number of effects, including enhancement of the taste of other ingredients. The current work focused on the impact of model flavorants on the taste of a mixture of propylene glycol and vegetable glycerine, solvents used in most e-cigarettes and related products. Methods A device delivered mixtures of propylene glycol and vegetable glycerine into the mouth in parallel with puffs of clean air (control) or odorized air. Aromas included two "fruity" esters ("pineapple" and "banana"), two confection-associated aromas ("vanilla" and "caramel/malty"), menthol (not a "sweet" aroma, but commonly used in e-cigarettes), and a "burnt" aroma not expected enhance flavor. Twenty young adults, aged 18-25, rated the sweetness, bitterness, and pleasantness of all stimuli (within-subjects design). Results Both fruity aromas significantly enhanced sweetness, both confection-associated aromas significantly enhanced pleasantness, and the caramel/malty aroma significantly reduced bitterness. Menthol and the "burnt" aroma had no measurable effects on the taste of solvent mixtures. Conclusion Some flavorants modulated the taste of solvents commonly used in e-cigarettes in ways consistent with an enhanced sensory profile. Implications If similar effects occur in actual products, improved flavor profiles could facilitate continued use, particularly in non-smokers experimenting with e-cigarettes and related products.
Collapse
|
63
|
Proserpio C, Laureati M, Invitti C, Cattaneo C, Pagliarini E. BMI and gender related differences in cross-modal interaction and liking of sensory stimuli. Food Qual Prefer 2017. [DOI: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.09.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
|
64
|
Itobe T, Kumazawa K. Taste-aroma Interactions in Lemon-flavored Model Beverages: Influence of Sweeteners on Aroma Perception and In Vivo Aroma Release. FOOD SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY RESEARCH 2017. [DOI: 10.3136/fstr.23.51] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/03/2022]
|
65
|
Maier JX. Single-neuron responses to intraoral delivery of odor solutions in primary olfactory and gustatory cortex. J Neurophysiol 2016; 117:1293-1304. [PMID: 28003413 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00802.2016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/11/2016] [Revised: 11/30/2016] [Accepted: 12/15/2016] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Smell plays a major role in our perception of food. Odorants released inside the mouth during consumption are combined with taste and texture qualities of a food to guide flavor preference learning and food choice behavior. Here, we built on recent physiological findings that implicated primary sensory cortex in multisensory flavor processing. Specifically, we used extracellular recordings in awake rats to characterize responses of single neurons in primary olfactory (OC) and gustatory cortex (GC) to intraoral delivery of odor solutions and compare odor responses to taste and plain water responses. The data reveal responses to olfactory, oral somatosensory, and gustatory qualities of intraoral stimuli in both OC and GC. Moreover, modality-specific responses overlap in time, indicating temporal convergence of multisensory, flavor-related inputs. The results extend previous work suggesting a role for primary OC in mediating influences of taste on smell that characterize flavor perception and point to an integral role for GC in olfactory processing.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Food perception is inherently multisensory, taking into account taste, smell, and texture qualities. However, the neural mechanisms underlying flavor perception remain unknown. Recording neural activity directly from the rat brain while animals consume multisensory flavor stimuli, we demonstrate that information about odor, taste, and mouthfeel of food converges on primary taste and smell cortex. The results suggest that processing of naturalistic, multisensory information involves an interacting network of primary sensory areas.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joost X Maier
- Department of Neurobiology and Anatomy, Wake Forest School of Medicine, Winston Salem, North Carolina
| |
Collapse
|
66
|
Arvisenet G, Guichard E, Ballester J. Taste-aroma interaction in model wines: Effect of training and expertise. Food Qual Prefer 2016. [DOI: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.05.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
67
|
