1
|
Kübler S, Langsdorf L, Meyer M, Schubert T. Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation of the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex Modulates Voluntary Task-order Coordination in Dual-task Situations. J Cogn Neurosci 2025; 37:602-620. [PMID: 39499530 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_02270] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2024]
Abstract
Dual tasks (DTs) require additional control processes that temporally coordinate the processing of the two component tasks. Previous studies employing imaging as well as noninvasive stimulation techniques have demonstrated that the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) is causally involved in these task-order coordination processes. However, in these studies, participants were instructed to match their processing order to an externally provided and mandatory order criterion during DT processing. Hence, it is still unknown whether the dlPFC is also recruited for rather voluntary order control processes, which are required in situations that allow for intentional and internally generated order choices. To address this issue, in two experiments, we applied anodal (Experiment 1) and cathodal (Experiment 2) transcranial direct current stimulation during a random-order DT in which participants could freely decide about their order of task processing. In our results, we found facilitatory and inhibitory effects on voluntary task-order coordination because of anodal and cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation, respectively. This was indicated by shorter RTs when participants intentionally switched the task order relative to the preceding trial during anodal as well as a reduced tendency to switch the task order relative to the preceding trial during cathodal stimulation compared with the sham stimulation. Overall, these findings indicate that the dlPFC is also causally involved in voluntary task-order coordination processes. In particular, we argue that the dlPFC is recruited for intentionally updating and implementing task-order information that is necessary for scheduling the processing of two temporally overlapping tasks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sebastian Kübler
- Martin Luther University Halle-Wittenberg
- Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
2
|
Yeung AWK, Robertson M, Uecker A, Fox PT, Eickhoff SB. Trends in the sample size, statistics, and contributions to the BrainMap database of activation likelihood estimation meta-analyses: An empirical study of 10-year data. Hum Brain Mapp 2023; 44:1876-1887. [PMID: 36479854 PMCID: PMC9980884 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.26177] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/26/2022] [Revised: 11/18/2022] [Accepted: 11/28/2022] [Indexed: 12/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The literature of neuroimaging meta-analysis has been thriving for over a decade. A majority of them were coordinate-based meta-analyses, particularly the activation likelihood estimation (ALE) approach. A meta-evaluation of these meta-analyses was performed to qualitatively evaluate their design and reporting standards. The publications listed from the BrainMap website were screened. Six hundred and three ALE papers published during 2010-2019 were included and analysed. For reporting standards, most of the ALE papers reported their total number of Papers involved and mentioned the inclusion/exclusion criteria on Paper selection. However, most papers did not describe how data redundancy was avoided when multiple related Experiments were reported within one paper. The most prevalent repeated-measures correction methods were voxel-level FDR (54.4%) and cluster-level FWE (33.8%), with the latter quickly replacing the former since 2016. For study characteristics, sample size in terms of number of Papers included per ALE paper and number of Experiments per analysis seemed to be stable over the decade. One-fifth of the surveyed ALE papers failed to meet the recommendation of having >17 Experiments per analysis. For data sharing, most of them did not provide input and output data. In conclusion, the field has matured well in terms of rising dominance of cluster-level FWE correction, and slightly improved reporting on elimination of data redundancy and providing input data. The provision of Data and Code availability statements and flow chart of literature screening process, as well as data submission to BrainMap, should be more encouraged.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andy Wai Kan Yeung
- Oral and Maxillofacial RadiologyApplied Oral Sciences and Community Dental Care, Faculty of Dentistry, The University of Hong KongHong KongChina
| | - Michaela Robertson
- Research Imaging InstituteUniversity of Texas Health Science CenterSan AntonioTexasUSA
| | - Angela Uecker
- Research Imaging InstituteUniversity of Texas Health Science CenterSan AntonioTexasUSA
| | - Peter T. Fox
- Research Imaging InstituteUniversity of Texas Health Science CenterSan AntonioTexasUSA
- Department of RadiologyUniversity of Texas Health Science CenterSan AntonioTexasUSA
| | - Simon B. Eickhoff
- Institute of Systems Neuroscience, Medical FacultyHeinrich Heine University DüsseldorfDüsseldorfGermany
- Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine, Brain & Behaviour (INM‐7)Research Centre JülichJülichGermany
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Guseva M, Bogler C, Allefeld C, Haynes JD. Instruction effects on randomness in sequence generation. Front Psychol 2023; 14:1113654. [PMID: 37034908 PMCID: PMC10075230 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1113654] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/01/2022] [Accepted: 02/20/2023] [Indexed: 04/11/2023] Open
Abstract
Randomness is a fundamental property of human behavior. It occurs both in the form of intrinsic random variability, say when repetitions of a task yield slightly different behavioral outcomes, or in the form of explicit randomness, say when a person tries to avoid being predicted in a game of rock, paper and scissors. Randomness has frequently been studied using random sequence generation tasks (RSG). A key finding has been that humans are poor at deliberately producing random behavior. At the same time, it has been shown that people might be better randomizers if randomness is only an implicit (rather than an explicit) requirement of the task. We therefore hypothesized that randomization performance might vary with the exact instructions with which randomness is elicited. To test this, we acquired data from a large online sample (n = 388), where every participant made 1,000 binary choices based on one of the following instructions: choose either randomly, freely, irregularly, according to an imaginary coin toss or perform a perceptual guessing task. Our results show significant differences in randomness between the conditions as quantified by conditional entropy and estimated Markov order. The randomization scores were highest in the conditions where people were asked to be irregular or mentally simulate a random event (coin toss) thus yielding recommendations for future studies on randomization behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maja Guseva
- Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience, Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany
- Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
- Department of Psychology, Humboldt Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
- *Correspondence: Maja Guseva,
| | - Carsten Bogler
- Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience, Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Carsten Allefeld
- Department of Psychology, City University of London, London, United Kingdom
| | - John-Dylan Haynes
- Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience, Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany
- Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
- Department of Psychology, City University of London, London, United Kingdom
- Berlin Center for Advanced Neuroimaging, Charité - Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Albuquerque MR, Hooper B, de Sousa Viana IO, Mesquita PHC, Santos TM, Apolinário‐Souza T, de Sousa Fortes L, Gonçalves DAP. Do executive function performance, gaze behavior, and pupil size change during incremental acute physical exercise? Psychophysiology 2022; 60:e14233. [PMID: 36537715 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14233] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/04/2022] [Revised: 11/08/2022] [Accepted: 11/22/2022] [Indexed: 12/24/2022]
Abstract
Several studies have investigated the interaction between acute physical exercise and cognitive performance. However, few studies have investigated this issue during acute high-intensity exercise. In the present study, we evaluated executive functions (EFs) during incremental exercise in three different intensities [below lactate threshold (LT), at LT, and above LT], measuring EFs performance, gaze behavior, and pupil diameter. Twenty subjects were familiarized with the EFs test and participated in a graded maximal exercise test on a cycle ergometer on the first visit. On the second visit, they performed the EFs task at rest and while exercising at three different intensities using mobile eye-tracking glasses. Our results showed that the psychophysiological measures differed between the conditions. Regarding EFs performance, during exercise above LT, the subjects showed worse accuracy when compared with rest (p < .001) and below LT (p < .001). In addition, the response time (RT) at LT and above LT was shorter than in the rest condition (p < .050). Further, RT was faster (p = .002) in the above LT than in the below LT condition. In addition, the gaze behavior measures indicated that exercise, independently of the intensity, improves the number of fixations with shorter fixation durations compared to the rest condition (p < .050). Additionally, we found no significant differences in average and peak pupil diameter between conditions. In conclusion, exercise at LT improves the EFs performance while exercising above LT worsens EFs performance. However, there were no significant differences in average and peak pupil diameter between conditions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maicon Rodrigues Albuquerque
- Neurosciences of Physical Activity and Sports Research Group Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais – UFMG Belo Horizonte Brazil
| | - Beatriz Hooper
- Neurosciences of Physical Activity and Sports Research Group Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais – UFMG Belo Horizonte Brazil
| | - Iasmin Oliveira de Sousa Viana
- Neurosciences of Physical Activity and Sports Research Group Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais – UFMG Belo Horizonte Brazil
| | | | - Tony Meireles Santos
- Graduate Program of Physical Education Pernambuco Federal University Recife Brazil
| | - Tércio Apolinário‐Souza
- Escola de Educação Física, Fisioterapia e Danca Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul Porto Alegre Brazil
| | - Leonardo de Sousa Fortes
- Associate Graduate Program of Physical Education Universidade Federal da Paraíba João Pessoa Brazil
| | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Fu X, Yang X, Cui X, Liu F, Li H, Yan M, Xie G, Guo W. Overlapping and segregated changes of functional hubs in melancholic depression and non-melancholic depression. J Psychiatr Res 2022; 154:123-131. [PMID: 35933856 DOI: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2022.07.043] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/24/2022] [Revised: 07/13/2022] [Accepted: 07/20/2022] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Previous research found associations between neuropsychiatric disorders and patterns of highly connected "hub" nodes, which are crucial in coordinating brain functions. Melancholic depression is considered a relatively distinct and homogenous subtype of major depressive disorder (MDD), which responds better to pharmacological treatments than placebos or psychotherapies. Accordingly, melancholic depression probably has distinct neuropathological underpinnings. This study aims to examine the overlapping and segregated changes of functional hubs in melancholic and non-melancholic MDD. METHODS Thirty-one melancholic patients, 28 non-melancholic patients, and 32 healthy controls were included. Resting-state functional imaging data were analyzed using global functional connectivity. RESULTS Both melancholic and non-melancholic patients had increased GFC in the bilateral insula and decreased GFC in the PCC/precuneus compared to HCs. The distinction was that melancholic patients showed increased GFC in the bilateral thalamus, right inferior parietal lobule (IPL), and left cerebellum Crus I and decreased GFC in the left temporal lobe, whereas non-melancholic patients showed increased GFC in the left superior parietal lobe. Additionally, compared with non-melancholic patients, melancholic individuals displayed significant increases of GFC in the left IPL and cerebellum. CONCLUSION Increased GFC of the insula and decreased GFC of the PCC and precuneus are the common abnormalities of melancholic and non-melancholic MDD. Hyperconnectivity of the IPL and cerebellum might be distinctive neuropathological features of melancholic MDD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xiaoya Fu
- Department of Psychiatry, and National Clinical Research Center for Mental Disorders, The Second Xiangya Hospital of Central South University, Changsha, 410011, Hunan, China
| | - Xiaolun Yang
- Department of Stomatology, The Second Xiangya Hospital of Central South University, Changsha, 410011, Hunan, China
| | - Xilong Cui
- Department of Psychiatry, and National Clinical Research Center for Mental Disorders, The Second Xiangya Hospital of Central South University, Changsha, 410011, Hunan, China
| | - Feng Liu
- Department of Radiology, Tianjin Medical University General Hospital, Tianjin, 300000, China
| | - Huabing Li
- Department of Radiology, The Second Xiangya Hospital of Central South University, Changsha, 410011, Hunan, China
| | - Meiqi Yan
- Department of Psychiatry, and National Clinical Research Center for Mental Disorders, The Second Xiangya Hospital of Central South University, Changsha, 410011, Hunan, China
| | - Guangrong Xie
- Department of Psychiatry, and National Clinical Research Center for Mental Disorders, The Second Xiangya Hospital of Central South University, Changsha, 410011, Hunan, China.
| | - Wenbin Guo
- Department of Psychiatry, and National Clinical Research Center for Mental Disorders, The Second Xiangya Hospital of Central South University, Changsha, 410011, Hunan, China; Department of Psychiatry, The Third People's Hospital of Foshan, Foshan, Guangdong, 528000, China; Department of Psychiatry, Qiqihar Medical University, Qiqihar, Heilongjiang, 161006, China.
