1
|
Hancock N, Redmond SM, Fox AB, Ash AC, Hogan TP. Word Reading and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in Children With Developmental Language Disorder. AMERICAN JOURNAL OF SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY 2025:1-17. [PMID: 40268687 DOI: 10.1044/2025_ajslp-24-00052] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 04/25/2025]
Abstract
PURPOSE This study evaluated the relationship between word reading and symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in school-age children with and without developmental language disorder (DLD), considering the influence of cognitive-linguistic mechanisms associated with dyslexia (phonological memory) and ADHD (working memory). METHOD Community ascertainment and blinded assessments identified 46 confirmed DLD and 76 cases of typical language development from a screening sample of 420 second and third graders. Language, word reading, ADHD symptoms, nonverbal intelligence, working memory, and phonological memory were assessed. RESULTS In all models, phonological memory was associated with word reading, and working memory was associated with ADHD symptoms. Additionally, in the new model of association that accounted for comorbidity with cognitive-linguistic indices, named the "Reading, ADHD, and Language (RE.A.L.) Comorbidity Model," word reading was uniquely mediated by phonological memory. CONCLUSIONS Findings highlight the unique role cognitive-linguistic indices associated with dyslexia and ADHD play in explaining the relationship between DLD, word reading, and ADHD symptoms. Results indicate that ADHD symptoms did not predict poor word reading; only phonological memory mediated the relationship between DLD and word reading difficulties associated with dyslexia. Similarly, working memory was associated with ADHD symptoms only. Findings underline the importance of including cognitive-linguistic indices associated with dyslexia and ADHD in evaluating word reading and ADHD symptoms in children with DLD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Norma Hancock
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, MGH Institute of Health Professions, Boston, MA
| | - Sean M Redmond
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, The University of Utah, Salt Lake City
| | - Annie B Fox
- School of Healthcare Leadership, MGH Institute of Health Professions, Boston, MA
| | - Andrea C Ash
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, The University of Utah, Salt Lake City
| | - Tiffany P Hogan
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, MGH Institute of Health Professions, Boston, MA
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Walquist-Sørli L, Caglar-Ryeng Ø, Furnes B, Nergård-Nilssen T, Donolato E, Melby-Lervåg M. Are Speech Sound Difficulties Risk Factors for Difficulties in Language and Reading Skills? A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2025; 68:164-177. [PMID: 39626051 DOI: 10.1044/2024_jslhr-24-00170] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/03/2025]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Children with speech sound difficulties often require educational psychology services, yet systematic reviews examining the association between these difficulties and language or reading problems are lacking. This meta-analysis examines whether these children are at higher risk of language and reading difficulties compared to their peers. METHOD The study analyzed 290 effect sizes from cross-sectional and longitudinal studies that compared language and reading skills between children with speech sound difficulties and controls. Additionally, we evaluated 37 effect sizes from correlational studies in general populations to examine the relationship between speech sound skills and language or reading skills. RESULTS Children with speech sound difficulties showed significant concurrent language (Hedges' g = -0.60) and reading (Hedges' g = -0.58) problems. Correlational studies also demonstrated a relationship between speech sound skills and language (r = .23) and reading (r = .23) skills. Phonological awareness and study quality were significant moderators. Longitudinal studies showed persistent or increasing group differences over time in language (Hedges' g = -0.85) and reading (Hedges' g = -0.50). These findings were consistent regardless of the severity or types of speech sound difficulties, nonverbal IQ, country, age, and publication year. However, a precision-effect test and the precision-effect estimate with standard errors analysis suggested a potential decrease in effect size due to publication bias from small sample sizes in primary studies. CONCLUSION Children with speech sound difficulties are at an increased risk of language and reading difficulties, emphasizing the need for broader language assessments and early interventions to mitigate future academic difficulties. SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.27849828.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Ømur Caglar-Ryeng
- Department of Education, UiT The Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø
| | | | | | - Enrica Donolato
- CREATE - Centre for Research on Equality in Education, University of Oslo, Norway
- Department of Special Needs Education, University of Oslo, Norway
| | - Monica Melby-Lervåg
- Department of Education, UiT The Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø
- CREATE - Centre for Research on Equality in Education, University of Oslo, Norway
- Department of Special Needs Education, University of Oslo, Norway
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Miniscalco C, Reinholdson AC, Gillberg C, Johnels JÅ. Speech sound error patterns may signal language disorder in Swedish preschool children with autism. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE & COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2024; 59:2516-2527. [PMID: 39137266 DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.13099] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/02/2023] [Accepted: 07/25/2024] [Indexed: 08/15/2024]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Within cohorts of children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) there is considerable variation in terms of language ability. In the past, it was believed that children with ASD either had delayed articulation and phonology skills or excelled in those areas compared to other language domains. Very little is known about speech sound ability in relation to language ability and non-verbal ability in Swedish preschool children with ASD. AIM The current study aimed to describe language variation in a group of 4-6-year-old children with ASD, focusing on in-depth analyses of speech sound error patterns with and without non-phonological language disorder and concomitant non-verbal delays. METHOD & PROCEDURES We examined and analysed the speech sound skills (including consonant inventory, percentage of correct consonants and speech sound error patterns) in relation to receptive language skills in a sample of preschool children who had screened positive for ASD in a population-based screening at 2.5 years of age. Seventy-three children diagnosed with ASD participated and were divided into subgroups based on their receptive language (i.e., non-phonological language) and non-verbal abilities. OUTCOMES & RESULTS The subgroup division revealed that 29 children (40%) had language delay/disorder without concurrent non-verbal general cognitive delay (ALD), 27 children (37%) had language delay/disorder with non-verbal general cognitive delay (AGD), and 17 children (23%) had language and non-verbal abilities within the normal range (ALN). Results revealed that children with ALD and children with AGD both had atypical speech sound error patterns significantly more often than the children with ALN. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS This study showed that many children who had screened positive for ASD before age 3 years - with or without non-verbal general cognitive delays - had deficits in language as well as in speech sound ability. However, individual differences were considerable. Our results point to speech sound error patterns as a potential clinical marker for language problems (disorder/delay) in preschool children with ASD. WHAT THIS PAPER ADDS What is already known on the subject Children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) have deficits in social communication, restricted interests and repetitive behaviour. They show very considerable variation in both receptive and expressive language abilities. Previously, articulation and phonology were viewed as either delayed in children with ASD or superior compared with other (non-phonological) language domains. What this paper adds to existing knowledge Children with ASD and language disorders also have problems with speech sound error patterns. What are the potential or actual clinical implications of this work? About 75% of children with ASD experience language delays/disorders, as well as speech sound problems, related to speech sound error patterns. Understanding/acknowledging these phonological patterns and their implications can help in the diagnosis and intervention of speech sound disorders in children with ASD. Direct intervention targeting phonology might lead to language gains, but more research is needed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carmela Miniscalco
- Gillberg Neuropsychiatry Centre, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology, Sahlgrenska Academy, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
- Child and Adolescent Neuropsychiatry Unit, Queen Silvia Children's Hospital, Sahlgrenska University Hospital, Gothenburg, Sweden
| | - Anna-Clara Reinholdson
- Child and Adolescent Neuropsychiatry Unit, Queen Silvia Children's Hospital, Sahlgrenska University Hospital, Gothenburg, Sweden
| | - Christopher Gillberg
- Gillberg Neuropsychiatry Centre, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology, Sahlgrenska Academy, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
- Child and Adolescent Neuropsychiatry Unit, Queen Silvia Children's Hospital, Sahlgrenska University Hospital, Gothenburg, Sweden
| | - Jakob Åsberg Johnels
- Gillberg Neuropsychiatry Centre, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology, Sahlgrenska Academy, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
- Child and Adolescent Neuropsychiatry Unit, Queen Silvia Children's Hospital, Sahlgrenska University Hospital, Gothenburg, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Larsen E, Murton O, Song X, Joachim D, Watts D, Kapczinski F, Venesky L, Hurowitz G. Validating the efficacy and value proposition of mental fitness vocal biomarkers in a psychiatric population: prospective cohort study. Front Psychiatry 2024; 15:1342835. [PMID: 38505797 PMCID: PMC10948552 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1342835] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/22/2023] [Accepted: 02/14/2024] [Indexed: 03/21/2024] Open
Abstract
Background The utility of vocal biomarkers for mental health assessment has gained increasing attention. This study aims to further this line of research by introducing a novel vocal scoring system designed to provide mental fitness tracking insights to users in real-world settings. Methods A prospective cohort study with 104 outpatient psychiatric participants was conducted to validate the "Mental Fitness Vocal Biomarker" (MFVB) score. The MFVB score was derived from eight vocal features, selected based on literature review. Participants' mental health symptom severity was assessed using the M3 Checklist, which serves as a transdiagnostic tool for measuring depression, anxiety, post-traumatic stress disorder, and bipolar symptoms. Results The MFVB demonstrated an ability to stratify individuals by their risk of elevated mental health symptom severity. Continuous observation enhanced the MFVB's efficacy, with risk ratios improving from 1.53 (1.09-2.14, p=0.0138) for single 30-second voice samples to 2.00 (1.21-3.30, p=0.0068) for data aggregated over two weeks. A higher risk ratio of 8.50 (2.31-31.25, p=0.0013) was observed in participants who used the MFVB 5-6 times per week, underscoring the utility of frequent and continuous observation. Participant feedback confirmed the user-friendliness of the application and its perceived benefits. Conclusions The MFVB is a promising tool for objective mental health tracking in real-world conditions, with potential to be a cost-effective, scalable, and privacy-preserving adjunct to traditional psychiatric assessments. User feedback suggests that vocal biomarkers can offer personalized insights and support clinical therapy and other beneficial activities that are associated with improved mental health risks and outcomes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | | | - Devon Watts
- Neuroscience Graduate Program, Department of Health Sciences, McMaster University, Hamilton, ON, Canada
- St. Joseph’s Healthcare Hamilton, Hamilton, ON, Canada
| | - Flavio Kapczinski
- Neuroscience Graduate Program, Department of Health Sciences, McMaster University, Hamilton, ON, Canada
- Department of Psychiatry, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Brazil
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Donolato E, Toffalini E, Rogde K, Nordahl‐Hansen A, Lervåg A, Norbury C, Melby‐Lervåg M. Oral language interventions can improve language outcomes in children with neurodevelopmental disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. CAMPBELL SYSTEMATIC REVIEWS 2023; 19:e1368. [PMID: 38024782 PMCID: PMC10680434 DOI: 10.1002/cl2.1368] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/01/2023]
Abstract
Background Young people who fail to develop language as expected face significant challenges in all aspects of life. Unfortunately, language disorders are common, either as a distinct condition (e.g., Developmental Language Disorder) or as a part of another neurodevelopmental condition (e.g., autism). Finding ways to attenuate language problems through intervention has the potential to yield great benefits not only for the individual but also for society as a whole. Objectives This meta-analytic review examined the effect of oral language interventions for children with neurodevelopmental disorders. Search Methods The last electronic search was conducted in April 2022. Selection Criteria Intervention studies had to target language skills for children from 2 to 18 years of age with Developmental Language Disorder, autism, intellectual disability, Down syndrome, Fragile X syndrome, and Williams syndrome in randomised controlled trials or quasi-experimental designs. Control groups had to include business-as-usual, waiting list, passive or active conditions. However, we excluded studies in which the active control group received a different type, delivery, or dosage of another language intervention. Eligible interventions implemented explicit and structured activities (i.e., explicit instruction of vocabulary, narrative structure or grammatical rules) and/or implicit and broad activities (i.e., shared book reading, general language stimulation). The intervention studies had to assess language skills in receptive and/or expressive modalities. Data Collection and Analysis The search provided 8195 records after deduplication. Records were screened by title and abstract, leading to full-text examinations of 448 records. We performed Correlated and Hierarchical Effects models and ran a retrospective power analysis via simulation. Publication bias was assessed via p-curve and precision-effect estimate. Main Results We examined 38 studies, with 46 group comparisons and 108 effects comparing pre-/post-tests and eight studies, with 12 group comparisons and 21 effects at follow-up. The results showed a mean effect size of d = 0.27 at the post-test and d = 0.18 at follow-up. However, there was evidence of publication bias and overestimation of the mean effects. Effects from the meta-analysis were significantly related to these elements: (1) receptive vocabulary and omnibus receptive measures showed smaller effect sizes relative to expressive vocabulary, grammar, expressive and receptive discourse, and omnibus expressive tests; and (2) the length of the intervention, where longer sessions conducted over a longer period of time were more beneficial than brief sessions and short-term interventions. Neither moderators concerning participants' characteristics (children's diagnosis, diagnostic status, age, sex, and non-verbal cognitive ability and severity of language impairment), nor those regarding of the treatment components and implementation of the language interventions (intervention content, setting, delivery agent, session structure of the intervention or total number of sessions) reached significance. The same occurred to indicators of study quality. The risk of bias assessment showed that reporting quality for the studies examined in the review was poor. Authors’ Conclusions In sum, the current evidence base is promising but inconclusive. Pre-registration and replication of more robust and adequately powered trials, which include a wider range of diagnostic conditions, together with more long-term follow-up comparisons, are needed to drive evidence-based practice and policy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Kristin Rogde
