1
|
Otárola-Jiménez J, Nataraj N, Bisch-Knaden S, Hansson BS, Knaden M. Oviposition experience affects oviposition preference in Drosophila melanogaster. iScience 2024; 27:110472. [PMID: 39129830 PMCID: PMC11315110 DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2024.110472] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/01/2024] [Revised: 05/02/2024] [Accepted: 07/04/2024] [Indexed: 08/13/2024] Open
Abstract
Learning, memorizing, and recalling of potential ovipositing sites can influence oviposition preference. Classical conditioning experiments have shown that vinegar flies can learn the association of olfactory, gustatory, or visual stimuli with either positive or negative unconditioned stimuli. However, less is known about whether similar associations are formed in an ecologically more relevant context like during oviposition. Our experiments reveal that Drosophila melanogaster females increase their preference for substrates they have already experienced. However, this change of preference requires that the flies not only smelled or touched the substrates but also oviposited on them. We furthermore show that such an experience results in long-term memory lasting for at least 4 days, i.e., a duration that so far was shown only for aversive conditioning. Our study thus reveals a different form of associative learning in D. melanogaster that might be highly relevant for settling novel ecological niches.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julio Otárola-Jiménez
- Department of Evolutionary Neuroethology, Max-Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology, 07745 Jena, Germany
- Chemistry School, University of Costa Rica, San Pedro, San José 11501-2060, Costa Rica
| | - Nandita Nataraj
- Department of Evolutionary Neuroethology, Max-Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology, 07745 Jena, Germany
| | - Sonja Bisch-Knaden
- Department of Evolutionary Neuroethology, Max-Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology, 07745 Jena, Germany
| | - Bill S. Hansson
- Department of Evolutionary Neuroethology, Max-Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology, 07745 Jena, Germany
| | - Markus Knaden
- Department of Evolutionary Neuroethology, Max-Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology, 07745 Jena, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Das D, Ghosh G, Dutta A, Sherpa RD, Ghosh P, Hui SP, Ghosh S. Fruit ripening retardant Daminozide induces cognitive impairment, cell specific neurotoxicity, and genotoxicity in Drosophila melanogaster. Neurotoxicology 2024; 103:123-133. [PMID: 38851594 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuro.2024.06.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/26/2024] [Revised: 05/30/2024] [Accepted: 06/04/2024] [Indexed: 06/10/2024]
Abstract
BACKGROUND We explored neurotoxic and genotoxic effects of Daminozide, a fruit ripening retardant, on the brain of Drosophila melanogaster, based on our previous finding of DNA fragmentation in larval brain cell in the flies experimentally exposed to this chemicals. METHODS Adult flies were subjected to two distinct concentrations of daminozide (200 mg/L and 400 mg/L) mixed in culture medium, followed by an examination of specific behaviors such as courtship conditioning and aversive phototaxis, which serve as indicators of cognitive functions. We investigated brain histology and histochemistry to assess the overall toxicity of daminozide, focusing on neuron type-specific effects. Additionally, we conducted studies on gene expression specific to neuronal function. Statistical comparisons were then made between the exposed and control flies across all tested attributes. RESULTS The outcome of behavioral assays suggested deleterious effects of Daminozide on learning, short term and long term memory function. Histological examination of brain sections revealed cellular degeneration, within Kenyon cell neuropiles in Daminozide-exposed flies. Neurone specific Immuno-histochemistry study revealed significant reduction of dopaminergic and glutaminergic neurones with discernible reduction in cellular counts, alteration in cell and nuclear morphology among daminozide exposed flies. Gene expression analyses demonstrated upregulation of rutabaga (rut), hb9 and down regulation of PKa- C1, CrebB, Ace and nAchRbeta-1 in exposed flies which suggest dysregulation of gene functions involved in motor neuron activity, learning, and memory. CONCLUSION Taken together, our findings suggests that Daminozide induces multifaceted harmful impacts on the neural terrain of Drosophila melanogaster, posing a threat to its cognitive abilities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Debasmita Das
- Department of Zoology, University of Calcutta, Kolkata, India
| | - Gaurab Ghosh
- Department of Biological Sciences, Indian Institute of Science Education & Research (IISER)- Kolkata Mohanpur Campus, Mohanpur, Nadia, West Bengal, India
| | - Arthita Dutta
- Department of Zoology, University of Calcutta, Kolkata, India
| | - Rinchen D Sherpa
- S. N. Pradhan Centre for Neurosciences, University of Calcutta, Kolkata, India
| | - Papiya Ghosh
- Department of Zoology, Bijoykrishna Girls' College. Howrah. India
| | - Subhra Prakash Hui
- S. N. Pradhan Centre for Neurosciences, University of Calcutta, Kolkata, India
| | - Sujay Ghosh
- Department of Zoology, University of Calcutta, Kolkata, India.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Chandak P, Chakraborti U, Annagiri S. Using pupae as appetitive reinforcement to study visual and tactile associative learning in the Ponerine ant Diacamma indicum. Sci Rep 2023; 13:15609. [PMID: 37731033 PMCID: PMC10511714 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-42439-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/14/2023] [Accepted: 09/10/2023] [Indexed: 09/22/2023] Open
Abstract
Associative learning is of great importance to animals, as it enhances their ability to navigate, forage, evade predation and improve fitness. Even though associative learning abilities of Hymenopterans have been explored, many of these studies offered food as appetitive reinforcement. In the current study, we focus on tactile and visual cue learning in an ant Diacamma indicum using a Y-maze setup with pupa as a positive reinforcement. Using pupa as a reward resulted in a significantly higher proportion of ants completing the training in a shorter time as compared to using food as reinforcement. Ants spent significantly more time in the conditioned arm for both visual cues (white dots or black dots) and tactile cues (rough or smooth surfaces) presented on the floor when associated with pupa, thus showing that they were capable of associative learning. On encountering a conflict between visual and tactile cues during the test, ants chose to spend significantly more time on the arm with the tactile cues indicating that they had made a stronger association between pupa and the tactile cue as compared to the visual cue during training. Using pupa as an ecologically relevant reward, we show that these solitary foraging ants living in small colonies are capable of visual and tactile associative learning and are likely to learn tactile cues over visual cues in association with pupa.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Parth Chandak
- Behaviour and Ecology Lab, Department of Biological Sciences, Indian Institute of Science Education and Research, Mohanpur, Kolkata, 741246, India
| | - Udipta Chakraborti
- Behaviour and Ecology Lab, Department of Biological Sciences, Indian Institute of Science Education and Research, Mohanpur, Kolkata, 741246, India
| | - Sumana Annagiri
- Behaviour and Ecology Lab, Department of Biological Sciences, Indian Institute of Science Education and Research, Mohanpur, Kolkata, 741246, India.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Lei Z, Henderson K, Keleman K. A neural circuit linking learning and sleep in Drosophila long-term memory. Nat Commun 2022; 13:609. [PMID: 35105888 PMCID: PMC8807839 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-022-28256-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/16/2021] [Accepted: 01/07/2022] [Indexed: 12/02/2022] Open
Abstract
Animals retain some but not all experiences in long-term memory (LTM). Sleep supports LTM retention across animal species. It is well established that learning experiences enhance post-learning sleep. However, the underlying mechanisms of how learning mediates sleep for memory retention are not clear. Drosophila males display increased amounts of sleep after courtship learning. Courtship learning depends on Mushroom Body (MB) neurons, and post-learning sleep is mediated by the sleep-promoting ventral Fan-Shaped Body neurons (vFBs). We show that post-learning sleep is regulated by two opposing output neurons (MBONs) from the MB, which encode a measure of learning. Excitatory MBONs-γ2α'1 becomes increasingly active upon increasing time of learning, whereas inhibitory MBONs-β'2mp is activated only by a short learning experience. These MB outputs are integrated by SFS neurons, which excite vFBs to promote sleep after prolonged but not short training. This circuit may ensure that only longer or more intense learning experiences induce sleep and are thereby consolidated into LTM.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zhengchang Lei
- Janelia Research Campus, HHMI, 19700 Helix Dr, Ashburn, VA, 20147, USA
| | - Kristin Henderson
- Janelia Research Campus, HHMI, 19700 Helix Dr, Ashburn, VA, 20147, USA
| | - Krystyna Keleman
- Janelia Research Campus, HHMI, 19700 Helix Dr, Ashburn, VA, 20147, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Mechanisms of a near-orthogonal ultra-fast evolution of human behaviour as a source of culture development. Behav Brain Res 2020; 384:112521. [DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.112521] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/11/2019] [Revised: 01/24/2020] [Accepted: 01/29/2020] [Indexed: 12/15/2022]
|
6
|
Hu H, Kahrizi K, Musante L, Fattahi Z, Herwig R, Hosseini M, Oppitz C, Abedini SS, Suckow V, Larti F, Beheshtian M, Lipkowitz B, Akhtarkhavari T, Mehvari S, Otto S, Mohseni M, Arzhangi S, Jamali P, Mojahedi F, Taghdiri M, Papari E, Soltani Banavandi MJ, Akbari S, Tonekaboni SH, Dehghani H, Ebrahimpour MR, Bader I, Davarnia B, Cohen M, Khodaei H, Albrecht B, Azimi S, Zirn B, Bastami M, Wieczorek D, Bahrami G, Keleman K, Vahid LN, Tzschach A, Gärtner J, Gillessen-Kaesbach G, Varaghchi JR, Timmermann B, Pourfatemi F, Jankhah A, Chen W, Nikuei P, Kalscheuer VM, Oladnabi M, Wienker TF, Ropers HH, Najmabadi H. Genetics of intellectual disability in consanguineous families. Mol Psychiatry 2019; 24:1027-1039. [PMID: 29302074 DOI: 10.1038/s41380-017-0012-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 119] [Impact Index Per Article: 23.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/05/2016] [Revised: 10/19/2017] [Accepted: 10/30/2017] [Indexed: 01/17/2023]
Abstract
Autosomal recessive (AR) gene defects are the leading genetic cause of intellectual disability (ID) in countries with frequent parental consanguinity, which account for about 1/7th of the world population. Yet, compared to autosomal dominant de novo mutations, which are the predominant cause of ID in Western countries, the identification of AR-ID genes has lagged behind. Here, we report on whole exome and whole genome sequencing in 404 consanguineous predominantly Iranian families with two or more affected offspring. In 219 of these, we found likely causative variants, involving 77 known and 77 novel AR-ID (candidate) genes, 21 X-linked genes, as well as 9 genes previously implicated in diseases other than ID. This study, the largest of its kind published to date, illustrates that high-throughput DNA sequencing in consanguineous families is a superior strategy for elucidating the thousands of hitherto unknown gene defects underlying AR-ID, and it sheds light on their prevalence.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hao Hu
- Max-Planck-Institute for Molecular Genetics, 14195, Berlin, Germany.,Guangzhou Women and Children's Medical Center, 510623, Guangzhou, China
| | - Kimia Kahrizi
- Genetics Research Center, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, 19857, Iran
| | - Luciana Musante
- Max-Planck-Institute for Molecular Genetics, 14195, Berlin, Germany
| | - Zohreh Fattahi
- Genetics Research Center, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, 19857, Iran
| | - Ralf Herwig
- Max-Planck-Institute for Molecular Genetics, 14195, Berlin, Germany
| | - Masoumeh Hosseini
- Genetics Research Center, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, 19857, Iran
| | - Cornelia Oppitz
- IMP-Research Institute of Molecular Pathology, 1030, Vienna, Austria
| | - Seyedeh Sedigheh Abedini
- Genetics Research Center, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, 19857, Iran
| | - Vanessa Suckow
- Max-Planck-Institute for Molecular Genetics, 14195, Berlin, Germany
| | - Farzaneh Larti
- Genetics Research Center, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, 19857, Iran
| | - Maryam Beheshtian
- Genetics Research Center, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, 19857, Iran
| | | | - Tara Akhtarkhavari
- Genetics Research Center, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, 19857, Iran
| | - Sepideh Mehvari
- Genetics Research Center, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, 19857, Iran
| | - Sabine Otto
- Max-Planck-Institute for Molecular Genetics, 14195, Berlin, Germany