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Dugas CS, Keltner-Dorman E, Hart J. Differential effects from cognitive rehabilitation and high-definition tDCS in posterior cortical atrophy: A single-case experimental design. Neuropsychol Rehabil 2022; 32:1620-1642. [PMID: 34037503 DOI: 10.1080/09602011.2021.1927761] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/21/2020] [Accepted: 05/04/2021] [Indexed: 02/08/2023]
Abstract
Posterior cortical atrophy (PCA) is a progressive neurodegenerative syndrome characterized by visual-perceptual deficits, which impact daily life. Recent research has focused on non-pharmacological techniques to ameliorate these deficits, with the most common being cognitive rehabilitation. The present study investigates the differential effects of high definition-transcranial direct current stimulation (HD-tDCS) and cognitive rehabilitation in a single-case experimental design with two separate experimental phases in a patient with PCA. Experimental Phase 1 consisted of 10 sessions of HD-tDCS targeting the pre-SMA/dACC and Phase 2 consisted of 10 sessions of cognitive rehabilitation. Normed and standardized scores from figure copy and recall tests served as the primary outcome measures for visuospatial processing. The participant showed no immediate or long-term changes in visuospatial measures following HD-tDCS intervention. However, cognitive rehabilitation showed immediate improvement in visuospatial memory (figure recall) and clinically significant improvement in visuospatial construction (figure copy). Visuospatial construction gains remained in the low average range in the 10-week follow-up while visuospatial memory returned to baseline. Results indicated differential effects between HD-tDCS and cognitive rehabilitation with cognitive rehabilitation showing clinically significant improvement in primary outcome measures with sustained improvement in the long-term follow-up measure. Additional research is warranted to confirm these effects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christine Sofka Dugas
- Department of Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences, University of Texas at Dallas, Dallas, TX, USA
| | - Elena Keltner-Dorman
- Department of Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences, University of Texas at Dallas, Dallas, TX, USA
| | - John Hart
- Department of Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences, University of Texas at Dallas, Dallas, TX, USA
- Department of Neurology, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, Dallas, TX, USA
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, Dallas, TX, USA
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Liljenström H. Consciousness, decision making, and volition: freedom beyond chance and necessity. Theory Biosci 2022; 141:125-140. [PMID: 34046848 PMCID: PMC9184456 DOI: 10.1007/s12064-021-00346-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/25/2020] [Accepted: 05/04/2021] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
What is the role of consciousness in volition and decision-making? Are our actions fully determined by brain activity preceding our decisions to act, or can consciousness instead affect the brain activity leading to action? This has been much debated in philosophy, but also in science since the famous experiments by Libet in the 1980s, where the current most common interpretation is that conscious free will is an illusion. It seems that the brain knows, up to several seconds in advance what "you" decide to do. These studies have, however, been criticized, and alternative interpretations of the experiments can be given, some of which are discussed in this paper. In an attempt to elucidate the processes involved in decision-making (DM), as an essential part of volition, we have developed a computational model of relevant brain structures and their neurodynamics. While DM is a complex process, we have particularly focused on the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) for its emotional, and the lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC) for its cognitive aspects. In this paper, we present a stochastic population model representing the neural information processing of DM. Simulation results seem to confirm the notion that if decisions have to be made fast, emotional processes and aspects dominate, while rational processes are more time consuming and may result in a delayed decision. Finally, some limitations of current science and computational modeling will be discussed, hinting at a future development of science, where consciousness and free will may add to chance and necessity as explanation for what happens in the world.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hans Liljenström
- Agora for Biosystems, SLU, P.O. Box 7032, SE-75007, Uppsala, Sweden.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Kang W, Pineda Hernández S, Wang J, Malvaso A. Instruction-based learning: A review. Neuropsychologia 2022; 166:108142. [PMID: 34999133 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2022.108142] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/12/2021] [Revised: 12/22/2021] [Accepted: 01/03/2022] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
Humans are able to learn to implement novel rules from instructions rapidly, which is termed "instruction-based learning" (IBL). This remarkable ability is very important in our daily life in both learning individually or working as a team, and almost every psychology experiment starts with instructing participants. Many recent progresses have been made in IBL research both psychologically and neuroscientifically. In this review, we discuss the role of language in IBL, the importance of the first trial performance in IBL, why IBL should be considered as a goal-directed behavior, intelligence and IBL, cognitive flexibility and IBL, how behaviorally relevant information is processed in the lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC), how the lateral frontal cortex (LFC) networks work as a functional hierarchy during IBL, and the cortical and subcortical contributions to IBL. Finally, we develop a neural working model for IBL and provide some sensible directions for future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Weixi Kang
- Computational, Cognitive and Clinical Neuroimaging Laboratory, Division of Brain Sciences, Department of Medicine, Imperial College London, UK.
| | | | - Junxin Wang
- School of Nursing, Beijing University of Chinese Medicine, China
| | - Antonio Malvaso
- Neurology Unit, IRCCS San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milan, Italy; Neuroimaging Research Unit, Division of Neuroscience, IRCCS San Raffaele Scientific Institute, Milan, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Fuentes-Claramonte P, Ramiro N, Torres L, Argila-Plaza I, Salgado-Pineda P, Soler-Vidal J, García-León MÁ, Albacete A, Bosque C, Panicalli F, Boix E, Munuera J, Tristany J, Sarró S, Bernardo M, Salvador R, McKenna PJ, Pomarol-Clotet E. Negative schizophrenic symptoms as prefrontal cortex dysfunction: Examination using a task measuring goal neglect. NEUROIMAGE: CLINICAL 2022; 35:103119. [PMID: 35870381 PMCID: PMC9421442 DOI: 10.1016/j.nicl.2022.103119] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/24/2020] [Revised: 06/10/2022] [Accepted: 07/12/2022] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Negative schizophrenic symptoms have been considered to reflect prefrontal cortex dysfunction. Functional imaging support for this theory is however weak, perhaps due to the tasks used. We examined negative symptom patients using a novel executive task measuring volitional behaviour. Comparison to patients without negative symptoms revealed prefrontal hypoactivation.
Background The negative symptoms of schizophrenia have been proposed to reflect prefrontal cortex dysfunction. However, this proposal has not been consistently supported in functional imaging studies, which have also used executive tasks that may not capture key aspects of negative symptoms such as lack of volition. Method Twenty-four DSM-5 schizophrenic patients with high negative symptoms (HNS), 25 with absent negative symptoms (ANS) and 30 healthy controls underwent fMRI during performance of the Computerized Multiple Elements Test (CMET), a task designed to measure poor organization of goal directed behaviour or ‘goal neglect’. Negative symptoms were rated using the PANSS and the Clinical Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms (CAINS). Results On whole brain analysis, the ANS patients showed no significant clusters of reduced activation compared to the healthy controls. In contrast, the HNS patients showed hypoactivation compared to the healthy controls in the left anterior frontal cortex, the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), the anterior insula bilaterally and the bilateral inferior parietal cortex. When compared to the ANS patients, the HNS patients showed reduced activation in the left anterior frontal cortex, the left DLPFC and the left inferior parietal cortex. After controlling for disorganization scores, differences remained in clusters in the left anterior frontal cortex and the bilateral inferior parietal cortex. Conclusions This study provides evidence that reduced prefrontal activation, perhaps especially in the left anterior frontal cortex, is a brain functional correlate of negative symptoms in schizophrenia. The simultaneous finding of reduced inferior parietal cortex activation was unexpected, but could reflect this region’s involvement in cognitive control, particularly the ‘regulative’ component of this.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paola Fuentes-Claramonte
- FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain; CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Barcelona, Spain
| | - Núria Ramiro
- Psychiatry department, Hospital Sant Rafael, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Llanos Torres
- Hospital Mare de Dèu de la Mercé, Unitat Polivalent, Barcelona, Spain
| | | | - Pilar Salgado-Pineda
- FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain; CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Barcelona, Spain
| | - Joan Soler-Vidal
- FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain; CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Barcelona, Spain; Benito Menni Complex Assistencial en Salut Mental, Sant Boi de Llobregat, Barcelona, Spain
| | - María Ángeles García-León
- FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain; CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Barcelona, Spain
| | - Auria Albacete
- FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain; CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Barcelona, Spain
| | - Clara Bosque
- Benito Menni Complex Assistencial en Salut Mental, Sant Boi de Llobregat, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Francesco Panicalli
- Benito Menni Complex Assistencial en Salut Mental, Sant Boi de Llobregat, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Ester Boix
- Mental Health Department, Hospital de Mataró, Mataró, Spain
| | - Josep Munuera
- Diagnostic Imaging Department, Hospital Sant Joan de Déu, Barcelona, Spain
| | | | - Salvador Sarró
- FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain; CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Barcelona, Spain
| | - Miquel Bernardo
- CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Barcelona, Spain; Barcelona Clínic Schizophrenia Unit, Hospital Clínic of Barcelona, Institute of Neuroscience, Barcelona, Spain; Universitat de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain; Institut d'investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), Barcelona, Spain
| | - Raymond Salvador
- FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain; CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Barcelona, Spain
| | - Peter J McKenna
- FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain; CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Barcelona, Spain.
| | - Edith Pomarol-Clotet
- FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalàries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain; CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental), Barcelona, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Si R, Rowe JB, Zhang J. Functional localization and categorization of intentional decisions in humans: A meta-analysis of brain imaging studies. Neuroimage 2021; 242:118468. [PMID: 34390878 PMCID: PMC8463837 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118468] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/12/2020] [Revised: 06/22/2021] [Accepted: 08/10/2021] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Brain-imaging research on intentional decision-making often employs a "free-choice" paradigm, in which participants choose among options with identical values or outcomes. Although the medial prefrontal cortex has commonly been associated with choices, there is no consensus on the wider network that underlies diverse intentional decisions and behaviours. Our systematic literature search identified 35 fMRI/PET experiments using various free-choice paradigms, with appropriate control conditions using external instructions. An Activation Likelihood Estimate (ALE) meta-analysis showed that, compared with external instructions, intentional decisions consistently activate the medial and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the left insula and the inferior parietal lobule. We then categorized the studies into four different types according to their experimental designs: reactive motor intention, perceptual intention, inhibitory intention, and cognitive intention. We conducted conjunction and contrast meta-analyses to identify consistent and selective spatial convergence of brain activation within each specific category of intentional decision. Finally, we used meta-analytic decoding to probe cognitive processes underlying free choices. Our findings suggest that the neurocognitive process underlying intentional decision incorporates anatomically separated components subserving distinct cognitive and computational roles.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ruoguang Si
- Cardiff University Brain Research Imaging Centre, School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Cardiff CF24 4HQ, United Kingdom.
| | - James B Rowe
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 0QQ, United Kingdom; Medical Research Council Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge CB2 7EF, United Kingdom
| | - Jiaxiang Zhang
- Cardiff University Brain Research Imaging Centre, School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Cardiff CF24 4HQ, United Kingdom.
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Belli VD, Orcioli-Silva D, Beretta VS, Vitório R, Zampier VC, Nóbrega-Sousa P, Conceição NRD, Gobbi LTB. Prefrontal Cortical Activity During Preferred and Fast Walking in Young and Older Adults: An fNIRS Study. Neuroscience 2021; 473:81-89. [PMID: 34455013 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2021.08.019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/04/2020] [Revised: 08/10/2021] [Accepted: 08/19/2021] [Indexed: 12/11/2022]
Abstract
Age-related changes may affect the performance during fast walking speed. Although, several studies have been focused on the contribution of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) during challenging walking tasks, the neural mechanism underling fast walking speed in older people remain poorly understood. Therefore, the aim of this study was to investigate the influence of aging on PFC activity during overground walking at preferred and fast speeds. Twenty-five older adults (67.37 ± 5.31 years) and 24 young adults (22.70 ± 1.30 years) walked overground in two conditions: preferred speed and fast walking speed. Five trials were performed for each condition. A wireless functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) system measured PFC activity. Gait parameters were evaluated using the GAITRite system. Overall, older adults presented higher PFC activity than young adults in both conditions. Speed-related change in PFC activity was observed for older adults, but not for young adults. Older adults significantly increased activity in the left PFC from the preferred to fast walking condition whereas young adults had similar levels of PFC activity across conditions. Our findings suggest that older adults need to recruit additional prefrontal cognitive resources to control walking, indicating a compensatory mechanism. In addition, left PFC seems to be involved in the modulation of gait speed in older adults.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Vinicius de Belli
- São Paulo State University (UNESP), Institute of Biosciences, Posture and Gait Studies Laboratory (LEPLO), Rio Claro, Brazil
| | - Diego Orcioli-Silva
- São Paulo State University (UNESP), Institute of Biosciences, Posture and Gait Studies Laboratory (LEPLO), Rio Claro, Brazil; Graduate Program in Movement Sciences, São Paulo State University (UNESP), Rio Claro, Brazil
| | - Victor Spiandor Beretta
- São Paulo State University (UNESP), Institute of Biosciences, Posture and Gait Studies Laboratory (LEPLO), Rio Claro, Brazil; Graduate Program in Movement Sciences, São Paulo State University (UNESP), Rio Claro, Brazil
| | - Rodrigo Vitório
- São Paulo State University (UNESP), Institute of Biosciences, Posture and Gait Studies Laboratory (LEPLO), Rio Claro, Brazil; Graduate Program in Movement Sciences, São Paulo State University (UNESP), Rio Claro, Brazil; Department of Neurology, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, OR, United States
| | - Vinicius Cavassano Zampier
- São Paulo State University (UNESP), Institute of Biosciences, Posture and Gait Studies Laboratory (LEPLO), Rio Claro, Brazil; Graduate Program in Movement Sciences, São Paulo State University (UNESP), Rio Claro, Brazil
| | - Priscila Nóbrega-Sousa
- São Paulo State University (UNESP), Institute of Biosciences, Posture and Gait Studies Laboratory (LEPLO), Rio Claro, Brazil; Graduate Program in Movement Sciences, São Paulo State University (UNESP), Rio Claro, Brazil
| | - Núbia Ribeiro da Conceição
- São Paulo State University (UNESP), Institute of Biosciences, Posture and Gait Studies Laboratory (LEPLO), Rio Claro, Brazil; Graduate Program in Movement Sciences, São Paulo State University (UNESP), Rio Claro, Brazil
| | - Lilian Teresa Bucken Gobbi
- São Paulo State University (UNESP), Institute of Biosciences, Posture and Gait Studies Laboratory (LEPLO), Rio Claro, Brazil; Graduate Program in Movement Sciences, São Paulo State University (UNESP), Rio Claro, Brazil.