- Department of Special Needs EducationUniversity of OsloOsloNorway
| | | | | | - Courtenay Norbury
- Division of Psychology & Language SciencesUniversity College LondonLondonUK
| | | |
Collapse
|
6
|
Sheldrick RC, Marakovitz S, Garfinkel D, Perrin EC, Carter AS. Prevalence and Co-Occurrence of Developmental and Emotional-Behavioral Problems in Young Children. Acad Pediatr 2023; 23:623-630. [PMID: 36417968 PMCID: PMC10149553 DOI: 10.1016/j.acap.2022.11.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/13/2022] [Revised: 11/14/2022] [Accepted: 11/15/2022] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE A limited number of studies have estimated the prevalence of emotional-behavioral disorders among young children. None have assessed their co-occurrence with developmental delays using standardized assessment tools. Our objective was to estimate the prevalence of emotional-behavioral disorders and their co-occurrence with developmental delays among young children (2-5.5 years). METHODS Parents of young children (N = 987) enrolled from pediatric waiting rooms completed developmental-behavioral screening questionnaires. Based on results, 585 families were invited to and 439 completed evaluations that included structured diagnostic interviews with parents to assess child psychopathology (Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment (PAPA)), developmental testing with children (including the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, third Edition for children ≤ 42 months; Differential Ability Scales, second Edition for older children), and videotaped observation to establish whether autism risk was sufficient to warrant further evaluation. RESULTS According to PAPA algorithms, 23.0% of children met criteria for a DSM-IV disorder, while 9% qualified for a developmental delay. Presence of delay doubled the odds of having a DSM-IV disorder (OR = 2.1; CI: 1.02-4.3), and presence of disorder doubled the odds of having a moderate-to-severe developmental delay (OR=2.0; CI: 1.10-3.50). Prevalence of DSM-IV disorders (48.8% (95% CI: 33.5-64.5%)) and developmental delays (57.5% (95% CI: 41.7-71.9%)) were both higher among children at risk for autism. While developmental delay did not vary by race/ethnicity, prevalence of DSM-IV disorders was lower among African-American/Black (10.6%; CI: 2.9-18.3) and Hispanic/Latino children (11.1%; CI: 4.3-17.9). CONCLUSIONS Developmental delays and emotional-behavioral disorders among young children seen in pediatric settings are characterized by high prevalence and substantial co-occurrence.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Susan Marakovitz
- Division of Developmental-Behavioral Pediatrics (S Marakovitz, D Garfinkel, and EC Perrin), Tufts Medical Center, Boston, Mass
| | - Daryl Garfinkel
- Division of Developmental-Behavioral Pediatrics (S Marakovitz, D Garfinkel, and EC Perrin), Tufts Medical Center, Boston, Mass
| | - Ellen C Perrin
- Division of Developmental-Behavioral Pediatrics (S Marakovitz, D Garfinkel, and EC Perrin), Tufts Medical Center, Boston, Mass
| | - Alice S Carter
- Department of Psychology (AS Carter), University of Massachusetts Boston
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Rashaid AB, Alqhazo M, Newbury DF, Kanaan H, El-Khateeb M, Abukashabeh A, Al-Tamimi F. Evaluation of elements in hair samples of children with developmental language disorder (DLD). Nutr Neurosci 2023; 26:138-147. [PMID: 35034571 DOI: 10.1080/1028415x.2021.2022068] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/27/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Recent studies have highlighted a role for trace elements and toxic metals across neurodevelopmental disorders, including developmental stuttering, Autistic Spectrum Disorders (ASD), and Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). However, these environmental influences have yet to be explored in relation to Developmental Language Disorder (DLD). METHODS Elemental hair composition of seven elements; zinc (64Zn), magnesium (26Mg), iron (57Fe), potassium (39K), aluminum (27Al), lead (208Pb), and barium (138Ba) were analyzed in hair samples from 35 children affected by DLD and 35 controls with typical language development (TLD) using both inductive coupled plasma optical emission spectroscopy (ICP-OES) and inductive coupled plasma mass spectroscopy (ICP-MS). RESULTS The concentration of 64Zn was significantly lower in the hair of DLD group compared to the TLD control group. All other elements showed similar levels between cases and controls. This pilot study demonstrates the utility of trace elements and toxic metals screening in relation to language disorders and the use of hair samples in such investigations. CONCLUSION The finding that zinc levels differed between cases and controls could represent a clinically relevant result and should be replicated in larger sample size across time. A wider battery of related elements will help to better understand the role of trace elements and toxic metals in DLD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ayat Bani Rashaid
- Department of Chemistry, Jordan University of Science and Technology, Irbid Jordan
| | - Mazin Alqhazo
- Department of Rehabilitation Sciences, Applied Medical Sciences, Jordan University of Science and Technology, Irbid Jordan
| | - Dianne F Newbury
- Department of Biological and Medical Sciences, Faculty of Health and Life Sciences, Oxford Brookes University, Oxford, UK
| | - Heba Kanaan
- Department of Chemistry, Jordan University of Science and Technology, Irbid Jordan
| | - Mohammad El-Khateeb
- Department of Chemistry, Jordan University of Science and Technology, Irbid Jordan
| | - Ahmad Abukashabeh
- Chemical and Physical Analyses Laboratories, Jordan Atomic Energy Commission, Amman, Jordan
| | - Feda Al-Tamimi
- Department of Rehabilitation Sciences, Applied Medical Sciences, Jordan University of Science and Technology, Irbid Jordan
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
McGregor KK, Smolak E, Jones M, Oleson J, Eden N, Arbisi-Kelm T, Pomper R. What Children with Developmental Language Disorder Teach Us About Cross-Situational Word Learning. Cogn Sci 2022; 46:e13094. [PMID: 35122309 PMCID: PMC9285947 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13094] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/05/2021] [Revised: 12/20/2021] [Accepted: 12/27/2021] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Children with developmental language disorder (DLD) served as a test case for determining the role of extant vocabulary knowledge, endogenous attention, and phonological working memory abilities in cross‐situational word learning. First‐graders (Mage = 7 years; 3 months), 44 with typical development (TD) and 28 with DLD, completed a cross‐situational word‐learning task comprised six cycles, followed by retention tests and independent assessments of attention, memory, and vocabulary. Children with DLD scored lower than those with TD on all measures of learning and retention, a performance gap that emerged in the first cycle of the cross‐situational protocol and that we attribute to weaknesses in initial encoding. Over cycles, children with DLD learned words at a similar rate as their TD peers but they were less flexible in their strategy use, demonstrating a propose‐but‐verify approach but never a statistical aggregation approach. Also, they drew upon different mechanisms to support their learning. Attention played a greater role for the children with DLD, whereas extant vocabulary size played a greater role for the children with TD. Children navigate the problem space of cross‐situational learning via varied routes. This conclusion is offered as motivation for theorists to capture all learners, not just the most typical ones.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Karla K McGregor
- Center for Childhood Deafness, Language and Learning, Boys Town National Research Hospital.,Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Iowa
| | - Erin Smolak
- Center for Childhood Deafness, Language and Learning, Boys Town National Research Hospital
| | | | | | - Nichole Eden
- Center for Childhood Deafness, Language and Learning, Boys Town National Research Hospital
| | - Timothy Arbisi-Kelm
- Center for Childhood Deafness, Language and Learning, Boys Town National Research Hospital
| | - Ronald Pomper
- Center for Childhood Deafness, Language and Learning, Boys Town National Research Hospital
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Coelho RM, Drummond C, Mota NB, Erthal P, Bernardes G, Lima G, Molina R, Sudo FK, Tannock R, Mattos P. Network analysis of narrative discourse and attention-deficit hyperactivity symptoms in adults. PLoS One 2021; 16:e0245113. [PMID: 33826632 PMCID: PMC8026017 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0245113] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/13/2020] [Accepted: 12/23/2020] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Previous research investigating language in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) has demonstrated several deficits in many aspects. However, no previous study employed quantitative methodology providing objective measures that could be compared among different studies with diverse samples. To fill this gap, we used network analysis to investigate how ADHD symptomatology impacts narrative discourse, a complex linguistic task considered to be an ecological measure of language. Fifty-eight adults (34 females and 24 males) with a mean age of 26 years old and a mean of 17 years of educational level were administered the Adult Self-Rating Scale for ADHD symptomatology. They also completed the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, the Beck Depression Inventory and the Urgency, Premeditation, Perseverance, Sensation Seeking Behavior Scale. Intelligence quotient was calculated. Individuals were asked to tell a story based on a wordless picture book. Speech was recorded and transcribed as an input to SpeechGraphs software. Parameters were total number of words (TNW), number of loops of one node (L1), repeated edges (RE), largest strongly connected component (LSC) and average shortest path (ASP). Verbosity was controlled. Statistical analysis was corrected for multiples comparisons and partial correlations were performed for confounding variables. After controlling for anxiety, depression, IQ, and impulsiveness ADHD symptomatology was positively correlated with L1 and negatively correlated with LSC. TNW was positively correlated with ADHD symptoms. In a subdomain analysis, both inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity were negatively correlated with LSC. Only hyperactivity-impulsivity positively correlated with TNW and L1. Results indicated a correlation between ADHD symptoms and lower connectedness in narrative discourse (as indicated by higher L1 and lower LSC), as well as higher total number of words (TNW). Our results suggest that the higher the number of ADHD symptoms, the less connectivity among words, and a higher number of words in narrative discourse.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Cláudia Drummond
- Institute D'Or for Research and Education (IDOR), Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil.,Department of speech and hearing pathology, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro (UFRJ), Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil
| | | | - Pilar Erthal
- Institute D'Or for Research and Education (IDOR), Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil
| | - Gabriel Bernardes
- Institute D'Or for Research and Education (IDOR), Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil
| | - Gabriel Lima
- Institute D'Or for Research and Education (IDOR), Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil
| | - Raquel Molina
- Institute D'Or for Research and Education (IDOR), Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil
| | - Felipe Kenji Sudo
- Institute D'Or for Research and Education (IDOR), Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil
| | - Rosemary Tannock
- Research Institute, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, Canada
| | - Paulo Mattos
- Institute D'Or for Research and Education (IDOR), Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil.,Institute of Psychiatry, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro (UFRJ), Rio de Janeiro, RJ, Brazil
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Atypical communication characteristics among clinic-referred youth with and without autism spectrum disorder: Stability and associations with clinical correlates. Dev Psychopathol 2020; 32:1240-1253. [PMID: 32938518 DOI: 10.1017/s095457942000070x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
Atypical communication characteristics (ACCs), such as speech delay, odd pitch, and pragmatic difficulties, are common features of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) as are the symptoms of a wide range of psychiatric disorders. Using a simple retrospective method, this study aimed to better understand the relation and stability of ACCs with a broad range of psychiatric symptoms among large, well-characterized samples of clinic-referred children and adolescents with and without ASD. Youth with ASD had higher rates and a more variable pattern of developmental change in ACCs than the non-ASD diagnostic group. Latent class analysis yielded three ACC stability subgroups within ASD: Stable ACCs, Mostly Current-Only ACCs, and Little Professors. Subgroups exhibited differences in severity of ASD symptomatology, co-occurring psychiatric symptoms, and other correlates. Our findings provide support for the clinical utility of characterizing caregiver-perceived changes in ACCs in identifying children at risk for co-occurring psychopathology and other clinically relevant variables.
