1
|
Causse M, Parmentier FB, Mouratille D, Thibaut D, Kisselenko M, Fabre E. Busy and confused? High risk of missed alerts in the cockpit: an electrophysiological study. Brain Res 2022; 1793:148035. [DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2022.148035] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/05/2022] [Revised: 07/08/2022] [Accepted: 07/27/2022] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
2
|
Reward magnitude enhances early attentional processing of auditory stimuli. COGNITIVE, AFFECTIVE & BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE 2022; 22:268-280. [PMID: 34811706 DOI: 10.3758/s13415-021-00962-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 10/06/2021] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Reward associations are known to shape the brain's processing of visual stimuli, but relatively less is known about how reward associations impact the processing of auditory stimuli. We leveraged the high-temporal resolution of electroencephalography (EEG) and event-related potentials (ERPs) to investigate the influence of low- and high-magnitude stimulus-reward associations in an auditory oddball task. We associated fast, correct detection of certain auditory target stimuli with larger monetary rewards, and other auditory targets with smaller rewards. We found enhanced attentional processing of the more highly rewarded target stimuli, as evidenced by faster behavioral detection of those stimuli compared with lower-rewarded stimuli. Neurally, higher-reward associations enhanced the early sensory processing of auditory targets. Targets associated with higher-magnitude rewards had higher amplitude N1 and mismatch negativity (MMN) ERP components than targets associated with lower-magnitude rewards. Reward did not impact the latency of these early components. Higher-reward magnitude also decreased the latency and increased the amplitude of the longer-latency P3 component, suggesting that reward also can enhance the final processing stages of auditory target stimuli. These results provide insight into how the sensory and attentional neural processing of auditory stimuli is modulated by stimulus-reward associations and the magnitude of those associations, with higher-magnitude reward associations yielding enhanced auditory processing at both early and late stages compared with lower-magnitude reward associations.
Collapse
|
3
|
Kim HK, Blumberger DM, Daskalakis ZJ. Neurophysiological Biomarkers in Schizophrenia-P50, Mismatch Negativity, and TMS-EMG and TMS-EEG. Front Psychiatry 2020; 11:795. [PMID: 32848953 PMCID: PMC7426515 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00795] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/23/2020] [Accepted: 07/24/2020] [Indexed: 12/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Impaired early auditory processing is a well characterized finding in schizophrenia that is theorized to contribute to clinical symptoms, cognitive impairment, and social dysfunction in patients. Two neurophysiological measures of early auditory processing, P50 gating ("P50") and mismatch negativity (MMN), which measure sensory gating and detection of change in auditory stimuli, respectively, are consistently shown to be impaired in patients with schizophrenia. Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) may also be a potential method by which sensory processing can be assessed, since TMS paradigms can be used to measure GABAB-mediated cortical inhibition that is linked with sensory gating. In this review, we examine the potential of P50, MMN and two TMS paradigms, cortical silent period (CSP) and long-interval intracortical inhibition (LICI), as endophenotypes as well as their ability to be used as predictive markers for interventions targeted at cognitive and psychosocial functioning. Studies consistently support a link between MMN, P50, and cognitive dysfunction, with robust evidence for a link between MMN and psychosocial functioning in schizophrenia as well. Importantly, studies have demonstrated that MMN can be used to predict performance in social and cognitive training tasks. A growing body of studies also supports the potential of MMN to be used as an endophenotype, and future studies are needed to determine if MMN can be used as an endophenotype specifically in schizophrenia. P50, however, has weaker evidence supporting its use as an endophenotype. While CSP and LICI are not as extensively investigated, growing evidence is supporting their potential to be used as an endophenotype in schizophrenia. Future studies that assess the ability of P50, MMN, and TMS neurophysiological measures to predict performance in cognitive and social training programs may identify markers that inform clinical decisions in the treatment of neurocognitive impairments in schizophrenia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Helena K Kim
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Daniel M Blumberger
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada.,Temerty Centre for Therapeutic Brain Intervention, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Zafiris J Daskalakis
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada.,Temerty Centre for Therapeutic Brain Intervention, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Cycowicz YM. Orienting and memory to unexpected and/or unfamiliar visual events in children and adults. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2019; 36:100615. [PMID: 30685577 PMCID: PMC6969219 DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100615] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/17/2018] [Revised: 01/10/2019] [Accepted: 01/11/2019] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
For children, new experiences occur very often, and learning to differentiate between old and new events is a fundamental process necessary for appropriate reactions to stimuli. Thus the present study is concerned with maturation of brain responses to repeated novel events. We examined the effect of repetition of familiar (meaningful) and unfamiliar (meaningless) symbols on the event-related-potentials (ERPs) recorded during novelty oddball and recognition memory tasks from children, adolescents and young adults. During the novelty oddball task, repetition of the familiar symbols elicited a reduction in the novelty P3 in the ERPs of all age groups, while repetition of the unfamiliar symbols elicited a reduction in novelty P3 amplitude only in children. As expected, recognition memory performance improved with age and was better for familiar than unfamiliar symbols. For all age groups, ERPs to correctly recognized familiar old symbols elicited a larger positivity than ERPs to correctly identified new symbols, indicating a reliable memory effect. However, ERPs to unfamiliar old and new symbols did not differ in adults and adolescents but did differ in children. The data suggest that children process familiar visual symbols in a similar fashion to that of adults, and that children process unfamiliar symbols differently from adults.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yael M Cycowicz
- Division of Child and Adolescents Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University Vagelos College of Physicians and Surgeons, New York, NY, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Jungnickel E, Gramann K. Mobile Brain/Body Imaging (MoBI) of Physical Interaction with Dynamically Moving Objects. Front Hum Neurosci 2016; 10:306. [PMID: 27445747 PMCID: PMC4921999 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00306] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/08/2016] [Accepted: 06/03/2016] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
The non-invasive recording and analysis of human brain activity during active movements in natural working conditions is a central challenge in Neuroergonomics research. Existing brain imaging approaches do not allow for an investigation of brain dynamics during active behavior because their sensors cannot follow the movement of the signal source. However, movements that require the operator to react fast and to adapt to a dynamically changing environment occur frequently in working environments like assembly-line work, construction trade, health care, but also outside the working environment like in team sports. Overcoming the restrictions of existing imaging methods would allow for deeper insights into neurocognitive processes at workplaces that require physical interactions and thus could help to adapt work settings to the user. To investigate the brain dynamics accompanying rapid volatile movements we used a visual oddball paradigm where participants had to react to color changes either with a simple button press or by physically pointing towards a moving target. Using a mobile brain/body imaging approach (MoBI) including independent component analysis (ICA) with subsequent backprojection of cluster activity allowed for systematically describing the contribution of brain and non-brain sources to the sensor signal. The results demonstrate that visual event-related potentials (ERPs) can be analyzed for simple button presses and physical pointing responses and that it is possible to quantify the contribution of brain processes, muscle activity and eye movements to the signal recorded at the sensor level even for fast volatile arm movements with strong jerks. Using MoBI in naturalistic working environments can thus help to analyze brain dynamics in natural working conditions and help improving unhealthy or inefficient work settings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Evelyn Jungnickel