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Brockett AT, Roesch MR. Reactive and Proactive Adaptation of Cognitive and Motor Neural Signals during Performance of a Stop-Change Task. Brain Sci 2021; 11:617. [PMID: 34064876 PMCID: PMC8151620 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci11050617] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/09/2021] [Revised: 05/04/2021] [Accepted: 05/06/2021] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
The ability to inhibit or suppress unwanted or inappropriate actions, is an essential component of executive function and cognitive health. The immense selective pressure placed on maintaining inhibitory control processes is exemplified by the relatively small number of instances in which these systems completely fail in the average person's daily life. Although mistakes and errors do inevitably occur, inhibitory control systems not only ensure that this number is low, but have also adapted behavioral strategies to minimize future failures. The ability of our brains to adapt our behavior and appropriately engage proper motor responses is traditionally depicted as the primary domain of frontal brain areas, despite evidence to the fact that numerous other brain areas contribute. Using the stop-signal task as a common ground for comparison, we review a large body of literature investigating inhibitory control processes across frontal, temporal, and midbrain structures, focusing on our recent work in rodents, in an effort to understand how the brain biases action selection and adapts to the experience of conflict.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Adam T. Brockett
- Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742, USA;
- Program in Neuroscience and Cognitive Science, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742, USA
| | - Matthew R. Roesch
- Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742, USA;
- Program in Neuroscience and Cognitive Science, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742, USA
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
The Potential Role of Dopamine in Mediating Motor Function and Interpersonal Synchrony. Biomedicines 2021; 9:biomedicines9040382. [PMID: 33916451 PMCID: PMC8066519 DOI: 10.3390/biomedicines9040382] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/22/2020] [Revised: 03/29/2021] [Accepted: 03/31/2021] [Indexed: 12/28/2022] Open
Abstract
Motor functions in general and motor planning in particular are crucial for our ability to synchronize our movements with those of others. To date, these co-occurring functions have been studied separately, and as yet it is unclear whether they share a common biological mechanism. Here, we synthesize disparate recent findings on motor functioning and interpersonal synchrony and propose that these two functions share a common neurobiological mechanism and adhere to the same principles of predictive coding. Critically, we describe the pivotal role of the dopaminergic system in modulating these two distinct functions. We present attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) as an example of a disorder that involves the dopaminergic system and describe deficits in motor and interpersonal synchrony. Finally, we suggest possible directions for future studies emphasizing the role of dopamine modulation as a link between social and motor functioning.
Collapse
|
14
|
Anterior cingulate cortex and adaptive control of brain and behavior. INTERNATIONAL REVIEW OF NEUROBIOLOGY 2020; 158:283-309. [PMID: 33785148 DOI: 10.1016/bs.irn.2020.11.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/03/2022]
Abstract
Research examining the functional underpinnings of anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and its relationship to cognitive control have been described as "perennially controversial" and a "Rorschach Test" for modern neuroscience. Although there is near universal agreement that ACC is important for the adaptation of behavior, debate, despite decades of work, stems from the exact manner in which ACC goes about doing this. This chapter provides a brief overview of the various past and present theoretical arguments and research surrounding ACC function, and highlights an emerging literature of single unit ACC recordings from several species that support these theories. We will finish the chapter by focusing on our work examining the firing of single neurons in rat dorsal medial striatum (DMS) and ACC, and examining DMS's dependency on ACC to accurately signal adaptive behavioral output. Ultimately, we will conclude that ACC carries a myriad of signals (error detection, reinforcement/feedback, value, response conflict, etc.) necessary for the modulation of attention and task-relevant/irrelevant signals so that difficult decisions can be made and action plans adapted when necessary.
Collapse
|
15
|
Travers E, Haggard P. The Readiness Potential reflects the internal source of action, rather than decision uncertainty. Eur J Neurosci 2020; 53:1533-1544. [PMID: 33236376 DOI: 10.1111/ejn.15063] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/10/2020] [Revised: 11/10/2020] [Accepted: 11/10/2020] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Voluntary actions are preceded by a Readiness Potential (RP), a slow EEG (electroencephalogram) component generated in medial frontal cortical areas. The RP is classically thought to be specific to internally-driven decisions to act, and to reflect post-decision motor preparation. Recent work suggests instead that it may reflect noise or conflict during the decision itself, with internally driven decisions tending to be more random, more conflicted and thus more uncertain than externally driven actions. To contrast accounts based on endogenicity with accounts based on uncertainty, we recorded EEG in a task where participants decided to act or withhold action to accept or reject visually presented gambles, and used multivariate methods to extract an RP-like component. We found no difference in amplitude of this component between actions driven by strong versus weak evidence, suggesting that the RP may not reflect uncertainty. In contrast, the same RP-like component showed higher amplitudes prior to actions performed without any external evidence (guesses) than for actions performed in response to equivocal, conflicting evidence. This supports the view that the RP reflects the internal source of action, rather than decision uncertainty.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eoin Travers
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, UK
| | - Patrick Haggard
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Takashima S, Ogawa CY, Najman FA, Ramos RT. The volition, the mode of movement selection and the readiness potential. Exp Brain Res 2020; 238:2113-2123. [PMID: 32656650 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-020-05878-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/26/2019] [Accepted: 07/08/2020] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
A growing body of evidence suggests that the mode of movement selection is relevant for the readiness potential, namely, internal (or free) selection of movements is associated with increased readiness potential amplitudes compared to predetermined or externally guided selection. It is little acknowledged, however, that this finding may be ascribed to the different expression of volition (i.e., conscious experience of choice) rather than to the mode of movement selection per se. To probe this issue, we conducted two experiments: in Experiment 1, a mental task was employed to distract sixteen volunteers from the selection and performance of incidental movements, which consisted of pressing one of two buttons according to either free or externally guided modes of movement selection; in Experiment 2, another sixteen individuals performed the same motor task, however, they were encouraged to attend to their intention to act. As result, the increased readiness potential amplitude before freely selected movements was found exclusively in Experiment 2. More detailed analysis suggested that the attention to the initiation of movements was associated with greater readiness potential in its medial and late portion, while the attention to the movement selection, with more global increase of the component. The study suggests that much of the higher demands on motor preparatory activities ascribed to the internal selection of movements in previous studies actually depends on individual's attention and, thus, probably corresponds to volitional processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shiro Takashima
- Department of Psychiatry, Faculdade de Medicina, Instituto de Psiquiatria, LIM23, Hospital das Clinicas HCFMUSP, Universidade de Sao Paulo, Rua Doutor Ovídio Pires de Campos, 785, Cerqueira César, São Paulo, SP, 05403-010, Brazil.
| | - Carolina Y Ogawa
- Department of Psychiatry, Faculdade de Medicina, Instituto de Psiquiatria, LIM23, Hospital das Clinicas HCFMUSP, Universidade de Sao Paulo, Rua Doutor Ovídio Pires de Campos, 785, Cerqueira César, São Paulo, SP, 05403-010, Brazil
| | - Fernando Araujo Najman
- Instituto de Matematica e Estatistica, RIDC NeuroMat, Universidade de Sao Paulo, Av. Prof. Luciano Gualberto, 117, Butantã, São Paulo, SP, 05508-010, Brazil
| | - Renato T Ramos
- Department of Psychiatry, Faculdade de Medicina, Instituto de Psiquiatria, LIM23, Hospital das Clinicas HCFMUSP, Universidade de Sao Paulo, Rua Doutor Ovídio Pires de Campos, 785, Cerqueira César, São Paulo, SP, 05403-010, Brazil
- Department of Psychiatry, Frederick W. Thompson Anxiety Disorders Centre, Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre, University of Toronto, 2075 Bayview Avenue, K3W42, Toronto, ON, M4N 3M5, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Abstract
As all human activities, verbal communication is fraught with errors. It is estimated that humans produce around 16,000 words per day, but the word that is selected for production is not always correct and neither is the articulation always flawless. However, to facilitate communication, it is important to limit the number of errors. This is accomplished via the verbal monitoring mechanism. A body of research over the last century has uncovered a number of properties of the mechanisms at work during verbal monitoring. Over a dozen routes for verbal monitoring have been postulated. However, to date a complete account of verbal monitoring does not exist. In the current paper we first outline the properties of verbal monitoring that have been empirically demonstrated. This is followed by a discussion of current verbal monitoring models: the perceptual loop theory, conflict monitoring, the hierarchical state feedback control model, and the forward model theory. Each of these models is evaluated given empirical findings and theoretical considerations. We then outline lacunae of current theories, which we address with a proposal for a new model of verbal monitoring for production and perception, based on conflict monitoring models. Additionally, this novel model suggests a mechanism of how a detected error leads to a correction. The error resolution mechanism proposed in our new model is then tested in a computational model. Finally, we outline the advances and predictions of the model.
Collapse
|
18
|
Wang Q, She S, Luo L, Li H, Ning Y, Ren J, Wu Z, Huang R, Zheng Y. Abnormal Contingent Negative Variation Drifts During Facial Expression Judgment in Schizophrenia Patients. Front Hum Neurosci 2020; 14:274. [PMID: 32760264 PMCID: PMC7371930 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00274] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/05/2020] [Accepted: 06/18/2020] [Indexed: 02/05/2023] Open
Abstract
Schizophrenia patients often show impaired facial expression recognition, which leads to difficulties in adaptation to daily life. However, it remains unclear whether the deficit is at the perceptual or higher cognitive level of facial emotion processing. Recent studies have shown that earlier face-evoked event-related potential (ERP) components such as N170 and P100 can effectively distinguish schizophrenia patients from healthy controls; however, findings for later waveforms are ambiguous. To clarify this point, in this study we compared electroencephalographic signals in schizophrenia patients and control subjects during a facial expression judgment task. We found that group effects of the occipital N170 and frontal lobe contingent negative variation (CNV) were both significant. The effect sizes (ESs) of N170 and CNV amplitudes were generally medium or small, whereas that of CNV slope for an upright face was large (>0.8). Moreover, N170 amplitude and CNV slope but not CNV amplitude was correlated with Personal and Social Performance (PSP) Scale score. These results suggest that the slope of CNV drift during facial expression processing has a potential clinical value for schizophrenia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Qian Wang
- Department of Clinical Psychology, Sanbo Brain Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
| | - Shenglin She
- Department of General Psychiatry, The Affiliated Brain Hospital of Guangzhou Medical University (Guangzhou Huiai Hospital), Guangzhou, China
| | - Lu Luo
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences, Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Haijing Li
- Department of General Psychiatry, The Affiliated Brain Hospital of Guangzhou Medical University (Guangzhou Huiai Hospital), Guangzhou, China
| | - Yuping Ning
- Department of General Psychiatry, The Affiliated Brain Hospital of Guangzhou Medical University (Guangzhou Huiai Hospital), Guangzhou, China
| | - Jianjuan Ren
- Department of General Psychiatry, The Affiliated Brain Hospital of Guangzhou Medical University (Guangzhou Huiai Hospital), Guangzhou, China
| | - Zhangying Wu
- Department of General Psychiatry, The Affiliated Brain Hospital of Guangzhou Medical University (Guangzhou Huiai Hospital), Guangzhou, China
| | - Rongcheng Huang
- Department of General Psychiatry, The Affiliated Brain Hospital of Guangzhou Medical University (Guangzhou Huiai Hospital), Guangzhou, China
| | - Yingjun Zheng
- Department of General Psychiatry, The Affiliated Brain Hospital of Guangzhou Medical University (Guangzhou Huiai Hospital), Guangzhou, China
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Magan D, Yadav RK. Neural mechanism of attention control in long-term preksha meditation. Med Hypotheses 2020; 143:109953. [PMID: 32679421 DOI: 10.1016/j.mehy.2020.109953] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/26/2020] [Revised: 05/23/2020] [Accepted: 06/04/2020] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Meditation is a complex mental process-practiced widely for stress management and promotion of health- exerts beneficial effects on physical and mental health, and cognitive performance. However, until now, few theoretical neural mechanisms of meditation had been proposed, still not completely elucidated. We have previously evaluated the brain activity during Preksha meditation using an innovative 18FDG-PET methodological approach. Based on our previous study observations, we proposed here the neurophysiological framework of long-term and regular practice of preksha meditation. This mechanism will provide the scientific evidence to understand the attention control mechanisms resulting from the advanced state of preksha meditation. This might have multiple clinical applications as well as effective in a healthy population for attention-related tasks. Also, it is expected that the present neural model will provide a scientific platform for future clinical studies of meditation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dipti Magan
- Department of Physiology, All India Institute of Medical Sciences, Bathinda 151001, India.
| | - Raj Kumar Yadav
- Department of Physiology, All India Institute of Medical Sciences, New Delhi 110029, India.