Collapse
|
11
|
McGregor KK, Eden N, Arbisi-Kelm T, Oleson J. The Fast-Mapping Abilities of Adults With Developmental Language Disorder. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2020; 63:3117-3129. [PMID: 32787708 PMCID: PMC7890218 DOI: 10.1044/2020_jslhr-19-00418] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/27/2019] [Revised: 04/17/2020] [Accepted: 06/15/2020] [Indexed: 05/19/2023]
Abstract
Purpose The aim of the study was to determine the integrity of fast mapping among adults with developmental language disorder (DLD). Method Forty-eight adults with DLD or typical language development (TD) were presented with 24 novel words and photos of their unfamiliar referents from the semantic categories of mammal, bird, fruit, or insect in two conditions. In the fast-mapping condition, 12 of the 24 unfamiliar referents were presented, one at a time alongside a familiar referent (e.g., a dog) and a question (e.g., Is the tail of the torato up?). In the explicit-encoding condition, the other 12 unfamiliar referents were presented alone, one at a time, with a label (e.g., This is a spimer). Immediately after exposure (T1) and again after a 1-day interval (T2), memory for the word-to-exemplar link was measured with a three-alternative forced-choice test, requiring the participant to match a spoken word to one of three pictured referents from the training set. At T2, memory for semantic category information was measured with a four-alternative forced-choice test, requiring the participant to match a spoken word to one of four prototypical silhouettes representing each of the semantic categories. Results Performance on word-to-exemplar link recognition was stronger for words learned in the explicit-encoding than the fast-mapping condition and stronger for the TD group than the DLD group. Time was not a significant factor as both groups maintained posttraining levels of performance after a 1-day retention interval. Performance on semantic category recognition was stronger for words learned in the explicit-encoding than the fast-mapping condition and stronger for the TD group than the DLD group. The lower category recognition performance of the DLD group was related to their lower nonverbal IQ scores. Conclusion Contexts that allow for explicit encoding yield better learning of word-to-referent links than contexts that allow for fast mapping in both stronger and weaker learners. Adults with DLD have difficulty learning the link between words and referents, whether trained via fast mapping or explicit encoding and whether tested with exemplar or category referents. Retention is a relative strength for adults with DLD. Supplemental Material https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.12765551.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Karla K. McGregor
- Boys Town National Research Hospital, Omaha, NE
- University of Iowa, Iowa City
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
Özcebe E, Noyan Erbas A, Karahan Tiğrak T. Analysis of behavioural characteristics of children with developmental language disorders. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY 2020; 22:30-36. [PMID: 30744427 DOI: 10.1080/17549507.2019.1571631] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/23/2018] [Revised: 11/02/2018] [Accepted: 01/15/2019] [Indexed: 06/09/2023]
Abstract
Purpose: Developmental language disorder (DLD) is common in early childhood and there may be an increased risk of co-occurring social, emotional, and behavioural problems related to the language problems of children. The aim of this study was to determine whether children with DLD experience more emotional and/or behavioural problems as compared to their typically developing peers.Method: A prospective case-control study design was used to perform a comparison between children with DLD and their typically developing peers. Thirty-eight Turkish children with DLD (mean age 38.21 months, standard deviation 8.15 months) and 30 Turkish children with typical language development (mean age 37.03 months, standard deviation 9.01 months) participated in the study.Result: The results of the analysis indicate that children with DLD have significantly higher scores for emotional reactivity, anxiety/depression, somatic complaints, withdrawnness, sleep problems, attention problems, and aggressive behaviours as compared with their typically developing peers.Conclusion: This study provides evidence to support children with DLD being at risk of other emotional and/or behavioural problems. An in-depth analysis of the behavioural characteristics of these children may be required to prevent and/or reduce the co-occurrence of other problems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Esra Özcebe
- Department of Speech and Language Therapy, Faculty of Health Sciences, Hacettepe University, Ankara, Turkey
| | - Aysin Noyan Erbas
- Department of Speech and Language Therapy, Faculty of Health Sciences, Hacettepe University, Ankara, Turkey
| | - Tuğçe Karahan Tiğrak
- Department of Speech and Language Therapy, Faculty of Health Sciences, Hacettepe University, Ankara, Turkey
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
van der Straaten TFK, Rieffe C, Soede W, Netten AP, Dirks E, Oudesluys-Murphy AM, Dekker FW, Böhringer S, Frijns JHM. Quality of life of children with hearing loss in special and mainstream education: A longitudinal study. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 2020; 128:109701. [PMID: 31606686 DOI: 10.1016/j.ijporl.2019.109701] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/22/2019] [Revised: 09/29/2019] [Accepted: 09/30/2019] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES To compare the quality of life (QoL) of children with hearing loss (HL) and children with normal hearing (NH) and to examine how the QoL of children with HL changes over time, considering language skills, type of hearing device, degree of HL, and type of education. METHODS AND MATERIALS This longitudinal study included 62 children with HL and their parents. Developmental outcome data were collected at two time points, when the mean ages of the children were 4 and 11 years. The Pediatric Quality of Life (PedsQL™) questionnaire, which includes assessments of Physical, Emotional, Social, and School functioning, was completed by parents at both time points and by the children with HL at the second time point. Receptive and expressive language skills at 4 years were assessed by the Reynell Developmental Language Scale. Results were compared with a Dutch normative sample. RESULTS The QoL of children with HL was similar to that of children with NH at both time points on two of the four QoL scales, Emotional and Physical functioning. On the other two scales, Social and School functioning, children with HL who attended special education and children who switched to mainstream education showed lower scores than children with HL who were consistently in mainstream education and lower scores than children with NH. The School QoL of children with HL decreased over time, as did the School QoL of children with NH. Social QoL of children with cochlear implants decreased over time, but this was not the case in children with hearing aids. Language skills and the degree of HL did not clinically improve the QoL over time of preschool children with HL. CONCLUSIONS The QoL of children with HL in mainstream education and the Physical and Emotional QoL of all children with HL were satisfactory. It is essential to develop specific guidance regarding school activities for children with HL in special education and for children with HL who switch to mainstream education in order to increase their social QoL.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tirza F K van der Straaten
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology and Head & Neck Surgery, Leiden University Medical Center, P.O. Box 9600, 2300 RC, Leiden, the Netherlands
| | - Carolien Rieffe
- Department of Developmental Psychology, Leiden University, P.O. Box 9555, 2300 RB, Leiden, the Netherlands; Dutch Foundation for the Deaf and Hard of Hearing Child, Lutmastraat 167, 1073 GX, Amsterdam, the Netherlands; Department of Psychology and Human Development, University College London, London WC1H 0AA, United Kingdom
| | - Wim Soede
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology and Head & Neck Surgery, Leiden University Medical Center, P.O. Box 9600, 2300 RC, Leiden, the Netherlands
| | - Anouk P Netten
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology and Head & Neck Surgery, Leiden University Medical Center, P.O. Box 9600, 2300 RC, Leiden, the Netherlands
| | - Evelien Dirks
- Dutch Foundation for the Deaf and Hard of Hearing Child, Lutmastraat 167, 1073 GX, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | - Anne Marie Oudesluys-Murphy
- Willem-Alexander Children's Hospital, Department of Social Pediatrics, Leiden University Medical Center, P.O. Box 9600, 2300 RC, Leiden, the Netherlands
| | - Friedo W Dekker
- Department of Clinical Epidemiology, Leiden University Medical Center, P.O. Box 9600, 2300 RC, Leiden, the Netherlands
| | - Stefan Böhringer
- Department of Medical Statistics, Leiden University Medical Center, P.O. Box 9600, 2300 RC, Leiden, the Netherlands
| | - Johan H M Frijns
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology and Head & Neck Surgery, Leiden University Medical Center, P.O. Box 9600, 2300 RC, Leiden, the Netherlands; Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, Leiden University Medical Center, P.O. Box 9600, 2300 RC, Leiden, the Netherlands.