| | - Marzieh Mohseni
- Genetics Research Center, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, 19857, Iran
| | - Sanaz Arzhangi
- Genetics Research Center, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, 19857, Iran
| | - Payman Jamali
- Shahrood Genetic Counseling Center, Welfare Office, Semnan, 36156, Iran
| | - Faezeh Mojahedi
- Mashhad Medical Genetic Counseling Center, Mashhad, 91767, Iran
| | - Maryam Taghdiri
- Shiraz Genetic Counseling Center, Welfare Office, Shiraz, Iran
| | - Elaheh Papari
- Genetics Research Center, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, 19857, Iran
| | | | - Saeide Akbari
- Genetics Research Center, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, 19857, Iran
| | - Seyed Hassan Tonekaboni
- Pediatric Neurology Research Center, Mofid Children's Hospital, Shahid Beheshti University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, 15468, Iran
| | - Hossein Dehghani
- Genetics Research Center, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, 19857, Iran
| | - Mohammad Reza Ebrahimpour
- Genetics Research Center, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, 19857, Iran
| | - Ingrid Bader
- Kinderzentrum München, Technische Universität München, 81377, München, Germany
| | - Behzad Davarnia
- Genetics Research Center, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, 19857, Iran
| | - Monika Cohen
- Children's Center Munich, 81377, Munich, Germany
| | - Hossein Khodaei
- Meybod Genetics Research Center, Welfare Organization, Yazd, 89651, Iran
| | - Beate Albrecht
- Institute of Human Genetics, University Hospital Essen, 45122, Essen, Germany
| | - Sarah Azimi
- Genetics Research Center, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, 19857, Iran
| | - Birgit Zirn
- Genetikum Counseling Center, 70173, Stuttgart, Germany
| | - Milad Bastami
- Genetics Research Center, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, 19857, Iran
| | - Dagmar Wieczorek
- Institute of Human Genetics and Anthropology, Heinrich Heine University Düsseldorf, 40225, Düsseldorf, Germany
| | - Gholamreza Bahrami
- Genetics Research Center, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, 19857, Iran
| | - Krystyna Keleman
- IMP-Research Institute of Molecular Pathology, 1030, Vienna, Austria.,Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Janelia Research Campus, Ashburn, VA, 20147, USA
| | - Leila Nouri Vahid
- Genetics Research Center, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, 19857, Iran
| | - Andreas Tzschach
- Max-Planck-Institute for Molecular Genetics, 14195, Berlin, Germany.,Institute of Clinical Genetics, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
| | - Jutta Gärtner
- University Medical Center, Georg August University Göttingen, 37075, Göttingen, Germany
| | | | | | - Bernd Timmermann
- Max-Planck-Institute for Molecular Genetics, 14195, Berlin, Germany
| | | | - Aria Jankhah
- Shiraz Genetic Counseling Center, Shiraz, 71346, Iran
| | - Wei Chen
- Berlin Institute for Medical Systems Biology, Max Delbrueck Center for Molecular Medicine, 13125, Berlin, Germany
| | - Pooneh Nikuei
- Molecular Medicine Research Center, Hormozgan Health Institute, Hormozgan University of Medical Sciences, Bandar Abbas, Iran
| | | | - Morteza Oladnabi
- Genetics Research Center, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, 19857, Iran
| | - Thomas F Wienker
- Max-Planck-Institute for Molecular Genetics, 14195, Berlin, Germany
| | - Hans-Hilger Ropers
- Max-Planck-Institute for Molecular Genetics, 14195, Berlin, Germany. .,Institute of Human Genetics, University Medicine, Mainz, Germany.
| | - Hossein Najmabadi
- Genetics Research Center, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, 19857, Iran. .,Kariminejad - Najmabadi Pathology & Genetics Centre, Tehran, 14667-13713, Iran.
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Horiuchi J. Recurrent loops: Incorporating prediction error and semantic/episodic theories into Drosophila associative memory models. GENES BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR 2019; 18:e12567. [PMID: 30891930 PMCID: PMC6900151 DOI: 10.1111/gbb.12567] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/05/2018] [Revised: 02/27/2019] [Accepted: 03/16/2019] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
In 2003, Martin Heisenberg et al. presented a model of how associative memories could be encoded and stored in the insect brain. This model was extremely influential in the Drosophila memory field, but did not incorporate several important mammalian concepts, including ideas of separate episodic and semantic types of memory and prediction error hypotheses. In addition, at that time, the concept of memory traces recurrently entering and exiting the mushroom bodies, brain areas where associative memories are formed and stored, was unknown. In this review, I present a simple updated model incorporating these ideas, which may be useful for future studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Junjiro Horiuchi
- Department of higher brain functions and dementias, Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of Medical Science, Tokyo, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Dag U, Lei Z, Le JQ, Wong A, Bushey D, Keleman K. Neuronal reactivation during post-learning sleep consolidates long-term memory in Drosophila. eLife 2019; 8:42786. [PMID: 30801246 PMCID: PMC6428568 DOI: 10.7554/elife.42786] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/14/2018] [Accepted: 02/22/2019] [Indexed: 01/04/2023] Open
Abstract
Animals consolidate some, but not all, learning experiences into long-term memory. Across the animal kingdom, sleep has been found to have a beneficial effect on the consolidation of recently formed memories into long-term storage. However, the underlying mechanisms of sleep dependent memory consolidation are poorly understood. Here, we show that consolidation of courtship long-term memory in Drosophila is mediated by reactivation during sleep of dopaminergic neurons that were earlier involved in memory acquisition. We identify specific fan-shaped body neurons that induce sleep after the learning experience and activate dopaminergic neurons for memory consolidation. Thus, we provide a direct link between sleep, neuronal reactivation of dopaminergic neurons, and memory consolidation. Why do some memories fade after only a few seconds, whereas others last a lifetime? Studies suggest that part of the explanation has to do with sleep. Experiments in rodents show that neural circuits that are active during learning become active again when an animal sleeps. This process of reactivation, which may be akin to dreaming, helps strengthen specific memories and move them into long-term storage. But the complexity of the mammalian brain has made it difficult to pin down the underlying mechanisms. One possible solution is to study the mechanisms in a simpler brain with fewer neurons, such as that of the fruit fly Drosophila. Dag, Lei et al. have now used molecular genetic tools to explore how sleep supports a specific type of learning in male fruit flies, called courtship learning. Female fruit flies that have recently mated will reject the courtship efforts of other males. A male fly that experiences repeated rejections therefore learns to avoid mated females in future. This type of memory can last for at least a day – a long time in the life of a fly. Dag, Lei et al. show that males that experience repeated rejections subsequently spend more time asleep than control males. Preventing this sleep hinders the males from learning from their experience. But how does this process work? During sleep, specific dopamine neurons that were active during the learning episode become active once again. Blocking this reactivation prevents the flies from learning from their rejections. By contrast, artificially activating the dopamine neurons enables flies with only limited experience of rejection to learn to avoid mated females. Dag, Lei et al. show that neurons called vFB cells control this process. The vFB neurons both induce sleep and reactivate the memory-inducing dopamine neurons. These findings in fruit flies thus reveal a direct causal link between sleep, reactivation of memory traces, and persistence of memories. They also show that fruit flies are a valid model for exploring the neural and molecular mechanisms connecting sleep and long-term memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ugur Dag
- Janelia Research Campus, Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Ashburn, United States
| | - Zhengchang Lei
- Janelia Research Campus, Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Ashburn, United States
| | - Jasmine Q Le
- Janelia Research Campus, Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Ashburn, United States
| | - Allan Wong
- Janelia Research Campus, Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Ashburn, United States
| | - Daniel Bushey
- Janelia Research Campus, Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Ashburn, United States
| | - Krystyna Keleman
- Janelia Research Campus, Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Ashburn, United States
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Dore AA, McDowall L, Rouse J, Bretman A, Gage MJG, Chapman T. The role of complex cues in social and reproductive plasticity. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2018; 72:124. [PMID: 30100665 PMCID: PMC6060796 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-018-2539-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/01/2018] [Revised: 06/20/2018] [Accepted: 06/22/2018] [Indexed: 01/13/2023]
Abstract
Phenotypic plasticity can be a key determinant of fitness. The degree to which the expression of plasticity is adaptive relies upon the accuracy with which information about the state of the environment is integrated. This step might be particularly beneficial when environments, e.g. the social and sexual context, change rapidly. Fluctuating temporal dynamics could increase the difficulty of determining the appropriate level of expression of a plastic response. In this review, we suggest that new insights into plastic responses to the social and sexual environment (social and reproductive plasticity) may be gained by examining the role of complex cues (those comprising multiple, distinct sensory components). Such cues can enable individuals to more accurately monitor their environment in order to respond adaptively to it across the whole life course. We briefly review the hypotheses for the evolution of complex cues and then adapt these ideas to the context of social and sexual plasticity. We propose that the ability to perceive complex cues can facilitate plasticity, increase the associated fitness benefits and decrease the risk of costly 'mismatches' between phenotype and environment by (i) increasing the robustness of information gained from highly variable environments, (ii) fine-tuning responses by using multiple strands of information and (iii) reducing time lags in adaptive responses. We conclude by outlining areas for future research that will help to determine the interplay between complex cues and plasticity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alice A. Dore
- School of Biological Sciences, University of East Anglia, Norwich Research Park, Norwich, NR4 7TJ UK
| | - Laurin McDowall
- School of Biology, Faculty of Biological Sciences, University of Leeds, Leeds, LS2 9JT UK
| | - James Rouse
- School of Biology, Faculty of Biological Sciences, University of Leeds, Leeds, LS2 9JT UK
| | - Amanda Bretman
- School of Biology, Faculty of Biological Sciences, University of Leeds, Leeds, LS2 9JT UK
| | - Matthew J. G. Gage
- School of Biological Sciences, University of East Anglia, Norwich Research Park, Norwich, NR4 7TJ UK
| | - Tracey Chapman
- School of Biological Sciences, University of East Anglia, Norwich Research Park, Norwich, NR4 7TJ UK
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Liefting M, Hoedjes KM, Le Lann C, Smid HM, Ellers J. Selection for associative learning of color stimuli reveals correlated evolution of this learning ability across multiple stimuli and rewards. Evolution 2018; 72:1449-1459. [PMID: 29768649 PMCID: PMC6099215 DOI: 10.1111/evo.13498] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/20/2017] [Accepted: 04/15/2018] [Indexed: 01/19/2023]
Abstract
We are only starting to understand how variation in cognitive ability can result from local adaptations to environmental conditions. A major question in this regard is to what extent selection on cognitive ability in a specific context affects that ability in general through correlated evolution. To address this question, we performed artificial selection on visual associative learning in female Nasonia vitripennis wasps. Using appetitive conditioning in which a visual stimulus was offered in association with a host reward, the ability to learn visual associations was enhanced within 10 generations of selection. To test for correlated evolution affecting this form of learning, the ability to readily form learned associations in females was also tested using an olfactory instead of a visual stimulus in the appetitive conditioning. Additionally, we assessed whether the improved associative learning ability was expressed across sexes by color-conditioning males with a mating reward. Both females and males from the selected lines consistently demonstrated an increased associative learning ability compared to the control lines, independent of learning context or conditioned stimulus. No difference in relative volume of brain neuropils was detected between the selected and control lines.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maartje Liefting