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Noguera C, Carmona D, Rueda A, Fernández R, Cimadevilla JM. Shall We Dance? Dancing Modulates Executive Functions and Spatial Memory. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF ENVIRONMENTAL RESEARCH AND PUBLIC HEALTH 2020; 17:ijerph17061960. [PMID: 32192128 PMCID: PMC7143315 DOI: 10.3390/ijerph17061960] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/17/2020] [Revised: 03/10/2020] [Accepted: 03/12/2020] [Indexed: 12/23/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Aging is generally considered to be related to physical and cognitive decline. This is especially prominent in the frontal and parietal lobes, underlying executive functions and spatial memory, respectively. This process could be successfully mitigated in certain ways, such as through the practice of aerobic sports. With regard to this, dancing integrates physical exercise with music and involves retrieval of complex sequences of steps and movements creating choreographies. METHODS In this study, we compared 26 non-professional salsa dancers (mean age 55.3 years, age-range 49-70 years) with 20 non-dancers (mean age 57.6 years, age-range 49-70 years) by assessing two variables: their executive functions and spatial memory performance. RESULTS results showed that dancers scored better that non-dancers in our tests, outperforming controls in executive functions-related tasks. Groups did not differ in spatial memory performance. CONCLUSIONS This work suggests that dancing can be a valid way of slowing down the natural age-related cognitive decline. A major limitation of this study is the lack of fitness assessment in both groups. In addition, since dancing combines multiple factors like social contact, aerobic exercise, cognitive work with rhythms, and music, it is difficult to determine the weight of each variable.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carmen Noguera
- Department of Psychology, University of Almeria, 04120 Almería, Spain; (C.N.); (D.C.); (A.R.)
- Health Research Center, University of Almeria, 04120 Almería, Spain;
| | - Dolores Carmona
- Department of Psychology, University of Almeria, 04120 Almería, Spain; (C.N.); (D.C.); (A.R.)
| | - Adrián Rueda
- Department of Psychology, University of Almeria, 04120 Almería, Spain; (C.N.); (D.C.); (A.R.)
| | - Rubén Fernández
- Health Research Center, University of Almeria, 04120 Almería, Spain;
- Department of Nursing, Physiotherapy and Medicine, University of Almeria, 04120 Almería, Spain
| | - José Manuel Cimadevilla
- Department of Psychology, University of Almeria, 04120 Almería, Spain; (C.N.); (D.C.); (A.R.)
- Health Research Center, University of Almeria, 04120 Almería, Spain;
- Correspondence: ; Tel.: +34-950-214-637
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Rubin-Falcone H, Weber J, Kishon R, Ochsner K, Delaparte L, Doré B, Raman S, Denny BT, Oquendo MA, Mann JJ, Miller JM. Neural predictors and effects of cognitive behavioral therapy for depression: the role of emotional reactivity and regulation. Psychol Med 2020; 50:146-160. [PMID: 30739618 DOI: 10.1017/s0033291718004154] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/18/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) is an effective treatment for many patients suffering from major depressive disorder (MDD), but predictors of treatment outcome are lacking, and little is known about its neural mechanisms. We recently identified longitudinal changes in neural correlates of conscious emotion regulation that scaled with clinical responses to CBT for MDD, using a negative autobiographical memory-based task. METHODS We now examine the neural correlates of emotional reactivity and emotion regulation during viewing of emotionally salient images as predictors of treatment outcome with CBT for MDD, and the relationship between longitudinal change in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) responses and clinical outcomes. Thirty-two participants with current MDD underwent baseline MRI scanning followed by 14 sessions of CBT. The fMRI task measured emotional reactivity and emotion regulation on separate trials using standardized images from the International Affective Pictures System. Twenty-one participants completed post-treatment scanning. Last observation carried forward was used to estimate clinical outcome for non-completers. RESULTS Pre-treatment emotional reactivity Blood Oxygen Level-Dependent (BOLD) signal within hippocampus including CA1 predicted worse treatment outcome. In contrast, better treatment outcome was associated with increased down-regulation of BOLD activity during emotion regulation from time 1 to time 2 in precuneus, occipital cortex, and middle frontal gyrus. CONCLUSIONS CBT may modulate the neural circuitry of emotion regulation. The neural correlates of emotional reactivity may be more strongly predictive of CBT outcome. The finding that treatment outcome was predicted by BOLD signal in CA1 may suggest overgeneralized memory as a negative prognostic factor in CBT outcome.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Harry Rubin-Falcone
- Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
- Division of Molecular Imaging and Neuropathology, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| | - Jochen Weber
- Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| | - Ronit Kishon
- Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| | - Kevin Ochsner
- Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| | - Lauren Delaparte
- Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA
| | - Bruce Doré
- Annenberg School for Communication, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Sudha Raman
- Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
- Division of Molecular Imaging and Neuropathology, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| | - Bryan T Denny
- Department of Psychology, Rice University, Houston, TX, USA
| | - Maria A Oquendo
- Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - J John Mann
- Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
- Division of Molecular Imaging and Neuropathology, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| | - Jeffrey M Miller
- Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
- Division of Molecular Imaging and Neuropathology, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Seghezzi S, Giannini G, Zapparoli L. Neurofunctional correlates of body-ownership and sense of agency: A meta-analytical account of self-consciousness. Cortex 2019; 121:169-178. [PMID: 31629195 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.08.018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/27/2019] [Revised: 07/31/2019] [Accepted: 08/29/2019] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
Self-consciousness consists of several dissociable experiences, including the sense of ownership of one's body and the sense of agency over one's action consequences. The relationship between body-ownership and the sense of agency has been described by different neurocognitive models, each providing specific neurofunctional predictions. According to an "additive" model, the sense of agency entails body-ownership, while an alternative "independence" hypothesis suggests that they represent two qualitatively different processes, underpinned by distinct brain systems. We propose a third "interactive" model, arguing the interdependence between body-ownership and the sense of agency: these constructs might represent different experiences with specific and exclusive brain correlates, but they also could partly overlap at the neurofunctional level. Here we test these three neurocognitive models by reviewing the available neurofunctional literature of body-ownership and the sense of agency, with a quantitative meta-analytical approach that allowed us to compare their neural correlates statistically. We identified (i) a body-ownership-specific network including the left inferior parietal lobule and the left extra-striate body area, (ii) a sense-of-agency-specific network including the left SMA, the left posterior insula, the right postcentral gyrus, and the right superior temporal lobe and (iii) a shared network in the left middle insula. These results provide support for the interactive neurocognitive model of body-ownership and the sense of agency. Body-ownership involves a sensory network in which multisensory inputs are integrated to be self-attributed. On the other hand, the sense of agency is specifically associated with premotor and sensory-motor areas, typically involved in generating motor predictions and in action monitoring. Finally, body-ownership and the sense of agency interact at the level of the left middle insula, a high-level multisensory hub engaged in body and action awareness in general.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Silvia Seghezzi
- Department of Psychology and NeuroMi - Milan Centre for Neuroscience, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milano, Italy; PhD Program in Neuroscience, School of Medicine and Surgery, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milano, Italy
| | - Gianluigi Giannini
- Department of Psychology and NeuroMi - Milan Centre for Neuroscience, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milano, Italy
| | | |
Collapse
|
23
|
Nair S, Nenert RE, Allendorfer JB, Goodman AM, Vannest J, Mirman D, Szaflarski JP. Sex, Age, and Handedness Modulate the Neural Correlates of Active Learning. Front Neurosci 2019; 13:961. [PMID: 31572114 PMCID: PMC6749092 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00961] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/23/2019] [Accepted: 08/27/2019] [Indexed: 12/18/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Self-generation of material compared to passive learning results in mproved memory performance; this may be related to recruitment of a fronto-temporal encoding network. Using a verbal paired-associate learning fMRI task, we examined the effects of sex, age, and handedness on the neural correlates of self-generation. METHODS Data from 174 healthy English-speaking participants (78M, 56 atypically handed; ages 19-76) were preprocessed using AFNI and FSL. Independent component analysis was conducted using GIFT (Group ICA fMRI Toolbox). Forty-one independent components were temporally sorted by task time series. Retaining correlations (r > 0.25) resulted in three task-positive ("generate") and three task-negative ("read") components. Using participants' back-projected components, we evaluated the effects of sex, handedness, and aging on activation lateralization and localization in task-relevant networks with two-sample t-tests. Further, we examined the linear relationship between sex and neuroimaging data with multiple regression, covarying for scanner, age, and handedness. RESULTS Task-positive components identified using ICA revealed a fronto-parietal network involved with self-generation, while task-negative components reflecting passive reading showed temporo-occipital involvement. Compared to older adults, younger adults exhibited greater task-positive involvement of the left inferior frontal gyrus and insula, whereas older adults exhibited reduced prefrontal lateralization. Greater involvement of the left angular gyrus in task-positive encoding networks among right-handed individuals suggests the reliance on left dominant semantic processing areas may be modulated by handedness. Sex effects on task-related encoding networks while controlling for age and handedness suggest increased right hemisphere recruitment among males compared to females, specifically in the paracentral lobe during self-generation and the suparmarginal gyrus during passive reading. IMPLICATIONS Identified neuroimaging differences suggest that sex, age, and handedness are factors in the differential recruitment of encoding network regions for both passive and active learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sangeeta Nair
- Department of Neurology, The University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL, United States
- Department of Psychology, The University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL, United States
| | - Rodolphe E. Nenert
- Department of Neurology, The University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL, United States
| | - Jane B. Allendorfer
- Department of Neurology, The University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL, United States
| | - Adam M. Goodman
- Department of Neurology, The University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL, United States
| | - Jennifer Vannest
- Department of Pediatrics, Division of Neurology, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center, Pediatric Neuroimaging Research Consortium, University of Cincinnati College of Medicine, Cincinnati, OH, United States
| | - Daniel Mirman
- Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
| | - Jerzy P. Szaflarski
- Department of Neurology, The University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL, United States
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Kim J, Shin JH, Ryu JK, Jung JH, Kim CH, Lee HB, Kim DH, Lee SK, Roh D. Physical performance is more strongly associated with cognition in schizophrenia than psychiatric symptoms. Eur Psychiatry 2019; 61:72-78. [PMID: 31349152 DOI: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2019.06.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/13/2019] [Revised: 06/24/2019] [Accepted: 06/27/2019] [Indexed: 10/26/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Although neurocognitive dysfunction and physical performance are known to be impaired in patients with schizophrenia, evidence regarding the relationship between these two domains remains insufficient. Thus, we aimed to investigate the relationship between various physical performance domains and cognitive domains in individuals with schizophrenia, while considering other disorder-related clinical symptoms. METHODS Sixty patients with schizophrenia participated in the study. Cardiorespiratory fitness and functional mobility were evaluated using the step test and supine-to-standing (STS) test, respectively. Executive function and working memory were assessed using the Stroop task and Sternberg working memory (SWM) task, respectively. Clinical symptoms were evaluated using the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale, Beck Depression Inventory, and State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Multivariate analyses were performed to adjust for relevant covariates and identify predictive factors associated with neurocognition. RESULTS Multiple regression analysis revealed that the step test index was most strongly associated with reaction time in the Stroop task (β = 0.434, p = 0.001) and SWM task (β = 0.331, p = 0.026), while STS test time was most strongly associated with accuracy on the Stoop task (β=-0.418, p = 0.001) and SWM task (β=-0.383, p = 0.007). Total cholesterol levels were positively associated with Stroop task accuracy (β=-0.307, p = 0.018) after controlling for other clinical correlates. However, clinical symptoms were not associated with any variables in Stroop or SWM task. CONCLUSIONS The present findings demonstrate the relationship between physical performance and neurocognition in patients with schizophrenia. Considering that these factors are modifiable, exercise intervention may help to improve cognitive symptoms in patients with schizophrenia, thereby leading to improvements in function and prognosis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jiheon Kim
- Mind-neuromodulation Laboratory and Department of Psychiatry, Chuncheon Sacred Heart Hospital, Hallym University College of Medicine, 77 Sakju-ro, Chuncheon, Gangwon-Do, 24253, Republic of Korea
| | - Ji-Hyeon Shin
- Department of Otolaryngology-Head and Neck Surgery, College of Medicine, The Catholic University of Korea, 271 Cheonbo-ro, Uijeongbu, Gyungki-Do, 11765, Republic of Korea
| | - Jeh-Kwang Ryu
- Institute for Cognitive Science, College of Humanities, Seoul National University, 1 Gwanak-ro, Gwanak-Gu, Seoul, 08826, Republic of Korea
| | - Jae Hoon Jung
- Mind-neuromodulation Laboratory and Department of Psychiatry, Chuncheon Sacred Heart Hospital, Hallym University College of Medicine, 77 Sakju-ro, Chuncheon, Gangwon-Do, 24253, Republic of Korea
| | - Chan-Hyung Kim
- Department of Psychiatry, Severance Hospital, Yonsei University College of Medicine, 50-1 Yonsei-ro, Seodaemun-Gu, Seoul, 03722, Republic of Korea
| | - Hwa-Bock Lee
- Gwangmyeong Mental Health Welfare Center, 613 Ori-ro, Gwangmyeong-si, Gyungki-do, 14303, Republic of Korea
| | - Do Hoon Kim
- Mind-neuromodulation Laboratory and Department of Psychiatry, Chuncheon Sacred Heart Hospital, Hallym University College of Medicine, 77 Sakju-ro, Chuncheon, Gangwon-Do, 24253, Republic of Korea
| | - Sang-Kyu Lee
- Mind-neuromodulation Laboratory and Department of Psychiatry, Chuncheon Sacred Heart Hospital, Hallym University College of Medicine, 77 Sakju-ro, Chuncheon, Gangwon-Do, 24253, Republic of Korea
| | - Daeyoung Roh
- Mind-neuromodulation Laboratory and Department of Psychiatry, Chuncheon Sacred Heart Hospital, Hallym University College of Medicine, 77 Sakju-ro, Chuncheon, Gangwon-Do, 24253, Republic of Korea.