| | | |
Collapse
|
14
|
Hooper SR, Montgomery JW, Brown TT, Swartz CW, Reed MS, Wasileski TJ, Levine MD. Prevalence of Writing Problems Across Three Middle School Samples. SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW 2019. [DOI: 10.1080/02796015.1993.12085677] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
15
|
Sheldrick RC, Schlichting LE, Berger B, Clyne A, Ni P, Perrin EC, Vivier PM. Establishing New Norms for Developmental Milestones. Pediatrics 2019; 144:peds.2019-0374. [PMID: 31727860 PMCID: PMC6889967 DOI: 10.1542/peds.2019-0374] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 08/23/2019] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES Pediatric surveillance of young children depends on providers' assessment of developmental milestones, yet normative data are sparse. Our objectives were to develop new norms for common milestones to aid in clinical interpretation of milestone attainment. METHODS We analyzed responses to the developmental screening form of the Survey of Well-being of Young Children from 41 465 screens across 3 states. Associations between developmental status and a range of child characteristics were analyzed, and norms for individual questions were compared to guidelines regarding attainment of critical milestones from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). RESULTS A contemporary resource of normative data for developmental milestone attainment was established. Lower developmental status was associated with child age in the presence of positive behavioral screening scores (P < .01), social determinants of health (P < .01), Medicaid (P < .01), male sex (P < .01), and child race (P < .01). Comparisons between Survey of Well-being of Young Children developmental questions and CDC guidelines reveal that a high percentage of children are reported to pass milestones by the age at which the CDC states that "most children pass" and that an even higher percentage of children are reported to pass milestones by the age at which the CDC states that parents should "act early." An interactive data visualization tool that can assist clinicians in real-time developmental screening and surveillance interpretation is also provided. CONCLUSIONS Detailed normative data on individual developmental milestones can help clinicians guide caregivers' expectations for milestone attainment, thereby offering greater specificity to CDC guidelines.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- R. Christopher Sheldrick
- Department of Health Law, Policy, and Management, School of Public Health, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts
| | - Lauren E. Schlichting
- Hassenfeld Child Health Innovation Institute, Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island
| | - Blythe Berger
- Rhode Island Department of Health, Providence, Rhode Island; and
| | - Ailis Clyne
- Rhode Island Department of Health, Providence, Rhode Island; and
| | - Pensheng Ni
- Department of Health Law, Policy, and Management, School of Public Health, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts
| | - Ellen C. Perrin
- Division of Developmental-Behavioral Pediatrics, School of Medicine, Tufts University and Floating Hospital for Children at Tufts Medical Center, Boston, Massachusetts
| | - Patrick M. Vivier
- Hassenfeld Child Health Innovation Institute, Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
St Clair MC, Skeen S, Marlow M, Tomlinson M. Relationships between concurrent language ability and mental health outcomes in a South African sample of 13-year-olds. PLoS One 2019; 14:e0221242. [PMID: 31487290 PMCID: PMC6728123 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0221242] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/25/2019] [Accepted: 07/14/2019] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Children and adolescents with delayed or disordered language development are at increased risk of a number of negative outcomes, including social and emotional problems and mental health difficulties. Yet, in low- and middle- income countries, where risk factors for compromised language development are known to be prevalent, there is a lack of research on the association between child and adolescent language ability and mental health outcomes. This study evaluates data from a cross-sectional study in Khayelitsha, a semi-urban impoverished community near Cape Town, South Africa. To measure language ability, behaviour and mental health, adolescents aged 13 (n = 200) were assessed using the Riddles subtest of the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children Version 2, the parent report Child Behaviour Checklist, and the self-report Moods and Feelings Questionnaire and the Self-Esteem Questionnaire. We conducted univariate and multivariate analyses to determine associations between language skills, self-esteem and mental health in this group of adolescents. Poor language ability was related to a range of concurrent adverse difficulties, such as attention deficits, self-esteem problems, social withdrawal, and depressive symptoms. Increased levels of language ability were related to better psychosocial profiles. In some cases, only individuals with a low level of language (bottom 10% of sample) were at increased risk of maladaptive outcomes. This study replicates the well-established relationship between language ability and poorer mental health found within high income countries in an upper middle-income country setting. Locally accessible support for children with reduced language ability is required, given the longer-term consequences of poorer mental health.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Sarah Skeen
- Institute for Life Course Health Research, Department of Global Health, Stellenbosch University, Stellenbosch, South Africa
| | - Marguerite Marlow
- Institute for Life Course Health Research, Department of Global Health, Stellenbosch University, Stellenbosch, South Africa
| | - Mark Tomlinson
- Institute for Life Course Health Research, Department of Global Health, Stellenbosch University, Stellenbosch, South Africa
- School of Nursing and Midwifery, Queen’s University Belfast, Belfast, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Lisa R, Pola R, Franz P, Jessica M. Developmental language disorder: Maternal stress level and behavioural difficulties of children with expressive and mixed receptive-expressive DLD. JOURNAL OF COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2019; 80:1-10. [PMID: 30999162 DOI: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2019.03.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/26/2018] [Revised: 03/18/2019] [Accepted: 03/31/2019] [Indexed: 06/09/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE The current study aimed to compare bullying and victimisation behaviour, levels of emotional and behavioural problems and maternal stress in children with expressive and mixed receptive-expressive language disorders. METHODS We measured the bullying and victimisation behaviour, the emotional and behavioural problems as well as the maternal stress in a sample of 4-9-year-old children with language disorders (N = 65). RESULTS Our main findings were that although bullying and victimisation were common in both groups; the children with expressive language disorders tended to be perpetrators (teacher-reported) while the children with mixed language disorders experienced more bullying and tended to be victims (self-reported); the latters' mothers perceived significantly more stress compared to mothers of children with expressive language disorders. Furthermore, the results of the study indicate that, although the number of children within the family was identified as the strongest predictor explaining maternal stress, maternal stress is a multifactorial concept influenced by many factors. CONCLUSION For a successful intervention in children with developmental language disorders, it seems to be very important to identify parental stressors beyond the treatment of the child's problems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rennecke Lisa
- Zentrum für Klinische Psychologie und Rehabilitation (ZKPR) der Universität Bremen, [Centre forClinicalPsychology and Rehabilitationof the University of Bremen], Grazer Str. 6 (Raum 3380), 28359 Bremen, Germany.
| | - Ronniger Pola
- Zentrum für Klinische Psychologie und Rehabilitation (ZKPR) der Universität Bremen, [Centre forClinicalPsychology and Rehabilitationof the University of Bremen], Grazer Str. 6 (Raum 3380), 28359 Bremen, Germany
| | - Petermann Franz
- Zentrum für Klinische Psychologie und Rehabilitation (ZKPR) der Universität Bremen, [Centre forClinicalPsychology and Rehabilitationof the University of Bremen], Grazer Str. 6 (Raum 3380), 28359 Bremen, Germany
| | - Melzer Jessica
- Zentrum für Klinische Psychologie und Rehabilitation (ZKPR) der Universität Bremen, [Centre forClinicalPsychology and Rehabilitationof the University of Bremen], Grazer Str. 6 (Raum 3380), 28359 Bremen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Combiths PN, Barlow JA, Richard JT, Pruitt-Lord SL. Treatment Targets for Co-Occurring Speech-Language Impairment: A Case Study. PERSPECTIVES OF THE ASHA SPECIAL INTEREST GROUPS 2019; 4:240-256. [PMID: 31214657 PMCID: PMC6581461 DOI: 10.1044/2019_pers-sig1-2018-0013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
PURPOSE The intersection of speech and language impairments is severely understudied. Despite repeatedly documented overlap and co-occurrence, treatment research for children with combined phonological and morphosyntactic deficits is limited. Especially little is known about optimal treatment targets for combined phonological-morphosyntactic intervention. We offer a clinically focused discussion of the existing literature pertaining to interventions for children with combined deficits and present a case study exploring the utility of a complex treatment target in word-final position for co-occurring speech and language impairment. METHOD Within a school setting, a kindergarten child (age 5;2) with co-occurring phonological disorder and developmental language disorder received treatment targeting a complex consonant cluster in word-final position inflected with third-person singular morphology. RESULTS For this child, training a complex consonant cluster in word-final position resulted in generalized learning to untreated consonants and clusters across word positions. However, morphological generalization was not demonstrated consistently across measures. CONCLUSION These preliminary findings suggest that training complex phonology in word-final position can result in generalized learning to untreated phonological targets. However, limited improvement in morphology and word-final phonology highlights the need for careful monitoring of cross-domain treatment outcomes and additional research to identify the characteristics of treatment approaches, techniques, and targets that induce cross-domain generalization learning in children with co-occurring speech-language impairment.