- Animal EcologyVrije Universiteit AmsterdamAmsterdam1081 HVthe Netherlands
- Applied Zoology/Animal EcologyFreie Universität BerlinBerlinD‐12163Germany
| | - Katja M. Hoedjes
- Laboratory of EntomologyWageningen UniversityWageningen6700 AAthe Netherlands
- Department of Ecology and EvolutionUniversity of LausanneLausanneCH‐1015Switzerland
| | - Cécile Le Lann
- Animal EcologyVrije Universiteit AmsterdamAmsterdam1081 HVthe Netherlands
- CNRS, ECOBIO (Ecosystèmes, Biodiversité, Evolution)UMR 6553, Université de RennesRennesF‐35000France
| | - Hans M. Smid
- Laboratory of EntomologyWageningen UniversityWageningen6700 AAthe Netherlands
| | - Jacintha Ellers
- Animal EcologyVrije Universiteit AmsterdamAmsterdam1081 HVthe Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Todd RM, Manaligod MG. Implicit guidance of attention: The priority state space framework. Cortex 2018; 102:121-138. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.08.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/12/2017] [Revised: 05/09/2017] [Accepted: 08/01/2017] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
|
12
|
Zhao X, Lenek D, Dag U, Dickson BJ, Keleman K. Persistent activity in a recurrent circuit underlies courtship memory in Drosophila. eLife 2018; 7:31425. [PMID: 29322941 PMCID: PMC5800849 DOI: 10.7554/elife.31425] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/22/2017] [Accepted: 01/09/2018] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Recurrent connections are thought to be a common feature of the neural circuits that encode memories, but how memories are laid down in such circuits is not fully understood. Here we present evidence that courtship memory in Drosophila relies on the recurrent circuit between mushroom body gamma (MBγ), M6 output, and aSP13 dopaminergic neurons. We demonstrate persistent neuronal activity of aSP13 neurons and show that it transiently potentiates synaptic transmission from MBγ>M6 neurons. M6 neurons in turn provide input to aSP13 neurons, prolonging potentiation of MBγ>M6 synapses over time periods that match short-term memory. These data support a model in which persistent aSP13 activity within a recurrent circuit lays the foundation for a short-term memory. Memories help to shape behavior, and can last from a few seconds to an entire lifetime. Working memory, in which information is temporarily held available for use in an ongoing task, is the most fleeting form of memory. It relies on persistent activation of a network of nerve cells or neurons that represent the information in question. Strengthening the connections between those neurons may result in a longer-lasting memory. But the mechanisms that support the formation of memories of different durations are not fully understood. Zhao et al. have now explored these mechanisms in the fruit fly by studying memory for courtship behavior. Inexperienced male fruit flies will attempt to court both virgin females and females who have recently mated. But the latter reject courtship attempts, and male fruit flies therefore learn to avoid them. This is known as courtship memory, and it relies on a network of neurons within a region of the fruit fly brain called the mushroom body. Within the mushroom body, dopamine neuron sends signals to a neuron called the Kenyon cell, which in turn sends signals to a mushroom body output neuron. The latter activates circuits responsible for decision-making and movement. But it also activates the dopamine neuron, thereby forming a recurrent circuit or loop. When the courtship is rejected, the dopamine neuron becomes persistently active, which generates a working memory of the experience. If the circuit is activated again during this period of persistent firing, the working memory may be converted into a longer-lasting memory. The results of Zhao et al. provide insights into the mechanisms by which memories form and undergo strengthening. They suggest that distinct processes within a single neural circuit give rise to memories of different durations. Recurrent loops are also present within the brains of mammals. Similar processes may thus support the formation and persistence of our own memories.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Daniela Lenek
- Research Institute of Molecular Pathology, Vienna, Austria
| | - Ugur Dag
- Janelia Research Campus, Ashburn, United States
| | - Barry J Dickson
- Janelia Research Campus, Ashburn, United States.,Queensland Brain Institute, University of Queensland, St Lucia, Australia
| | - Krystyna Keleman
- Janelia Research Campus, Ashburn, United States.,Research Institute of Molecular Pathology, Vienna, Austria
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Trannoy S, Kravitz EA. Strategy changes in subsequent fights as consequences of winning and losing in fruit fly fights. Fly (Austin) 2016; 11:129-138. [PMID: 27834611 DOI: 10.1080/19336934.2016.1259041] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022] Open
Abstract
In competition for food, territory and mates, male fruit flies (Drosophila melanogaster) engage in agonistic encounters with conspecifics. The fighting strategies used to obtain these resources are influenced by previous and present experience, environmental cues, and the internal state of the animal including hormonal and genetic influences. Animals that experience prior defeats show submissive behavior and are more likely to lose 2nd contests, while animals that win 1st fights are more aggressive and have a higher probability of winning 2nd contests. In a recent report, we examined these loser and winner effects in greater detail and demonstrated that both winners and losers show short-term memory of the results of previous bouts while only losers demonstrate a longer-term memory that requires protein synthesis. The recent findings also suggested that an individual recognition mechanism likely exists that can serve important roles in evaluating the fighting ability of opponents and influencing future fighting strategy. In this article, we follow up on these results by asking how previous defeated and victorious flies change their fighting strategies in the presence of 2nd losing and winning flies, by searching for evidence of territory marking, and discussing the existing literature in light of our findings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Séverine Trannoy
- a Department of Neurobiology , Harvard Medical School , Boston , MA , USA
| | - Edward A Kravitz
- a Department of Neurobiology , Harvard Medical School , Boston , MA , USA
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Abstract
Localized protein translation is critical in many biological contexts, particularly in highly polarized cells, such as neurons, to regulate gene expression in a spatiotemporal manner. The cytoplasmic polyadenylation element-binding (CPEB) family of RNA-binding proteins has emerged as a key regulator of mRNA transport and local translation required for early embryonic development, synaptic plasticity, and long-term memory (LTM). Drosophila Orb and Orb2 are single members of the CPEB1 and CPEB2 subfamilies of the CPEB proteins, respectively. At present, the identity of the mRNA targets they regulate is not fully known, and the binding specificity of the CPEB2 subfamily is a matter of debate. Using transcriptome-wide UV cross-linking and immunoprecipitation, we define the mRNA-binding sites and targets of Drosophila CPEBs. Both Orb and Orb2 bind linear cytoplasmic polyadenylation element-like sequences in the 3' UTRs of largely overlapping target mRNAs, with Orb2 potentially having a broader specificity. Both proteins use their RNA-recognition motifs but not the Zinc-finger region for RNA binding. A subset of Orb2 targets is translationally regulated in cultured S2 cells and fly head extracts. Moreover, pan-neuronal RNAi knockdown of these targets suggests that a number of these targets are involved in LTM. Our results provide a comprehensive list of mRNA targets of the two CPEB proteins in Drosophila, thus providing insights into local protein synthesis involved in various biological processes, including LTM.
Collapse
|
15
|
Xu S, Elefant F. Tip off the HAT- Epigenetic control of learning and memory by Drosophila Tip60. Fly (Austin) 2016; 9:22-8. [PMID: 26327426 DOI: 10.1080/19336934.2015.1080887] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Disruption of epigenetic gene control mechanisms involving histone acetylation in the brain causes cognitive impairment, a debilitating hallmark of most neurodegenerative disorders. Histone acetylation regulates cognitive gene expression via chromatin packaging control in neurons. Unfortunately, the histone acetyltransferases (HATs) that generate such neural epigenetic signatures and their mechanisms of action remain unclear. Our recent findings provide insight into this question by demonstrating that Tip60 HAT action is critical for morphology and function of the mushroom body (MB), the learning and memory center in the Drosophila brain. We show that Tip60 is robustly produced in MB Kenyon cells and extending axonal lobes and that targeted MB Tip60 HAT loss results in axonal outgrowth disruption. Functional consequences of loss and gain of Tip60 HAT levels in the MB are evidenced by defects in memory. Tip60 ChIP-Seq analysis reveals enrichment for genes that function in cognitive processes and accordingly, key genes representing these pathways are misregulated in the Tip60 HAT mutant fly brain. Remarkably, increasing levels of Tip60 in the MB rescues learning and memory deficits resulting from Alzheimer's disease associated amyloid precursor protein (APP) induced neurodegeneration. Our studies highlight the potential of HAT activators as a therapeutic option for cognitive disorders.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Songjun Xu
- a Department of Biology ; Drexel University ; Philadelphia , PA USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Krüttner S, Traunmüller L, Dag U, Jandrasits K, Stepien B, Iyer N, Fradkin LG, Noordermeer JN, Mensh BD, Keleman K. Synaptic Orb2A Bridges Memory Acquisition and Late Memory Consolidation in Drosophila. Cell Rep 2015; 11:1953-65. [PMID: 26095367 PMCID: PMC4508346 DOI: 10.1016/j.celrep.2015.05.037] [Citation(s) in RCA: 56] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/02/2014] [Revised: 04/14/2015] [Accepted: 05/21/2015] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
Abstract
To adapt to an ever-changing environment, animals consolidate some, but not all, learning experiences to long-term memory. In mammals, long-term memory consolidation often involves neural pathway reactivation hours after memory acquisition. It is not known whether this delayed-reactivation schema is common across the animal kingdom or how information is stored during the delay period. Here, we show that, during courtship suppression learning, Drosophila exhibits delayed long-term memory consolidation. We also show that the same class of dopaminergic neurons engaged earlier in memory acquisition is also both necessary and sufficient for delayed long-term memory consolidation. Furthermore, we present evidence that, during learning, the translational regulator Orb2A tags specific synapses of mushroom body neurons for later consolidation. Consolidation involves the subsequent recruitment of Orb2B and the activity-dependent synthesis of CaMKII. Thus, our results provide evidence for the role of a neuromodulated, synapse-restricted molecule bridging memory acquisition and long-term memory consolidation in a learning animal. Drosophila exhibits late long-term memory consolidation DA-aSP13 neurons mediate both memory acquisition and memory consolidation Drosophila CPEB, Orb2, acts downstream of DopR1 in the MB γ neurons Synaptic Orb2A is required during memory acquisition and Orb2B during consolidation
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sebastian Krüttner
- Research Institute of Molecular Pathology, Doktor Bohr Gasse 7, A-1030 Vienna, Austria; Friedrich Miescher Institute for Biomedical Research, Maulbeerstrasse 66, 4058 Basel, Switzerland
| | - Lisa Traunmüller
- Research Institute of Molecular Pathology, Doktor Bohr Gasse 7, A-1030 Vienna, Austria; Biozentrum, University of Basel, Klingelbergstrasse 50-70, 4056 Basel, Switzerland
| | - Ugur Dag
- Janelia Research Campus, HHMI, 19700 Helix Drive, Ashburn, VA 20147, USA
| | - Katharina Jandrasits
- Research Institute of Molecular Pathology, Doktor Bohr Gasse 7, A-1030 Vienna, Austria
| | - Barbara Stepien
- Research Institute of Molecular Pathology, Doktor Bohr Gasse 7, A-1030 Vienna, Austria; The Max Planck Institute of Molecular Cell Biology and Genetics, Pfotenhauerstrasse 108, 01307 Dresden, Germany
| | - Nirmala Iyer
- Janelia Research Campus, HHMI, 19700 Helix Drive, Ashburn, VA 20147, USA
| | - Lee G Fradkin
- Department of Molecular Cell Biology, Leiden University Medical Center, 2300 RA Leiden, the Netherlands
| | - Jasprina N Noordermeer
- Department of Molecular Cell Biology, Leiden University Medical Center, 2300 RA Leiden, the Netherlands
| | - Brett D Mensh
- Janelia Research Campus, HHMI, 19700 Helix Drive, Ashburn, VA 20147, USA
| | - Krystyna Keleman
- Janelia Research Campus, HHMI, 19700 Helix Drive, Ashburn, VA 20147, USA; Research Institute of Molecular Pathology, Doktor Bohr Gasse 7, A-1030 Vienna, Austria.