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Piras F, Piras F, Banaj N, Ciullo V, Vecchio D, Edden RAE, Spalletta G. Cerebellar GABAergic correlates of cognition-mediated verbal fluency in physiology and schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2019; 139:582-594. [PMID: 30887499 DOI: 10.1111/acps.13027] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 03/11/2019] [Indexed: 12/11/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Defective cerebellar GABAergic inhibitory control may participate to the cognitive impairments seen in SZ. We tested the prediction of a model for the relationship between cerebellar GABA concentration and the associative/executive processes required by verbal fluency in patients with schizophrenia (SZ) and matched healthy controls (HC). METHOD Magnetic resonance spectroscopy of GABA was performed using a 3 Tesla scanner and verbal fluency assessed by the Controlled Word (WFT) and Semantic (SFT) Fluency tests. Cerebellar GABA measurements were obtained using the MEGA-PRESS acquisition sequence. Linear correlations between cerebellar GABA levels and the WFT, SFT score were performed to test differences between correlation coefficients of SZ and HC. Quantile regressions between GABA levels and the WFT score were performed. RESULTS Higher cerebellar GABA concentration was associated in SZ with lower phonemic fluency and reduced number of switches among subcategories as opposed to what observed in HC (with higher cerebellar GABA associated with higher number of words and phonemic switches). GABA levels explained phonemic fluency in SZ performing above the group mean. CONCLUSION Studying cerebellar GABA provides a valid heuristic to explore the molecular mechanisms of SZ. This is crucial for developing pharmacological treatments to improve cognition and functional recovery in SZ.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- F Piras
- Neuropsychiatry Laboratory, IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy
| | - F Piras
- Neuropsychiatry Laboratory, IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy
| | - N Banaj
- Neuropsychiatry Laboratory, IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy
| | - V Ciullo
- Neuropsychiatry Laboratory, IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy
| | - D Vecchio
- Neuropsychiatry Laboratory, IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy
| | - R A E Edden
- The Russell H. Morgan Department of Radiology and Radiological Science, The Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, USA.,F.M. Kirby Center for Functional Brain Imaging, Kennedy Krieger Institute, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - G Spalletta
- Neuropsychiatry Laboratory, IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy.,Beth K. and Stuart C. Yudofsky Division of Neuropsychiatry, Menninger Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, USA
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Seghezzi S, Zirone E, Paulesu E, Zapparoli L. The Brain in (Willed) Action: A Meta-Analytical Comparison of Imaging Studies on Motor Intentionality and Sense of Agency. Front Psychol 2019; 10:804. [PMID: 31031676 PMCID: PMC6473038 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00804] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/16/2018] [Accepted: 03/25/2019] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Voluntary actions can be fractionated in different phenomena: from the emergence of intentions and the ensuing motor plans and actions, to the anticipation and monitoring of their outcomes, to the appreciation of their congruency with intentions and to the eventual emergence of a sense of agency. It follows that motor intention and the sense of agency should occur at different stages in the normal generation of willed actions. Both these processes have been associated with a fronto-parietal motor network, but no study has investigated to what extent the two experiences can be dissociated for the brain regions involved. To this end, we assessed the PET/fMRI literature on agency and intentionality using a meta-analytic technique based on a hierarchical clustering algorithm. Beside a shared brain network involving the meso-frontal and prefrontal regions, the middle insula and subcortical structures, we found that motor intention and the sense of agency are functionally underpinned by separable sets of brain regions: an “intentionality network,” involving the rostral area of the mesial frontal cortex (middle cingulum and pre-supplementary motor area), the anterior insula and the parietal lobules, and a “self-agency network,” which involves the posterior areas of the mesial frontal cortex (the SMA proper), the posterior insula, the occipital lobe and the cerebellum. We were then able to confirm this functional organization by a subsequent seed-based fMRI resting-state functional connectivity analysis, with seeds derived from the intentionality/sense of agency specific clusters of the medial wall of the frontal lobe. Our results suggest the existence of a rostro-caudal gradient within the mesial frontal cortex, with the more anterior regions linked to the concept of motor intentionality and the brain areas located more posteriorly associated with the direct monitoring between the action and its outcome. This suggestion is reinforced by the association between the sense of agency and the activation of the occipital lobes, to suggest a direct comparison between the movement and its external (e.g., visual) consequences. The shared network may be important for the integration of intentionality and agency in a coherent appreciation of self-generated actions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Silvia Seghezzi
- Department of Psychology and NeuroMI - Milan Centre for Neuroscience, University of Milan-Bicocca, Milan, Italy.,Ph.D. Program in Neuroscience, School of Medicine and Surgery, University of Milan-Bicocca, Milan, Italy
| | - Eleonora Zirone
- fMRI Unit, IRCCS Orthopedic Institute Galeazzi, Milan, Italy
| | - Eraldo Paulesu
- Department of Psychology and NeuroMI - Milan Centre for Neuroscience, University of Milan-Bicocca, Milan, Italy.,fMRI Unit, IRCCS Orthopedic Institute Galeazzi, Milan, Italy
| | - Laura Zapparoli
- fMRI Unit, IRCCS Orthopedic Institute Galeazzi, Milan, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
de Jong BM. Free Will Emerges From a Multistage Process of Target Assignment and Body-Scheme Recruitment for Free Effector Selection. Front Psychol 2019; 10:388. [PMID: 30873085 PMCID: PMC6401622 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00388] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/30/2018] [Accepted: 02/07/2019] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Self-intended action implies an initial stage of assigning an external entity as target of action, with subsequent recruitment of body-scheme information serving the free selection of an appropriate effector system to achieve the action aim. This plurality underscores the concept that neuronal response freedom underlying the generation of such action is not necessarily restricted to a singular cerebral event at its initiation, but that such freedom is embedded in a series of successive processing steps. In this respect, action intention initially concerns the transition of a neutral object into a target of action, while the “will” to act further crystalizes with the recruitment of one’s body scheme. The latter is a prerequisite for effector selection and indeed complements the emerging sense of agency. This temporal order of neuronal events fits a model of fronto-parietal interactions associated with volition. A concise behavioral experiment is additionally described, in which successively displayed balls represent either a recognizable object with distinct shape and color features, or a target of action. Instructions to write down the ball’s characteristics were alternated by the command ”action.” When shifting from a neutral object to an action target, the ball was placed in one of three backgrounds: empty, an outdoor goal or indoor basket. In response to the action command, subjects reported intended actions such as kicking, seizing, throwing and heading, thus implicitly referring to the foot, hand, or head as chosen effector. For the latter the parietal cortex is strongly implicated, not only concerning predefined but also free selection. Although subjects were free to choose what to do with the ball, the environmental cues of the ball strongly influenced their choices. These results illustrate the temporal order in fronto-parietal processing associated with initial target assignment, instantly followed by the embodiment of will, i.e., the recruitment of body-scheme information for possible effector selection. Such multistage neuronal processing underlying free action selection underscores that the onset of brain signals prior to the perceived sense of free will is not a valid argument to reduce free will to an illusion.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bauke M de Jong
- Department of Neurology, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Abstract
Volition refers to a capacity for endogenous action, particularly goal-directed endogenous action, shared by humans and some other animals. It has long been controversial whether a specific set of cognitive processes for volition exist in the human brain, and much scientific thinking on the topic continues to revolve around traditional metaphysical debates about free will. At its origins, scientific psychology had a strong engagement with volition. This was followed by a period of disenchantment, or even outright hostility, during the second half of the twentieth century. In this review, I aim to reinvigorate the scientific approach to volition by, first, proposing a range of different features that constitute a new, neurocognitively realistic working definition of volition. I then focus on three core features of human volition: its generativity (the capacity to trigger actions), its subjectivity (the conscious experiences associated with initiating voluntary actions), and its teleology (the goal-directed quality of some voluntary actions). I conclude that volition is a neurocognitive process of enormous societal importance and susceptible to scientific investigation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Patrick Haggard
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London WC1N 3AZ, United Kingdom
- Institute of Philosophy, School of Advanced Study, University of London, London WC1E 7HU, United Kingdom
- Laboratoire de Neurosciences Cognitives, Département d’Études Cognitives, École Normale Supérieure, 75005 Paris, France
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Viswanathan S, Wang BA, Abdollahi RO, Daun S, Grefkes C, Fink GR. Freely chosen and instructed actions are terminated by different neural mechanisms revealed by kinematics-informed EEG. Neuroimage 2018; 188:26-42. [PMID: 30521953 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.12.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/29/2018] [Revised: 11/06/2018] [Accepted: 12/02/2018] [Indexed: 10/27/2022] Open
Abstract
Neurophysiological accounts of human volition are dominated by debates on the origin of voluntary choices but the neural consequences that follow such choices remain poorly understood. For instance, could one predict whether or not an action was chosen voluntarily based only on how that action is motorically executed? We investigated this possibility by integrating scalp electroencephalograms and index-finger accelerometer recordings acquired while people chose between pressing a left or right button either freely or as instructed by a visual cue. Even though freely selected and instructed actions were executed with equal vigor, the timing of the movement to release the button was comparatively delayed for freely selected actions. This chronometric difference was six-times larger for the β-oscillations over the sensorimotor cortex that characteristically accompany an action's termination. This surprising modulation of an action's termination by volition was traceable to volition-modulated differences in how the competing yet non-selected action was represented and regulated.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shivakumar Viswanathan
- Cognitive Neuroscience, Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-3), Research Centre Jülich, 52425, Jülich, Germany; Department of Neurology, University Hospital Cologne, 50924, Cologne, Germany.
| | - Bin A Wang
- Cognitive Neuroscience, Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-3), Research Centre Jülich, 52425, Jülich, Germany; Department of Neurology, Ruhr-University Bochum, 44789, Bochum, Germany
| | - Rouhollah O Abdollahi
- Cognitive Neuroscience, Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-3), Research Centre Jülich, 52425, Jülich, Germany
| | - Silvia Daun
- Cognitive Neuroscience, Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-3), Research Centre Jülich, 52425, Jülich, Germany; Heisenberg Research Group of Computational Biology, Department of Animal Physiology, Institute of Zoology, University of Cologne, 50674, Cologne, Germany
| | - Christian Grefkes
- Cognitive Neuroscience, Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-3), Research Centre Jülich, 52425, Jülich, Germany; Department of Neurology, University Hospital Cologne, 50924, Cologne, Germany
| | - Gereon R Fink
- Cognitive Neuroscience, Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-3), Research Centre Jülich, 52425, Jülich, Germany; Department of Neurology, University Hospital Cologne, 50924, Cologne, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Leach J. ‘Give-up-itis’ revisited: Neuropathology of extremis. Med Hypotheses 2018; 120:14-21. [DOI: 10.1016/j.mehy.2018.08.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/14/2018] [Revised: 08/01/2018] [Accepted: 08/07/2018] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
31
|
Kang JM, Cho YS, Park S, Lee BH, Sohn BK, Choi CH, Choi JS, Jeong HY, Cho SJ, Lee JH, Lee JY. Montreal cognitive assessment reflects cognitive reserve. BMC Geriatr 2018; 18:261. [PMID: 30376815 PMCID: PMC6208087 DOI: 10.1186/s12877-018-0951-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 96] [Impact Index Per Article: 13.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/15/2017] [Accepted: 10/17/2018] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Background The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) is known to have discriminative power for patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI). Recently Cognitive Reserve (CR) has been introduced as a factor that compensates cognitive decline. We aimed to assess whether the MoCA reflects CR. Furthermore, we assessed whether there were any differences in the efficacy between the MoCA and the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) in reflecting CR. Methods MoCA, MMSE, and the Cognitive Reserve Index questionnaire (CRIq) were administered to 221 healthy participants. Normative data and associated factors of the MoCA were identified. Correlation and regression analyses of the MoCA, MMSE and CRIq scores were performed, and the MoCA score was compared with the MMSE score to evaluate the degree to which the MoCA reflected CR. Results The MoCA reflected total CRIq score (CRI; B = 0.076, P < 0.001), CRI-Education (B = 0.066, P < 0.001), and CRI-Working activity (B = 0.025, P = 0.042), while MMSE reflected total CRI (B = 0.044, P < 0.001) and CRI-Education (B = 0.049, P < 0.001) only. The MoCA differed from the MMSE in the reflection of total CRI (Z = 2.30). Conclusion In this study, we show that the MoCA score reflects CR more sensitively than the MMSE score. Therefore, we suggest that MoCA can be used to assess CR and early cognitive decline. Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (10.1186/s12877-018-0951-8) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jae Myeong Kang
- Department of Psychiatry, Gil Medical Center, Gachon University College of Medicine, Incheon, Republic of Korea
| | - Young-Sung Cho
- Department of Psychiatry, SMG-SNU Boramae Medical Center, Boramae-Ro 5-Gil, Shindaebang-dong, Dongjak-gu, Seoul, Republic of Korea.,Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, Seoul National University College of Medicine, Boramae-Ro 5-Gil, Shindaebang-dong, Dongjak-gu, Seoul, Republic of Korea
| | - Soowon Park
- Department of Education, Sejong University, Seoul, Republic of Korea
| | - Byung Ho Lee
- Department of Psychology, Salisbury University, Salisbury, Maryland, USA
| | - Bo Kyung Sohn
- Department of Psychiatry, SMG-SNU Boramae Medical Center, Boramae-Ro 5-Gil, Shindaebang-dong, Dongjak-gu, Seoul, Republic of Korea
| | - Chi Hyun Choi
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, Seoul National University College of Medicine, Boramae-Ro 5-Gil, Shindaebang-dong, Dongjak-gu, Seoul, Republic of Korea
| | - Jeong-Seok Choi
- Department of Psychiatry, SMG-SNU Boramae Medical Center, Boramae-Ro 5-Gil, Shindaebang-dong, Dongjak-gu, Seoul, Republic of Korea.,Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, Seoul National University College of Medicine, Boramae-Ro 5-Gil, Shindaebang-dong, Dongjak-gu, Seoul, Republic of Korea
| | - Hee Yeon Jeong
- Department of Psychiatry, SMG-SNU Boramae Medical Center, Boramae-Ro 5-Gil, Shindaebang-dong, Dongjak-gu, Seoul, Republic of Korea.,Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, Seoul National University College of Medicine, Boramae-Ro 5-Gil, Shindaebang-dong, Dongjak-gu, Seoul, Republic of Korea
| | - Seong-Jin Cho
- Department of Psychiatry, Gil Medical Center, Gachon University College of Medicine, Incheon, Republic of Korea
| | - Jae-Hong Lee
- Department of Neurology, Asan Medical Center, University of Ulsan College of Medicine, Seoul, Republic of Korea
| | - Jun-Young Lee
- Department of Psychiatry, SMG-SNU Boramae Medical Center, Boramae-Ro 5-Gil, Shindaebang-dong, Dongjak-gu, Seoul, Republic of Korea. .,Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, Seoul National University College of Medicine, Boramae-Ro 5-Gil, Shindaebang-dong, Dongjak-gu, Seoul, Republic of Korea.