Amsellem S, Ohla K. Perceived Odor-Taste Congruence Influences Intensity and Pleasantness Differently. Chem Senses 2016; 41:677-84. [PMID: 27384192 DOI: 10.1093/chemse/bjw078] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The role of congruence in cross-modal interactions has received little attention. In most experiments involving cross-modal pairs, congruence is conceived of as a binary process according to which cross-modal pairs are categorized as perceptually and/or semantically matching or mismatching. The present study investigated whether odor-taste congruence can be perceived gradually and whether congruence impacts other facets of subjective experience, that is, intensity, pleasantness, and familiarity. To address these questions, we presented food odorants (chicken, orange, and 3 mixtures of the 2) and tastants (savory-salty and sour-sweet) in pairs varying in congruence. Participants were to report the perceived congruence of the pairs along with intensity, pleasantness, and familiarity. We found that participants could perceive distinct congruence levels, thereby favoring a multilevel account of congruence perception. In addition, familiarity and pleasantness followed the same pattern as the congruence while intensity was highest for the most congruent and the most incongruent pairs whereas intensities of the intermediary-congruent pairs were reduced. Principal component analysis revealed that pleasantness and familiarity form one dimension of the phenomenological experience of odor-taste pairs that was orthogonal to intensity. The results bear implications for the understanding the behavioral underpinnings of perseverance of habitual food choices.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sherlley Amsellem
- Psychophysiology of Food Perception, German Institute of Human Nutrition Potsdam-Rehbruecke (DIfE), Arthur-Scheunert-Allee 114-116, 14558 Nuthetal, Germany
| | - Kathrin Ohla
- Psychophysiology of Food Perception, German Institute of Human Nutrition Potsdam-Rehbruecke (DIfE), Arthur-Scheunert-Allee 114-116, 14558 Nuthetal, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
68
|
|
69
|
Delime P, O'Mahony K, Desforges N, Taylor AJ, Hort J. Comparing the relative sensitivity of ortho- and retronasal perception of a strawberry flavour model using omission testing. FLAVOUR FRAG J 2016. [DOI: 10.1002/ffj.3326] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Perrine Delime
- Sensory Science Centre; University of Nottingham, Sutton Bonington Campus; Loughborough LE12 5RD UK
| | - Kate O'Mahony
- Sensory Science Centre; University of Nottingham, Sutton Bonington Campus; Loughborough LE12 5RD UK
| | - Neil Desforges
- WALTHAM Centre for Pet Nutrition, Mars Petcare; Waltham-on-the-Wolds LE14 4RT UK
| | - Andrew J. Taylor
- WALTHAM Centre for Pet Nutrition, Mars Petcare; Waltham-on-the-Wolds LE14 4RT UK
| | - Joanne Hort
- Sensory Science Centre; University of Nottingham, Sutton Bonington Campus; Loughborough LE12 5RD UK
| |
Collapse
|
70
|
Isogai T, Wise PM. The Effects of Odor Quality and Temporal Asynchrony on Modulation of Taste Intensity by Retronasal Odor. Chem Senses 2016; 41:557-66. [PMID: 27143280 DOI: 10.1093/chemse/bjw059] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The experiments had 2 main goals: 1) to add to the sparse literature on how retronasal aromas interact with bitter tastes, and 2) to determine whether modulation of taste intensity by aroma depends on temporal contiguity, as one might expect if flavor interactions depend on cross-modal binding (similar to object perception in other modalities). An olfactometer-gustometer allowed independent oral presentation of odorized air and liquid samples. First, using simultaneous presentation of odors and tastes (Experiments 1a-d) we found that a "sweet-smelling" aroma enhanced the rated sweetness of sucrose and decreased the rated bitterness of sucrose octaacetate (SOA), and that a "bitter-smelling" aroma enhanced the bitterness of SOA and decreased the sweetness of sucrose. Thus, with respect to effects on taste intensity, sweet and bitter aromas mimicked mixture-interactions between sweet and bitter tastes under current conditions. Next (Experiment 2), both odors were again paired with both tastes, with a parametric manipulation of odor onset. Odor presentation ranged from before taste delivery to after taste delivery. Enhancement of taste intensity was greatest with simultaneous onset, and greatly attenuated with offsets of 1s. These results are consistent with the idea that enhancement of taste by retronasal aroma depends on a temporal binding window like many other cross-modal interactions. The effects of temporal offsets on suppression of taste were inconclusive. These findings are discussed within the context of past work on odor-taste interactions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tomoyuki Isogai
