1
|
Parelman JM, Doré BP, Cooper N, O’Donnell MB, Chan HY, Falk EB. Overlapping Functional Representations of Self- and Other-Related Thought are Separable Through Multivoxel Pattern Classification. Cereb Cortex 2022; 32:1131-1141. [PMID: 34398230 PMCID: PMC8924429 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhab272] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/17/2021] [Revised: 07/14/2021] [Accepted: 07/15/2021] [Indexed: 12/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Self-reflection and thinking about the thoughts and behaviors of others are important skills for humans to function in the social world. These two processes overlap in terms of the component processes involved, and share overlapping functional organizations within the human brain, in particular within the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC). Several functional models have been proposed to explain these two processes, but none has directly explored the extent to which they are distinctly represented within different parts of the brain. This study used multivoxel pattern classification to quantify the separability of self- and other-related thought in the MPFC and expanded this question to the entire brain. Using a large-scale mega-analytic dataset, spanning three separate studies (n = 142), we find that self- and other-related thought can be reliably distinguished above chance within the MPFC, posterior cingulate cortex and temporal lobes. We highlight subcomponents of the ventral MPFC that are particularly important in representing self-related thought, and subcomponents of the orbitofrontal cortex robustly involved in representing other-related thought. Our findings indicate that representations of self- and other-related thought in the human brain are described best by a distributed pattern rather than stark localization or a purely ventral to dorsal linear gradient in the MPFC.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jacob M Parelman
- Annenberg School for Communication, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA
| | - Bruce P Doré
- Desautels Faculty of Management, McGill University, H3A 1G5, Montreal, Canada
| | - Nicole Cooper
- Annenberg School for Communication, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA
| | | | - Hang-Yee Chan
- Amsterdam School of Communication Research, University of Amsterdam, Nieuwe Achtergracht 166, 1018 WV Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Emily B Falk
- Annenberg School for Communication, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Stephenson LJ, Edwards SG, Bayliss AP. From Gaze Perception to Social Cognition: The Shared-Attention System. PERSPECTIVES ON PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2021; 16:553-576. [PMID: 33567223 PMCID: PMC8114330 DOI: 10.1177/1745691620953773] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
When two people look at the same object in the environment and are aware of each other's attentional state, they find themselves in a shared-attention episode. This can occur through intentional or incidental signaling and, in either case, causes an exchange of information between the two parties about the environment and each other's mental states. In this article, we give an overview of what is known about the building blocks of shared attention (gaze perception and joint attention) and focus on bringing to bear new findings on the initiation of shared attention that complement knowledge about gaze following and incorporate new insights from research into the sense of agency. We also present a neurocognitive model, incorporating first-, second-, and third-order social cognitive processes (the shared-attention system, or SAS), building on previous models and approaches. The SAS model aims to encompass perceptual, cognitive, and affective processes that contribute to and follow on from the establishment of shared attention. These processes include fundamental components of social cognition such as reward, affective evaluation, agency, empathy, and theory of mind.
Collapse
|
3
|
Courtney AL, Meyer ML. Self-Other Representation in the Social Brain Reflects Social Connection. J Neurosci 2020; 40:5616-5627. [PMID: 32541067 PMCID: PMC7363469 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.2826-19.2020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/27/2019] [Revised: 03/22/2020] [Accepted: 04/29/2020] [Indexed: 12/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Social connection is critical to well-being, yet how the brain reflects our attachment to other people remains largely unknown. We combined univariate and multivariate brain imaging analyses to assess whether and how the brain organizes representations of others based on how connected they are to our own identity. During an fMRI scan, female and male human participants (N = 43) completed a self- and other-reflection task for 16 targets: the self, five close others, five acquaintances, and five celebrities. In addition, they reported their subjective closeness to each target and their own trait loneliness. We examined neural responses to the self and others in a brain region that has been associated with self-representation (mPFC) and across the whole brain. The structure of self-other representation in the mPFC and across the social brain appeared to cluster targets into three social categories: the self, social network members (including close others and acquaintances), and celebrities. Moreover, both univariate activation in mPFC and multivariate self-other similarity in mPFC and across the social brain increased with subjective self-other closeness ratings. Critically, participants who were less socially connected (i.e., lonelier) showed altered self-other mapping in social brain regions. Most notably, in mPFC, loneliness was associated with reduced representational similarity between the self and others. The social brain apparently maintains information about broad social categories as well as closeness to the self. Moreover, these results point to the possibility that feelings of chronic social disconnection may be mirrored by a "lonelier" neural self-representation.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Social connection is critical to well-being, yet how the brain reflects our attachment to people remains unclear. We found that the social brain stratifies neural representations of people based on our subjective connection to them, separately clustering people who are and are not in our social network. Moreover, the people we feel closest to are represented most closely to ourselves. Finally, lonelier individuals also appeared to have a "lonelier" neural self-representation in the mPFC, as loneliness attenuated the similarity between self and other neural representations in this region. The social brain appears to map our interpersonal ties, and alterations in this map may help explain why lonely individuals endorse statements such as "people are around me but not with me."