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Zordan MA, Sandrelli F. Circadian Clock Dysfunction and Psychiatric Disease: Could Fruit Flies have a Say? Front Neurol 2015; 6:80. [PMID: 25941512 PMCID: PMC4403521 DOI: 10.3389/fneur.2015.00080] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/13/2015] [Accepted: 03/24/2015] [Indexed: 12/15/2022] Open
Abstract
There is evidence of a link between the circadian system and psychiatric diseases. Studies in humans and mammals suggest that environmental and/or genetic disruption of the circadian system leads to an increased liability to psychiatric disease. Disruption of clock genes and/or the clock network might be related to the etiology of these pathologies; also, some genes, known for their circadian clock functions, might be associated to mental illnesses through clock-independent pleiotropy. Here, we examine the features which we believe make Drosophila melanogaster a model apt to study the role of the circadian clock in psychiatric disease. Despite differences in the organization of the clock system, the molecular architecture of the Drosophila and mammalian circadian oscillators are comparable and many components are evolutionarily related. In addition, Drosophila has a rather complex nervous system, which shares much at the cell and neurobiological level with humans, i.e., a tripartite brain, the main neurotransmitter systems, and behavioral traits: circadian behavior, learning and memory, motivation, addiction, social behavior. There is evidence that the Drosophila brain shares some homologies with the vertebrate cerebellum, basal ganglia, and hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis, the dysfunctions of which have been tied to mental illness. We discuss Drosophila in comparison to mammals with reference to the: organization of the brain and neurotransmitter systems; architecture of the circadian clock; clock-controlled behaviors. We sum up current knowledge on behavioral endophenotypes, which are amenable to modeling in flies, such as defects involving sleep, cognition, or social interactions, and discuss the relationship of the circadian system to these traits. Finally, we consider if Drosophila could be a valuable asset to understand the relationship between circadian clock malfunction and psychiatric disease.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mauro Agostino Zordan
- Department of Biology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
- Cognitive Neuroscience Center, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
| | | |
Collapse
|
18
|
Abstract
Disruption of epigenetic gene control mechanisms in the brain causes significant cognitive impairment that is a debilitating hallmark of most neurodegenerative disorders, including Alzheimer's disease (AD). Histone acetylation is one of the best characterized of these epigenetic mechanisms that is critical for regulating learning- and memory- associated gene expression profiles, yet the specific histone acetyltransferases (HATs) that mediate these effects have yet to be fully characterized. Here, we investigate an epigenetic role for the HAT Tip60 in learning and memory formation using the Drosophila CNS mushroom body (MB) as a well-characterized cognition model. We show that Tip60 is endogenously expressed in the Kenyon cells, the intrinsic neurons of the MB, and in the MB axonal lobes. Targeted loss of Tip60 HAT activity in the MB causes thinner and shorter axonal lobes while increasing Tip60 HAT levels cause no morphological defects. Functional consequences of both loss and gain of Tip60 HAT levels in the MB are evidenced by defects in immediate-recall memory. Our ChIP-Seq analysis reveals that Tip60 target genes are enriched for functions in cognitive processes, and, accordingly, key genes representing these pathways are misregulated in the Tip60 HAT mutant fly brain. Remarkably, we find that both learning and immediate-recall memory deficits that occur under AD-associated, amyloid precursor protein (APP)-induced neurodegenerative conditions can be effectively rescued by increasing Tip60 HAT levels specifically in the MB. Together, our findings uncover an epigenetic transcriptional regulatory role for Tip60 in cognitive function and highlight the potential of HAT activators as a therapeutic option for neurodegenerative disorders.
Collapse
|
19
|
Reuter MS, Musante L, Hu H, Diederich S, Sticht H, Ekici AB, Uebe S, Wienker TF, Bartsch O, Zechner U, Oppitz C, Keleman K, Jamra RA, Najmabadi H, Schweiger S, Reis A, Kahrizi K. NDST1missense mutations in autosomal recessive intellectual disability. Am J Med Genet A 2014; 164A:2753-63. [DOI: 10.1002/ajmg.a.36723] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/07/2014] [Accepted: 07/09/2014] [Indexed: 12/13/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Miriam S. Reuter
- Institute of Human Genetics; Friedrich-Alexander-Universität Erlangen-Nürnberg; Erlangen Germany
| | - Luciana Musante
- Max-Planck-Institute for Molecular Genetics; Dept. Human Molecular Genetics; Berlin Germany
| | - Hao Hu
- Max-Planck-Institute for Molecular Genetics; Dept. Human Molecular Genetics; Berlin Germany
| | - Stefan Diederich
- Institute of Human Genetics; University Medical Center of the Johannes Gutenberg University Mainz; Mainz Germany
| | - Heinrich Sticht
- Institute of Biochemistry; Friedrich-Alexander-Universität Erlangen-Nürnberg; Erlangen Germany
| | - Arif B. Ekici
- Institute of Human Genetics; Friedrich-Alexander-Universität Erlangen-Nürnberg; Erlangen Germany
| | - Steffen Uebe
- Institute of Human Genetics; Friedrich-Alexander-Universität Erlangen-Nürnberg; Erlangen Germany
| | - Thomas F. Wienker
- Max-Planck-Institute for Molecular Genetics; Dept. Human Molecular Genetics; Berlin Germany
| | - Oliver Bartsch
- Institute of Human Genetics; University Medical Center of the Johannes Gutenberg University Mainz; Mainz Germany
| | - Ulrich Zechner
- Institute of Human Genetics; University Medical Center of the Johannes Gutenberg University Mainz; Mainz Germany
| | | | - Krystyna Keleman
- Research Institute of Molecular Pathology; Vienna Austria
- Janelia Farm Research Campus; Howard Hughes Medical Institute; Ashburn Virginia
| | - Rami Abou Jamra
- Institute of Human Genetics; Friedrich-Alexander-Universität Erlangen-Nürnberg; Erlangen Germany
| | - Hossein Najmabadi
- Genetics Research Center; University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences; Tehran Iran
- Kariminejad-Najmabadi Pathology & Genetics Center Tehran; Tehran Iran
| | - Susann Schweiger
- Institute of Human Genetics; University Medical Center of the Johannes Gutenberg University Mainz; Mainz Germany
| | - André Reis
- Institute of Human Genetics; Friedrich-Alexander-Universität Erlangen-Nürnberg; Erlangen Germany
| | - Kimia Kahrizi
- Genetics Research Center; University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences; Tehran Iran
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
McBride SMJ, Holloway SL, Jongens TA. Using Drosophila as a tool to identify pharmacological therapies for fragile X syndrome. DRUG DISCOVERY TODAY. TECHNOLOGIES 2014; 10:e129-36. [PMID: 24050241 DOI: 10.1016/j.ddtec.2012.09.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/05/2023]
Abstract
Despite obvious differences such as the ability to fly, the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster is similar to humans at many different levels of complexity. Studies of development, cell growth and division, metabolism and even cognition, have borne out these similarities. For example, Drosophila bearing mutations in the fly gene homologue of the known human disease fragile X are affected in fundamentally similar ways as affected humans. The ramification of this degree of similarity is that Drosophila, as a model organism, is a rich resource for learning about human cells, development and even human cognition and behavior. Drosophila has a short generation time of ten days, is cheap to propagate and maintain and has a vast array of genetic tools available to it; making Drosophila an extremely attractive organism for the study of human disease. Here, we summarize research from our lab and others using Drosophila to understand the human neurological disease, called fragile X. We focus on the Drosophila model of fragile X, its characterization, and use as a tool to identify potential drugs for the treatment of fragile X. Several clinical trials are in progress now that were motivated by this research.