- Department of Psychology and Ergonomics, Biological Psychology and Neuroergonomics, Institute of Psychology and Ergonomics, Berlin Institute of Technology Berlin, Germany
| | - Klaus Gramann
- Department of Psychology and Ergonomics, Biological Psychology and Neuroergonomics, Institute of Psychology and Ergonomics, Berlin Institute of TechnologyBerlin, Germany; Center for Advanced Neurological Engineering, University of CaliforniaSan Diego, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Distinguishing shyness and sociability in children: An event-related potential study. J Exp Child Psychol 2016; 142:291-311. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.08.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/30/2015] [Revised: 07/28/2015] [Accepted: 08/23/2015] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
|
7
|
Zhang C, Pugh KR, Mencl WE, Molfese PJ, Frost SJ, Magnuson JS, Peng G, Wang WSY. Functionally integrated neural processing of linguistic and talker information: An event-related fMRI and ERP study. Neuroimage 2015; 124:536-549. [PMID: 26343322 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.064] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/25/2014] [Revised: 08/15/2015] [Accepted: 08/28/2015] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Speech signals contain information of both linguistic content and a talker's voice. Conventionally, linguistic and talker processing are thought to be mediated by distinct neural systems in the left and right hemispheres respectively, but there is growing evidence that linguistic and talker processing interact in many ways. Previous studies suggest that talker-related vocal tract changes are processed integrally with phonetic changes in the bilateral posterior superior temporal gyrus/superior temporal sulcus (STG/STS), because the vocal tract parameter influences the perception of phonetic information. It is yet unclear whether the bilateral STG is also activated by the integral processing of another parameter - pitch, which influences the perception of lexical tone information and is related to talker differences in tone languages. In this study, we conducted separate functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and event-related potential (ERP) experiments to examine the spatial and temporal loci of interactions of lexical tone and talker-related pitch processing in Cantonese. We found that the STG was activated bilaterally during the processing of talker changes when listeners attended to lexical tone changes in the stimuli and during the processing of lexical tone changes when listeners attended to talker changes, suggesting that lexical tone and talker processing are functionally integrated in the bilateral STG. It extends the previous study, providing evidence for a general neural mechanism of integral phonetic and talker processing in the bilateral STG. The ERP results show interactions of lexical tone and talker processing 500-800ms after auditory word onset (a simultaneous posterior P3b and a frontal negativity). Moreover, there is some asymmetry in the interaction, such that unattended talker changes affect linguistic processing more than vice versa, which may be related to the ambiguity that talker changes cause in speech perception and/or attention bias to talker changes. Our findings have implications for understanding the neural encoding of linguistic and talker information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Caicai Zhang
- Department of Chinese and Bilingual Studies, The Hong Kong Polytechnic University, Hong Kong, China; Key Laboratory of Human-Machine Intelligence-Synergy Systems, Shenzhen Institutes of Advanced Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shenzhen, China.
| | - Kenneth R Pugh
- Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, CT, USA; Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT, USA; Department of Linguistics, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA
| | - W Einar Mencl
- Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, CT, USA; Department of Linguistics, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA
| | - Peter J Molfese
- Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, CT, USA; Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT, USA
| | | | - James S Magnuson
- Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, CT, USA; Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT, USA
| | - Gang Peng
- Key Laboratory of Human-Machine Intelligence-Synergy Systems, Shenzhen Institutes of Advanced Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shenzhen, China; CUHK-PKU-UST Joint Research Centre for Language and Human Complexity, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China; Department of Linguistics and Modern Languages, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China.
| | - William S-Y Wang
- Key Laboratory of Human-Machine Intelligence-Synergy Systems, Shenzhen Institutes of Advanced Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shenzhen, China; CUHK-PKU-UST Joint Research Centre for Language and Human Complexity, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China; Department of Linguistics and Modern Languages, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China; Department of Electronic Engineering, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Gamble ML, Woldorff MG. Rapid Context-based Identification of Target Sounds in an Auditory Scene. J Cogn Neurosci 2015; 27:1675-84. [PMID: 25848684 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_00814] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
To make sense of our dynamic and complex auditory environment, we must be able to parse the sensory input into usable parts and pick out relevant sounds from all the potentially distracting auditory information. Although it is unclear exactly how we accomplish this difficult task, Gamble and Woldorff [Gamble, M. L., & Woldorff, M. G. The temporal cascade of neural processes underlying target detection and attentional processing during auditory search. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991), 2014] recently reported an ERP study of an auditory target-search task in a temporally and spatially distributed, rapidly presented, auditory scene. They reported an early, differential, bilateral activation (beginning at 60 msec) between feature-deviating target stimuli and physically equivalent feature-deviating nontargets, reflecting a rapid target detection process. This was followed shortly later (at 130 msec) by the lateralized N2ac ERP activation, that reflects the focusing of auditory spatial attention toward the target sound and parallels the attentional-shifting processes widely studied in vision. Here we directly examined the early, bilateral, target-selective effect to better understand its nature and functional role. Participants listened to midline-presented sounds that included target and nontarget stimuli that were randomly either embedded in a brief rapid stream or presented alone. The results indicate that this early bilateral effect results from a template for the target that utilizes its feature deviancy within a stream to enable rapid identification. Moreover, individual-differences analysis showed that the size of this effect was larger for participants with faster RTs. The findings support the hypothesis that our auditory attentional systems can implement and utilize a context-based relational template for a target sound, making use of additional auditory information in the environment when needing to rapidly detect a relevant sound.
Collapse
|
9
|
Stimulus complexity effects on the event-related potentials to task-irrelevant stimuli. Biol Psychol 2013; 94:82-9. [DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.05.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/19/2012] [Revised: 04/27/2013] [Accepted: 05/13/2013] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
|
10
|
Reiche M, Hartwigsen G, Widmann A, Saur D, Schröger E, Bendixen A. Involuntary attentional capture by speech and non-speech deviations: A combined behavioral–event-related potential study. Brain Res 2013; 1490:153-60. [DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.10.055] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/06/2012] [Revised: 10/25/2012] [Accepted: 10/26/2012] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
11
|
Jahshan C, Cadenhead KS, Rissling AJ, Kirihara K, Braff DL, Light GA. Automatic sensory information processing abnormalities across the illness course of schizophrenia. Psychol Med 2012; 42:85-97. [PMID: 21740622 PMCID: PMC3193558 DOI: 10.1017/s0033291711001061] [Citation(s) in RCA: 141] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Deficits in automatic sensory discrimination, as indexed by a reduction in the mismatch negativity (MMN) and P3a event-related potential amplitudes, are well documented in chronic schizophrenia. However, MMN and P3a have not been sufficiently studied early in the course of psychotic illness. The present study aimed to investigate MMN, P3a and reorienting negativity (RON) across the course of schizophrenia. METHOD MMN, P3a, and RON were assessed in 118 subjects across four groups: (1) individuals at risk for psychosis (n=26); (2) recent-onset patients (n=31); (3) chronic patients (n=33); and (4) normal controls (n=28) using a duration-deviant auditory oddball paradigm. RESULTS Frontocentral deficits in MMN and P3a were present in all patient groups. The at-risk group's MMN and P3a amplitudes were intermediate to those of the control and recent-onset groups. The recent-onset and chronic patients, but not the at-risk subjects, showed significant RON amplitude reductions, relative to the control group. Associations between MMN, P3a, RON and psychosocial functioning were present in the chronic patients. In the at-risk subjects, P3a and RON deficits were significantly associated with higher levels of negative symptoms. CONCLUSIONS Abnormalities in the automatic processes of sensory discrimination, orienting and reorienting of attention are evident in the early phases of schizophrenia and raise the possibility of progressive worsening across stages of the illness. The finding that MMN and P3a, but not RON, were reduced before psychosis onset supports the continued examination of these components as potential early biomarkers of schizophrenia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carol Jahshan