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrea L Courtney
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, California 94305
| | - Meghan L Meyer
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Salhi L, Bergström ZM. Intact strategic retrieval processes in older adults: no evidence for age-related deficits in source-constrained retrieval. Memory 2020; 28:348-361. [PMID: 31984857 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2020.1719161] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
Aging is thought to involve impairments to cognitive control functions that support episodic memory, for example by enabling people to strategically constrain their retrieval search towards a specific context ("source") in order to facilitate retrieval of goal-relevant memories. The "memory-for-foils" paradigm investigates source-constrained retrieval by assessing whether incidental encoding of new foils during an old/new recognition test differs depending on the type of processing that was previously used during study of the old items in the test. If it does, it suggests that people process foils differently as a result of engaging in source-constrained retrieval attempts. Young adults typically show differences in incidental encoding foils, but such differences have not been found in older adults. Here, we compared source-constrained retrieval and reward effects on incidental foil encoding between younger and older adults, to assess if age-related reductions in strategic retrieval processing are accompanied by differences in responsiveness to external rewards. The results showed only minor effects of rewards on memory processing, in younger adults only. Contrary to prior findings, older adults had equivalent overall memory performance and spontaneously constrained retrieval to the same extent as the young group, showing that aging-related impairments to strategic retrieval processes are not inevitable.
Collapse
|
5
|
The Role of the Amygdala and the Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex in Emotional Regulation: Implications for Post-traumatic Stress Disorder. Neuropsychol Rev 2019; 29:220-243. [DOI: 10.1007/s11065-019-09398-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/26/2017] [Accepted: 02/14/2019] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
|
6
|
Fuentes-Claramonte P, Martín-Subero M, Salgado-Pineda P, Alonso-Lana S, Moreno-Alcázar A, Argila-Plaza I, Santo-Angles A, Albajes-Eizagirre A, Anguera-Camós M, Capdevila A, Sarró S, McKenna PJ, Pomarol-Clotet E, Salvador R. Shared and differential default-mode related patterns of activity in an autobiographical, a self-referential and an attentional task. PLoS One 2019; 14:e0209376. [PMID: 30608970 PMCID: PMC6319771 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0209376] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/10/2018] [Accepted: 12/04/2018] [Indexed: 01/17/2023] Open
Abstract
The default-mode network (DMN) comprises a set of brain regions that show deactivations during performance of attentionally demanding tasks, but also activation during certain processes including recall of autobiographical memories and processing information about oneself, among others. However, the DMN is not activated in a homogeneous manner during performance of such tasks, so it is not clear to what extent its activation patterns correspond to deactivation patterns seen during attention-demanding tasks. In this fMRI study we compared patterns of activation in response to an autobiographical memory task to those observed in a self/other-reflection task, and compared both to deactivations observed during the n-back working memory task. Autobiographical recall and self-reflection activated several common DMN areas, which were also deactivated below baseline levels by the n-back task. Activation in the medial temporal lobe was seen during autobiographical recall but not the self/other task, and right angular gyrus activity was specifically linked to other-reflection. ROI analysis showed that most, but not all DMN regions were activated above baseline levels during the autobiographical memory and self-reflection tasks. Our results provide evidence for the usefulness of the autobiographical memory task to study DMN activity and support the notion of interacting subsystems within this network.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paola Fuentes-Claramonte
- FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain
- CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Barcelona, Spain
| | - Marta Martín-Subero
- FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain
- CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Barcelona, Spain
- Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Pilar Salgado-Pineda
- FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain
- CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Barcelona, Spain
| | - Silvia Alonso-Lana
- FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain
- CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Barcelona, Spain
| | - Ana Moreno-Alcázar
- CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Barcelona, Spain
- Institut de Neuropsiquiatria i Addiccions, Centre Fòrum Research Unit, Parc de Salut Mar, Barcelona, Spain