Collapse
|
21
|
Baggio F, Bozzato A, Benna C, Leonardi E, Romoli O, Cognolato M, Tosatto SCE, Costa R, Sandrelli F. 2mit, an intronic gene of Drosophila melanogaster timeless2, is involved in behavioral plasticity. PLoS One 2013; 8:e76351. [PMID: 24098788 PMCID: PMC3786989 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076351] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/28/2012] [Accepted: 08/27/2013] [Indexed: 12/15/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Intronic genes represent ~6% of the total gene complement in Drosophila melanogaster and ~85% of them encode for proteins. We recently characterized the D. melanogastertimeless2 (tim2) gene, showing its active involvement in chromosomal stability and light synchronization of the adult circadian clock. The protein coding gene named 2mit maps on the 11thtim2 intron in the opposite transcriptional orientation. Methodology/Principal Findings Here we report the molecular and functional characterization of 2mit. The 2mit gene is expressed throughout Drosophila development, localizing mainly in the nervous system during embryogenesis and mostly in the mushroom bodies and ellipsoid body of the central complex in the adult brain. Insilico analyses revealed that 2mit encodes a putative leucine-Rich Repeat transmembrane receptor with intrinsically disordered regions, harboring several fully conserved functional interaction motifs in the cytosolic side. Using insertional mutations, tissue-specific over-expression, and down-regulation approaches, it was found that 2mit is implicated in adult short-term memory, assessed by a courtship conditioning assay. In D. melanogaster, tim2 and 2mit do not seem to be functionally related. Bioinformatic analyses identified 2MIT orthologs in 21 Drosophilidae, 4 Lepidoptera and in Apis mellifera. In addition, the tim2-2mit host-nested gene organization was shown to be present in A. mellifera and maintained among Drosophila species. Within the Drosophilidae 2mit-hosting tim2 intron, insilico approaches detected a neuronal specific transcriptional binding site which might have contributed to preserve the specific host-nested gene association across Drosophila species. Conclusions/Significance Taken together, these results indicate that 2mit, a gene mainly expressed in the nervous system, has a role in the behavioral plasticity of the adult Drosophila. The presence of a putative 2mit regulatory enhancer within the 2mit-hosting tim2 intron could be considered an evolutionary constraint potentially involved in maintaining the tim2-2mit host-nested chromosomal architecture during the evolution of Drosophila species.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Francesca Baggio
- Dipartimento di Biologia, Università degli Studi di Padova Padova, Italy
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
22
|
Dukas R, Dukas L. Learning about prospective mates in male fruit flies: effects of acceptance and rejection. Anim Behav 2012. [DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.09.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/05/2023]
|
23
|
Identification of gene expression changes associated with long-term memory of courtship rejection in Drosophila males. G3-GENES GENOMES GENETICS 2012; 2:1437-45. [PMID: 23173095 PMCID: PMC3484674 DOI: 10.1534/g3.112.004119] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/17/2012] [Accepted: 09/16/2012] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
Long-term memory formation in Drosophila melanogaster is an important neuronal function shaping the insect’s behavioral repertoire by allowing an individual to modify behaviors on the basis of previous experiences. In conditioned courtship or courtship suppression, male flies that have been repeatedly rejected by mated females during courtship advances are less likely than naïve males to subsequently court another mated female. This long-term courtship suppression can last for several days after the initial rejection period. Although genes with known functions in many associative learning paradigms, including those that function in cyclic AMP signaling and RNA translocation, have been identified as playing critical roles in long-term conditioned courtship, it is clear that additional mechanisms also contribute. We have used RNA sequencing to identify differentially expressed genes and transcript isoforms between naïve males and males subjected to courtship-conditioning regimens that are sufficient for inducing long-term courtship suppression. Transcriptome analyses 24 hours after the training regimens revealed differentially expressed genes and transcript isoforms with predicted and known functions in nervous system development, chromatin biology, translation, cytoskeletal dynamics, and transcriptional regulation. A much larger number of differentially expressed transcript isoforms were identified, including genes previously implicated in associative memory and neuronal development, including fruitless, that may play functional roles in learning during courtship conditioning. Our results shed light on the complexity of the genetics that underlies this behavioral plasticity and reveal several new potential areas of inquiry for future studies.
Collapse
|
24
|
Krüttner S, Stepien B, Noordermeer JN, Mommaas MA, Mechtler K, Dickson BJ, Keleman K. Drosophila CPEB Orb2A mediates memory independent of Its RNA-binding domain. Neuron 2012; 76:383-95. [PMID: 23083740 PMCID: PMC3480640 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.028] [Citation(s) in RCA: 56] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 08/13/2012] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Long-term memory and synaptic plasticity are thought to require the synthesis of new proteins at activated synapses. The CPEB family of RNA binding proteins, including Drosophila Orb2, has been implicated in this process. The precise mechanism by which these molecules regulate memory formation is however poorly understood. We used gene targeting and site-specific transgenesis to specifically modify the endogenous orb2 gene in order to investigate its role in long-term memory formation. We show that the Orb2A and Orb2B isoforms, while both essential, have distinct functions in memory formation. These two isoforms have common glutamine-rich and RNA-binding domains, yet Orb2A uniquely requires the former and Orb2B the latter. We further show that Orb2A induces Orb2 complexes in a manner dependent upon both its glutamine-rich region and neuronal activity. We propose that Orb2B acts as a conventional CPEB to regulate transport and/or translation of specific mRNAs, whereas Orb2A acts in an unconventional manner to form stable Orb2 complexes that are essential for memory to persist.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sebastian Krüttner
- Research Institute of Molecular Pathology, Dr. Bohrgasse 7, A-1030 Vienna, Austria
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
25
|
Vinauger C, Buratti L, Lazzari CR. Learning the way to blood: first evidence of dual olfactory conditioning in a blood-sucking insect, Rhodnius prolixus. I. Appetitive learning. J Exp Biol 2011; 214:3032-8. [DOI: 10.1242/jeb.056697] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
SUMMARY
It has been largely assumed that the individual experience of insects that are disease vectors might not only contribute to animal fitness, but also have an important influence on parasite transmission. Nevertheless, despite the invested efforts in testing the capacity to learn and remember information in blood-sucking insects, only little conclusive information has been obtained to date. Adapting a classical conditioning approach to our haematophagous model, we trained larvae of Rhodnius prolixus to associate l-lactic-acid, an odour perceived by these bugs but behaviourally neutral when presented alone, with food (i.e. positive reinforcement). Naive bugs – those exposed either to a conditioned stimulus (CS, l-lactic acid), unconditioned stimulus (US, heat) and reward (blood) alone or CS, US and reward in the absence of contingency – remained indifferent to the presence of an air stream loaded with l-lactic acid when tested in an olfactometer (random orientation), whereas the groups previously exposed to the contingency CS–US–reward (blood) were significantly attracted by l-lactic-acid. In a companion paper, the opposite, i.e. repellence, was induced in bugs exposed to the contingency of the same odour with a negative reinforcement. This constitutes the first evidence of olfactory conditioning in triatomine bugs, vectors of Chagas disease, and one of the few substantiations available to date of olfactory conditioning in haematophagous insects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Clément Vinauger
- Institut de Recherche sur la Biologie de l'Insecte, UMR CNRS 6035, Université François Rabelais, 37200 Tours, France
| | - Laura Buratti
- Institut de Recherche sur la Biologie de l'Insecte, UMR CNRS 6035, Université François Rabelais, 37200 Tours, France
| | - Claudio R. Lazzari
- Institut de Recherche sur la Biologie de l'Insecte, UMR CNRS 6035, Université François Rabelais, 37200 Tours, France
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Inoshita T, Martin JR, Marion-Poll F, Ferveur JF. Peripheral, central and behavioral responses to the cuticular pheromone bouquet in Drosophila melanogaster males. PLoS One 2011; 6:e19770. [PMID: 21625481 PMCID: PMC3098836 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0019770] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/03/2011] [Accepted: 04/04/2011] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Pheromonal communication is crucial with regard to mate choice in many animals including insects. Drosophila melanogaster flies produce a pheromonal bouquet with many cuticular hydrocarbons some of which diverge between the sexes and differently affect male courtship behavior. Cuticular pheromones have a relatively high weight and are thought to be -- mostly but not only -- detected by gustatory contact. However, the response of the peripheral and central gustatory systems to these substances remains poorly explored. We measured the effect induced by pheromonal cuticular mixtures on (i) the electrophysiological response of peripheral gustatory receptor neurons, (ii) the calcium variation in brain centers receiving these gustatory inputs and (iii) the behavioral reaction induced in control males and in mutant desat1 males, which show abnormal pheromone production and perception. While male and female pheromones induced inhibitory-like effects on taste receptor neurons, the contact of male pheromones on male fore-tarsi elicits a long-lasting response of higher intensity in the dedicated gustatory brain center. We found that the behavior of control males was more strongly inhibited by male pheromones than by female pheromones, but this difference disappeared in anosmic males. Mutant desat1 males showed an increased sensitivity of their peripheral gustatory neurons to contact pheromones and a behavioral incapacity to discriminate sex pheromones. Together our data indicate that cuticular hydrocarbons induce long-lasting inhibitory effects on the relevant taste pathway which may interact with the olfactory pathway to modulate pheromonal perception.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tsuyoshi Inoshita
- Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation, Unité Mixte de Recherche-6265 Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Unité Mixte de Recherche-1324 Institut National de la Recherche Agronomique, Université de Bourgogne, Dijon, France
| | - Jean-René Martin
- Laboratoire de Neurobiologie and Développement, Unité Propre de Recherche 3294, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique - Institut de Neurobiologie Alfred Fessard, Gif-sur-Yvette, France
| | - Frédéric Marion-Poll
- Physiologie de l'Insecte: Signalisation et Communication, Unité Mixte de Recherche-1272 Institut National de la Recherche Agronomique/Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Versailles, France
- AgroParisTech, Département Science de la Vie et Santé, Paris, France
| | - Jean-François Ferveur
- Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation, Unité Mixte de Recherche-6265 Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Unité Mixte de Recherche-1324 Institut National de la Recherche Agronomique, Université de Bourgogne, Dijon, France
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Mating alters gene expression patterns in Drosophila melanogaster male heads. BMC Genomics 2010; 11:558. [PMID: 20937114 PMCID: PMC3091707 DOI: 10.1186/1471-2164-11-558] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/20/2010] [Accepted: 10/11/2010] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Behavior is a complex process resulting from the integration of genetic and environmental information. Drosophila melanogaster rely on multiple sensory modalities for reproductive success, and mating causes physiological changes in both sexes that affect reproductive output or behavior. Some of these effects are likely mediated by changes in gene expression. Courtship and mating alter female transcript profiles, but it is not known how mating affects male gene expression. Results We used Drosophila genome arrays to identify changes in gene expression profiles that occur in mated male heads. Forty-seven genes differed between mated and control heads 2 hrs post mating. Many mating-responsive genes are highly expressed in non-neural head tissues, including an adipose tissue called the fat body. One fat body-enriched gene, female-specific independent of transformer (fit), is a downstream target of the somatic sex-determination hierarchy, a genetic pathway that regulates Drosophila reproductive behaviors as well as expression of some fat-expressed genes; three other mating-responsive loci are also downstream components of this pathway. Another mating-responsive gene expressed in fat, Juvenile hormone esterase (Jhe), is necessary for robust male courtship behavior and mating success. Conclusions Our study demonstrates that mating causes changes in male head gene expression profiles and supports an increasing body of work implicating adipose signaling in behavior modulation. Since several mating-induced genes are sex-determination hierarchy target genes, additional mating-responsive loci may be downstream components of this pathway as well.