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Naefgen C, Janczyk M. Smaller backward crosstalk effects for free choice tasks are not the result of immediate conflict adaptation. Cogn Process 2018; 20:73-85. [PMID: 30306368 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-018-0887-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/28/2018] [Accepted: 09/20/2018] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
In dual-task situations, mutual interference phenomena are often observed. One particularly interesting example of such phenomena is that even Task 1 performance is improved if Task 2 requires a compatible (e.g., both responses are given on the left side) instead of an incompatible response (e.g., one response is given on the right side, and the other on the left side). This is called the compatibility-based backward crosstalk effect (BCE). In a previous paper, we observed support for a critical role of stimulus-response (S-R) links in causing this effect: The BCE was smaller when one of the two tasks was a free choice task. However, an alternative explanation for this observation is that free choice tasks lead to immediate conflict adaptation, thereby reducing the interference from the other task. In the present two experiments, we tested this explanation by varying the amount of conflict assumed to be induced by a free choice task either sequentially (Exp. 1) or block-wise (Exp. 2). While we replicated a sequential modulation of the BCE with two forced choice tasks, we observed (1) no reduction of the BCE induced by (compatible) free choice trials nor (2) an effect of block-wise manipulations of the frequency of free choice trials on the size of the BCE. Thus, while the BCE is sensitive to sequential modulations induced by the (in)compatibility of two forced choice responses, which might point to conflict adaptation, the reduced BCE in dual-task situations involving a free choice task is likely due to its weaker S-R links.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christoph Naefgen
- Department of Psychology, Eberhard Karls University of Tübingen, Schleichstraße 4, 72076, Tübingen, Germany.
| | - Markus Janczyk
- Department of Psychology, Eberhard Karls University of Tübingen, Schleichstraße 4, 72076, Tübingen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Ishii K, Liang N, Asahara R, Takahashi M, Matsukawa K. Feedforward- and motor effort-dependent increase in prefrontal oxygenation during voluntary one-armed cranking. J Physiol 2018; 596:5099-5118. [PMID: 30175404 DOI: 10.1113/jp276956] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/08/2018] [Accepted: 08/31/2018] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
KEY POINTS Some cortical areas are believed to transmit a descending signal in association with motor intention and/or effort that regulates the cardiovascular system during exercise (termed central command). However, there was no evidence for the specific cortical area responding prior to arbitrary motor execution and in proportion to the motor effort. Using a multichannel near-infrared spectroscopy system, we found that the oxygenation of the dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal cortices on the right side increases in a feedforward- and motor effort-dependent manner during voluntary one-armed cranking with the right arm. This finding may suggest a role of the dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal cortices in triggering off central command and may help us to understand impaired regulation of the cardiovascular system in association with lesion of the prefrontal cortex. ABSTRACT Output from higher brain centres (termed central command) regulates the cardiovascular system during exercise in a feedforward- and motor effort-dependent manner. This study aimed to determine a cortical area responding prior to arbitrarily started exercise and in proportion to the effort during exercise. The oxygenation responses in the frontal and frontoparietal areas during one-armed cranking with the right arm were measured using multichannel near-infrared spectroscopy, as indexes of regional blood flow responses, in 20 subjects. The intensity of voluntary exercise was 30% and 60% of the maximal voluntary effort (MVE). At the start period of both voluntary cranking tasks, the oxygenation increased (P < 0.05) only in the lateral and dorsal part of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) and sensorimotor cortices. Then, the oxygenation increased gradually in all cortical areas during cranking at 60% MVE, while oxygenation increased only in the frontoparietal area and some of the frontal area during cranking at 30% MVE. The rating of perceived exertion to the cranking tasks correlated (P < 0.05) with the oxygenation responses on the right side of the lateral-DLPFC (r = 0.46) and VLPFC (r = 0.48) and the frontopolar areas (r = 0.47-0.49). Motor-driven passive one-armed cranking decreased the oxygenation in most cortical areas, except the contralateral frontoparietal areas. Accordingly, the lateral-DLPFC and VLPFC on the right side would respond in a feedforward- and motor effort-dependent manner during voluntary exercise with the right arm. Afferent inputs from mechanosensitive afferents may decrease the cortical oxygenation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kei Ishii
- Department of Integrative Physiology, Graduate School of Biomedical and Health Sciences, Hiroshima University, Hiroshima, Japan.,Automotive Human Factors Research Centre, National Institute of Advanced Industrial Science and Technology, Tsukuba, Japan
| | - Nan Liang
- Department of Integrative Physiology, Graduate School of Biomedical and Health Sciences, Hiroshima University, Hiroshima, Japan
| | - Ryota Asahara
- Department of Integrative Physiology, Graduate School of Biomedical and Health Sciences, Hiroshima University, Hiroshima, Japan
| | - Makoto Takahashi
- Department of Biomechanics, Graduate School of Biomedical and Health Sciences, Hiroshima University, Hiroshima, Japan
| | - Kanji Matsukawa
- Department of Integrative Physiology, Graduate School of Biomedical and Health Sciences, Hiroshima University, Hiroshima, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Chirino A, Hernandez-Castillo CR, Galvez V, Contreras A, Diaz R, Beltran-Parrazal L, Fernandez-Ruiz J. Motor and cognitive impairments in spinocerebellar ataxia type 7 and its correlations with cortical volumes. Eur J Neurosci 2018; 48:3199-3211. [PMID: 30255962 DOI: 10.1111/ejn.14148] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/29/2018] [Revised: 08/22/2018] [Accepted: 09/10/2018] [Indexed: 02/07/2023]
Abstract
Spinocerebellar Ataxia Type 7 (SCA7) is a neurodegenerative disorder caused by cytosine-adenine-guanine (CAG) repeat expansion. It is clinically characterized by ataxia and visual loss. To date, little is known about SCA7 cognitive impairments and its relationship with grey matter volume (GMV) changes. The aim of this study was to explore SCA7 patients' performance in specific components of auditory-verbal neuropsychological tests and to correlate their scores with genetic mutation, severity of ataxia and GMV. We assessed verbal memory and verbal fluency proficiencies in 31 genetically confirmed SCA7 patients, and compared their results with 32 healthy matched volunteers; we also correlated CAG repeats and severity of motor symptoms with performance in the auditory-verbal tests. SCA7 patients exhibited deficiencies in several components of these cognitive tasks, which were independent of motor impairments and showed no relation to CAG repeats. Based on Resonance Images performed in 27 patients we found association between ataxia severity and GMV in "sensoriomotor" cerebellum, as well as correlations of impaired verbal memory and semantic fluency scores with GMV in association cortices, including the right parahippocampal gyrus. To our knowledge, this is the first report of deficits in the organization of semantic information and in the evocation of verbal material, as well as greater susceptibility to proactive interference in SCA7 patients. These findings bring novel information about specific cognitive abilities in SCA7 patients, particularly verbal memory and fluency, and their relation with GMV variations in circumscribed brain regions, including association cortices known to have functional relationships with the cerebellum.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Amanda Chirino
- Departamento de Fisiología, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Ciudad de México, México
| | | | - Victor Galvez
- Laboratorio de Neurociencias cognitivas y desarrollo, Escuela de Psicología, Universidad Panamericana, Ciudad de México, México
| | - Anabel Contreras
- Centro de Investigaciones Cerebrales, Universidad Veracruzana, Xalapa, México
| | - Rosalinda Diaz
- Departamento de Fisiología, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Ciudad de México, México
| | | | - Juan Fernandez-Ruiz
- Departamento de Fisiología, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Ciudad de México, México.,Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Veracruzana, Xalapa, México
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
Abstract
Here we challenge and present evidence that expands the what, when, and whether anatomical model of intentional action, which states that internally driven decisions about the content and timing of our actions and about whether to act at all depend on separable neural systems, anatomically segregated along the medial wall of the frontal lobe. In our fMRI event-related paradigm, subjects acted following conditional cues or following their intentions. The content of the actions, their timing, or their very occurrence were the variables investigated, together with the modulating factor of intentionality. Besides a shared activation of the pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA) and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) for all components and the SMA proper for the when component, we found specific activations beyond the mesial prefrontal wall involving the parietal cortex for the what component or subcortical gray structures for the when component. Moreover, we found behavioral, functional, anatomical, and brain connectivity evidence that the self-driven decisions on whether to act require a higher interhemispheric cooperation: This was indexed by a specific activation of the corpus callosum whereby the less the callosal activation, the greater was the decision cost at the time of the action in the whether trials. Furthermore, tractography confirmed that the fibers passing through the callosal focus of activation connect the two sides of the frontal lobes involved in intentional trials. This is evidence of non-unitary neural foundations for the processes involved in intentional actions with the pre-SMA/ACC operating as an intentional hub. These findings may guide the exploration of specific instances of disturbed intentionality.