- Mental Illness Research, Education and Clinical Center, Veterans Affairs Greater Los Angeles Healthcare System, Los Angeles, California
| | - Kristin S. Cadenhead
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
| | - Anthony J. Rissling
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
| | - Kenji Kirihara
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
| | - David L. Braff
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
| | - Gregory A. Light
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Brown CR, Barry RJ, Clarke AR. ERPs to infrequent auditory stimuli in two- and three-stimulus versions of the inter-modal oddball task. Int J Psychophysiol 2009; 74:174-82. [DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.08.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/15/2009] [Revised: 06/02/2009] [Accepted: 08/27/2009] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
|
13
|
Sawaki R, Katayama J. Difficulty of Discrimination Modulates Attentional Capture by Regulating Attentional Focus. J Cogn Neurosci 2009; 21:359-71. [DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21022] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Abstract
Attentional capture for distractors is enhanced by increasing the difficulty of discrimination between the standard and the target in the three-stimulus oddball paradigm. In this study, we investigated the cognitive mechanism of this modulation of attentional capture. Event-related brain potentials were recorded from participants while they performed a visual three-stimulus oddball paradigm (frequent standard, rare target, and rare distractor). The discrimination difficulty between standard and target was manipulated in the central location. Distractor stimuli were presented in the central or surrounding locations. The P3a component was elicited by distractor stimuli and was used as a measure of attentional capture. The results revealed that discrimination difficulty had opposite effects on the P3a response between central and surrounding locations. With an increase in the difficulty of discrimination, the P3a response was enhanced when distractor stimuli were presented in the central location. In contrast, the P3a response was reduced when distractor stimuli were presented in a surrounding location. This finding suggests that spatial attention was focused by the difficulty of discrimination, and deviant processing was increased within its focus but decreased outside its focus. Therefore, attentional capture for deviant distractors is modulated by top–down controlled attentional focus.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Risa Sawaki
- 1Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan
- 2Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, Japan
| | | |
Collapse
|
14
|
Sawaki R, Katayama J. Distractor P3 is associated with attentional capture by stimulus deviance. Clin Neurophysiol 2008; 119:1300-9. [DOI: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.01.107] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/27/2007] [Revised: 12/25/2007] [Accepted: 01/25/2008] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
|
15
|
Chong H, Riis JL, McGinnis SM, Williams DM, Holcomb PJ, Daffner KR. To Ignore or Explore: Top–Down Modulation of Novelty Processing. J Cogn Neurosci 2008; 20:120-34. [DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Abstract
Attending to novelty is a critical element of human behavior and learning. Novel events can serve as task-irrelevant distracters or as potential sources of engagement by interesting or important aspects of one's environment. An optimally functioning brain should have the capacity to respond differentially to novel events depending on the circumstances in which they occur. In the present study, a subject-controlled variant of the visual novelty oddball paradigm was employed under two different conditions in which novel stimuli were characterized either as distracters from a main task or as potentially meaningful “invitations” to explore the environment. Differences in context, derived from varying the emphasis of task instructions, strongly modulated both the behavioral and electrophysiological response to novelty. This modulation was not observed for processing earlier than the P3 component. Subjects who encountered novel events that served as distracters limited the amount of attention and processing resources they appropriated. Remarkably, under this condition, there were no differences in overall P3 amplitude, late positive slow-wave activity, or viewing duration between rare novel and frequent standard events. In contrast, subjects who encountered novel events as potential opportunities to explore augmented the attention and processing resources directed toward these events (as reflected by a larger P3 amplitude, late positive slow-wave activity, and longer viewing durations). Our results suggest that the processing of novelty within the visual modality involves several stages, including: (1) the relatively automatic detection of unfamiliar, novel stimuli (indexed by the N2); (2) the voluntary allocation of resources determined by the broader context in which a novel event occurs (indexed by the P3); and (3) the sustained processing of novelty (indexed by late positive slow-wave activity). This study provides evidence of the brain's ability to generate differential responses to novel events according to the circumstances under which they are encountered. It also points to a greater degree of top–down modulation of the processing of novelty than has been previously emphasized. We suggest that less commonly studied variables, such as subject control, may provide additional insight into the different ways in which novelty is processed.
Collapse
|
16
|
Gaál ZA, Csuhaj R, Molnár M. Age-dependent changes of auditory evoked potentials--effect of task difficulty. Biol Psychol 2007; 76:196-208. [PMID: 17767993 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.07.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/27/2006] [Revised: 07/26/2007] [Accepted: 07/26/2007] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
The goal of this study was to evaluate the patterns of age-dependent changes of P3 components of auditory event-related potentials exploring the effects of task difficulty. The participants (age span: 19-68 years, n=55, divided into five age groups) took part in an easy and in a difficult two-tone oddball frequency discrimination task with speed or accuracy instructions, and in a novelty oddball task. The latency of the P3 components increased with aging. While in the easy task a linear P3b latency increase could be seen, in the difficult tasks (difficult frequency discrimination or distracting novel stimuli) an accelerated latency increase was observed for the P3b and P3a. In the two-tone oddball paradigm age had no effect on P3b amplitude, but in the novelty oddball task the amplitude of P3 potentials decreased with age. These results indicate that distracting stimuli increase task demands, and in difficult tasks decay can be observed more easily due to the accumulation of various processing mechanisms characterizing aging.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zsófia Anna Gaál
- Nonlinear Psychophysiology Research Group, Institute for Psychology, Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Szondi u. 83-85, Budapest, Hungary.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Sawaki R, Katayama J. Difficulty of discrimination modulates attentional capture for deviant information. Psychophysiology 2007; 44:374-82. [PMID: 17433096 DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00506.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
It has been reported that an increased difficulty of discrimination between standard and target enhances P3a for typical nontarget. To elucidate the mechanism of this effect on deviant processing, the P300 event-related brain potential (ERP) was elicited using a visual three-stimulus oddball paradigm (standard circle, .70, target circle, .15, and nontarget/target square, .15). Four task conditions were defined by a combination of two category types of rare square (nontarget or target) and two levels of discrimination difficulty between standard and target circles (easy or difficult). In the difficult conditions, P3a was elicited by both nontarget and target square. Our findings suggest that the difficulty of discrimination between standard and target enhances attentional capture, rather than inhibition, for deviant information. This study has implications for understanding the attentional mechanisms of deviant processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Risa Sawaki
- Graduate School of Education, Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan.