Collapse
|
28
|
A single social defeat reduces aggression in a highly aggressive strain of Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2010; 107:12682-6. [PMID: 20616023 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1007016107] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Genes and prior experience both influence the behavior of animals, but the relative contribution of each to fighting behavior in Drosophila remains unclear. To address this issue, we bred hyperaggressive flies by selecting winners of fights over 34-37 generations. Males of this strain initiate fights sooner, retaliate more often, and regularly defeat opponents from the nonselected parent Canton-S strain. After a defeat, however, these highly aggressive flies lose their second fights against socially naïve counterparts. Defeated flies also lunge and retaliate less after experiencing a loss, suggesting that the subsequent losses result from flies becoming less aggressive. Remarkably, flies that were once capable of engaging in high-intensity boxing and tussling patterns of behavior for extended periods of time often do not even engage in mid-intensity lunging after a single defeat. Furthermore, these formerly highly aggressive flies lose all competitive advantage over nonselected Canton-S after experiencing a loss. Lastly, females were more likely to copulate with males from the nonselected parent line than with the hyperaggressive strain.
Collapse
|
29
|
Abstract
Circadian clocks organize behavior and physiology to adapt to daily environmental cycles. Genetic approaches in the fruit fly, Drosophila melanogaster, have revealed widely conserved molecular gears of these 24-h timers. Yet much less is known about how these cell-autonomous clocks confer temporal information to modulate cellular functions. Here we discuss our current knowledge of circadian clock function in Drosophila, providing an overview of the molecular underpinnings of circadian clocks. We then describe the neural network important for circadian rhythms of locomotor activity, including how these molecular clocks might influence neuronal function. Finally, we address a range of behaviors and physiological systems regulated by circadian clocks, including discussion of specific peripheral oscillators and key molecular effectors where they have been described. These studies reveal a remarkable complexity to circadian pathways in this "simple" model organism.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ravi Allada
- Department of Neurobiology and Physiology, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL 60208, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
30
|
Donlea JM, Shaw PJ. Sleeping together using social interactions to understand the role of sleep in plasticity. ADVANCES IN GENETICS 2010; 68:57-81. [PMID: 20109659 DOI: 10.1016/s0065-2660(09)68003-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
Abstract
Social experience alters the expression of genes related to synaptic function and plasticity, induces elaborations in the morphology of neural structures throughout the brain (Volkmar and Greenough, 1972; Greenough et al., 1978; Technau, 2007), improves cognitive and behavioral performance (Pham et al., 1999a; Toscano et al., 2006) and alters subsequent sleep (Ganguly-Fitzgerald et al., 2006). In this review, we discuss the plastic mechanisms that are induced in response to social experience and how social enrichment can provide insight into the biological functions of sleep.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jeffrey M Donlea
- Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology, Washington University School of Medicine, Campus Box 8108, St. Louis, Missouri, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
31
|
ELLIS LL, CARNEY GE. Drosophila melanogastermales respond differently at the behavioural and genome-wide levels toDrosophila melanogasterandDrosophila simulansfemales. J Evol Biol 2009; 22:2183-91. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2009.01834.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
|
32
|
Moran CN, Kyriacou CP. Functional neurogenomics of the courtship song of male Drosophila melanogaster. Cortex 2009; 45:18-34. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.05.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/20/2007] [Revised: 01/17/2008] [Accepted: 05/25/2008] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
|
33
|
Affiliation(s)
- Rozi Andretic
- The Neuroscience Institute, San Diego, California 92121
| | - Paul Franken
- Center for Integrative Genomics (CIG), University of Lausanne, 1015 Lausanne, Switzerland;
| | - Mehdi Tafti
- Center for Integrative Genomics (CIG), University of Lausanne, 1015 Lausanne, Switzerland;
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Sex-specific splicing in Drosophila: widespread occurrence, tissue specificity and evolutionary conservation. Genetics 2008; 181:421-34. [PMID: 19015538 DOI: 10.1534/genetics.108.096743] [Citation(s) in RCA: 59] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/25/2023] Open
Abstract
Many genes in eukaryotic genomes produce multiple transcripts through a variety of molecular mechanisms including alternative splicing. Alternatively spliced transcripts often encode functionally distinct proteins, indicating that gene regulation at this level makes an important contribution to organismal complexity. The multilevel splicing cascade that regulates sex determination and sex-specific development in Drosophila is a classical example of the role of alternative splicing in cell differentiation. Recent evidence suggests that a large proportion of genes in the Drosophila genome may be spliced in a sex-biased fashion, raising the possibility that alternative splicing may play a more general role in sexually dimorphic development and physiology. However, the prevalence of sex-specific splicing and the extent to which it is shared among genotypes are not fully understood. Genetic variation in the splicing of key components of the sex determination pathway is known to influence the expression of downstream target genes, suggesting that alternative splicing at other loci may also vary in functionally important ways. In this study, we used exon-specific microarrays to examine 417 multitranscript genes for evidence of sex-specific and genotype-specific splicing in 80 different genotypes of Drosophila melanogaster. Most of these loci showed sex-biased splicing, whereas genotype-specific splicing was rare. One hundred thirty-five genes showed different alternative transcript use in males vs. females. Real-time PCR analysis of 6 genes chosen to represent a broad range of biological functions showed that most sex-biased splicing occurs in the gonads. However, somatic tissues, particularly adult heads, also show evidence of sex-specific splicing. Comparison of splicing patterns at orthologous loci in seven Drosophila species shows that sexual biases in alternative exon representation are highly conserved, indicating that sex-specific splicing is an ancient feature of Drosophila biology. To investigate potential mechanisms of sex-biased splicing, we used real-time PCR to examine the expression of six known regulators of alternative splicing in males vs. females. We found that all six loci are themselves spliced sex specifically in gonads and heads, suggesting that regulatory hierarchies based on alternative splicing may be an important feature of sexual differentiation.
Collapse
|
35
|
Savvateeva-Popova E, Medvedeva A, Popov A, Evgen'ev M. Role of non-coding RNAs in neurodegeneration and stress response in Drosophila. Biotechnol J 2008; 3:1010-21. [PMID: 18702036 DOI: 10.1002/biot.200800120] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/23/2022]
Abstract
The inherent limitations of genetic analysis in humans and other mammals as well as striking conservation of most genes controlling nervous system functioning in flies and mammals made Drosophila an attractive model to investigate various aspects of brain diseases. Since RNA research has made great progress in recent years here we present an overview of studies demonstrating the role of various non-coding RNAs in neurodegeneration and stress response in Drosophila as a model organism. We put special emphasis on the role of non-coding micro RNAs, hsr-omega transcripts, and artificial small highly structured RNAs as triggers of neuropathology including aggregates formation, cognitive abnormalities and other symptoms. Cellular stress is a conspicuous feature of many neurodegenerative diseases and the production of specialized proteins protects the nerve cells against aggregates formation. Therefore, herein we describe some data implicating various classes of non-coding RNAs in stress response in Drosophila. All these findings highlight Drosophila as an important model system to investigate various brain diseases potentially mediated by some non-coding RNAs including polyglutamine diseases, Alzheimer's disease, Huntigton's disease, and many others.
Collapse
|
36
|
Non-coding RNA as a trigger of neuropathologic disorder phenotypes in transgenic Drosophila. J Neural Transm (Vienna) 2008; 115:1629-42. [PMID: 18779919 DOI: 10.1007/s00702-008-0078-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/29/2008] [Accepted: 06/01/2008] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
At most, many protein-misfolding diseases develop as environmentally induced sporadic disorders. Recent studies indicate that the dynamic interplay between a wide repertoire of noncoding RNAs and the environment play an important role in brain development and pathogenesis of brain disorders. To elucidate this new issue, novel animal models which reproduce the most prominent disease manifestations are required. For this, transgenic Drosophila strains were constructed to express small highly structured, non-coding RNA under control of a heat shock promoter. Expression of the RNA induced formation of intracellular aggregates revealed by Thioflafin T in embryonic cell culture and Congo Red in the brain of transgenic flies. Also, this strongly perturbed the brain control of locomotion monitored by the parameters of sound production and memory retention of young 5-day-old males. This novel model demonstrates that expression of non-coding RNA alone is sufficient to trigger neuropathology.