Collapse
|
36
|
Takashima S, Cravo AM, Sameshima K, Ramos RT. The effect of conscious intention to act on the Bereitschaftspotential. Exp Brain Res 2018; 236:2287-2297. [PMID: 29860629 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-018-5302-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/31/2017] [Accepted: 05/28/2018] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
The current study investigated the effect of conscious intention to act on the Bereitschaftspotential. Situations in which the awareness of acting is minimally expressed were generated by asking 16 participants to press a button after performing a mental imagery task based on animal pictures (automatic condition). The affective responses induced by the pictures were controlled by selecting the animals according to different valences, threatening and neutral. The Bereitschaftspotential associated with the button presses was compared to the observed when similar movements were performed under the basic instructions of the self-paced movement paradigm (willed condition). Enhanced Bereitschaftspotential amplitudes were observed in the willed condition with respect to the automatic condition. This effect was manifested as a negative slope at medial frontocentral sites during the last 500 ms before movement onset. The valence of the pictures did not affect the motor preparatory potentials. The results suggest that significant part of the NS' subcomponent of the readiness potential is associated with the attention to-and, presumably, awareness of-intention to move, possibly reflecting cortical activation from supplementary motor areas. Secondarily, our findings supports that the feeling of threat does not influence the Bereitschaftspotential associated with automatic movements. Regarding methodological issues, the behavioural model of spontaneous voluntary movements proposed in automatic condition can benefit investigations on purely motor (or non-cognitive) subcomponents of the Bereitschaftspotential.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shiro Takashima
- Department of Psychiatry, LIM23, Hospital das Clinicas HCFMUSP, Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de Sao Paulo, Rua Doutor Ovídio Pires de Campos, 785-Cerqueira César, 05403010, Sao Paulo, SP, Brazil.
| | - André M Cravo
- Centro de Matemática Computação e Cognição, Universidade Federal do ABC, R. Santa Adélia, 166-Bangu, 09210170, Santo André, SP, Brazil
| | - Koichi Sameshima
- Department of Radiology, Hospital das Clinicas HCFMUSP, Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de Sao Paulo, Av. Dr. Arnaldo 455-Cerqueira César, 05403010, Sao Paulo, SP, Brazil
| | - Renato T Ramos
- Department of Psychiatry, LIM23, Hospital das Clinicas HCFMUSP, Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de Sao Paulo, Rua Doutor Ovídio Pires de Campos, 785-Cerqueira César, 05403010, Sao Paulo, SP, Brazil
- Frederick W. Thompson Anxiety Disorders Centre, Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre, Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
Phillips HN, Cope TE, Hughes LE, Zhang J, Rowe JB. Monitoring the past and choosing the future: the prefrontal cortical influences on voluntary action. Sci Rep 2018; 8:7247. [PMID: 29739978 PMCID: PMC5940796 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-018-25127-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/15/2018] [Accepted: 04/10/2018] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Choosing between equivalent response options requires the resolution of ambiguity. One could facilitate such decisions by monitoring previous actions and implementing transient or arbitrary rules to differentiate response options. This would reduce the entropy of chosen actions. We examined voluntary action decisions during magnetoencephalography, identifying the spatiotemporal correlates of stimulus- and choice-entropy. Negative correlations between frontotemporal activity and entropy of past trials were observed after participants’ responses, reflecting sequential monitoring of recent events. In contrast, choice entropy correlated negatively with prefrontal activity, before and after participants’ response, consistent with transient activation of latent response-sets ahead of a decision and updating the monitor of recent decisions after responding. Individual differences in current choices were related to the strength of the prefrontal signals that reflect monitoring of the statistical regularities in previous events. Together, these results explain individual expressions of voluntary action, through differential engagement of prefrontal areas to guide sequential decisions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- H N Phillips
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 0SZ, UK. .,Medical Research Council, Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, CB2 7EF, UK.
| | - T E Cope
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 0SZ, UK
| | - L E Hughes
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 0SZ, UK.,Medical Research Council, Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, CB2 7EF, UK
| | - J Zhang
- School of Psychology, University of Cardiff, Cardiff, CF2 2AT, UK
| | - J B Rowe
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 0SZ, UK.,Medical Research Council, Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, CB2 7EF, UK.,Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| |
Collapse
|
38
|
Sreedharan RM, James JS, Kesavadas C, Thomas SV. Language lateralization in pre-adolescent children: FMRI study using visual verb generation and word pair paradigms. Indian J Radiol Imaging 2018; 28:146-151. [PMID: 30050235 PMCID: PMC6038212 DOI: 10.4103/ijri.ijri_211_17] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022] Open
Abstract
Abstract
Background: FMRI is a noninvasive tool for mapping language networks, especially in children. We conducted FMRI studies in children in the age group 8- 12 years using 2 different paradigms for assessing language networks and lateralization. Aim: To map language networks in pre-adolescent children and to calculate lateralization index using two different visual paradigms. Methods and Materials: The study was conducted in normal right handed children in the age group 8-12 years. Sixteen normal subjects underwent FMRI using 2 paradigms- visual verb generation (VVG), word pairs paradigm (WPP) to stimulate language areas. FMRI data analysis was done using SPM8 (statistical parametric Mapping) software. Total activated voxels were calculated for each hemispheres in the pre-defined ROIs for both paradigms. Results: FMRI showed left language lateralization in 13 out of 16 children with both VVG and WPP and bilateral language lateralization in two subjects. With VVG there was more significant activation in the left inferior triangular gyrus (ITG) (P < 0.001), left inferior opercular gyrus (IOG) (P < 0.01), left middle frontal gyrus (MFG) (P < 0.05), left and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (P < 0.05). Left posterior superior temporal gyrus (STG or WA) (P < 0.001), Left AG (P < 0.03), Left SMG (P < 0.05) were significantly activated with WP paradigm. Conclusion: Our FMRI studies showed that VGP predominantly activated frontal language areas and WPP predominantly activated temperoparietal language areas. Several other brain regions were also involved in language processing apart from the classical language areas.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ruma M Sreedharan
- Department of Radiology, Government Medical College Hospital, Trivandrum, Kerala, India
| | - Jija S James
- Departments of Imaging Sciences and Interventional Radiology, Sree Chitra Thirunal Institute for Medical Sciences and Technology, Trivandrum, Kerala, India
| | - Chandrasekharan Kesavadas
- Departments of Imaging Sciences and Interventional Radiology, Sree Chitra Thirunal Institute for Medical Sciences and Technology, Trivandrum, Kerala, India
| | - Sanjeev V Thomas
- Departments of Imaging Sciences and Interventional Neurology, Sree Chitra Thirunal Institute for Medical Sciences and Technology, Trivandrum, Kerala, India
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
Rens N, Bode S, Burianová H, Cunnington R. Proactive Recruitment of Frontoparietal and Salience Networks for Voluntary Decisions. Front Hum Neurosci 2018; 11:610. [PMID: 29311873 PMCID: PMC5733024 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00610] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/08/2017] [Accepted: 11/29/2017] [Indexed: 12/02/2022] Open
Abstract
There is evidence that neural patterns are predictive of voluntary decisions, but findings come from paradigms that have typically required participants to make arbitrary choices decisions in highly abstract experimental tasks. It remains to be seen whether proactive neural activity reflects upcoming choices for individuals performing decisions in more complex, dynamic, scenarios. In this functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) study, we investigated proactive neural activity for voluntary decisions compared with instructed decisions in a virtual environment, which more closely mimicked a real-world decision. Using partial least squares (PLS) analysis, we found that the frontoparietal and salience networks were associated with voluntary choice selection from a time at which decisions were abstract and preceded external stimuli. Using multi-voxel pattern analysis (MVPA), we showed that participants’ choices, which were decodable from motor and visual cortices, could be predicted with lower accuracy for voluntary decisions than for instructed decisions. This corresponded to eye-tracking data showing that participants made a greater number of fixations to alternative options during voluntary choices, which might have resulted in less stable choice representations. These findings suggest that voluntary decisions engage proactive choice selection, and that upcoming choices are encoded in neural representations even while individuals continue to consider their options in the environment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Natalie Rens
- Queensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
| | - Stefan Bode
- Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, VIC, Australia.,Department of Psychology, University of Cologne, Cologne, Germany
| | - Hana Burianová
- Centre for Advanced Imaging, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia.,Department of Psychology, Swansea University, Swansea, United Kingdom
| | - Ross Cunnington
- Queensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia.,School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
40
|
Liu P, Yu Y, Gao S, Sun J, Yang X, Liu P, Qin W. Structural Integrity in the Genu of Corpus Callosum Predicts Conflict-induced Functional Connectivity Between Medial Frontal Cortex and Right Posterior Parietal Cortex. Neuroscience 2017; 366:162-171. [PMID: 29080715 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.10.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/15/2017] [Revised: 10/12/2017] [Accepted: 10/13/2017] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
Studies using the flanker task have reported that response conflict is detected by the medial frontal cortex (MFC). As a conflict alert system, the MFC shows enhanced functional communication with task-related regions. Previous studies have revealed individual differences in functional connectivity during cognitive task performance. However, the mechanisms underlying these individual differences remain unclear. In the current study, electroencephalography (EEG) was recorded while 30 subjects performed a flanker task that was modified to exclude feature integration and contingency learning. The diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) data were collected the day before the EEG session. FCz-P3/4 theta phase synchronization was used to measure functional connectivity between the MFC and posterior parietal cortex (PPC). Hierarchical regression analyses were used to assess the relationship between MFC-PPC conflict-induced theta phase synchronization and white matter integrity in significant regions derived from tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS) analysis. As expected, MFC-PPC theta phase synchronization was significantly enhanced during conflict, suggesting a conflict-induced functional connectivity. However, these findings were only found in the right hemisphere, which may be related to the asymmetrical role of the bilateral PPC in response conflict processing. Furthermore, hierarchical regression analyses revealed that 44% of individual variability in FCz-P4 conflict-induced theta phase synchronization could be explained by variations in axial diffusivity (AD) in the genu of the corpus callosum (gCC). These results demonstrated that structural integrity in the gCC predicts conflict-induced functional connectivity between the MFC and right PPC.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Peng Liu
- Engineering Research Center of Molecular and Neuro Imaging of the Ministry of Education, School of Life Science and Technology, Xidian University, Xi'an, Shaanxi 710071, China; School of Computer and Communication, Lanzhou University of Technology, Lanzhou, Gansu 710050, China
| | - Yang Yu
- Engineering Research Center of Molecular and Neuro Imaging of the Ministry of Education, School of Life Science and Technology, Xidian University, Xi'an, Shaanxi 710071, China
| | - Shudan Gao
- Engineering Research Center of Molecular and Neuro Imaging of the Ministry of Education, School of Life Science and Technology, Xidian University, Xi'an, Shaanxi 710071, China
| | - Jinbo Sun
- Engineering Research Center of Molecular and Neuro Imaging of the Ministry of Education, School of Life Science and Technology, Xidian University, Xi'an, Shaanxi 710071, China
| | - Xuejuan Yang
- Engineering Research Center of Molecular and Neuro Imaging of the Ministry of Education, School of Life Science and Technology, Xidian University, Xi'an, Shaanxi 710071, China
| | - Peng Liu
- Engineering Research Center of Molecular and Neuro Imaging of the Ministry of Education, School of Life Science and Technology, Xidian University, Xi'an, Shaanxi 710071, China
| | - Wei Qin
- Engineering Research Center of Molecular and Neuro Imaging of the Ministry of Education, School of Life Science and Technology, Xidian University, Xi'an, Shaanxi 710071, China.
| |
Collapse
|
41
|
Funahashi S. Prefrontal Contribution to Decision-Making under Free-Choice Conditions. Front Neurosci 2017; 11:431. [PMID: 28798662 PMCID: PMC5526964 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00431] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/13/2017] [Accepted: 07/12/2017] [Indexed: 12/02/2022] Open
Abstract
Executive function is thought to be the coordinated operation of multiple neural processes and allows to accomplish a current goal flexibly. The most important function of the prefrontal cortex is the executive function. Among a variety of executive functions in which the prefrontal cortex participates, decision-making is one of the most important. Although the prefrontal contribution to decision-making has been examined using a variety of behavioral tasks, recent studies using fMRI have shown that the prefrontal cortex participates in decision-making under free-choice conditions. Since decision-making under free-choice conditions represents the very first stage for any kind of decision-making process, it is important that we understand its neural mechanism. Although few studies have examined this issue while a monkey performed a free-choice task, those studies showed that, when the monkey made a decision to subsequently choose one particular option, prefrontal neurons showing selectivity to that option exhibited transient activation just before presentation of the imperative cue. Further studies have suggested that this transient increase is caused by the irregular fluctuation of spontaneous firing just before cue presentation, which enhances the response to the cue and biases the strength of the neuron's selectivity to the option. In addition, this biasing effect was observed only in neurons that exhibited sustained delay-period activity, indicating that this biasing effect not only influences the animal's decision for an upcoming choice, but also is linked to working memory mechanisms in the prefrontal cortex.