- Product Development Department, Megmilk Snow Brand Co., Ltd., 1-1-2 Minamidai, 350-1165 Kawagoe, Saitama, Japan and
| | - Paul M Wise
- Monell Chemical Senses Center, 3500 Market Street, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA
| |
Collapse
|
71
|
Individual Differences in Retronasal Odor Responsiveness: Effects of Aging and Concurrent Taste. CHEMOSENS PERCEPT 2016. [DOI: 10.1007/s12078-016-9206-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
72
|
Niimi J, Overington AR, Silcock P, Bremer PJ, Delahunty CM. Cross-modal taste and aroma interactions: Cheese flavour perception and changes in flavour character in multicomponent mixtures. Food Qual Prefer 2016. [DOI: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2015.08.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
73
|
|
74
|
Cross-modal interactions for custard desserts differ in obese and normal weight Italian women. Appetite 2016; 100:203-9. [PMID: 26911260 DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.02.033] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/15/2015] [Revised: 01/18/2016] [Accepted: 02/16/2016] [Indexed: 02/02/2023]
Abstract
The effects of variation in odors and thickening agents on sensory properties and acceptability of a model custard dessert were investigated in normal weight and obese women. Subjects rated their liking and the intensity of sensory properties (sweetness, vanilla and butter flavors, and creaminess) of 3 block samples (the first varied in vanilla aroma, the second varied in butter aroma and the third varied in xanthan gum). Significant differences were found in acceptability and intensity ratings in relation to body mass index. The addition of butter aroma in the custard was the most effective way to elicit odor-taste, odor-flavor and odor-texture interactions in obese women. In this group, butter aroma, signaling energy dense products, increased the perception of sweetness, vanilla flavor and creaminess, which are all desirable properties in a custard, while maintaining a high liking degree. Understanding cross-modal interactions in relation to nutritional status is interesting in order to develop new food products with reduced sugar and fat, that are still satisfying for the consumer. This could have important implications to reduce caloric intake and tackle the obesity epidemic.
Collapse
|
75
|
Sun X, Veldhuizen MG, Babbs AE, Sinha R, Small DM. Perceptual and Brain Response to Odors Is Associated with Body Mass Index and Postprandial Total Ghrelin Reactivity to a Meal. Chem Senses 2016; 41:233-48. [PMID: 26826114 DOI: 10.1093/chemse/bjv081] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 12/17/2015] [Indexed: 12/11/2022] Open
Abstract
Animal studies have shown that olfactory sensitivity is greater when fasted than when fed. However, human research has generated inconsistent results. One possible explanation for these conflicting findings is metabolic health. Many metabolic peptides, including ghrelin, are moderated by adiposity and influence olfaction and olfactory-guided behaviors. We tested whether the effect of a meal on the perceived intensity of suprathreshold chemosensory stimuli is influenced by body mass index and/or metabolic response to a meal. We found that overweight or obese (n = 13), but not healthy weight (n = 20) subjects perceived odors, but not flavored solutions, as more intense when hungry than when sated. This effect was correlated with reduced postprandial total ghrelin suppression (n = 23) and differential brain response to odors in the cerebellum, as measured with functional magnetic resonance imaging. In contrast, it was unrelated to circulating leptin, glucose, insulin, triglycerides, or free fatty acids; or to odor pleasantness or sniffing (n = 24). These findings demonstrate that the effect of a meal on suprathreshold odor intensity perception is associated with metabolic measures such as body weight and total ghrelin reactivity, supporting endocrine influences on olfactory perception.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xue Sun
- Yale Interdepartmental Neuroscience Program, Yale School of Medicine, 333 Cedar Street, New Haven, CT 06510, USA, The John B. Pierce Laboratory, 290 Congress Avenue, New Haven, CT 06519, USA,