Collapse
|
37
|
Abstract
The mushroom body (MB) of the insect brain has important roles in odor learning and memory and in diverse other brain functions. To elucidate the anatomical basis underlying its function, we studied how the MB of Drosophila is organized by its intrinsic and extrinsic neurons. We screened for the GAL4 enhancer-trap strains that label specific subsets of these neurons and identified seven subtypes of Kenyon cells and three other intrinsic neuron types. Laminar organization of the Kenyon cell axons divides the pedunculus into at least five concentric strata. The alpha', beta', alpha, and beta lobes are each divided into three strata, whereas the gamma lobe appears more homogeneous. The outermost stratum of the alpha/beta lobes is specifically connected with a small, protruded subregion of the calyx, the accessory calyx, which does not receive direct olfactory input. As for the MB extrinsic neurons (MBENs), we found three types of antennal lobe projection neurons, among which two are novel. In addition, we resolved 17 other types of MBENs that arborize in the calyx, lobes, and pedunculus. Lobe-associated MBENs arborize in only specific areas of the lobes, being restricted along their longitudinal axes, forming two to five segmented zones in each lobe. The laminar arrangement of the Kenyon cell axons and segmented organization of the MBENs together divide the lobes into smaller synaptic units, possibly facilitating characteristic interaction between intrinsic and extrinsic neurons in each unit for different functional activities along the longitudinal lobe axes and between lobes. Structural differences between lobes are also discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nobuaki K Tanaka
- Institute of Molecular and Cellular Biosciences, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo 113-0032, Japan
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
38
|
Abstract
The reproductive biology of Drosophila melanogaster is described and critically discussed, primarily with regard to genetic studies of sex-specific behavior and its neural underpinnings. The investigatory history of this system includes, in addition to a host of recent neurobiological analyses of reproductive phenotypes, studies of mating as well as the behaviors leading up to that event. Courtship and mating have been delved into mostly with regard to male-specific behavior and biology, although a small number of studies has also pointed to the neural substrates of female reproduction. Sensory influences on interactions between courting flies have long been studied, partly by application of mutants and partly by surgical experiments. More recently, molecular-genetic approaches to sensations passing between flies in reproductive contexts have aimed to "dissect" further the meaning of separate sensory modalities. Notable among these are olfactory and contact-chemosensory stimuli, which perhaps have received an inordinate amount of attention in terms of the possibility that they could comprise the key cues involved in triggering and sustaining courtship actions. But visual and auditory stimuli are heavily involved as well--appreciated mainly from older experiments, but analyzable further using elementary approaches (single-gene mutations mutants and surgeries), as well as by applying the molecularly defined factors alluded to above. Regarding regulation of reproductive behavior by components of Drosophila's central nervous system (CNS), once again significant invigoration of the relevant inquiries has been stimulated and propelled by identification and application of molecular-genetic materials. A distinct plurality of the tools applied involves transposons inserted in the fly's chromosomes, defining "enhancer-trap" strains that can be used to label various portions of the nervous system and, in parallel, disrupt their structure and function by "driving" companion transgenes predesigned for these experimental purposes. Thus, certain components of interneuronal routes, functioning along pathways whose starting points are sensory reception by the peripheral nervous system (PNS), have been manipulated to enhance appreciation of sexually important sensory modalities, as well as to promote understanding of where such inputs end up within the CNS: Where are reproductively related stimuli processed, such that different kinds of sensation would putatively be integrated to mediate sex-specific behavioral readouts? In line with generic sensory studies that have tended to concentrate on chemical stimuli, PNS-to-CNS pathways focused upon in reproductive experiments relying on genic enhancers have mostly involved smell and taste. Enhancer traps have also been applied to disrupt various regions within the CNS to ask about the various ganglia, and portions thereof, that contribute to male- or female-specific behavior. These manipulations have encompassed structural or functional disruptions of such regions as well as application of molecular-genetic tricks to feminize or masculinize a given component of the CNS. Results of such experiments have, indeed, identified certain discrete subsets of centrally located ganglia that, on the one hand, lead to courtship defects when disrupted or, on the other, must apparently maintain sex-specific identity if the requisite courtship actions are to be performed. As just implied, perturbations of certain neural tissues not based on manipulating "sex factors" might lead to reproductive behavioral abnormalities, even though changing the sexual identity of such structures would not necessarily have analogous consequences. It has been valuable to uncover these sexually significant subsets of the Drosophila nervous system, although it must be said that not all of the transgenically based dissection outcomes are in agreement. Thus, the good news is that not all of the CNS is devoted to courtship control, whereby any and all locales disrupted might have led to sex-specific deficits; but the bad news is that the enhancer-trap approach to these matters has not led to definitive homing-in on some tractable number of mutually agreed-upon "courtship centers" within the brain or within the ventral nerve cord (VNC). The latter neural region, which comprises about half of the fly's CNS, is underanalyzed as to its sex-specific significance: How, for example, are various kinds of sensory inputs to posteriorly located PNS structures processed, such that they eventually end up modulating brain functions underlying courtship? And how are sex-specific motor outputs mediated by discrete collections of neurons within VNC ganglia--so that, for instance, male-specific whole-animal motor actions and appendage usages are evoked? These behaviors can be thought of as fixed action patterns. But it is increasingly appreciated that elements of the fly's reproductive behavior can be modulated by previous experience. In this regard, the neural substrates of conditioned courtship are being more and more analyzed, principally by further usages of various transgenic types. Additionally, a set of molecular neurogenetic experiments devoted to experience-dependent courtship was based on manipulations of a salient "sex gene" in D. melanogaster. This well-defined factor is called fruitless (fru). The gene, its encoded products, along with their behavioral and neurobiological significance, have become objects of frenetic attention in recent years. How normal, mutated, and molecularly manipulated forms of fru seem to be generating a good deal of knowledge and insight about male-specific courtship and mating is worthy of much attention. This previews the fact that fruitless matters are woven throughout this chapter as well as having a conspicuous section allocated to them. Finally, an acknowledgment that the reader is being subjected to lengthy preview of an article about this subject is given. This matter is mentioned because--in conjunction with the contemporary broadening and deepening of this investigatory area--brief summaries of its findings are appearing with increasing frequency. This chapter will, from time to time, present our opinion that a fair fraction of the recent minireviews are replete with too many catch phrases about what is really known. This is one reason why the treatment that follows not only attempts to describe the pertinent primary reports in detail but also pauses often to discuss our views about current understandings of sex-specific behavior in Drosophila and its underlying biology.
Collapse
|
39
|
Knight D, Iliadi K, Charlton MP, Atwood HL, Boulianne GL. Presynaptic plasticity and associative learning are impaired in a Drosophila presenilin null mutant. Dev Neurobiol 2007; 67:1598-613. [PMID: 17562530 DOI: 10.1002/dneu.20532] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
Abstract
Alzheimer's disease is a neurodegenerative disorder characterized by progressive memory and cognitive decline that is associated with changes in synaptic plasticity and neuronal cell loss. Recent evidence suggests that some of these defects may be due to a loss of normal presenilin activity. Here, we have examined the effect of loss of Drosophila presenilin (psn) function on synaptic plasticity and learning. Basal transmitter release was elevated in psn mutants while both paired pulse synaptic plasticity and post-tetanic potentiation were impaired. These defects in synaptic strength and plasticity were not due to developmental defects in NMJ morphology. We also found that psn null terminals take up significantly less FM 4-64 than control terminals when loaded with high frequency stimulation, suggesting a defect in synaptic vesicle availability or mobilization. To determine whether these reductions in synaptic plasticity had any impact on learning, we tested the larvae for defects in associative learning. Using both olfactory and visual learning assays, we found that associative learning is impaired in psn mutants compared with controls. Both the learning and synaptic defects could be rescued by expression of a full length psn transgene suggesting the defects are specifically due to a loss of psn function. Taken together, these results provide the first evidence of learning and synaptic defects in a Drosophila psn mutant and strongly suggest a presynaptic role for presenilin in normal neuronal function.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David Knight
- Program in Developmental and Stem Cell Biology, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, Ontario, Canada M5G 1L7
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
40
|
Harden MV, Newton LA, Lloyd RC, Whitlock KE. Olfactory imprinting is correlated with changes in gene expression in the olfactory epithelia of the zebrafish. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2007; 66:1452-66. [PMID: 17013923 DOI: 10.1002/neu.20328] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
Abstract
Odors experienced as juveniles can have significant effects on the behavior of mature organisms. A dramatic example of this occurs in salmon, where the odors experienced by developing fish determine the river to which they return as adults. Further examples of olfactory memories are found in many animals including vertebrates and invertebrates. Yet, the cellular and molecular bases underlying the formation of olfactory memory are poorly understood. We have devised a series of experiments to determine whether zebrafish can form olfactory memories much like those observed in salmonids. Here we show for the first time that zebrafish form and retain olfactory memories of an artificial odorant, phenylethyl alcohol (PEA), experienced as juveniles. Furthermore, we demonstrate that exposure to PEA results in changes in gene expression within the olfactory sensory system. These changes are evident by in situ hybridization in the olfactory epithelium of the developing zebrafish. Strikingly, our analysis by in situ hybridization demonstrates that the transcription factor, otx2, is up regulated in the olfactory sensory epithelia in response to PEA. This increase is evident at 2-3 days postfertilization and is maintained in the adult animals. We propose that the changes in otx2 gene expression are manifest as an increase in the number of neuronal precursors in the cells olfactory epithelium of the odor-exposed fish. Thus, our results reveal a role for the environment in controlling gene expression in the developing peripheral nervous system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maegan V Harden
- Department of Molecular Biology and Genetics, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York 14853, USA
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
41
|
Isiegas C, Park A, Kandel ER, Abel T, Lattal KM. Transgenic inhibition of neuronal protein kinase A activity facilitates fear extinction. J Neurosci 2006; 26:12700-7. [PMID: 17151273 PMCID: PMC2910919 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.2743-06.2006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 60] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/27/2006] [Revised: 10/03/2006] [Accepted: 10/04/2006] [Indexed: 01/27/2023] Open
Abstract
Much is known about the neurobiology of memory storage for learned fear. In contrast, the molecular mechanisms underlying extinction of fear memory are just beginning to be delineated. Here, we investigate the role of protein kinase A (PKA) in extinction of memory for contextual fear by using conventional and temporally regulated transgenic approaches that allow us to inhibit PKA activity in neurons within brain regions thought to be involved in extinction. Strikingly, reduction of PKA activity facilitated the development of extinction, without interfering with storage of the original fear memory. Moreover, inhibition of PKA facilitated extinction of both recent and remote contextual fear memories. The finding that PKA, which is required for the acquisition of fear memory, is a constraint for extinction provides the first genetic support for the idea that fear extinction is itself a genuine learning process with its own specific molecular requirements, rather than simply the erasure of a previously learned process. Further, these experiments represent the first genetic evidence that protein kinases may be constraints for the extinction of fear.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carolina Isiegas
- Department of Biology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 19104
| | - Alice Park
- Department of Genetics, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, Connecticut 06250
| | - Eric R. Kandel
- Center for Neurobiology and Behavior and Howard Hughes Medical Institute, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York, New York 10032, and
| | - Ted Abel
- Department of Biology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 19104
| | - K. Matthew Lattal
- Department of Behavioral Neuroscience, Oregon Health and Science University, Portland, Oregon 97239
| |
Collapse
|
42
|
Yurkovic A, Wang O, Basu AC, Kravitz EA. Learning and memory associated with aggression in Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2006; 103:17519-24. [PMID: 17088536 PMCID: PMC1634832 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0608211103] [Citation(s) in RCA: 111] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Male Drosophila melanogaster (Canton-S strain) exhibit aggression in competition for resources, to defend territory, and for access to mates. In the study reported here, we asked: (i) how long flies fight; (ii) whether flies adopt distinct winning and losing strategies as hierarchical relationships are established; (iii) whether flies exhibit experience-dependent changes in fighting strategies in later fights; and (iv) whether flies fight differently in second fights against familiar or unfamiliar opponents. The results showed that flies fought for up to 5 h. As hierarchical relationships were established, behavioral strategies changed: winners progressively lunged more and retreated less, whereas losers progressively lunged less and retreated more. Encounters between flies were frequent during the first 10 min of pairing and then dropped significantly. To ask whether flies remembered previous fights, they were re-paired with familiar or unfamiliar opponents after 30 min of separation. In familiar pairings, there were fewer encounters during the first 10 min of fighting than in unfamiliar pairings, and former losers fought differently against familiar winners than unfamiliar winners. Former losers lost or no decision was reached in all second fights in pairings with familiar or unfamiliar winners or with naive flies. Winner/winner, loser/loser, and naive/naive pairings revealed that losers used low-intensity strategies in later fights and were unlikely to form new hierarchical relationships, compared with winners or socially naive flies. These results strongly support the idea that learning and memory accompany the changes in social status that result from fruit fly fights.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alexandra Yurkovic