| | | |
Collapse
|
18
|
Volpe U, Mucci A, Bucci P, Merlotti E, Galderisi S, Maj M. The cortical generators of P3a and P3b: A LORETA study. Brain Res Bull 2007; 73:220-30. [PMID: 17562387 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.03.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 191] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/02/2006] [Revised: 03/01/2007] [Accepted: 03/01/2007] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
The P3 is probably the most well known component of the brain event-related potentials (ERPs). Using a three-tone oddball paradigm two different components can be identified: the P3b elicited by rare target stimuli and the P3a elicited by the presentation of rare non-target stimuli. Although the two components may partially overlap in time and space, they have a different scalp topography suggesting different neural generators. The present study is aimed at defining the scalp topography of the two P3 components by means of reference-independent methods and identifying their electrical cortical generators by using the low-resolution electromagnetic tomography (LORETA). ERPs were recorded during a three-tone oddball task in 32 healthy, right-handed university students. The scalp topography of the P3 components was assessed by means of the brain electrical microstates technique and their cortical sources were evaluated by LORETA. P3a and P3b showed different scalp topography and cortical sources. The P3a electrical field had a more anterior distribution as compared to the P3b and its generators were localized in cingulate, frontal and right parietal areas. P3b sources included bilateral frontal, parietal, limbic, cingulate and temporo-occipital regions. Differences in scalp topography and cortical sources suggest that the two components reflect different neural processes. Our findings on cortical generators are in line with the hypothesis that P3a reflects the automatic allocation of attention, while P3b is related to the effortful processing of task-relevant events.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- U Volpe
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Naples SUN, Largo Madonna delle Grazie, 80138 Naples, Italy.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Cycowicz YM, Friedman D. Visual novel stimuli in an ERP novelty oddball paradigm: effects of familiarity on repetition and recognition memory. Psychophysiology 2007; 44:11-29. [PMID: 17241137 DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00481.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
The orienting response, the brain's reaction to novel and/or out of context familiar events, is reflected by the novelty P3 of the ERP. Contextually novel events also engender high rates of recognition memory. We examined, under incidental and intentional conditions, the effects of visual symbol familiarity on the novelty P3 recorded during an oddball task and on the parietal episodic memory (EM) effect, an index of recollection. Repetition of familiar, but not unfamiliar, symbols elicited a reduction in the novelty P3. Better recognition performance for the familiar symbols was associated with a robust parietal EM effect, which was absent for the unfamiliar symbols in the incidental task. These data demonstrate that processing of novel events depends on expectation and whether stimuli have preexisting representations in long-term semantic memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yael M Cycowicz
- Cognitive Electrophysiology Laboratory, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York City, New York 10032, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
20
|
Devrim-Uçok M, Keskin-Ergen HY, Uçok A. Novelty P3 and P3b in first-episode schizophrenia and chronic schizophrenia. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2006; 30:1426-34. [PMID: 16828218 DOI: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2006.05.019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/25/2006] [Revised: 05/28/2006] [Accepted: 05/29/2006] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
The objective of this study was to evaluate P3b and novelty P3 responses in patients with first-episode schizophrenia (FES) and chronic schizophrenia (CS). P3b is consistently reported to be reduced in CS. However, novelty P3 results in CS are controversial. Novelty P3 is not studied, and there are only a few P3b studies in patients with FES. Subject groups comprised 31 patients with FES and 36 younger control subjects, and 26 patients with CS and 35 older control subjects. Automatically elicited auditory novelty P3 and effortfully elicited auditory P3b potentials were assessed. P3b amplitudes were reduced in both patients with FES and CS relative to their controls. CS and FES patients did not differ in P3b amplitude. Novelty P3 amplitude was reduced in patients with CS. Novelty P3 amplitude in patients with FES did not differ from their controls. P3b amplitude reduction may be a trait marker of schizophrenia and may not progress over the course of illness, although this can only be definitively determined by longitudinal studies. Novelty P3 amplitude reduction present in patients with CS, is not found at the onset of illness. Novelty P3 seems unaffected early in the disease process.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Müge Devrim-Uçok
- Department of Physiology, University of Istanbul, Istanbul Medical Faculty, 34093 Capa-Istanbul, Turkey.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
21
|
Beauchemin M, De Beaumont L, Vannasing P, Turcotte A, Arcand C, Belin P, Lassonde M. Electrophysiological markers of voice familiarity. Eur J Neurosci 2006; 23:3081-6. [PMID: 16819998 DOI: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04856.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 55] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Our ability to discriminate and recognize human voices is amongst the most important functions of the human auditory system. The current study sought to determine whether electrophysiological markers could be used as objective measures of voice familiarity, by looking at the electrophysiological responses [mismatch negativity (MMN) and P3a] when the infrequent stimulus presented is a familiar voice as opposed to an unfamiliar voice. Results indicate that the MMN elicited by a familiar voice is greater than that elicited by an unfamiliar voice at FCz. The familiar voice also produced a greater P3a wave than that triggered by the unfamiliar voice at Fz. As both the MMN and the P3a were elicited as participants were instructed not to pay attention to incoming stimulation, these findings suggest that voice recognition is a particularly potent preattentive process whose neural representations can be objectively described through electrophysiological assessments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maude Beauchemin
- Centre de Recherche, Hôpital Ste-Justine, 3175, Côte-Sainte-Catherine, Montréal QC, Canada H3T 1C5
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
22
|
Zimmer H. Habituation of the orienting response as reflected by the skin conductance response and by endogenous event-related brain potentials. Int J Psychophysiol 2006; 60:44-58. [PMID: 16023235 DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.05.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/09/2004] [Revised: 04/14/2005] [Accepted: 05/02/2005] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
The paper is concerned with the question of whether endogenous components of the auditory event-related brain potential (ERP) qualify for showing habituation of the orienting response (OR). Although response decrements have been found in nearly every ERP component, this question is still of current concern because a true selective response inhibition proving habituation of the OR is still lacking. The question has been tackled using single-trial ERP measurements in classical variants of the repetition/change paradigm commonly used in the traditional OR research on autonomous responses such as the skin conductance response (SCR). Results on 120 adults indicate that at least two endogenous components of the ERP, an anterior slow negative wave and a posterior slow positive wave, meet essential requirements of habituation: like the exemplary OR component, the SCR, both slow waves declined systematically with repeated stimulations and, more than that, recovered in response to fundamental changes. In the same way, an anterior positivity resembling the novelty P3 levelled off systematically with the stimulations, but without showing recovery. Thus, in so far as habituation of the OR is conceptualised as a selective inhibitory central nervous system process which can be assumed to have taken place only if a systematic (usually exponential) response decrement is followed by a recovery, the generalised decrement of the P3 cannot be equated with habituation, whereas the selective response diminution of both slow waves would have to be regarded as typical of habituation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Heinz Zimmer
- Institute of Psychology, University of Mainz, 55099 Mainz, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Muller-Gass A, Stelmack RM, Campbell KB. The effect of visual task difficulty and attentional direction on the detection of acoustic change as indexed by the Mismatch Negativity. Brain Res 2006; 1078:112-30. [PMID: 16497283 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.12.125] [Citation(s) in RCA: 71] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/13/2005] [Revised: 12/06/2005] [Accepted: 12/29/2005] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Näätänen's model of auditory processing purports that attention does not affect the MMN. The present study investigates this claim through two different manipulations. First, the effect of visual task difficulty on the passively elicited MMN is assessed. Second, the MMNs elicited by stimuli under attended and ignored conditions are compared. In Experiment 1, subjects were presented with mixed sequences of equiprobable auditory and visual stimuli. The auditory stimuli consisted of standard (80 dB SPL 1000 Hz), frequency deviant (1050 Hz), and intensity deviant (70 dB SPL) tone pips. In a first instance, subjects were instructed to ignore the auditory stimulation and engage in an easy and difficult visual discrimination task (focused condition). Subsequently, they were asked to attend to both modalities and detect visual and auditory deviant stimuli (divided condition). The results indicate that the passively elicited MMN to frequency and intensity deviants did not significantly vary with visual task difficulty, in spite of the fact that the easy and difficult tasks showed a wide variation in performance. The manipulation of the attentional direction (focused vs. divided conditions) did result in a significant effect on the MMN elicited by the intensity, but not frequency, deviant. The intensity MMN was larger at frontal sites when subjects' attention was directed to both modalities as compared to only the visual modality. The attentional effect on the MMN to the intensity deviants only may be due to the specific deviant feature or the poorer perceptual discriminability of this deviant from the standard. Experiment 2 was designed to address this issue. The methods of Experiment 2 were identical to those of Experiment 1 with the exception that the intensity deviant (60 dB SPL) was made to be more perceptible than the frequency deviant (1016 Hz) when compared to the standard stimulus (80 dB SPL 1000 Hz). The results of Experiment 2 also demonstrated that the passively elicited MMN was not affected by large variations in visual task difficulty; this provides convincing evidence that the MMN is independent of visual task demands. Similarly to Experiment 1, the direction of attention again had a significant effect on the MMN. In Experiment 2, however, the frequency MMN (and not the intensity MMN) was larger at frontal sites during divided attention compared to focused visual attention. The most parsimonious explanation of these results is that attention enhances the discriminability of the deviant from the standard background stimulation. As such, small acoustic changes would benefit from attention whereas the discriminability of larger changes may not be significantly enhanced.