| | - Maria G Veldhuizen
- The John B. Pierce Laboratory, 290 Congress Avenue, New Haven, CT 06519, USA, Department of Psychiatry, Yale School of Medicine, 300 George Street, New Haven, CT 06511, USA and
| | - Amanda E Babbs
- The John B. Pierce Laboratory, 290 Congress Avenue, New Haven, CT 06519, USA
| | - Rajita Sinha
- Department of Psychiatry, Yale School of Medicine, 300 George Street, New Haven, CT 06511, USA and
| | - Dana M Small
- Yale Interdepartmental Neuroscience Program, Yale School of Medicine, 333 Cedar Street, New Haven, CT 06510, USA, The John B. Pierce Laboratory, 290 Congress Avenue, New Haven, CT 06519, USA, Department of Psychiatry, Yale School of Medicine, 300 George Street, New Haven, CT 06511, USA and Department of Psychology, Yale University, 2 Hillhouse Avenue, New Haven, CT 06520, USA
| |
Collapse
|
76
|
|
77
|
Nishimura T, Goto S, Miura K, Takakura Y, Egusa AS, Wakabayashi H. Umami compounds enhance the intensity of retronasal sensation of aromas from model chicken soups. Food Chem 2015; 196:577-83. [PMID: 26593530 DOI: 10.1016/j.foodchem.2015.09.036] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/31/2015] [Revised: 08/18/2015] [Accepted: 09/10/2015] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
We examined the influence of taste compounds on retronasal aroma sensation using a model chicken soup. The aroma intensity of a reconstituted flavour solution from which glutamic acid (Glu), inosine 5'-monophosphate (IMP), or phosphate was omitted was significantly lower (p<0.05) than that of the model soup. The aroma intensity of 0.4% NaCl solution containing the aroma chicken model (ACM) with added Glu and IMP was significantly higher (p<0.05) than that of 0.4% NaCl solution containing only ACM. The quantitative analyses showed that adding monosodium glutamate (MSG) to aqueous aroma solution containing only ACM enhanced the intensity of retronasal aroma sensation by 2.5-folds with increasing MSG concentration from 0% to 0.3%. Sensation intensity using an umami solution with added MSG and IMP was significantly higher than that with only MSG when the MSG concentration was 0.05%, 0.075%, or 0.1%. However, it plateaued when MSG concentration was beyond 0.3%.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Toshihide Nishimura
- Faculty of Food Science, Department of Applied Life Science, Nippon Veterinary and Life Science University, 1-7-1 Kyonancho, Musashino-shi, Tokyo 180-8602, Japan.
| | - Shingo Goto
- Faculty of Food Science, Department of Applied Life Science, Nippon Veterinary and Life Science University, 1-7-1 Kyonancho, Musashino-shi, Tokyo 180-8602, Japan
| | - Kyo Miura
- Faculty of Food Science, Department of Applied Life Science, Nippon Veterinary and Life Science University, 1-7-1 Kyonancho, Musashino-shi, Tokyo 180-8602, Japan
| | - Yukiko Takakura
- Institute of Food Sciences & Technologies, Ajinomoto Co., Inc., 1-1, Suzuki-cho, Kawasaki-ku, Kawasaki-shi 210-8681, Japan
| | - Ai S Egusa
- Faculty of Food Science, Department of Applied Life Science, Nippon Veterinary and Life Science University, 1-7-1 Kyonancho, Musashino-shi, Tokyo 180-8602, Japan
| | - Hidehiko Wakabayashi
- Institute of Food Sciences & Technologies, Ajinomoto Co., Inc., 1-1, Suzuki-cho, Kawasaki-ku, Kawasaki-shi 210-8681, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
78
|
Niimi J, Eddy AI, Overington AR, Silcock P, Bremer PJ, Delahunty CM. Sensory Interactions between Cheese Aroma and Taste. J SENS STUD 2015. [DOI: 10.1111/joss.12155] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Jun Niimi
- Sensory Science Research Centre, Department of Food Science; University of Otago; Dunedin New Zealand
- CSIRO Animal; Food and Health Sciences; North Ryde NSW Australia
| | - Andrew I. Eddy
- CSIRO Animal; Food and Health Sciences; North Ryde NSW Australia
| | - Amy R. Overington
- Fonterra Research and Development Centre; Palmerston North New Zealand
| | - Patrick Silcock
- Sensory Science Research Centre, Department of Food Science; University of Otago; Dunedin New Zealand
| | - Phil J. Bremer
- Sensory Science Research Centre, Department of Food Science; University of Otago; Dunedin New Zealand
| | | |
Collapse
|
79
|
Abstract