- Department of Neurobiology, Harvard Medical School, 220 Longwood Avenue, Boston, MA 02115
| | - Oulu Wang
- Department of Neurobiology, Harvard Medical School, 220 Longwood Avenue, Boston, MA 02115
| | - Alo C. Basu
- Department of Neurobiology, Harvard Medical School, 220 Longwood Avenue, Boston, MA 02115
| | - Edward A. Kravitz
- Department of Neurobiology, Harvard Medical School, 220 Longwood Avenue, Boston, MA 02115
- *To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
43
|
Manoli DS, Meissner GW, Baker BS. Blueprints for behavior: genetic specification of neural circuitry for innate behaviors. Trends Neurosci 2006; 29:444-51. [PMID: 16806511 DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2006.06.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 85] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/12/2005] [Revised: 04/10/2006] [Accepted: 06/07/2006] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Innate behaviors offer a unique opportunity to use genetic analysis to dissect and characterize the neural substrates of complex behavioral programs. Courtship in Drosophila involves a complex series of stereotyped behaviors that include numerous exchanges of multimodal sensory information over time. As we will discuss in this review, recent work has demonstrated that male-specific expression of Fruitless transcription factors (Fru(M) proteins) is necessary and sufficient to confer the potential for male courtship behaviors. Fru(M) factors program neurons of the male central and peripheral nervous systems whose function is dedicated to sexual behaviors. This circuitry seems to integrate sensory information to define behavioral states and regulate conserved neural elements for sex-specific behavioral output. The principles that govern the circuitry specified by Fru(M) expression might also operate in subcortical networks that govern innate behaviors in mammals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Devanand S Manoli
- Medical Scientist Training Program, Neurosciences Program and Department of Biological Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305-5020, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
44
|
van Wijk M, Wadman WJ, Sabelis MW. Gross morphology of the central nervous system of a phytoseiid mite. EXPERIMENTAL & APPLIED ACAROLOGY 2006; 40:205-16. [PMID: 17242982 DOI: 10.1007/s10493-006-9039-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/06/2006] [Accepted: 11/16/2006] [Indexed: 05/13/2023]
Abstract
The general morphology of the central nervous system is analysed in intact females of the predatory mite, Phytoseiulus persimilis (Acari: Phytoseiidae), using a nucleic acid label (YOYO-1) and confocal laser scanning microscopy. The somata of all cells that comprise the synganglion reside in the cortex. The cortex harbours an estimated total of 10,000 cells. The somata are densely packed in the cortex and cells residing in the inner cortex may only occupy about 1.8 microm. As in all Arachnida, the synganglion is divided in a sub- and a supra-oesophageal nervous mass. Both the cortex and the neuropil appear continuous between these two nervous masses. The sub-oesophageal nervous mass mainly consists of the four paired pedal ganglia that are each associated with a leg. The prominent olfactory lobes are ventrally associated with the first pedal ganglia. A small opisthosomal ganglion occupies the most caudal part of the sub-oesophageal ganglion. The rostral part of the supra-oesophageal nervous mass consists of the paired cheliceral and palpal ganglia. The supra-oesophageal ganglion is the largest ganglion in the supra-oesophageal nervous mass and unlike all other ganglia it is not associated with any of the major nerves. It is therefore more likely involved in secondary information processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michiel van Wijk
- Institute for Biodiversity and Ecosystem Dynamics, Section Population Biology, University of Amsterdam, Kruislaan 320, 1098 SM, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
45
|
Siwicki KK, Riccio P, Ladewski L, Marcillac F, Dartevelle L, Cross SA, Ferveur JF. The role of cuticular pheromones in courtship conditioning of Drosophila males. Learn Mem 2005; 12:636-45. [PMID: 16287720 PMCID: PMC1356183 DOI: 10.1101/lm.85605] [Citation(s) in RCA: 55] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
Courtship conditioning is an associative learning paradigm in Drosophila melanogaster, wherein male courtship behavior is modified by experience with unreceptive, previously mated females. While the training experience with mated females involves multiple sensory and behavioral interactions, we hypothesized that female cuticular hydrocarbons function as a specific chemosensory conditioned stimulus in this learning paradigm. The effects of training with mated females were determined in courtship tests with either wild-type virgin females as courtship targets, or with target flies of different genotypes that express distinct cuticular hydrocarbon (CH) profiles. Results of tests with female targets that lacked the normal CH profile, and with male targets that expressed typically female CH profiles, indicated that components of this CH profile are both necessary and sufficient cues to elicit the effects of conditioning. Results with additional targets indicated that the female-specific 7,11-dienes, which induce naive males to court, are not essential components of the conditioned stimulus. Rather, the learned response was significantly correlated with the levels of 9-pentacosene (9-P), a compound found in both males and females of many Drosophila strains and species. Adding 9-P to target flies showed that it stimulates courting males to attempt to copulate, and confirmed its role as a component of the conditioned stimulus by demonstrating dose-dependent increases in the expression of the learned response. Thus, 9-P can contribute significantly to the conditioned suppression of male courtship toward targets that express this pheromone.
Collapse
|
46
|
Ferveur JF. Cuticular hydrocarbons: their evolution and roles in Drosophila pheromonal communication. Behav Genet 2005; 35:279-95. [PMID: 15864443 DOI: 10.1007/s10519-005-3220-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 352] [Impact Index Per Article: 18.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/01/2004] [Accepted: 02/01/2005] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
I review the recent literature on cuticular hydrocarbons (CHs) in Drosophila. First, the major structural features of CHs are examined in a variety of species with regard to phylogeny. The genetic bases of the CH variation between and within species have been investigated with some of the genes involved characterized and manipulated. The effect of non-genetic factors as temperature, food and development is also examined with regard to CH production. Using a model involving the stimulating or the inhibiting role of CHs, it is possible to speculate on the mechanisms of CH perception and on the role(s) that these substances could play on sexual isolation and on the evolution of pheromonal communication.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jean-François Ferveur
- Unité de Recherche 5548 Associée au Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Faculté des Sciences, Université de Bourgogne, 6 Bd Gabriel, 21000 , Dijon, France.
| |
Collapse
|
47
|
Abstract
The olfactory nervous system of insects and mammals exhibits many similarities, which suggests that the mechanisms for olfactory learning may be shared. Molecular genetic investigations of Drosophila learning have uncovered numerous genes whose gene products are essential for olfactory memory formation. Recent studies of the products of these genes have continued to expand the range of molecular processes known to underlie memory formation. Recent research has also broadened the neuroanatomical areas thought to mediate olfactory learning to include the antennal lobes in addition to a previously accepted and central role for the mushroom bodies. The roles for neurons extrinsic to the mushroom body neurons are becoming better defined. Finally, the genes identified to participate in Drosophila olfactory learning have conserved roles in mammalian organisms, highlighting the value of Drosophila for gene discovery.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ronald L Davis
- Department of Molecular and Cellular Biology, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX 77030, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
48
|
Buschbeck EK, Hoy RR. The development of a long, coiled, optic nerve in the stalk-eyed fly Cyrtodiopsis whitei. Cell Tissue Res 2005; 321:491-504. [PMID: 16010600 DOI: 10.1007/s00441-005-1142-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/14/2004] [Accepted: 04/06/2005] [Indexed: 02/02/2023]
Abstract
In the stalk-eyed fly Cyrtodiopsis whitei (Diopsidae; Diptera), the relatively long optic nerve develops within the tight lumen of a very short eyestalk. Axonal growth is generally considered in terms of path finding, selective fasciculation, and towing. Physical forces that are necessary for axon lengthening are generated either by the growth cone or by the growth of surrounding tissues. Therefore, it is surprising to encounter a loosely coiled nerve apparently lacking any attachments that could allow for pull, or towing, of the nerve. In this study, we used histological sections and whole-mount preparations to confirm that the optic nerve of the stalk-eyed fly indeed elongates without the external application of tension to the nerve. Secondly, we examined the distribution of cytoskeletal elements and selected proteins that may be involved in axon extension. Staining against the vesicle fusion proteins SNAP-24 and SNAP-25 consistently results in stronger staining in the rapidly extending optic nerve than in a control nerve, suggesting a possible role of these proteins in the extension process. On a gross morphological level, SNAP-24/25 as well as the cytoskeletal elements actin and tubulin are uniformly distributed throughout the lengths of the growing nerve, suggesting that nerve elongation is distributed rather than localized. Finally, we identified glia as a possible source for tension within the nerve bundle. Glia proliferate rapidly in the optic nerve but not in the control nerve. Much work continues to focus on the growth of axons in culture, but this study is one of the few that considers the dynamics of nerve bundle extension as a whole.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elke K Buschbeck
- Department of Biological Sciences, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH 45221-0006, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
49
|
Svetec N, Ferveur JF. Social experience and pheromonal perception can change male-male interactions in Drosophila melanogaster. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2005; 208:891-8. [PMID: 15755887 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.01454] [Citation(s) in RCA: 66] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Social interaction with conspecifics can influence the developing brain and behaviour of the exposed animal. This experience can involve the exchange and retention of visual, chemical, acoustic and tactile signals. When several Drosophila melanogaster male flies are associated with mated females in the presence of food, they show frequent aggressive interactions. To measure the role of social experience on male-male interaction, two tester males - naive or exposed to sibling(s) during a variable period of their adult development - were confronted in the absence of female and food. The two males displayed homosexual courtship and aggressive behaviours, the frequency, intensity and directionality of which varied according to their experience. The effect of social experience was greatly enhanced between transgenic males partially defective for pheromonal perception, indicating that male inhibitory pheromones are normally used to repress male-male interaction.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicolas Svetec
- Unité de Recherche 5548 Associée au Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Faculté des Sciences, Université de Bourgogne, 6, Bd Gabriel, 21 000 Dijon, France
| | | |
Collapse
|
50
|
Ejima A, Smith BP, Lucas C, Levine JD, Griffith LC. Sequential learning of pheromonal cues modulates memory consolidation in trainer-specific associative courtship conditioning. Curr Biol 2005; 15:194-206. [PMID: 15694302 PMCID: PMC2805828 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.01.035] [Citation(s) in RCA: 81] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/20/2004] [Revised: 11/26/2004] [Accepted: 11/29/2004] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Associative memory formation requires that animals choose predictors for experiences they need to remember. When an artificial odor is paired with an aversive experience, that odor becomes the predictor. In more natural settings, however, animals can have multiple salient experiences that need to be remembered and prioritized. The mechanisms by which animals deal with multiple experiences are incompletely understood. RESULTS Here we show that Drosophila males can be trained to discriminate between different types of female pheromones; they suppress courtship specifically to the type of female that was associated with unsuccessful courtship. Such "trainer-specific" learning is mediated by hydrocarbon olfactory cues and modifies the male's processing of those cues. Animals that are unable to use olfactory cues can still learn by using other sensory modalities, but memory in this case is not specific to the trainer female's maturation state. Concurrent and serial presentation of different pheromones demonstrates that the ability to consolidate memory of pheromonal cues can be modified by the temporal order in which they appear. CONCLUSION Suppression of memory by new learning demonstrates that the dynamics of memory consolidation are subject to plasticity in Drosophila. This type of metaplasticity is essential for navigation of experience-rich natural environments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aki Ejima
- Department of Biology and Volen Center for Complex Systems, Brandeis University MS008, 415 South Street, Waltham, Massachusetts 02454-9110
| | - Benjamin P.C. Smith
- Department of Biology, University of Toronto at Mississauga, 3359 Mississauga Road North, Mississauga, Ontario L5L 1C6, Canada
| | - Christophe Lucas
- Department of Biology, University of Toronto at Mississauga, 3359 Mississauga Road North, Mississauga, Ontario L5L 1C6, Canada
| | - Joel D. Levine
- Department of Biology, University of Toronto at Mississauga, 3359 Mississauga Road North, Mississauga, Ontario L5L 1C6, Canada
| | - Leslie C. Griffith
- Department of Biology and Volen Center for Complex Systems, Brandeis University MS008, 415 South Street, Waltham, Massachusetts 02454-9110
- Correspondence:
| |
Collapse
|