Collapse
|
19
|
Kang E, Gadow KD, Lerner MD. Atypical Communication Characteristics, Differential Diagnosis, and the Autism Spectrum Disorder Phenotype in Youth. JOURNAL OF CLINICAL CHILD AND ADOLESCENT PSYCHOLOGY 2019; 49:251-263. [DOI: 10.1080/15374416.2018.1539912] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Erin Kang
- Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
20
|
Thurm A, Manwaring SS, Jimenez CC, Swineford L, Farmer C, Gallo R, Maeda M. SOCIOEMOTIONAL AND BEHAVIORAL PROBLEMS IN TODDLERS WITH LANGUAGE DELAY. Infant Ment Health J 2018; 39:569-580. [PMID: 30105861 PMCID: PMC6245647 DOI: 10.1002/imhj.21735] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
Abstract
Toddlers with language delay are at risk for persistent developmental and behavioral difficulties; however, the association between socioemotional/behavior problems and language in young children is not well understood. This study explored socioemotional/behavior problems in a unique sample of toddlers with language delays using a measure developed explicitly for this age group. Toddlers identified by 18 months with receptive and expressive language delay (LD; n = 30) or typical development (TD; n = 61) were evaluated at 18 and 24 months of age using the Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (ITSEA) and the Mullen Scales of Early Learning. Compared to toddlers who had TD, toddlers with LD had significantly more concerning scores at 18 and 24 months on all ITSEA domains. The rate of "clinical concern" on most domains was not high in either group, except that >60% of LD toddlers were in the clinical concern range on the Competence domain. Socioemotional/behavioral problems were dimensionally related to receptive and expressive language, with greater language delay associated with more concerning ITSEA scores. Socioemotional and behavioral problems are related to receptive and expressive language abilities in 18- and 24-month-olds, indicating the need for screening of both types of concerns in toddlers identified with potential language delays.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Audrey Thurm
- Pediatrics & Developmental Neuroscience Branch, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD
| | - Stacy S. Manwaring
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT
| | | | - Lauren Swineford
- Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, Washington State University, Spokane, WA
| | - Cristan Farmer
- Pediatrics & Developmental Neuroscience Branch, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD
| | - Renee Gallo
- Pediatrics & Developmental Neuroscience Branch, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD
| | - Mika Maeda
- Pediatrics & Developmental Neuroscience Branch, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Chan KM, Fugard AJ. Assessing speech, language and communication difficulties in children referred for ADHD: A qualitative evaluation of a UK child and adolescent mental health service. Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry 2018; 23:442-456. [PMID: 29589469 DOI: 10.1177/1359104517753510] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is one of the most common childhood neuropsychiatric disorders and is highly comorbid with speech, language and communication difficulties (SLCDs). However, it is unclear how often SLCDs are identified in ADHD referrals in routine practice and whether there are unidentified SLCDs within this population. METHOD A thematic analysis was conducted on a random sample of case notes from 18 referrals for ADHD made to a child and adolescent mental health service (CAMHS) in London, United Kingdom. Analyses aimed to identify (a) the types of SLCDs detected during assessment, (b) at which point of the episode of care these SLCDs were suspected and (c) whether a referral or consultation was made to a speech and language therapist (SLT) for further evaluation. RESULTS Out of 18 cases investigated, 15 were found to have possible SLCDs based on case notes and reports provided by external agencies. However, only four were referred by CAMHS for further assessment. It is unclear what, if any, steps other external agencies took. Themes describing types of SLCDs, comorbidities and the process of identification are discussed. CONCLUSION The analysis of this service's case notes revealed a range of different routes to the identification of SLCDs, and it was unclear what steps were taken as a result of assessment. A limitation is that this is just one service and the results may not generalise. However, given the similarity in practitioner training received across the country and that practitioners move from service to service, there are grounds for repeating the study in other services. We recommend a more structured approach to identifying SLCDs and recording assessment and treatment decisions made.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Andrew Jb Fugard
- 2 Research Department of Clinical, Educational, and Health Psychology, University College London, UK.,3 Department of Psychosocial Studies, Birkbeck, University of London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Tambyraja SR, Rhoad-Drogalis A, Khan KS, Justice LM, Sawyer BE. Inattentiveness and Language Abilities in Preschoolers: A Latent Profile Analysis. JOURNAL OF ABNORMAL CHILD PSYCHOLOGY 2018; 47:245-257. [PMID: 29936661 DOI: 10.1007/s10802-018-0451-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Growing evidence suggests that early symptoms of inattentiveness may affect the language development and academic success of young children. In the present study, we examined the extent to which profiles of inattentiveness and language could be discerned within a heterogeneous group of preschoolers attending early childhood special education programs (n = 461). Based on parent-reported observations of children's symptoms of inattentiveness and direct assessments of children's language skills (grammar, vocabulary, and narrative ability), three distinct profiles were identified. The three groups, representing levels of severity (at risk, almost average, above average), differed not only by their end of year performance, but also with respect to which their abilities changed over the course of the academic year. Children in the poorest performing profile had poorer mean scores in the spring of their preschool year on all measures, but exhibited patterns of gain that exceeded or equaled their peers in higher-performing groups, in the domains of vocabulary and grammar. Examination of subsequent kindergarten reading skills suggested that profile differences remained consistent. Findings underscore the associations between early symptoms of inattentiveness and language difficulties, and further indicate that these relations extend to the acquisition of early reading skills. Future research is needed to corroborate these findings with more robust measures of attention, and to understand the long-term associations between inattentiveness, language and literacy, and potential effects on these associations from early intervention.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sherine R Tambyraja
- Crane Center for Early Childhood Research and Policy, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, 43201, USA.
| | - A Rhoad-Drogalis
- Crane Center for Early Childhood Research and Policy, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, 43201, USA
| | - K S Khan
- Crane Center for Early Childhood Research and Policy, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, 43201, USA
| | - L M Justice
- Crane Center for Early Childhood Research and Policy, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, 43201, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
23
|
Tchoungui Oyono L, Pascoe M, Singh S. The Prevalence of Speech and Language Disorders in French-Speaking Preschool Children From Yaoundé (Cameroon). JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2018; 61:1238-1250. [PMID: 29710276 DOI: 10.1044/2018_jslhr-l-16-0400] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/13/2016] [Accepted: 01/11/2018] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE The purpose of this study was to determine the prevalence of speech and language disorders in French-speaking preschool-age children in Yaoundé, the capital city of Cameroon. METHOD A total of 460 participants aged 3-5 years were recruited from the 7 communes of Yaoundé using a 2-stage cluster sampling method. Speech and language assessment was undertaken using a standardized speech and language test, the Evaluation du Langage Oral (Khomsi, 2001), which was purposefully renormed on the sample. A predetermined cutoff of 2 SDs below the normative mean was applied to identify articulation, expressive language, and receptive language disorders. Fluency and voice disorders were identified using clinical judgment by a speech-language pathologist. RESULTS Overall prevalence was calculated as follows: speech disorders, 14.7%; language disorders, 4.3%; and speech and language disorders, 17.1%. In terms of disorders, prevalence findings were as follows: articulation disorders, 3.6%; expressive language disorders, 1.3%; receptive language disorders, 3%; fluency disorders, 8.4%; and voice disorders, 3.6%. CONCLUSION Prevalence figures are higher than those reported for other countries and emphasize the urgent need to develop speech and language services for the Cameroonian population.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lilly Tchoungui Oyono
- Division of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Department of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Cape Town, South Africa
| | - Michelle Pascoe
- Division of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Department of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Cape Town, South Africa
| | - Shajila Singh
- Division of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Department of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Cape Town, South Africa
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Abstract
Intellectual disability (ID) and language disorders are neurodevelopmental conditions arising in early childhood. Child psychiatrists are likely to encounter children with ID and language disorders because both are strongly associated with challenging behaviors and mental disorder. Because early intervention is associated with optimal outcomes, child psychiatrists must be aware of their signs and symptoms, particularly as related to delays in cognitive and adaptive function. Optimal management of both ID and language disorders requires a multidisciplinary, team-based, and family centered approach. Child psychiatrists play an important role on this team, given their expertise with contextualizing and treating challenging behaviors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Natasha Marrus
- Department of Psychiatry, Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Washington University in St Louis, 660 South Euclid Avenue, Box 8504, St Louis, MO 63110, USA.
| | - Lacey Hall
- Department of Psychology, St. Jude Children's Research Hospital, 262 Danny Thomas Place, Memphis, TN 38105, USA
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Kim SJ, Ko YK, Seo EY, Oh GA. Prevalence of Speech Sound Disorders in 6-Year-Old Children in Korea. COMMUNICATION SCIENCES & DISORDERS 2017. [DOI: 10.12963/csd.17403] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
|
26
|
Korrel H, Mueller KL, Silk T, Anderson V, Sciberras E. Research Review: Language problems in children with Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder - a systematic meta-analytic review. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2017; 58:640-654. [PMID: 28186338 DOI: 10.1111/jcpp.12688] [Citation(s) in RCA: 55] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 11/21/2016] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Children with Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) appear to have a higher risk of language problems compared with typically developing children, although the types of language problems experienced are less clear. This review aims to establish the types of language problems experienced by children with ADHD according to systematically reviewed literature and determine the empirical evidence for language problems in children with ADHD compared with non-ADHD controls. METHODS A standardized search protocol was used on databases: CINAHL, Medline, and PsychINFO. We identified studies with the following inclusion criteria: (a) confirmed ADHD status at the time of the study, (b) inclusion of a non-ADHD control group, (c) use of a validated language measure, and (d) age ≤ 18. t-Tests, Pearson's r, and Hedges g effect sizes (ES) were calculated using summary statistics. Random effects meta-analyses were conducted for the language domain suitable for analysis. Publication bias was investigated using both the trim and fill and p-curve techniques. RESULTS Twenty-one studies were included in the systematic review (ADHD = 1,209; Control = 1,101), within which 60 of 68 separate analyses found significant differences between the ADHD and control group on the language measures (p < .05). Follow-up meta-analyses found evidence for large deficits in the ADHD groups overall (10/11 studies met p < .05; weighted mean ES [WMES]: 1.04); expressive (10/10 met p < .05; WMES: 1.23); receptive (12/14 met p < .05; WMES: 0.97), and pragmatic language (4/4 studies met p < .05; WMES: 0.98) compared with controls. CONCLUSIONS This study demonstrates that children with ADHD have poorer performance on measures of overall, expressive, receptive, and pragmatic language compared with controls. A screening of language functioning may be a valuable addition to the assessment of ADHD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hannah Korrel
- Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, Parkville, Vic., Australia.,The Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Vic., Australia
| | - Kathryn L Mueller
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, USA
| | - Tim Silk
- Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, Parkville, Vic., Australia.,Department of Paediatrics, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Vic., Australia
| | - Vicki Anderson
- Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, Parkville, Vic., Australia.,The Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Vic., Australia.,Department of Paediatrics, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Vic., Australia.,The Royal Children's Hospital, Parkville, Vic., Australia
| | - Emma Sciberras
- Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, Parkville, Vic., Australia.,Department of Paediatrics, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Vic., Australia.,School of Psychology, Deakin University, Geelong, Vic., Australia
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Durkin K, Toseeb U, Pickles A, Botting N, Conti-Ramsden G. Learning to drive in young adults with language impairment. TRANSPORTATION RESEARCH. PART F, TRAFFIC PSYCHOLOGY AND BEHAVIOUR 2016; 42:195-204. [PMID: 27974878 PMCID: PMC5139688 DOI: 10.1016/j.trf.2016.07.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/30/2015] [Revised: 06/16/2016] [Accepted: 07/23/2016] [Indexed: 06/06/2023]
Abstract
Language impairment (LI) is a common developmental disorder which affects many aspects of young people's functional skills and engagement with society. Little is known of early driving behaviour in those with this disability. This longitudinal study examines early driving experience in a sample of young adults with LI, compared with a sample of typically developing age-matched peers (AMPs). At age 24 years, significantly fewer participants with LI had acquired a driving licence. A crucial hurdle for those with LI appeared to be the Theory part of the (UK) test. Logistic regression analysis indicated that language ability and a measure of independence at age 17 contributed to the prediction of licence possession at age 24. There was no evidence of differences in traffic violations or accident rates between those with and without LI. There is little evidence that young people with LI are at greater risk on the roads than peers without LI, but some individuals with LI might benefit from support in the course of preparation for driving and in the driving test.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kevin Durkin
- University of Strathclyde, School of Psychological Sciences and Health, UK
| | - Umar Toseeb
- The University of Manchester, School of Psychological Sciences, UK
| | - Andrew Pickles
- King’s College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, UK
| | - Nicola Botting
- City University London, Language and Communication Science, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
28
|
Ruhl KL, Hughes CA, Camarata SM. Analysis of the Expressive and Receptive Language Characteristics of Emotionally Handicapped Students Served in Public School Settings. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2016. [DOI: 10.1177/152574019201400205] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Until the past few years, language characteristics of students receiving services for mild/moderate behavior disorders (BD) have been neglected for research on the language of the more severely disturbed population. However, recent investigations of the language characteristics of mild/moderately BD students have produced sufficient evidence to warrant further study. The purpose of the present study was to examine the language skills (i.e., morphology, syntax, and semantics) of a group of 30 mild/moderately BD students served in public school, resource classrooms. Overall results from the Test of Language Development-Intermediate (TOLD-I) (Hammill & Newcomer, 1982), the Test for Auditory Comprehension of Language-Revised (TACL-R) (Carrow-Woolfolk, 1985), the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised (PPVT-R) (Dunn & Dunn, 1982), and the Expressive One Word Vocabulary Test (EOWVT) (Gardner, 1979) revealed that the BD students fell a minimum of one standard deviation below the normative mean on all but one (i.e., Word Classes and Relations subtest of the TACL-R) of the measures. Further within subject analyses indicated the students were having difficulty with both receptive and expressive language. These findings substantiate the fact that mild/moderate BD students should be considered at risk for language disorders.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kathy L. Ruhl
- The Pennsylvania State University University Park, PA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
29
|
Carson DK, Klee T, Lee S, Williams KC, Perry CK. Children's Language Proficiency at Ages 2 and 3 as Predictors of Behavior Problems, Social and Cognitive Development at Age 3. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2016. [DOI: 10.1177/152574019801900203] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
This study examined the relationship between language proficiency, behavior problems, and other areas of development in a sample of 36 children ranging from 36–40 months (M = 37.3; SD = 1.1). Although none of the children were in the clinical range of behavioral disturbance, the findings indicated a strong general association between deficiencies in both expressive and receptive language and children's behavioral difficulties. Specifically, deficits in expressive language at age 2 (M = 25.5 mo.; SD = 0.6 mo.) were more predictive of internalizing rather than externalizing behavior problems at age 3, including symptoms associated with anxiety and depression, withdrawal, sleep and somatic problems. However, deficits in receptive language at age 3 were more predictive of these same types of symptoms at age 3, along with more destructive kinds of behaviors. Scores on measures of receptive language abilities at age 2 were positively predictive of social and cognitive development at age 3. These findings indicate a need for early identification of language delays in young children to help prevent deficits in language that might be linked to behavior problems or delays in other areas of development. Further, our results suggest the need for early intervention, especially for children who display both language delays and behavioral difficulties.