Collapse
|
24
|
Nan Y, Knösche TR, Luo YJ. Counting in everyday life: discrimination and enumeration. Neuropsychologia 2005; 44:1103-13. [PMID: 16360184 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.10.020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/21/2004] [Revised: 10/27/2005] [Accepted: 10/27/2005] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Enumerating the number of items in a set accurately and quickly is a basic mathematical skill. This ability is especially crucial in the more real-life situations, where relevant items have to be discriminated from irrelevant distracters. Although much work has been done on the brain mechanisms and neural correlates of the enumeration and/or discrimination process, no agreement has been reached yet. We used event-related potentials (ERPs) to show the time course of brain activity elicited by a task that involved both enumeration and discrimination at the same time. We found that even though the two processes run to some extent in parallel, discrimination seems to take place mainly in an earlier time window (from 100 ms after the stimulus onset) than enumeration (beyond 200 ms after the stimulus onset). Moreover, electrophysiological evidence based on the N2 and P3 components make it reasonable to argue for the existence of a dichotomy between subitizing (for sets of less than four items) and counting (for sets of four and more items). Source estimation suggests that subitizing and counting, though being distinct brain processes, do recruit similar brain areas.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yun Nan
- Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
25
|
Merrin EL, Floyd TC, Deicken RF, Lane PA. The Wisconsin Card Sort Test and P300 responses to novel auditory stimuli in schizophrenic patients. Int J Psychophysiol 2005; 60:330-48. [PMID: 16143413 DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.05.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/14/2004] [Revised: 05/15/2005] [Accepted: 05/28/2005] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
The authors studied the relationship between performance on the Wisconsin Card Sort Test (WCST) and P300 activity in schizophrenics and normal controls. Fourteen male predominantly medicated schizophrenics and matched non-ill controls were administered the WCST and tests of temporal lobe (delayed verbal and spatial memory) and general intellectual functioning (Shipley). Patients were rated with negative and positive symptom scales extracted from the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale. Subjects performed a tone discrimination task requiring identification of rare targets in both a standard oddball paradigm and a three-stimulus paradigm that included rare novel sounds. Reference independent data from 16 scalp electrodes yielded Global Field Power (GFP), from which P300 latency was determined. P300 amplitude measures included amplitude at this identified latency as well as amplitude integrated over a 100 ms time window centered over it. These amplitude measures were examined at six selected electrode locations. Schizophrenics produced smaller P300 responses that tended to be slower, but there were no group differences in the relationships between neuropsychological performance and P300 responses. Across diagnostic groups percent perseverative errors predicted lower integrated and peak P300 amplitude during the novel but not the standard oddball paradigm. The effect on integrated P300 amplitude was localized to anterior leads after novel stimuli. Negative symptoms predicted lower WCST performance, lower integrated P300 amplitude, and smaller GFP after novel stimuli. Positive symptoms predicted reduced overall GFP and specific but inconsistent reductions in parietal P300 amplitude. The results suggest relationships between dorsolateral prefrontal competence, P300 activity in response to stimulus novelty, and negative symptoms in schizophrenic patients, paralleling findings obtained from blood flow and other measures of brain activity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Edward L Merrin
- California Department of Corrections, Parole Outpatient Clinic, Suite A, Santa Rosa, 95403, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
26
|
Legrain V, Bruyer R, Guérit JM, Plaghki L. Involuntary orientation of attention to unattended deviant nociceptive stimuli is modulated by concomitant visual task difficulty. Evidence from laser evoked potentials. Clin Neurophysiol 2005; 116:2165-74. [PMID: 16055373 DOI: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.05.019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 54] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/19/2004] [Revised: 05/04/2005] [Accepted: 05/29/2005] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Recent laser evoked potential (LEP) studies showed that unattended rare intensity-deviant nociceptive stimuli enhance the LEP vertex positivity P2 ('P400 effect'). It was hypothesized to reflect an involuntary switch of attention to nociceptive events. If true the P400 effect (1) should be produced when attention is focused on a task in another sensory modality (primary task), and (2) should be modulated by the primary task difficulty. METHODS Subjects had to count the number of visual symbols presented on a screen. In a difficult condition, symbols were digits 1-4 (interference between amount and meaning). In an easy condition, symbols were letters X (no interference). Nociceptive CO2 laser stimuli were simultaneously delivered on the left hand. Occasional stronger deviant stimuli (16%) were presented at random. In additional sessions, the strong stimuli were presented alone in homogenous series (100%). RESULTS LEP amplitude at about 400 ms was larger for rare deviant than for homogenous stimuli. Visual task difficulty decreased LEP amplitude at this latency. Deviant stimuli seemed also to interfere with performance in the visual task. CONCLUSIONS The results give evidence for considering the P400 effect as reflecting an involuntary attentional shift to nociceptive events. SIGNIFICANCE The study provides electrophysiological evidences for an intrusive capacity of pain to attract attention and to decrease behavioural performance in concurrent processes. In turn, such an attentional shift is tampered if attention is very engaged in a concomitant task.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Valéry Legrain
- Unité de Neurosciences Cognitives (NESC), Faculté de Psychologie et des Sciences de l'Education, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
27
|
Levy DA, Granot R, Bentin S. Neural sensitivity to human voices: ERP evidence of task and attentional influences. Psychophysiology 2003; 40:291-305. [PMID: 12820870 DOI: 10.1111/1469-8986.00031] [Citation(s) in RCA: 59] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
In an earlier study, we found that human voices evoked a positive event-related potential (ERP) peaking at approximately 320 ms after stimulus onset, distinctive from those elicited by instrumental tones. Here we show that though similar in latency to the Novelty P3, this Voice-Sensitive Response (VSR) differs in antecedent conditions and scalp distribution. Furthermore, when participants were not attending to stimuli, the response to voices was undistinguished from other harmonic stimuli (strings, winds, and brass). During a task requiring attending to a feature other than timbre, voices were not distinguished from voicelike stimuli (strings), but were distinguished from other harmonic stimuli. We suggest that the component elicited by voices and similar sounds reflects the allocation of attention on the basis of stimulus significance (as opposed to novelty), and propose an explanation of the task and attentional factors that contribute to the effect.