- IMIM (Hospital del Mar Medical Research Institute), Barcelona, Spain
| | | | | | - Anton Albajes-Eizagirre
- FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain
- CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Barcelona, Spain
| | | | - Antoni Capdevila
- Radiology Unit, Hospital de la Santa Creu i Sant Pau (HSCSP), Barcelona, Spain
- CIBER-BBN (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red en Bioingeniería, Biomateriales y Nanomedicina), Barcelona, Spain
| | - Salvador Sarró
- FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain
- CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Barcelona, Spain
| | - Peter J. McKenna
- FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain
- CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Barcelona, Spain
| | - Edith Pomarol-Clotet
- FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain
- CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Barcelona, Spain
| | - Raymond Salvador
- FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain
- CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Barcelona, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Gutchess A, Kensinger EA. Shared Mechanisms May Support Mnemonic Benefits from Self-Referencing and Emotion. Trends Cogn Sci 2018; 22:712-724. [PMID: 29886010 PMCID: PMC6652178 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.05.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/26/2017] [Revised: 05/10/2018] [Accepted: 05/11/2018] [Indexed: 12/17/2022]
Abstract
The literatures on episodic memory for self-referential and emotional information have proceeded relatively independently, and most studies examining the effects of age on these memory processes have been interpreted within domain-specific frameworks. However, there is increasing evidence for shared mechanisms that contribute to episodic memory benefits in these two domains. We review this evidence and propose a model that incorporates overlapping as well as domain-specific contributions to episodic memory encoding of self-referential and emotional material. We discuss the implications for understanding the relatively intact memory of older adults for these classes of stimuli, and conclude with suggestions for future research to test key tenets and extensions of this shared-process model.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Angela Gutchess
- Aging, Culture, and Cognition Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, Waltham, MA, USA; Equal contributions.
| | - Elizabeth A Kensinger
- Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Boston College, Chestnut Hill, MA, USA; Equal contributions
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
van Schie CC, Chiu CD, Rombouts SARB, Heiser WJ, Elzinga BM. When compliments do not hit but critiques do: an fMRI study into self-esteem and self-knowledge in processing social feedback. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2018; 13:404-417. [PMID: 29490088 PMCID: PMC5928412 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsy014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/05/2017] [Revised: 02/15/2018] [Accepted: 02/22/2018] [Indexed: 01/15/2023] Open
Abstract
The way we view ourselves may play an important role in our responses to interpersonal interactions. In this study, we investigate how feedback valence, consistency of feedback with self-knowledge and global self-esteem influence affective and neural responses to social feedback. Participants (N = 46) with a high range of self-esteem levels performed the social feedback task in an MRI scanner. Negative, intermediate and positive feedback was provided, supposedly by another person based on a personal interview. Participants rated their mood and applicability of feedback to the self. Analyses on trial basis on neural and affective responses are used to incorporate applicability of individual feedback words. Lower self-esteem related to low mood especially after receiving non-applicable negative feedback. Higher self-esteem related to increased posterior cingulate cortex and precuneus activation (i.e. self-referential processing) for applicable negative feedback. Lower self-esteem related to decreased medial prefrontal cortex, insula, anterior cingulate cortex and posterior cingulate cortex activation (i.e. self-referential processing) during positive feedback and decreased temporoparietal junction activation (i.e. other referential processing) for applicable positive feedback. Self-esteem and consistency of feedback with self-knowledge appear to guide our affective and neural responses to social feedback. This may be highly relevant for the interpersonal problems that individuals face with low self-esteem and negative self-views.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Charlotte C van Schie
- Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, P.O. Box 9555, 2300 RB Leiden, The Netherlands.,Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, P.O.Box 9600, 2300 RC Leiden, The Netherlands
| | - Chui-De Chiu
- Department of Psychology, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Shatin, Hong Kong
| | - Serge A R B Rombouts
- Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, P.O. Box 9555, 2300 RB Leiden, The Netherlands.,Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, P.O.Box 9600, 2300 RC Leiden, The Netherlands.,Department of Radiology, Leiden University Medical Centre, P.O. Box 9600, 2300 RC Leiden, The Netherlands
| | - Willem J Heiser
- Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, P.O. Box 9555, 2300 RB Leiden, The Netherlands.,Mathematical Institute, Leiden University, P.O. Box 9512, 2300 RA Leiden, The Netherlands
| | - Bernet M Elzinga
- Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, P.O. Box 9555, 2300 RB Leiden, The Netherlands.,Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, P.O.Box 9600, 2300 RC Leiden, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Cooper RA, Richter FR, Bays PM, Plaisted-Grant KC, Baron-Cohen S, Simons JS. Reduced Hippocampal Functional Connectivity During Episodic Memory Retrieval in Autism. Cereb Cortex 2018; 27:888-902. [PMID: 28057726 PMCID: PMC5390398 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhw417] [Citation(s) in RCA: 60] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/21/2016] [Accepted: 12/23/2016] [Indexed: 01/26/2023] Open
Abstract
Increasing recent research has sought to understand the recollection impairments experienced by individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Here, we tested whether these memory deficits reflect a reduction in the probability of retrieval success or in the precision of memory representations. We also used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to study the neural mechanisms underlying memory encoding and retrieval in ASD, focusing particularly on the functional connectivity of core episodic memory networks. Adults with ASD and typical control participants completed a memory task that involved studying visual displays and subsequently using a continuous dial to recreate their appearance. The ASD group exhibited reduced retrieval success, but there was no evidence of a difference in retrieval precision. fMRI data revealed similar patterns of brain activity and functional connectivity during memory encoding in the 2 groups, though encoding-related lateral frontal activity predicted subsequent retrieval success only in the control group. During memory retrieval, the ASD group exhibited attenuated lateral frontal activity and substantially reduced hippocampal connectivity, particularly between hippocampus and regions of the fronto-parietal control network. These findings demonstrate notable differences in brain function during episodic memory retrieval in ASD and highlight the importance of functional connectivity to understanding recollection-related retrieval deficits in this population.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rose A Cooper
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 3EB, UK
| | | | - Paul M Bays
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 3EB, UK
| | | | - Simon Baron-Cohen
- Autism Research Centre, Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 8AH, UK
| | - Jon S Simons
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 3EB, UK
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Vogelsang DA, Gruber M, Bergström ZM, Ranganath C, Simons JS. Alpha Oscillations during Incidental Encoding Predict Subsequent Memory for New "Foil" Information. J Cogn Neurosci 2018; 30:667-679. [PMID: 29324072 PMCID: PMC6042834 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_01234] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
People can employ adaptive strategies to increase the likelihood that previously encoded information will be successfully retrieved. One such strategy is to constrain retrieval toward relevant information by reimplementing the neurocognitive processes that were engaged during encoding. Using EEG, we examined the temporal dynamics with which constraining retrieval toward semantic versus nonsemantic information affects the processing of new “foil” information encountered during a memory test. Time–frequency analysis of EEG data acquired during an initial study phase revealed that semantic compared with nonsemantic processing was associated with alpha decreases in a left frontal electrode cluster from around 600 msec after stimulus onset. Successful encoding of semantic versus nonsemantic foils during a subsequent memory test was related to decreases in alpha oscillatory activity in the same left frontal electrode cluster, which emerged relatively late in the trial at around 1000–1600 msec after stimulus onset. Across participants, left frontal alpha power elicited by semantic processing during the study phase correlated significantly with left frontal alpha power associated with semantic foil encoding during the memory test. Furthermore, larger left frontal alpha power decreases elicited by semantic foil encoding during the memory test predicted better subsequent semantic foil recognition in an additional surprise foil memory test, although this effect did not reach significance. These findings indicate that constraining retrieval toward semantic information involves reimplementing semantic encoding operations that are mediated by alpha oscillations and that such reimplementation occurs at a late stage of memory retrieval, perhaps reflecting additional monitoring processes.