Collapse
|
30
|
Dimitriadis SI. Identification of infants at high familiar risk for language-learning disorders (LLD) by combining machine learning techniques with EEG-based brain network metrics. Clin Neurophysiol 2016; 127:2692-4. [PMID: 27212116 DOI: 10.1016/j.clinph.2016.04.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/08/2016] [Revised: 04/15/2016] [Accepted: 04/18/2016] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Stavros I Dimitriadis
- Institute of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, School of Medicine, Cardiff University, CF24 4HQ Cardiff, UK; Cardiff University Brain Research Imaging Center (CUBRIC), School of Psychology, Cardiff University, CF24 4HQ Cardiff, UK; Artificial Intelligence and Information Analysis Laboratory, Department of Informatics, Aristotle University, 54124 Thessaloniki, Greece; NeuroInformatics Group, AUTH, Thessaloniki, Greece.
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Fitch RH, Tallal P. Neural Mechanisms of Language-Based Learning Impairments: Insights from Human Populations and Animal Models. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2016; 2:155-78. [PMID: 15006291 DOI: 10.1177/1534582303258736] [Citation(s) in RCA: 56] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
The acquisition of speech perception and consequent expression of language represent fundamental aspects of human functioning. Yet roughly 7% to 8% of children who are otherwise healthy and of normal intelligence exhibit unexplained delays and impairments in acquiring these skills. Ongoing research has revealed several key features of language disability that may pro-vide more direct insight into underlying anomalous neural functioning. For example, evidence supports a strong association between basic defects in processing rapidly changing acoustic information and emergent disruptions in speech perception, as well as cascading effects on other forms of language development (including reading). Considerable neurobiological research has thus focused on developmental factors that might deleteriously influence rapid sensory processing. Additional research focuses on mechanisms of neural plasticity, including how such brains might be “retrained” for improved processing of language. These and related findings from human clinical studies, electrophysiological studies, neuroimaging studies, and animal models are reviewed.
Collapse
|
32
|
Thapa KB, Okalidou A, Anastasiadou S. Teachers' screening estimations of speech-language impairments in primary school children in Nepal. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE & COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2016; 51:310-327. [PMID: 26757345 DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12209] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/07/2015] [Accepted: 08/11/2015] [Indexed: 06/05/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND The prevalence of speech-language impairments in children have been estimated for several languages, primarily in developed countries. However, prevalence data is lacking for developing countries, such as Nepal. AIMS (1) To obtain teacher estimates of incidence and overall prevalence of speech-language impairments and its subtypes as a function of gender, age and grade level; and (2) to validate the screening instrument on Nepalese children. METHODOLOGY The adapted teachers' screening instrument, namely adapted Teachers' Speech and Language Referral Checklist (a-TSLRC), was administered in 2776 (690 and 2086) primary school children aged 5;00-11;11 years (mean = 8;11 years). The screening was conducted at four different points in time, i.e. Incidences I and II, and each incidence consisted of a testing and a retesting phase. Prior to this, teachers were trained in forum meetings, and an information sheet containing an overview of speech-language impairments, and guidelines/criteria for marking the occurrence of speech-language impairments in the TSLRC were disseminated. OUTCOMES & RESULTS Overall prevalence of speech-language impairments in children was estimated as 8.11%. Specifically, overall speech problems were estimated as 4.68%, and language problems as 8.0%. Additionally, the prevalence by subtypes of speech-language impairments as categorized in the TSLRC were reported to be 2.95% for an articulation/phonological problems, 2.09% for stuttering, 3.42% for a voice problems, 4.97% for a receptive language problems and 7.74% for an expressive language problems. The internal consistency among items was sufficient and a good intra-rater reliability was obtained. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS The study indicates that the overall results of speech-language impairments in children via the adapted in-Nepalese criterion-referenced instrument are supported by international studies. In addition, justifiable reliability and validity was obtained. Therefore, based on these overall evidence, this instrument can be useful for the screening of speech-language impairments in primary school children in Nepal.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Krishna Bahadur Thapa
- University of Macedonia, Department of Educational and Social Policy, 156 Egnatias Street, P.O. Box 1591, Salonika 540 06, Greece
| | - Areti Okalidou
- University of Macedonia, Department of Educational and Social Policy, 156 Egnatias Street, P.O. Box 1591, Salonika 540 06, Greece
| | - Sofia Anastasiadou
- University of Western Macedonia, Department of Preschool Education, 3rd km Florinas-Nikis, P.O. Box 21, 53100 Florina, Greece
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Behavior problems and social competence in Brazilian children with specific language impairment. PSICOLOGIA-REFLEXAO E CRITICA 2016. [DOI: 10.1186/s41155-016-0027-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
|
34
|
Early identification: Language skills and social functioning in deaf and hard of hearing preschool children. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 2015; 79:2221-6. [PMID: 26514930 DOI: 10.1016/j.ijporl.2015.10.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/17/2015] [Revised: 10/09/2015] [Accepted: 10/11/2015] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Permanent childhood hearing impairment often results in speech and language problems that are already apparent in early childhood. Past studies show a clear link between language skills and the child's social-emotional functioning. The aim of this study was to examine the level of language and communication skills after the introduction of early identification services and their relation with social functioning and behavioral problems in deaf and hard of hearing children. STUDY DESIGN Nationwide cross-sectional observation of a cohort of 85 early identified deaf and hard of hearing preschool children (aged 30-66 months). METHODS Parents reported on their child's communicative abilities (MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory III), social functioning and appearance of behavioral problems (Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire). Receptive and expressive language skills were measured using the Reynell Developmental Language Scale and the Schlichting Expressive Language Test, derived from the child's medical records. RESULTS Language and communicative abilities of early identified deaf and hard of hearing children are not on a par with hearing peers. Compared to normative scores from hearing children, parents of deaf and hard of hearing children reported lower social functioning and more behavioral problems. Higher communicative abilities were related to better social functioning and less behavioral problems. No relation was found between the degree of hearing loss, age at amplification, uni- or bilateral amplification, mode of communication and social functioning and behavioral problems. CONCLUSION These results suggest that improving the communicative abilities of deaf and hard of hearing children could improve their social-emotional functioning.
Collapse
|
35
|
Mental health trajectories from adolescence to adulthood: Language disorder and other childhood and adolescent risk factors. Dev Psychopathol 2015; 28:489-504. [PMID: 26611829 DOI: 10.1017/s0954579415001054] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
Longitudinal research on mental health development beyond adolescence among nonclinical populations is lacking. This study reports on psychiatric disorder trajectories from late adolescence to young adulthood in relation to childhood and adolescent risk factors. Participants were recruited for a prospective longitudinal study tracing a community sample of 5-year-old children with communication disorders and a matched control cohort to age 31. Psychiatric disorders were measured at ages 19, 25, and 31. Known predictors of psychopathology and two school-related factors specifically associated with language disorder (LD) were measured by self-reports and semistructured interviews. The LD cohort was uniquely characterized by a significantly decreasing disorder trajectory in early adulthood. Special education was associated with differential disorder trajectories between LD and control cohorts, whereas maltreatment history, specific learning disorder, family structure, and maternal psychological distress were associated with consistent trajectories between cohorts. From late adolescence to young adulthood, childhood LD was characterized by a developmentally limited course of psychiatric disorder; maltreatment was consistently characterized by an elevated risk of psychiatric disorder regardless of LD history, whereas special education was associated with significantly decreasing risk of psychiatric disorder only in the presence of LD.