The reported effects of temperature on sweet taste in humans have generally been small and inconsistent. Here, we describe 3 experiments that follow up a recent finding that cooling from 37 to 21 °C does not reduce the initial sweetness of sucrose but increases sweet taste adaptation. In experiment 1, subjects rated the sweetness of sucrose, glucose, and fructose solutions at 5-41 °C by dipping the tongue tip into the solutions after 0-, 3-, or 10-s pre-exposures to the same solutions or to H2O; experiment 2 compared the effects of temperature on the sweetness of 3 artificial sweeteners (sucralose, aspartame, and saccharin); and experiment 3 employed a flow-controlled gustometer to rule out the possibility the effects of temperature in the preceding experiments were unique to dipping the tongue into a still taste solution. The results (i) confirmed that mild cooling does not attenuate sweetness but can increase sweet taste adaptation; (ii) demonstrated that cooling to 5-12 °C can directly reduce sweetness intensity; and (iii) showed that both effects vary across stimuli. These findings have implications for the TRPM5 hypothesis of thermal effects on sweet taste and raise the possibility that temperature also affects an earlier step in the T1R2-T1R3 transduction cascade.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Barry G Green
- The John B. Pierce Laboratory, 290 Congress Avenue, New Haven, CT 06519, USA and The Department of Surgery (Otolaryngology), Yale School of Medicine, 333 Cedar Street, New Haven, CT 06510, USA
| | - Danielle Nachtigal
- The John B. Pierce Laboratory, 290 Congress Avenue, New Haven, CT 06519, USA and
| |
Collapse
|
80
|
Emorine M, Septier C, Andriot I, Martin C, Salles C, Thomas-Danguin T. Combined heterogeneous distribution of salt and aroma in food enhances salt perception. Food Funct 2015; 6:1449-59. [DOI: 10.1039/c4fo01067a] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
Combining salt and aroma heterogeneous distribution in hot snacks improves saltiness perception and helps to maintain consumer liking. This approach is relevant for lowering salt content in food.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marion Emorine
- CNRS
- UMR6265 Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation
- F-21000 Dijon
- France
- INRA
| | - Chantal Septier
- CNRS
- UMR6265 Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation
- F-21000 Dijon
- France
- INRA
| | - Isabelle Andriot
- CNRS
- UMR6265 Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation
- F-21000 Dijon
- France
- INRA
| | - Christophe Martin
- CNRS
- UMR6265 Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation
- F-21000 Dijon
- France
- INRA
| | - Christian Salles
- CNRS
- UMR6265 Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation
- F-21000 Dijon
- France
- INRA
| | | |
Collapse
|
81
|
|
82
|
Tomita S, Terao Y, Hatano T, Nishimura R. Subtotal glossectomy preserving half the tongue base prevents taste disorder in patients with tongue cancer. Int J Oral Maxillofac Surg 2014; 43:1042-6. [DOI: 10.1016/j.ijom.2014.02.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/24/2013] [Revised: 01/06/2014] [Accepted: 02/17/2014] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
|
83
|
|
84
|
Niimi J, Eddy AI, Overington AR, Heenan SP, Silcock P, Bremer PJ, Delahunty CM. Aroma–taste interactions between a model cheese aroma and five basic tastes in solution. Food Qual Prefer 2014. [DOI: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2013.05.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
|
85
|
Gotow N, Kobayashi T, Kobayakawa T. Retronasal aroma allows feature extraction from taste of a traditional Japanese confection. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2013. [DOI: 10.1186/2044-7248-2-26] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
Abstract
Abstract
Background
Common foods consist of several taste qualities. Consumers perceive intensity of a particular taste quality after noticing it among other taste qualities when they eat common foods. We supposed that while one is eating the facility for noticing a taste quality present in a common food will differ among taste qualities which compose the common food. We, therefore, proposed a new measurement scale for food perception named ‘noticeability’. Furthermore, we found that consumers’ food perceptions to common foods were modified by retronasal aroma. In this study, in order to examine whether retronasal aroma affects the relationship between noticeability and perceived intensity for taste, we evaluated participants for noticeability and perceived intensity of five fundamental taste qualities (sweetness, saltiness, sourness, bitterness, and umami) under open and closed nostril conditions using one of the most popular traditional Japanese confections called ‘yokan’.