Collapse
|
11
|
Cooper RA, Plaisted-Grant KC, Baron-Cohen S, Simons JS. Reality Monitoring and Metamemory in Adults with Autism Spectrum Conditions. J Autism Dev Disord 2017; 46:2186-2198. [PMID: 26899724 PMCID: PMC4860197 DOI: 10.1007/s10803-016-2749-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Studies of reality monitoring (RM) often implicate medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) in distinguishing internal and external information, a region linked to autism-related deficits in social and self-referential information processing, executive function, and memory. This study used two RM conditions (self-other; perceived-imagined) to investigate RM and metamemory in adults with autism. The autism group showed a deficit in RM, which did not differ across source conditions, and both groups exhibited a self-encoding benefit on recognition and source memory. Metamemory for perceived-imagined information, but not for self-other information, was significantly lower in the autism group. Therefore, reality monitoring and metamemory, sensitive to mPFC function, appear impaired in autism, highlighting a difficulty in remembering and monitoring internal and external details of past events.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rose A Cooper
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge, CB2 3EB, UK
| | - Kate C Plaisted-Grant
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge, CB2 3EB, UK
| | - Simon Baron-Cohen
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge, CB2 3EB, UK.,Autism Research Centre, Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Jon S Simons
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge, CB2 3EB, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Richter FR, Cooper RA, Bays PM, Simons JS. Distinct neural mechanisms underlie the success, precision, and vividness of episodic memory. eLife 2016; 5. [PMID: 27776631 PMCID: PMC5079745 DOI: 10.7554/elife.18260] [Citation(s) in RCA: 136] [Impact Index Per Article: 17.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/28/2016] [Accepted: 09/26/2016] [Indexed: 12/27/2022] Open
Abstract
A network of brain regions have been linked with episodic memory retrieval, but limited progress has been made in identifying the contributions of distinct parts of the network. Here, we utilized continuous measures of retrieval to dissociate three components of episodic memory: retrieval success, precision, and vividness. In the fMRI scanner, participants encoded objects that varied continuously on three features: color, orientation, and location. Participants’ memory was tested by having them recreate the appearance of the object features using a continuous dial, and continuous vividness judgments were recorded. Retrieval success, precision, and vividness were dissociable both behaviorally and neurally: successful versus unsuccessful retrieval was associated with hippocampal activity, retrieval precision scaled with activity in the angular gyrus, and vividness judgments tracked activity in the precuneus. The ability to dissociate these components of episodic memory reveals the benefit afforded by measuring memory on a continuous scale, allowing functional parcellation of the retrieval network. DOI:http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.18260.001 Remembering is something we do countless times each day. The detail and vividness with which we can remember is part of what makes memories so precious. Given the significance and complexity of memories, it is perhaps unsurprising that several parts of the brain are needed for us to experience them. Indeed, the brain regions involved in memory all work so closely together that it is a challenge to identify what role each region plays. Richter, Cooper et al. aimed to design a memory task that could separate key characteristics of remembering, which would allow them to study links between each aspect and the different brain regions involved in memory. The resulting test involved showing people images of different objects whilst they were in an MRI medical imaging scanner. The people taking the test were asked to remember several objects that could vary in color, position and orientation. Participants were asked to rate how vividly they remembered the objects and then tried to precisely recreate their color, orientation and position. The test allowed Richter, Cooper et al. to link specific parts of the brain to certain aspects of remembering. The hippocampus, an area known to be important in memory processing, indicated whether or not information had been remembered. More vivid memories were linked to greater activity in a region called the precuneus, which plays a role in imagination. Lastly, activity in a third region – the angular gyrus – indicated the precision of each memory. Being able to study different aspects of memory using tests like this that collect detailed measurements could be important in identifying memory problems, for example, in people with brain diseases or head injuries, or after a stroke. Specifically, the methods developed by Richter, Cooper et al. could provide sensitive tools for detecting memory difficulties at an early stage. This may help more people to get treated sooner, potentially minimizing lasting complications. DOI:http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.18260.002
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Franziska R Richter
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
| | - Rose A Cooper
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
| | - Paul M Bays
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
| | - Jon S Simons
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Allan K, Morson S, Dixon S, Martin D, Cunningham SJ. Simulation-based mentalizing generates a "proxy" self-reference effect in memory. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2016; 70:1074-1084. [PMID: 27457683 DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1209532] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