Collapse
|
36
|
Zaimoğlu S, Türkdoğan D, Mazlum B, Bekiroğlu N, Tetik-Kabil A, Eyilikeder S. When Is EEG Indicated in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder? J Child Neurol 2015; 30:1785-93. [PMID: 25895916 DOI: 10.1177/0883073815580545] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/13/2014] [Accepted: 02/10/2015] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
The authors investigated the parameters for predicting epileptiform abnormalities in a group of children diagnosed with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). The sample consisted of 148 subjects aged between 6 and 13 (8.76 ± 1.26; 25.7% female) years. Subtypes of ADHD and comorbid psychiatric disorders were defined according to DSM-IV criteria. The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised was applied to all patients. Most of the subjects (89.2%) had wakefulness and sleep electroencephalography examinations lasting about one hour. The authors found out that the coexistence of speech sound disorder (odds ratio [OR] 3.90, 95% confidence interval [CI]: 1.61-9.48) and higher Digit Span test performance (OR 1.24, 95% CI: 1.06-1.44) predicted the presence of accompanying epileptiform abnormalities. The prevalence of epileptiform abnormalities was 26.4%, and they were frequently localized in the frontal (41%) and centrotemporal (28.2%) regions. Higher percentage of speech sound disorder co-occurrence (64%) in subjects with rolandic spikes suggests that epileptiform abnormalities associated with ADHD can be determined genetically at least in some cases. Pathophysiology of epileptiform abnormalities in ADHD might have complex genetic and maturational background.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sennur Zaimoğlu
- Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Istanbul, Institute of Neurological Sciences, Marmara University, Istanbul, Turkey
| | - Dilşad Türkdoğan
- Department of Child Neurology, Medical School, Marmara University, Istanbul, Turkey
| | - Betül Mazlum
- Department of Neuroscience, Institute of Experimental Medical Research, Istanbul University, Istanbul, Turkey
| | - Nural Bekiroğlu
- Department of Biostatistics and Bioinformatics, Medical School, Marmara University, Istanbul, Turkey
| | - Aylin Tetik-Kabil
- Foundation Development Year Psychological Counseling Center, School of Languages, Sabancı University, Istanbul, Turkey
| | - Seda Eyilikeder
- Department of Speech and Language Therapy, Anadolu University, Eskisehir, Turkey
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
Copy Number Variation Screen Identifies a Rare De Novo Deletion at Chromosome 15q13.1-13.3 in a Child with Language Impairment. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0134997. [PMID: 26262844 PMCID: PMC4532445 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0134997] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/03/2015] [Accepted: 07/17/2015] [Indexed: 01/15/2023] Open
Abstract
A significant proportion of children (up to 7% in the UK) present with pronounced language difficulties that cannot be explained by obvious causes like other neurological and medical conditions. A substantial genetic component is predicted to underlie such language problems. Copy number variants (CNVs) have been implicated in neurodevelopmental and psychiatric conditions, such as autism and schizophrenia, but it is not fully established to what extent they might contribute to language disorders. We conducted a CNV screen in a longitudinal cohort of young children with language-related difficulties (n = 85), focusing on single events at candidate loci. We detected a de novo deletion on chromosome 15q13.1–13.3. The adjacent 15q11-13.1 locus is disrupted in Prader-Willi and Angelman syndromes, while disruptions across the breakpoints (BP1-BP6) have previously been implicated in different neurodevelopmental phenotypes including autism, intellectual disability (ID), seizures and developmental delay (DD). This is the first report of a deletion at BP3-BP5 being linked to a deficit confined to language impairment, in the absence of ID, expanding the range of phenotypes that implicate the chromosome 15q13 locus.
Collapse
|
38
|
Motor Issues in Specific Language Impairment: a Window into the Underlying Impairment. CURRENT DEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS REPORTS 2015. [DOI: 10.1007/s40474-015-0051-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
39
|
Atkinson L, Beitchman J, Gonzalez A, Young A, Wilson B, Escobar M, Chisholm V, Brownlie E, Khoury JE, Ludmer J, Villani V. Cumulative risk, cumulative outcome: a 20-year longitudinal study. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0127650. [PMID: 26030616 PMCID: PMC4452593 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0127650] [Citation(s) in RCA: 58] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/27/2014] [Accepted: 04/17/2015] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
Cumulative risk (CR) models provide some of the most robust findings in the developmental literature, predicting numerous and varied outcomes. Typically, however, these outcomes are predicted one at a time, across different samples, using concurrent designs, longitudinal designs of short duration, or retrospective designs. We predicted that a single CR index, applied within a single sample, would prospectively predict diverse outcomes, i.e., depression, intelligence, school dropout, arrest, smoking, and physical disease from childhood to adulthood. Further, we predicted that number of risk factors would predict number of adverse outcomes (cumulative outcome; CO). We also predicted that early CR (assessed at age 5/6) explains variance in CO above and beyond that explained by subsequent risk (assessed at ages 12/13 and 19/20). The sample consisted of 284 individuals, 48% of whom were diagnosed with a speech/language disorder. Cumulative risk, assessed at 5/6-, 12/13-, and 19/20-years-old, predicted aforementioned outcomes at age 25/26 in every instance. Furthermore, number of risk factors was positively associated with number of negative outcomes. Finally, early risk accounted for variance beyond that explained by later risk in the prediction of CO. We discuss these findings in terms of five criteria posed by these data, positing a "mediated net of adversity" model, suggesting that CR may increase some central integrative factor, simultaneously augmenting risk across cognitive, quality of life, psychiatric and physical health outcomes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Joseph Beitchman
- Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
- University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| | | | | | - Beth Wilson
- Social Planning Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| | | | | | - Elizabeth Brownlie
- Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
- University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
40
|
Beitchman JH, Brownlie EB, Bao L. Age 31 mental health outcomes of childhood language and speech disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2014; 53:1102-10.e8. [PMID: 25245354 DOI: 10.1016/j.jaac.2014.07.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/08/2014] [Revised: 07/03/2014] [Accepted: 07/24/2014] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Language disorders are associated with emotional and behavioral problems in childhood and adolescence. Although clinical studies with small samples suggest that psychosocial difficulties continue into adulthood, adult mental health outcomes of childhood language disorders are not well known. The objective of this prospective longitudinal study is to determine whether the age 31 mental health outcomes of individuals who had childhood language disorders differ from the outcomes of typically developing controls. METHOD A 26-year cohort study followed up children with language or speech disorders from age 5 to age 31. The children were selected from a 1-in-3 random sample of 5-year-olds using a 3-stage screening and assessment process. A control group matched by sex, age, and classroom or school was also selected. Diagnoses were assigned with the Composite International Diagnostic Interview with the additional criterion that Global Assessment of Functioning scores indicated at least mild impairment. Dimensional psychosocial self-report measures were also administered. RESULTS Rates of diagnosis at age 31 years were equivalent between participants who had childhood language disorders and controls, with and without multiple imputation to estimate missing outcomes. Differences in rates of affective and substance use disorders could not be ruled out because of attrition in the cohort with language disorders, who were less likely to participate at age 31. Psychosocial scores for both cohorts were in the normal range. The cohort with language disorders had poorer self-rated physical health than controls. CONCLUSION Mild/moderate language disorders may not have significant long-term mental health consequences in early adulthood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joseph H Beitchman
- Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Toronto, ON, Canada; University of Toronto.
| | - E B Brownlie
- Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Toronto, ON, Canada; University of Toronto
| | - Lin Bao
- Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Toronto, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
41
|
Smolík F, Vávru P. Sentence imitation as a marker of SLI in Czech: disproportionate impairment of verbs and clitics. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2014; 57:837-849. [PMID: 24763390 DOI: 10.1044/2014_jslhr-l-12-0384] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/03/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE The authors examined sentence imitation as a potential clinical marker of specific language impairment (SLI) in Czech and its use to identify grammatical markers of SLI. METHOD Children with SLI and the age- and language-matched control groups (total N = 57) were presented with a sentence imitation task, a receptive vocabulary task, and digit span and nonword repetition tasks. Sentence imitations were scored for accuracy and error types. A separate count of inaccuracies for individual part-of-speech categories was performed. RESULTS Children with SLI had substantially more inaccurate imitations than the control groups. The differences in the memory measures could not account for the differences between children with SLI and the control groups in imitation accuracy, even though they accounted for the differences between the language-matched and age-matched control groups. The proportion of grammatical errors was larger in children with SLI than in the control groups. The categories that were most affected in imitations of children with SLI were verbs and clitics. CONCLUSION Sentence imitation is a sensitive marker of SLI. Verbs and clitics are the most vulnerable categories in Czech SLI. The pattern of errors suggests that impaired syntactic representations are the most likely source of difficulties in children with SLI.