Collapse
|
28
|
Senkowski D, Herrmann CS. Effects of task difficulty on evoked gamma activity and ERPs in a visual discrimination task. Clin Neurophysiol 2002; 113:1742-53. [PMID: 12417227 DOI: 10.1016/s1388-2457(02)00266-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 113] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE The present study examined oscillatory brain activity of the EEG gamma band and event-related potentials (ERPs) with relation to the difficulty of a visual discrimination task. METHODS Three tasks with identical stimulus material were performed by 9 healthy subjects. The tasks comprised a passive control task, and an easy and a hard visual discrimination task, requiring discrimination of the color of circles. EEG was recorded from 26 electrodes. A wavelet transform based on Morlet wavelets was employed for the analysis of gamma activity. RESULTS Evoked EEG gamma activity was enhanced by both discrimination tasks as compared to the passive control task. Within the two discrimination tasks, the latency of the evoked gamma peak was delayed for the harder task. Higher amplitudes of the ERP components N170 and P300 were found in both discrimination tasks as compared to the passive task. The N2b, which showed a maximum activation at about 260 ms, was increased in the hard discrimination task as compared to the easy discrimination task. CONCLUSIONS Our results indicate that early evoked gamma activity and N2b are related to the difficulty of visual discrimination processes. A delayed gamma activity in the hard task indicated a longer duration of stimulus processing, whereas the amplitude of the N2b directly indicates the level of task difficulty.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel Senkowski
- Max-Planck-Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, Postfach 500 355, 04303 Leipzig, Germany
| | | |
Collapse
|
29
|
Ornitz EM, Gehricke JG, Russell AT, Pynoos R, Siddarth P. Modulation of startle and the startle-elicited P300 by the conditions of the cued continuous performance task in school-age boys. Clin Neurophysiol 2001; 112:2209-23. [PMID: 11738191 DOI: 10.1016/s1388-2457(01)00686-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES This study compares the modulation of the startle response by conditions requiring response preparation, production, and inhibition during a cued continuous performance task (CPT) in children to the results of previous studies in adults and evaluates the modulation of the startle-elicited P300 under the same conditions. The latter variable, reflecting the cognitive processing of the startling stimulus (SS), has not been studied under these conditions. METHODS Normal boys completed a cued CPT in which the cue was the letter T, the go condition requiring a button press was an X following the T, and the no-go condition requiring response inhibition was a letter other than X following the T. SS were presented 450 ms following the letter of interest in each condition. The amplitudes of the startle-elicited P300 at Fz, Cz, and Pz and the startle blink were compared in the different CPT conditions. RESULTS The startle blink, measured by orbicularis oculi electromyography, was not inhibited by the no-go CPT condition as is the case in adults. The vertical electro-oculogram was actually largest in the no-go condition. The startle-elicited P300 showed a central predominance and was significantly larger in the no-go condition and in the cue condition than in the go condition. CONCLUSIONS The absence of inhibition of the startle response during the no-go condition probably reflects a relative inefficiency of prefrontal cortical mechanisms that mediate response inhibition in children compared to adults. The enhanced startle-elicited P300 in the no-go and cue conditions of the CPT reflects cognitive processing of the SS that has been influenced by response inhibition or its anticipation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- E M Ornitz
- Division of Child Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, University of California at Los Angeles (UCLA), 760 Westwood Plaza, Los Angeles, CA 90024, USA.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
30
|
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Visual event-related brain potentials (ERPs) were elicited using a 3-stimulus oddball paradigm to assess the P3a with passive stimulus processing. METHODS Young adults (n=12) were presented with a series of visual stimuli consisting of a solid circle standard stimulus (P=0.76) that was difficult to discriminate from a larger target circle (P=0.12), with a large square distractor stimulus (P=0.12) presented randomly in the series. Subjects were instructed in the passive condition to simply look at the stimuli and in the active condition to press a mouse key only to the target stimulus. ERPs were recorded from 15 scalp electrodes, with the amplitude and latency of the P300 from the distractor and target stimuli assessed. RESULTS The P3a from the distractor stimulus was similar in amplitude, scalp topography, and peak latency across the passive and active task conditions. The P3b from the target stimulus demonstrated much smaller amplitude, highly altered scalp topography, and longer latency for the passive compared to active task conditions. CONCLUSIONS The P3a can be obtained with visual stimuli in the 3-stimulus paradigm under passive viewing conditions. Theoretical implications and clinical applications are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Y W Jeon
- Department of Neuropsychiatry, Our Lady of Mercy Hospital, The Catholic University of Korea, #665 Bupyung-Dong, Bupyung-Gu, Inchon, South Korea
| | | |
Collapse
|
31
|
Winterer G, Egan MF, Rädler T, Coppola R, Weinberger DR. Event-related potentials and genetic risk for schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2001; 50:407-17. [PMID: 11566157 DOI: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01072-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 48] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Event-related potentials (ERPs) during an auditory oddball task were investigated in patients with schizophrenia and in their healthy siblings to explore the question of whether abnormalities of two-dimensional topographic scalp-distribution of P300 amplitude and latency relate to genetic risk for schizophrenia. We also examined the P50, N100, and P200-waves, elicited during the same task. METHODS We investigated 42 schizophrenic patients, 62 of their healthy siblings, and 34 unrelated normal control subjects with a standard auditory oddball paradigm and 16 electroencephalogram electrodes. Amplitudes and latencies of the ERPs P50, N100, P200, and P300 were topographically analyzed. RESULTS In the patients, P300 amplitude was significantly decreased in the range of 54%-58% over the left parietotemporal area. Siblings did not show decreased P300 amplitudes when compared with normal subjects. P300 latencies were unchanged in both groups. No significant group differences were observed for the other event-related potentials. CONCLUSIONS In line with previous studies, the P300 amplitude in schizophrenic patients was decreased over the left temporoparietal area; however, we found no evidence for a genetic trait effect in the event-related potential abnormality. Possible reasons for these largely negative findings are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- G Winterer
- Clinical Brain Disorders Branch, NIMH/NIH, Building 10, Room 4S229A MSC, Bethesda, MD 20892, USA
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
32
|
Abstract
Recent neuroimaging studies have provided evidence for localized perceptual specificity in the processing of human voice stimuli, paralleling the specificity for human faces. This study attempted to delineate the perceptual features of human voices yielding selective processing, and to characterize its time-course. Electrophysiological recordings revealed a positive potential peaking at 320 ms post-stimulus onset, in response to sung tones compared with fundamental-frequency-matched instrumental tones, when both categories were distracters in an oddball task. This voice-specific response (VSR) evoked under conditions different from those yielding positivity at that latency in other contexts, indicates the overriding salience of voice stimuli, possibly reflecting the operation of a gating system directing voice stimuli to be processed differently from other acoustic stimuli.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D A Levy
- Department of Psychology, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem 91905, Israel
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
33
|
Hozumi A, Hirata K, Tanaka H, Yamazaki K. Perseveration for novel stimuli in Parkinson's disease: an evaluation based on event-related potentials topography. Mov Disord 2000; 15:835-42. [PMID: 11009188 DOI: 10.1002/1531-8257(200009)15:5<835::aid-mds1012>3.0.co;2-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Event-related potential topography produced by novel and target stimuli was used to detect dysfunction of mental switching (perseveration) in nondemented patients with Parkinson's disease. The study participants were 15 patients with Parkinson's disease and 13 age-matched healthy control patients. Ten percent of the novelty tones with pitches of 125 and 500 Hz were added to 20% of the target tones that had a pitch of 1000 Hz. Patients were instructed to count the target tones. The modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test was used to evaluate frontal lobe function. Patients with Parkinson's disease showed a significant decrease in the achieved categories and an increase in perseverative errors in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. These results indicate that the cognitive impairment of patients with Parkinson's disease can be characterized as failure of mental switching related to frontal lobe dysfunction based on basal ganglia disturbance. As compared with the control patients, patients with Parkinson's disease had shorter P3 latencies to the novel stimuli and a more frontal distribution on the P3 map, especially for the 125-Hz stimuli. This characteristic of P3 to novel stimuli in the patients with Parkinson's disease, but not in the control patients, is categorized by P3a (novelty P3). Our findings suggest that decreased mental switching causes lack of novelty P3 habituation in patients with Parkinson's disease and that it is related to learning disabilities based on dysfunction of the frontal lobe and basal ganglia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A Hozumi
- Department of Neurology, Dokkyo University School of Medicine, Mibu, Tochigi, Japan
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
34
|
Abstract