The self-reference effect (SRE) in memory is thought to depend on specialized mechanisms that enhance memory for self-relevant information. We investigated whether these mechanisms can be engaged "by proxy" when we simulate other people, by asking participants to interact with two virtual partners: one similar and one dissimilar to self. Participants viewed pairs of objects and picked one for themselves, for their similar partner, or their dissimilar partner. A surprise memory test followed that required participants to identify which object of each pair was chosen, and for whom. Finally, participants were shown both partners' object pairs again, and asked to indicate their personal preference. Four key findings were observed. Overlap between participants' own choice and those made for their partner was significantly higher for the similar than the dissimilar partner, revealing participants' use of their own preferences to simulate the similar partner. Recollection of chosen objects was significantly higher for self than for both partners and, critically, was significantly higher for similar than dissimilar partners. Source confusion between self and the similar partner was also higher. These findings suggest that self-reference by proxy enhances memory for non-self-relevant material, and we consider the theoretical implications for functional interpretation of the SRE.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kevin Allan
- a School of Psychology, College of Life Sciences and Medicine , University of Aberdeen , Aberdeen , UK
| | - Suzannah Morson
- a School of Psychology, College of Life Sciences and Medicine , University of Aberdeen , Aberdeen , UK
| | - Susan Dixon
- a School of Psychology, College of Life Sciences and Medicine , University of Aberdeen , Aberdeen , UK
| | - Douglas Martin
- a School of Psychology, College of Life Sciences and Medicine , University of Aberdeen , Aberdeen , UK
| | - Sheila J Cunningham
- b Psychology Division, School of Social and Health Sciences , Abertay University , Dundee , UK
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Vogelsang DA, Bonnici HM, Bergström ZM, Ranganath C, Simons JS. Goal-directed mechanisms that constrain retrieval predict subsequent memory for new "foil" information. Neuropsychologia 2016; 89:356-363. [PMID: 27431039 PMCID: PMC5010040 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.07.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/10/2016] [Revised: 07/12/2016] [Accepted: 07/15/2016] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
To remember a previous event, it is often helpful to use goal-directed control processes to constrain what comes to mind during retrieval. Behavioral studies have demonstrated that incidental learning of new "foil" words in a recognition test is superior if the participant is trying to remember studied items that were semantically encoded compared to items that were non-semantically encoded. Here, we applied subsequent memory analysis to fMRI data to understand the neural mechanisms underlying the "foil effect". Participants encoded information during deep semantic and shallow non-semantic tasks and were tested in a subsequent blocked memory task to examine how orienting retrieval towards different types of information influences the incidental encoding of new words presented as foils during the memory test phase. To assess memory for foils, participants performed a further surprise old/new recognition test involving foil words that were encountered during the previous memory test blocks as well as completely new words. Subsequent memory effects, distinguishing successful versus unsuccessful incidental encoding of foils, were observed in regions that included the left inferior frontal gyrus and posterior parietal cortex. The left inferior frontal gyrus exhibited disproportionately larger subsequent memory effects for semantic than non-semantic foils, and significant overlap in activity during semantic, but not non-semantic, initial encoding and foil encoding. The results suggest that orienting retrieval towards different types of foils involves re-implementing the neurocognitive processes that were involved during initial encoding.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David A Vogelsang
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EB, UK; Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, CB2 3EB, UK
| | - Heidi M Bonnici
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EB, UK; Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, CB2 3EB, UK
| | | | - Charan Ranganath
- Center for Neuroscience, University of California at Davis, CA 95618, USA; Department of Psychology, University of California at Davis, CA 95616, USA
| | - Jon S Simons
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EB, UK; Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, CB2 3EB, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Morin A. The “self-awareness–anosognosia” paradox explained: How can one process be associated with activation of, and damage to, opposite sides of the brain? Laterality 2016; 22:105-119. [DOI: 10.1080/1357650x.2016.1173049] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
|
16
|
Tracy DK, Joyce DW, Shergill SS. Kaleidoscope. Br J Psychiatry 2015; 207:367-8. [PMID: 26429693 DOI: 10.1192/bjp.207.4.367] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
Life expectancy in England is related to income (and in London,
it can be mapped by a resident's nearest tube station1). The
arrival of a new government offers the opportunity to review the impact of
earlier policies designed to reduce health inequality. A new report from the
King's Fund2 has updated Michael Marmot's 2010 publication,
Fair Society, Healthy Lives, and using a wider range of
determinants of lifestyle and health, has found that income-related
inequalities in life expectancy have improved since that report. Department
of Health policies in the 2000s appear to have contributed to this, although
reductions in child and pensioner poverty and improvements in employment and
social housing have also impacted positively. Nevertheless, unemployment,
housing deprivation, and binge drinking remain key factors in lowering life
expectancy. The King's Fund report argues that a more nuanced and integrated
policy response for the NHS and other public services will be required by
the new government to continue to reduce inequality, but recognises that
implementation in an era of austerity poses challenges, particularly for the
most vulnerable.
Collapse
|