Collapse
|
42
|
Bornstein MH, Hahn CS, Suwalsky JTD. Language and internalizing and externalizing behavioral adjustment: developmental pathways from childhood to adolescence. Dev Psychopathol 2013; 25:857-78. [PMID: 23880396 PMCID: PMC4151616 DOI: 10.1017/s0954579413000217] [Citation(s) in RCA: 78] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
Two independent prospective longitudinal studies that cumulatively spanned the age interval from 4 years to 14 years used multiwave designs to investigate developmental associations between language and behavioral adjustment (internalizing and externalizing behavior problems). Altogether 224 children, their mothers, and teachers provided data. Series of nested path analysis models were used to determine the most parsimonious and plausible paths among the three constructs over and above stability in each across age and their covariation at each age. In both studies, children with poorer language skills in early childhood had more internalizing behavior problems in later childhood and in early adolescence. These developmental paths between language and behavioral adjustment held after taking into consideration children's nonverbal intellectual functioning, maternal verbal intelligence, education, parenting knowledge, and social desirability bias, as well as family socioeconomic status, and they applied equally to girls and boys.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marc H Bornstein
- Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
43
|
Raschle NM, Stering PL, Meissner SN, Gaab N. Altered neuronal response during rapid auditory processing and its relation to phonological processing in prereading children at familial risk for dyslexia. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2013; 24:2489-501. [PMID: 23599167 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bht104] [Citation(s) in RCA: 52] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
Developmental dyslexia (DD) is a learning disability affecting 5-17% of children. Although researchers agree that DD is characterized by deficient phonological processing (PP), its cause is debated. It has been suggested that altered rapid auditory processing (RAP) may lead to deficient PP in DD and studies have shown deficient RAP in individuals with DD. Functional neuroimaging (fMRI) studies have implicated hypoactivations in left prefrontal brain regions during RAP in individuals with DD. When and how these neuronal alterations evolve remains unknown. In this article, we investigate functional networks during RAP in 28 children with (n = 14) and without (n = 14) a familial risk for DD before reading onset (mean: 5.6 years). Results reveal functional alterations in left-hemispheric prefrontal regions during RAP in prereading children at risk for DD, similar to findings in individuals with DD. Furthermore, activation during RAP in left prefrontal regions positively correlates with prereading measures of PP and with neuronal activation during PP in posterior dorsal and ventral brain areas. Our results suggest that neuronal differences during RAP predate reading instruction and thus are not due to experience-dependent brain changes resulting from DD itself and that there is a functional relationship between neuronal networks for RAP and PP within the prereading brain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nora M Raschle
- Laboratories of Cognitive Neuroscience, Division of Developmental Medicine, Department of Medicine, Boston Children's Hospital, Boston, MA 02115, USA Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115, USA and
| | - Patrice L Stering
- Laboratories of Cognitive Neuroscience, Division of Developmental Medicine, Department of Medicine, Boston Children's Hospital, Boston, MA 02115, USA
| | - Sarah N Meissner
- Laboratories of Cognitive Neuroscience, Division of Developmental Medicine, Department of Medicine, Boston Children's Hospital, Boston, MA 02115, USA
| | - Nadine Gaab
- Laboratories of Cognitive Neuroscience, Division of Developmental Medicine, Department of Medicine, Boston Children's Hospital, Boston, MA 02115, USA Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115, USA and Harvard Graduate School of Education, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA
| |
Collapse
|
44
|
Lewis BA, Short EJ, Iyengar SK, Taylor HG, Freebairn L, Tag J, Avrich AA, Stein CM. Speech-Sound Disorders and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Symptoms. TOPICS IN LANGUAGE DISORDERS 2012; 32:247-263. [PMID: 24363479 PMCID: PMC3868495 DOI: 10.1097/tld.0b013e318261f086] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/09/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE The purpose of this study was to examine the association of speech-sound disorders (SSD) with symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) by the severity of the SSD and the mode of transmission of SSD within the pedigrees of children with SSD. PARTICIPANTS AND METHODS The participants were 412 children who were enrolled in a longitudinal family study of SSD. Children were grouped on the basis of the severity of their SSD as determined by their scores on the Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation and history of an SSD. Five severity groups were compared: no SSD, resolved SSD, mild SSD, mild-moderate SSD, and moderate-severe SSD. Participants were also coded for comorbid language impairment (LI), based on scores on a standardized language test. Pedigrees of children were considered to represent bilineal inheritance of disorders if there was a history for SSD on both the maternal and paternal sides of the family. Parents completed the ADHD rating scale and a developmental questionnaire for each of their children. RESULTS AND CONCLUSIONS Children with moderate-severe SSD had higher ratings on the inattention and hyperactive/impulsivity scales than children with no SSD. Children whose family pedigrees demonstrated bilineal inheritance had higher ratings of inattention than children without bilineal inheritance. To determine the best predictors of ADHD ratings, multiple linear regression analyses were conducted. LI was more predictive of ADHD symptoms than SSD severity, bilineal inheritance of SSD, age, or gender. Findings support that LI rather than SSD is associated with ADHD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Barbara A Lewis
- Departments of Psychological Sciences (Drs Lewis and Short and Mss Freebairn and Tag), Epidemiology and Biostatistics (Drs Iyengar and Stein and Ms Avrich), and Pediatrics (Dr Taylor), Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, Ohio
| | - Elizabeth J Short
- Departments of Psychological Sciences (Drs Lewis and Short and Mss Freebairn and Tag), Epidemiology and Biostatistics (Drs Iyengar and Stein and Ms Avrich), and Pediatrics (Dr Taylor), Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, Ohio
| | - Sudha K Iyengar
- Departments of Psychological Sciences (Drs Lewis and Short and Mss Freebairn and Tag), Epidemiology and Biostatistics (Drs Iyengar and Stein and Ms Avrich), and Pediatrics (Dr Taylor), Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, Ohio
| | - H Gerry Taylor
- Departments of Psychological Sciences (Drs Lewis and Short and Mss Freebairn and Tag), Epidemiology and Biostatistics (Drs Iyengar and Stein and Ms Avrich), and Pediatrics (Dr Taylor), Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, Ohio
| | - Lisa Freebairn
- Departments of Psychological Sciences (Drs Lewis and Short and Mss Freebairn and Tag), Epidemiology and Biostatistics (Drs Iyengar and Stein and Ms Avrich), and Pediatrics (Dr Taylor), Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, Ohio
| | - Jessica Tag
- Departments of Psychological Sciences (Drs Lewis and Short and Mss Freebairn and Tag), Epidemiology and Biostatistics (Drs Iyengar and Stein and Ms Avrich), and Pediatrics (Dr Taylor), Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, Ohio
| | - Allison A Avrich
- Departments of Psychological Sciences (Drs Lewis and Short and Mss Freebairn and Tag), Epidemiology and Biostatistics (Drs Iyengar and Stein and Ms Avrich), and Pediatrics (Dr Taylor), Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, Ohio
| | - Catherine M Stein
- Departments of Psychological Sciences (Drs Lewis and Short and Mss Freebairn and Tag), Epidemiology and Biostatistics (Drs Iyengar and Stein and Ms Avrich), and Pediatrics (Dr Taylor), Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, Ohio
| |
Collapse
|
45
|
Brownlie EB, Lazare K, Beitchman J. Validating a self-report screen for ADHD in early adulthood using childhood parent and teacher ratings. J Atten Disord 2012; 16:467-77. [PMID: 21903889 DOI: 10.1177/1087054711398902] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE This article evaluates the diagnostic utility of a self-report screening tool for adults based on Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed.; DSM-IV) ADHD criteria. METHOD Children with speech/language (S/L) impairment and typically developing controls had ADHD symptoms rated by parents and teachers at ages 5 and 12. At age 19, participants completed the Adult Attention Problems Scale (AAPS), an 18-item screen. Receiver operative characteristic curve analyses were used to assess the efficiency of this instrument in screening for ADHD. RESULTS The AAPS had moderate sensitivity and high specificity, but only for adults without a history of communication disorders. CONCLUSION The AAPS provides clinicians with the only self-report scales for ADHD in adulthood, validated with childhood ADHD symptoms assessed by multiple raters. However, scale characteristics were poor for the S/L-impaired cohort. Given the overlap between language impairment and ADHD, adult ADHD measures validated in S/L-impaired samples are needed.
Collapse
|
46
|
Monopoli WJ, Kingston S. The relationships among language ability, emotion regulation and social competence in second-grade students. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT 2012. [DOI: 10.1177/0165025412446394] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Relationships exist between language ability, emotion regulation, and social competence in preschool children. This study examines how these relationships function in elementary school children, and explores whether language ability partially mediates the relationship between emotion regulation and social competence. Second-grade students ( N = 67) completed an assessment of receptive vocabulary, and teachers rated emotional and social skills. Results show relationships between emotion regulation and social competence, and between language and aspects of social competence. There were compelling sex differences in the strength of the relationships between these variables. However, language did not act as a mediator. Future research should further investigate the role of language skills in social competence as children mature, and further investigate sex differences in these constructs.
Collapse
|
47
|
Tomblin JB, Mueller KL. How Can the Comorbidity with ADHD Aid Understanding of Language and Speech Disorders? TOPICS IN LANGUAGE DISORDERS 2012; 32:198-206. [PMID: 24817779 PMCID: PMC4013272 DOI: 10.1097/tld.0b013e318261c264] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/25/2023]
Abstract
This paper serves to provide a background for the topic of comorbidity than extends through this issue. Comorbidity is common within developmental disorders. It is shown that there are many possible reasons for comorbidity. Some of these can be viewed as artifacts as simple as chance occurrence or because of the way that the research participants were sampled. If these artifacts are eliminated, then comorbidity can be informative with respect to possible causes of the disorders that are comorbid. Several possible etiologic models are presented along with a general framework for considering levels of causality in developmental disorders.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J Bruce Tomblin
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Iowa
| | - Kathyrn L Mueller
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Iowa
| |
Collapse
|
48
|
Scerri TS, Morris AP, Buckingham LL, Newbury DF, Miller LL, Monaco AP, Bishop DV, Paracchini S. DCDC2, KIAA0319 and CMIP are associated with reading-related traits. Biol Psychiatry 2011; 70:237-45. [PMID: 21457949 PMCID: PMC3139836 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 128] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/26/2010] [Revised: 01/18/2011] [Accepted: 02/05/2011] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Several susceptibility genes have been proposed for dyslexia (reading disability; RD) and specific language impairment (SLI). RD and SLI show comorbidity, but it is unclear whether a common genetic component is shared. METHODS We have investigated whether candidate genes for RD and SLI affect specific cognitive traits or have broad effect on cognition. We have analyzed common risk variants within RD (MRPL19/C2ORF3, KIAA0319, and DCDC2) and language impairment (CMIP and ATP2C2) candidate loci in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children cohort (n = 3725), representing children born in southwest England in the early 1990s. RESULTS We detected associations between reading skills and KIAA0319, DCDC2, and CMIP. We show that DCDC2 is specifically associated with RD, whereas variants in CMIP and KIAA0319 are associated with reading skills across the ability range. The strongest associations were restricted to single-word reading and spelling measures, suggesting that these genes do not extend their effect to other reading and language-related skills. Inclusion of individuals with comorbidity tends to strengthen these associations. Our data do not support MRPL19/C2ORF3 as a locus involved in reading abilities nor CMIP/ATP2C2 as genes regulating language skills. CONCLUSIONS We provide further support for the role of KIAA0319 and DCDC2 in contributing to reading abilities and novel evidence that the language-disorder candidate gene CMIP is also implicated in reading processes. Additionally, we present novel data to evaluate the prevalence and comorbidity of RD and SLI, and we recommend not excluding individuals with comorbid RD and SLI when designing genetic association studies for RD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tom S. Scerri
- Wellcome Trust Centre for Human Genetics, University of Oxford, UK
| | - Andrew P. Morris
- Wellcome Trust Centre for Human Genetics, University of Oxford, UK
| | | | | | - Laura L. Miller
- School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, UK
| | | | | | - Silvia Paracchini
- Wellcome Trust Centre for Human Genetics, University of Oxford, UK,Address correspondence to Silvia Paracchini, D.Phil., Wellcome Trust Centre for Human Genetics, OX3 7BN Oxford, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
49
|
Tikkanen S, Stiles WB, Leiman M. Parent development in clinical child neurological assessment process: Encounters with the assimilation model. Psychother Res 2011; 21:593-607. [DOI: 10.1080/10503307.2011.594817] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022] Open
|
50
|
Chuang YC, Hsu CY, Chiu NC, Lin SP, Tzang RF, Yang CC. Other impairment associated with developmental language delay in preschool-aged children. J Child Neurol 2011; 26:714-7. [PMID: 21343604 DOI: 10.1177/0883073810389331] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
The aim of this study was to investigate developmental impairment in several domains that might be associated with developmental language delay. The records of 56 preschool children with developmental language delay and 31 nonimpaired children were reviewed. Children with language delay were more likely than those in the nonimpaired group to have cognitive developmental delay (Mental Development Index < 70) (P < .001) and gross and fine motor delay (gross: 28 [50%] versus 5 [16%], P = .002; fine: 34 [62%] versus 11 [35%], P = .02). Children with language delay were significantly more likely to have impairment than were nonimpaired children in gross motor, fine motor, comprehension-conceptual and personal-social (P = .01, P = .02, P = .01, P = .02, respectively) functional domains. Our findings indicate that preschool children with language delay have wide-ranging difficulties in development and function.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yao-Chia Chuang
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Mackay Memorial Hospital, Taipei, Taiwan.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|