BACKGROUND This study investigated attentional allocation in 39 Vietnam combat veterans, 25 with and 14 without posttraumatic stress disorder, assessing P300 amplitudes and latencies during both three-tone and novelty "oddball" tasks. METHODS The three-tone oddball task consisted of three stimuli: frequent tones (85%), rare target tones (7.5%), and rare distractor tones (7.5%). The novelty oddball task was identical to the three-tone task except that the rare distractor tones were replaced with nonrepeating novel sounds (7.5%). RESULTS Combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder showed significant P300 amplitude enhancements at frontal sites in response to distracting stimuli during the novelty but not during the three-tone oddball tasks. There were no amplitude differences in target tones during either task. CONCLUSIONS The data suggest that combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder demonstrate P300 responses consistent with a heightened orientation response to novel, distracting stimuli. This finding is consistent both with the clinical presentation of the disorder and with theoretical notions that individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder demonstrate information-processing biases towards vague or potentially threatening stimuli.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M Kimble
- VA Boston Healthcare System/Behavioral Science Division/National Center for PTSD, Boston University School of Medicine, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
35
|
Knott V, Mohr E, Haché N, Mahoney C, Mendis T. EEG and the passive P300 in dementia of the Alzheimer type. CLINICAL EEG (ELECTROENCEPHALOGRAPHY) 1999; 30:64-72. [PMID: 10358785 DOI: 10.1177/155005949903000207] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Quantitatively analyzed resting electroencephalographic (EEG) activity and P300 event-related potentials elicited with a passive tone sequence paradigm were examined in 30 patients with dementia of the Alzheimer type (DAT) and 15 age matched controls. Background electrical rhythms of DAT patients evidenced slowing as shown by increased absolute and relative amplitudes in slow frequency bands and decreased amplitudes in fast frequency bands (relative to controls). Electrical slowing was more evident in patients with higher clinical ratings of global intellectual deterioration. Passive P300 amplitude and latency did not differentiate patients and controls and were not related to severity of dementia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- V Knott
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Ottawa/Royal Ottawa Hospital, Ontario, Canada
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
36
|
Comerchero MD, Polich J. P3a, perceptual distinctiveness, and stimulus modality. BRAIN RESEARCH. COGNITIVE BRAIN RESEARCH 1998; 7:41-8. [PMID: 9714727 DOI: 10.1016/s0926-6410(98)00009-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 105] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
A three-stimulus oddball paradigm (target, standard, nontarget) was employed in which subjects responded to an infrequent target, when its discrimination from the frequent standard was difficult. In separate auditory and visual modality conditions, the stimulus characteristics of an infrequent nontarget were manipulated such that its perceptual distinctiveness from the target was varied systematically. For both the low and high distinctiveness conditions, target stimulus P300 amplitude was larger than the nontarget only at the parietal electrode. In contrast, nontarget P3a amplitude was larger and earlier than the target P300 over the frontal/central electrode sites. The distinctiveness manipulation between the target and nontarget produced larger P3a component profiles for the auditory compared to visual stimuli. The results support previous findings that target/standard stimulus context determines P3a generation for both auditory and visual stimulus modalities and suggest that the distinctiveness of the eliciting stimulus contributes to P3a amplitude. Theoretical implications are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M D Comerchero
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92037, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
37
|
Schröger E, Wolff C. Behavioral and electrophysiological effects of task-irrelevant sound change: a new distraction paradigm. BRAIN RESEARCH. COGNITIVE BRAIN RESEARCH 1998; 7:71-87. [PMID: 9714745 DOI: 10.1016/s0926-6410(98)00013-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 259] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
A distraction paradigm was utilized that is suited to yield reliable auditory distraction on an individual level even with rather small frequency deviances (7%). Distraction to these tiny deviants was achieved by embedding task-relevant aspects and task-irrelevant, distracting aspects of stimulation into the same perceptual object. Event-related potential (ERP) and behavioral effects of this newly developed paradigm were determined. Subjects received tones that could be of short or long duration equiprobably. They were instructed to press a response button to long-duration tones (targets). In oddball blocks, tones could be of standard frequency or of low-probability (p=0.1), deviant frequency. The task-irrelevant frequency deviants elicited MMN, N2b, and P3a components, and caused impoverished behavioral performance to targets. The usage of tiny distractors permits an interpretation of auditory distraction in terms of attention switching due to a particular memory-related change-detection process. On the basis of the results from an additional condition in which tones were of 10 different frequencies (involving those frequencies which served as standard and deviant in oddball blocks), it is argued that one important prerequisite for linking the neural mechanisms reflected in change-related brain waves to behavioral distraction effects may be regarded as fulfilled. The robustness of the distraction effects to tiny deviations was confirmed in two control experiments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- E Schröger
- Institut für Allgemeine Psychologie, Universität Leipzig, Seeburgstr. 14-20, D-04103 Leipzig, Germany
| | | |
Collapse
|
38
|
Basile LF, Rogers RL, Simos PG, Papanicolaou AC. Magnetoencephalographic evidence for common sources of long latency fields to rare target and rare novel visual stimuli. Int J Psychophysiol 1997; 25:123-37. [PMID: 9101337 DOI: 10.1016/s0167-8760(96)00715-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/04/2023]
Abstract
This study used magnetoencephalography to examine the possibility that different generators account for the long-latency event-related potential (P300), evoked by rare target and by rare non-target, novel visual stimuli, in a visual oddball counting task performed by seven subjects. As expected, P300 peak latency was longer in response to rare targets compared to novel, non-target stimuli. Two main source regions were found for the Target- as well as for the Novel-P300, one in the temporal and one in the occipital lobe. Centers of neural activity were observed in the vicinity of the superior temporal sulcus, in the hippocampal formation and parahippocampal gyrus and in the occipital extrastriate cortex. It appears that the brain structures which contributed to the generation of the P300 response to both the target and the novel visual stimuli overlapped to a great extent.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- L F Basile
- Department of Neurosurgery, University of Texas-Houston Health Science Center 77030, USA
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
39
|
Cycowicz YM, Friedman D, Rothstein M. An ERP developmental study of repetition priming by auditory novel stimuli. Psychophysiology 1996; 33:680-90. [PMID: 8961790 DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1996.tb02364.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 42] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/03/2023]
Abstract
Event-related potentials were recorded from participants 5-7, 9-11, 14-16, and 22-28 years old during an auditory novelty oddball task. In this task, stimuli about which the participant is not instructed (i.e., novel or uncategorized) typically elicit a more frontally oriented P3 scalp topography (novelty P3). In contrast, stimuli to which the participant must respond (i.e., target or precategorized) elicit a P3 with a more posterior scalp topography. Repetition of identical novel stimuli led to a similar reduction in novelty P3 amplitude for all age groups. Moreover, with repetition the shift in scalp topography of the novelty P3 to a more parietally oriented distribution was similar in children and adults. A second component, the P3(2) (assumed to be an analog of the P3b), exhibited a repetition priming effect in both the adults and the youngest children. The fact that age-related differences induced by novel repetition were small and not systematic indicates that the processing of novel information is similar across a wide age range.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Y M Cycowicz
- Cognitive Electrophysiology Laboratory, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York 10032, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
40
|
Katayama J, Polich J. P300, probability, and the three-tone paradigm. ELECTROENCEPHALOGRAPHY AND CLINICAL NEUROPHYSIOLOGY 1996; 100:555-62. [PMID: 8980420 DOI: 10.1016/s0168-5597(96)95171-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 70] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/03/2023]
Abstract
The effects of stimulus probability on P300 from a 3-tone paradigm were examined in two experiments. Experiment 1 manipulated the probability of the non-target tone as 0.10, 0.45, or 0.80, while the target tone probability was always 0.10. Experiment 2 manipulated the probability of 3 tones as 0.10, 0.30, or 0.60, with one of the infrequent tones assigned as the target in each condition. Subjects were required to press a button in response to the target stimulus in both experiments. The results indicated that the P300 to the target and the non-target were both affected by the probability of the eliciting stimulus, such that component amplitude was inversely related to probability; no reliable P300 latency effects were found. Target tones elicited larger P300 amplitude than the non-target tones at the same probability. The findings suggest that probability effects on P300 amplitude are independent of responding to a specific target stimulus and are discussed with reference to the clinical utility of the 3-tone paradigm.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J Katayama
- Faculty of Education, Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan.