Results
The taste quality showed that the highest noticeability and perceived intensity among five fundamental taste qualities for yokan was sweetness, independent of the nostril condition. For sweetness, a significant decrease of correlation between noticeability and perceived intensity was observed in response to retronasal aroma. On the other hand, for umami, correlation between noticeability and perceived intensity significantly increased with retronasal aroma.
Conclusions
As the retronasal aroma of yokan allowed feature extraction from taste by Japanese consumers, we reconfirmed that consumers’ food perceptions were modified by the retronasal aroma of a common food.
Collapse
|
86
|
Auffarth B. Understanding smell—The olfactory stimulus problem. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2013; 37:1667-79. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.06.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 53] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/30/2012] [Revised: 05/09/2013] [Accepted: 06/13/2013] [Indexed: 01/30/2023]
|
87
|
Small DM, Veldhuizen MG, Green B. Sensory neuroscience: taste responses in primary olfactory cortex. Curr Biol 2013; 23:R157-9. [PMID: 23428327 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.12.036] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
A new electrophysiological study in rodents demonstrates that taste-odor convergence occurs in posterior piriform olfactory cortex and calls for a reformulation of classic models of the central representation of flavor.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dana M Small
- The John B Pierce Laboratory, 290 Congress Avenue, New Haven, CT 06519, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
88
|
Abstract
Food perception and preference formation relies on the ability to combine information from both the taste and olfactory systems. Accordingly, psychophysical investigations in humans and behavioral work in animals has shown that the taste system plays an integral role in odor processing. However, the neural basis for the influence of taste (gustation) on odor (olfaction) remains essentially unknown. Here we tested the hypothesis that gustatory influence on olfactory processing occurs at the level of primary olfactory cortex. We recorded activity from single neurons in posterior olfactory (piriform) cortex (pPC) of awake rats while presenting basic taste solutions directly to the tongue. A significant portion of pPC neurons proved to respond selectively to taste stimuli. These taste responses were significantly reduced by blockade of the gustatory epithelium, were unaffected by blockade of the olfactory epithelium, and were independent of respiration behavior. In contrast, responses to olfactory stimuli, recorded from the same area, were reduced by nasal epithelial deciliation and phase-locked to the respiration cycle. These results identify pPC as a likely site for gustatory influences on olfactory processing, which play an important role in food perception and preference formation.
Collapse
|
89
|
|
90
|
Small DM. Flavor is in the brain. Physiol Behav 2012; 107:540-52. [PMID: 22542991 DOI: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.04.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 163] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/17/2012] [Revised: 04/10/2012] [Accepted: 04/12/2012] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
91
|
LEKSRISOMPONG P, LOPETCHARAT K, GUTHRIE B, DRAKE M. DESCRIPTIVE ANALYSIS OF CARBONATED REGULAR AND DIET LEMON-LIME BEVERAGES. J SENS STUD 2012. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1745-459x.2012.00389.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
|
92
|
Bojanowski V, Hummel T. Retronasal perception of odors. Physiol Behav 2012; 107:484-7. [PMID: 22425641 DOI: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.03.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 68] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/30/2012] [Revised: 03/01/2012] [Accepted: 03/02/2012] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
We perceive odors orthonasally during sniffing; in contrast, we perceive odors retronasally during eating when they enter the nose through the pharynx. There are clear differences between orthonasal and retronasal olfaction in neuronal processing and perception, so that these two pathways convey two distinct sensory signals. The perception of foods is based on the interaction between ortho- and retronasal smell, taste, trigeminal activation and texture, so it is difficult to investigate one of these factors in isolation. Specific clinical aspects include effects of retronasal olfaction on satiation and swallowing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Viola Bojanowski
- Smell and Taste Clinic, Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Technical University of Dresden Medical School, Fetscherstrasse 74, 01307 Dresden, Germany
| | | |
Collapse
|