| | | |
Collapse
|
41
|
Abstract
P300 event-related potentials (ERPs) from 1-, 2-, and 3-tone oddball paradigms were elicited and compared from the same subjects. In the 1-tone paradigm, only a target tone was presented, with the standard tone replaced by silence. The 2-tone paradigm was a typical oddball task, wherein the target and standard tones were presented every 2.0 s in a random order with a target-tone probability of 0.10. In the 3-tone paradigm, in addition to the infrequent target (p = 0.10) and the frequent standard (p = 0.80), infrequent nontarget tones (p = 0.10) also were presented. The subject responded with a button press only to the target stimulus in each task. The target stimulus in each paradigm elicited a P300 component with a parietal maximum distribution. No P300 amplitude differences were found among paradigms, although peak latency from the 1-tone paradigm was shorter than those from the other two tasks. Both P300 peak amplitude and latency demonstrated strong positive correlations between each pair of paradigms. The results suggest that P300 was produced by the same neural and cognitive mechanisms across tasks. The possible utility of each paradigm in clinical testing is discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J Katayama
- Faculty of Education, Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan.
| | | |
Collapse
|
42
|
Abstract
The theoretical and empirical backgrounds for the utility of the P300 event-related potential (ERP) as a measure of cognitive aging are summarized. P300 latency data from 32 different normative aging studies are then reviewed and assessed with meta-analytic procedures. Evaluation of moderator variables indicates that sample characteristics, stimulus factors, and task conditions contribute significantly to the "normal" change in peak latency that occurs with aging. These findings are critiqued in the context of previous reports, and implications are outlined for future applications of ERPs to normative aging. It is concluded that P300 latency can provide useful information about cognitive aging but that specific variables must be considered to obtain more precise results.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J Polich
- Department of Neuropharmacology, Scripps Research Institute, La Jolla, CA 92037, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
43
|
Grillon C, Sinha R, O'Malley SS. Effects of ethanol on the processing of low probability stimuli: an ERP study. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1995; 119:455-65. [PMID: 7480526 DOI: 10.1007/bf02245862] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/25/2023]
Abstract
The effect of a moderate dose of ethanol on the processing of low probability task-relevant and task-irrelevant stimuli was investigated using event-related potentials (ERPs). Sixteen subjects received alcoholic and placebo beverages on alternate days. ERPs were recorded from 15 locations on the scalp. The subjects were asked to press a button upon detection of rare target stimuli embedded among frequent standard and rare "novel" stimuli. Ethanol 1) reduced the amplitude of P3 to novel stimuli, but not P3 to target stimuli, 2) did not affect the mismatch negativity, and 3) delayed P3 latency and reaction time independently. These results suggest that 1) the processing of rare task-irrelevant stimuli is more vulnerable to the effects of ethanol than is the processing of task-relevant stimuli, and 2) ethanol impacts stimulus evaluation time and response production stages of information processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- C Grillon
- Yale University School of Medicine, GERU, New Haven, CT 06510, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
44
|
Oades RD, Zerbin D, Dittmann-Balcar A. The topography of event-related potentials in passive and active conditions of a 3-tone auditory oddball test. Int J Neurosci 1995; 81:249-64. [PMID: 7628914 DOI: 10.3109/00207459509004890] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/26/2023]
Abstract
Normalized event-related potential (ERP) data were analysed for topographical differences of ERP amplitude or latency in two conditions of a 3-tone oddball paradigm. The aim was to compare perception-related features relating to tone-type (passive non-task condition) with focussed attention-related features (active discrimination of target from non-target) in 5 ERP components from 23 young healthy subjects. The tones used were a common standard (70%, 0.8 KHz), a deviant standard (15%, 2 KHz) and a 1.4 KHz tone (15%, t) also used as the target (T). A site x tone interaction was obtained for P1 amplitude (augmenting with pitch anterior to posterior). The opposite tendency was seen for P2 to the right of midline maxima. No interaction was obtained for N1 amplitude. Condition became relevant for the N2-P3 complex. Frontal N2 amplitude increased after rare tones in the active condition. Posterior P3 peak size distinguished between tone (more widespread response to the common tone) and condition (more right-sided in the passive condition). The common tone elicited more widespread shift to the right than the rare tones. Latency was affected by condition from the P2 onwards and confirmed many of the amplitude interactions. This report extends and qualifies well-known main effects of tone and condition through main site effects to lateral sites. It supports claims of multiple sources of ERP components, except for N1 and P2. The contributions of these sources are influenced by tone-features (from P1) and the presence or absence of focussed attention (from the N2-P3 complex).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- R D Oades
- RLHK Clinic for Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Essen, Germany
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
45
|
Abstract
In the present experiment, event-related potentials (ERPs) were used to investigate the effect of anxiety on the attention to novel stimuli. A total of 16 healthy subjects participated in an experiment involving the anticipation of electric shocks. ERPs from 15 scalp sites were recorded in response to three types of auditory stimuli, and two attentional modes during threat and safe conditions. The three types of auditory stimuli were (1) frequent "standard" tones, (2) rare "target" tones, and (3) rare "novel" sounds. In the passive attentional mode, subjects passively listened to the stimuli. In the active attentional model, they had to Press a button in response to target tones. In each attentional mode, stimuli were presented under two conditions: threat (anticipation of shock) and safe (no-shock anticipation). P3 to the target stimuli was not affected by shock anticipation. The amplitude of P3 to the novel stimuli, however, was increased by the threat of shock in the passive but not in the active mode. The lack of impact of anxiety on P3 to the novel stimuli in the active mode is discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- C Grillon
- Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT 06510-3223
| | | |
Collapse
|
46
|
Abstract
Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) were recorded from young, middle-aged, and older adults to infrequent target and novel stimuli during a version of the oddball paradigm. Analyses of scalp distribution suggested that the shift to a more frontally oriented topography with increasing age was confined to the P3 component (as compared to N1 and P2) elicited by both target and novel stimuli. This first demonstration of an age-related shift in the scalp distribution of the novelty P3 elicited by auditory stimuli was associated with an age-related increase in the false-alarm rate to novel stimuli. These age-associated differences in scalp distribution and false-alarm rate are consistent with a change in frontal lobe activity with increasing age.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D Friedman
- New York State Psychiatric Institute, NY 10032
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
47
|
Abstract
The P3 component of the event-related potentials (ERPs) to auditory task-relevant and task-irrelevant stimuli in 'Attend Auditory' (i.e., reaction time task) and 'Attend Visual' (i.e., when the auditory stimuli were being ignored) conditions was investigated in 13 RDC/DSM-III diagnosed schizophrenic patients. ERPs were recorded from Fz, Cz, and Pz. Compared to controls, schizophrenics had a significantly smaller P3 in the Attend Auditory than in the Attend Visual condition and to the task-relevant than to the task-irrelevant stimuli. Furthermore, the patients' P3 response to the most salient task-irrelevant stimuli in the Attend visual condition was normal. The results are discussed as suggesting that schizophrenics either allocate relatively more resources to task-irrelevant than task-relevant stimuli or that they fail to habituate to task-irrelevant stimuli.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- C Grillon
- Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT 06510-3223
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|