Collapse
|
42
|
Zapparoli L, Seghezzi S, Paulesu E. The What, the When, and the Whether of Intentional Action in the Brain: A Meta-Analytical Review. Front Hum Neurosci 2017; 11:238. [PMID: 28567010 PMCID: PMC5434171 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00238] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/28/2017] [Accepted: 04/25/2017] [Indexed: 02/06/2023] Open
Abstract
In their attempt to define discrete subcomponents of intentionality, Brass and Haggard (2008) proposed their What, When, and Whether model (www-model) which postulates that the content, the timing and the possibility of generating an action can be partially independent both at the cognitive level and at the level of their neural implementation. The original proposal was based on a limited number of studies, which were reviewed with a discursive approach. To assess whether the model stands in front of the more recently published data, we performed a systematic review of the literature with a meta-analytic method based on a hierarchical clustering (HC) algorithm. We identified 15 PET/fMRI studies well-suited for this quest. HC revealed the existence of a rostro-caudal gradient within the medial prefrontal cortex, with the more anterior regions (the anterior cingulum) involved in more abstract decisions of whether to execute an action and the more posterior ones (the middle cingulum or the SMA) recruited in specifying the content and the timing components of actions. However, in contrast with the original www-model, this dissociation involves also brain regions well outside the median wall of the frontal lobe, in a component specific manner: the supramarginal gyrus for the what component, the pallidum and the thalamus for the when component, the putamen and the insula for the whether component. We then calculated co-activation maps on the three component-specific www clusters of the medial wall of the frontal/limbic lobe: to this end, we used the activation likelihood approach that we applied on the imaging studies on action contained in the BrainMap.org database. This analysis confirmed the main findings of the HC analyses. However, the BrainMap.org data analyses also showed that the aforementioned segregations are generated by paradigms in which subjects act in response to conditional stimuli rather than while driven by their own intentions. We conclude that the available data confirm that the neural underpinnings of intentionality can be fractionated in discrete components that are partially independent. We also suggest that intentionality manifests itself in discrete components through the boosting of general purpose action-related regions specialized for different aspects of action selection and inhibition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Eraldo Paulesu
- fMRI Unit, IRCCS Istituto Ortopedico GaleazziMilan, Italy.,Psychology Department and NeuroMI-Milan Centre for Neuroscience, University of Milano-BicoccaMilan, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
43
|
Jacobson JM, Nielsen NP, Minthon L, Warkentin S, Wiig EH. Multiple Rapid Automatic Naming Measures of Cognition: Normal Performance and Effects of Aging. Percept Mot Skills 2016; 98:739-53. [PMID: 15209286 DOI: 10.2466/pms.98.3.739-753] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Rapid automatic naming tasks are clinical tools for probing brain functions that underlie normal cognition. To compare performance for various stimuli in normal subjects and assess the effect of aging, we administered six single-dimension stimuli (color, form, number, letter, animal, and object) and five dual-dimension stimuli (color-form, color-number, color-letter, color-animal, and color-object) to 144 normal volunteers who ranged in age from 15 to 85 years. Rapid automatic naming times for letters and numbers were significantly less than for forms, animals, and objects. Rapid automatic naming times for color-number and color-letter stimuli were significantly less than for color-form, color-animal, or color-object stimuli. Age correlated significantly with rapid automatic naming time for each single-dimension stimulus and for color-form, color-number, color-animal, and color-object stimuli. Linear regression showed that rapid automatic naming times increased with age for aggregated color stimuli, aggregated single-dimension stimuli, and aggregated dual-dimension stimuli. This age effect persisted in subgroups less than 60 years of age and greater than 60 years of age. We conclude that normal performance time is dependent on the task, with letter and number stimuli eliciting most rapid responses, and that most rapid automatic naming times increase with age.
Collapse
|
44
|
Borchert RJ, Rittman T, Passamonti L, Ye Z, Sami S, Jones SP, Nombela C, Vázquez Rodríguez P, Vatansever D, Rae CL, Hughes LE, Robbins TW, Rowe JB. Atomoxetine Enhances Connectivity of Prefrontal Networks in Parkinson's Disease. Neuropsychopharmacology 2016; 41:2171-7. [PMID: 26837463 PMCID: PMC4856878 DOI: 10.1038/npp.2016.18] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/02/2015] [Revised: 01/22/2016] [Accepted: 01/26/2016] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
Cognitive impairment is common in Parkinson's disease (PD), but often not improved by dopaminergic treatment. New treatment strategies targeting other neurotransmitter deficits are therefore of growing interest. Imaging the brain at rest ('task-free') provides the opportunity to examine the impact of a candidate drug on many of the brain networks that underpin cognition, while minimizing task-related performance confounds. We test this approach using atomoxetine, a selective noradrenaline reuptake inhibitor that modulates the prefrontal cortical activity and can facilitate some executive functions and response inhibition. Thirty-three patients with idiopathic PD underwent task-free fMRI. Patients were scanned twice in a double-blind, placebo-controlled crossover design, following either placebo or 40-mg oral atomoxetine. Seventy-six controls were scanned once without medication to provide normative data. Seed-based correlation analyses were used to measure changes in functional connectivity, with the right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) a critical region for executive function. Patients on placebo had reduced connectivity relative to controls from right IFG to dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and to left IFG and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Atomoxetine increased connectivity from the right IFG to the dorsal anterior cingulate. In addition, the atomoxetine-induced change in connectivity from right IFG to dorsolateral prefrontal cortex was proportional to the change in verbal fluency, a simple index of executive function. The results support the hypothesis that atomoxetine may restore prefrontal networks related to executive functions. We suggest that task-free imaging can support translational pharmacological studies of new drug therapies and provide evidence for engagement of the relevant neurocognitive systems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robin J Borchert
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Timothy Rittman
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Luca Passamonti
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK,National Research Council, Institute of Bioimaging and Molecular Physiology, Catanzaro, Italy
| | - Zheng Ye
- Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
| | - Saber Sami
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Simon P Jones
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Cristina Nombela
- Systems and Automatic Control Engineering, Technical University of Cartagena, Cartagena, Spain
| | | | | | - Charlotte L Rae
- Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, University of Sussex, Brighton, UK,Department of Psychiatry, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, Brighton, UK
| | - Laura E Hughes
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK,MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Trevor W Robbins
- University of Cambridge Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, Cambridge, UK
| | - James B Rowe
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK,MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK,University of Cambridge Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, Cambridge, UK,Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Herchel Smith Building, Forvie Site, Robinson Way, Cambridge Biomedical Campus, Cambridge CB3 0SZ, UK, Tel: +44 1223 760695, Fax: +44 1223 336581, E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
45
|
Abstract
Cognitive neuroscientists have acquired powerful new tools for studying the functional neuroanatomy of the human brain. Traditional methods such as lesion analysis have been supplemented with electrical and mag netic field recording techniques that can measure the informational transactions of the brain in the scale of milliseconds and neuroimaging techniques that can provide structural details of the brain to a fraction of a millimeter. The most powerful new methods have been functional imaging techniques in which brain activity engendered by a sensory, motor, or cognitive task causes an increase in local blood flow and metabolism that can be imaged with high resolution. This article reviews recent progress in functional neuroimaging, with special emphasis on understanding the neural substrates of memory. The Neuroscientist 1:155-163, 1995
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gregory McCarthy
- Neuropsychology Laboratory VA Medical Center West Haven,
Connecticut Section of Neurosurgery and Department of Neurology Yale University
School of Medicine New Haven, Connecticut
| |
Collapse
|
46
|
Jiang C, Ranganathan VK, Zhang J, Siemionow V, Yue GH. Motor effort training with low exercise intensity improves muscle strength and descending command in aging. Medicine (Baltimore) 2016; 95:e3291. [PMID: 27310942 PMCID: PMC4998428 DOI: 10.1097/md.0000000000003291] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
Abstract
This study explored the effect of high mental effort training (MET) and conventional strength training (CST) on increasing voluntary muscle strength and brain signal associated with producing maximal muscle force in healthy aging. Twenty-seven older adults (age: 75 ± 7.9 yr, 8 women) were assigned into 1 of 3 groups: MET group-trained with low-intensity (30% maximal voluntary contraction [MVC]) physical exercise combined with MET, CST group-trained with high-intensity muscle contractions, or control (CTRL) group-no training of any kind. MET and CST lasted for 12 weeks (5 sessions/week). The participants' elbow flexion strength of the right arm, electromyography (EMG), and motor activity-related cortical potential (MRCP) directly related to the strength production were measured before and after training. The CST group had the highest strength gain (17.6%, P <0.001), the MET group also had significant strength gain (13.8%, P <0.001), which was not statistically different from that of the CST group even though the exercise intensity for the MET group was only at 30% MVC level. The CTRL group did not have significant strength changes. Surprisingly, only the MET group demonstrated a significant augmentation in the MRCP (29.3%, P <0.001); the MRCP increase in CST group was at boarder-line significance level (12.11%, P = 0.061) and that for CTRL group was only 4.9% (P = 0.539). These results suggest that high mental effort training combined with low-intensity physical exercise is an effective method for voluntary muscle strengthening and this approach is especially beneficial for those who are physically weak and have difficulty undergoing conventional strength training.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Changhao Jiang
- Human Performance and Engineering Research, Kessler Foundation, West Orange, NJ
- Key Lab of Sports Ability Evaluation and Comprehensive Research Lab of General Administration of Sports, Capital Institute of Physical Education, Beijing, China
| | - Vinoth K. Ranganathan
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Cleveland Clinic, Cleveland, OH
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Cleveland Clinic, Cleveland, OH
| | - Junmei Zhang
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Cleveland Clinic, Cleveland, OH
- Graduate School, Beijing Sports University, Beijing, China
| | - Vlodek Siemionow
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Cleveland Clinic, Cleveland, OH
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Cleveland Clinic, Cleveland, OH
| | - Guang H. Yue
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Cleveland Clinic, Cleveland, OH
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Cleveland Clinic, Cleveland, OH
- Human Performance and Engineering Research, Kessler Foundation, West Orange, NJ
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Rutgers New Jersey Medical School, Rutgers University, Newark, NJ
| |
Collapse
|
47
|
Manza P, Hu S, Ide JS, Farr OM, Zhang S, Leung HC, Li CSR. The effects of methylphenidate on cerebral responses to conflict anticipation and unsigned prediction error in a stop-signal task. J Psychopharmacol 2016; 30:283-93. [PMID: 26755547 PMCID: PMC4837899 DOI: 10.1177/0269881115625102] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/24/2022]
Abstract
To adapt flexibly to a rapidly changing environment, humans must anticipate conflict and respond to surprising, unexpected events. To this end, the brain estimates upcoming conflict on the basis of prior experience and computes unsigned prediction error (UPE). Although much work implicates catecholamines in cognitive control, little is known about how pharmacological manipulation of catecholamines affects the neural processes underlying conflict anticipation and UPE computation. We addressed this issue by imaging 24 healthy young adults who received a 45 mg oral dose of methylphenidate (MPH) and 62 matched controls who did not receive MPH prior to performing the stop-signal task. We used a Bayesian Dynamic Belief Model to make trial-by-trial estimates of conflict and UPE during task performance. Replicating previous research, the control group showed anticipation-related activation in the presupplementary motor area and deactivation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and parahippocampal gyrus, as well as UPE-related activations in the dorsal anterior cingulate, insula, and inferior parietal lobule. In group comparison, MPH increased anticipation activity in the bilateral caudate head and decreased UPE activity in each of the aforementioned regions. These findings highlight distinct effects of catecholamines on the neural mechanisms underlying conflict anticipation and UPE, signals critical to learning and adaptive behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Peter Manza
- Integrative Neuroscience Program, Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA
| | - Sien Hu
- Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA
| | - Jaime S Ide
- Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA Department of Biomedical Engineering, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA
| | - Olivia M Farr
- Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center/Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Sheng Zhang
- Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA
| | - Hoi-Chung Leung
- Integrative Neuroscience Program, Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA
| | - Chiang-shan R Li
- Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA Department of Neuroscience, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA Interdepartmental Neuroscience Program, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA
| |
Collapse
|
48
|
Karch S, Loy F, Krause D, Schwarz S, Kiesewetter J, Segmiller F, Chrobok AI, Keeser D, Pogarell O. Increased Event-Related Potentials and Alpha-, Beta-, and Gamma-Activity Associated with Intentional Actions. Front Psychol 2016; 7:7. [PMID: 26834680 PMCID: PMC4722116 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/24/2015] [Accepted: 01/04/2016] [Indexed: 12/02/2022] Open
Abstract
Objective: Internally guided actions are defined as being purposeful, self-generated and offering choices between alternatives. Intentional actions are essential to reach individual goals. In previous empirical studies, internally guided actions were predominantly related to functional responses in frontal and parietal areas. The aim of the present study was to distinguish event-related potentials and oscillatory responses of intentional actions and externally guided actions. In addition, we compared neurobiological findings of the decision which action to perform with those referring to the decision whether or not to perform an action. Methods: Twenty-eight subjects participated in adapted go/nogo paradigms, including a voluntary selection condition allowing participants to (1) freely decide whether to press the response button or (2) to decide whether they wanted to press the response button with the right index finger or the left index finger. Results: The reaction times were increased when participants freely decided whether and how they wanted to respond compared to the go condition. Intentional processes were associated with a fronto-centrally located N2 and P3 potential. N2 and P3 amplitudes were increased during intentional actions compared to instructed responses (go). In addition, increased activity in the alpha-, beta- and gamma-frequency range was shown during voluntary behavior rather than during externally guided responses. Conclusion: These results may indicate that an additional cognitive process is needed for intentional actions compared to instructed behavior. However, the neural responses were comparatively independent of the kind of decision that was made (1) decision which action to perform; (2) decision whether or not to perform an action). Significance: The study demonstrates the importance of fronto-central alpha-, beta-, and gamma oscillations for voluntary behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Susanne Karch
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich, Germany
| | - Fabian Loy
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Ludwig-Maximilians-UniversityMunich, Germany; Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatic Medicine, Ludwig-Maximilians-UniversityMunich, Germany
| | - Daniela Krause
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich, Germany
| | - Sandra Schwarz
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich, Germany
| | - Jan Kiesewetter
- Institute for Medical Education, Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich, Germany
| | - Felix Segmiller
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich, Germany
| | - Agnieszka I Chrobok
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich, Germany
| | - Daniel Keeser
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich, Germany
| | - Oliver Pogarell
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
49
|
Oestreich LKL, Mifsud NG, Ford JM, Roach BJ, Mathalon DH, Whitford TJ. Cortical Suppression to Delayed Self-Initiated Auditory Stimuli in Schizotypy: Neurophysiological Evidence for a Continuum of Psychosis. Clin EEG Neurosci 2016; 47:3-10. [PMID: 25898988 DOI: 10.1177/1550059415581708] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/29/2014] [Accepted: 03/19/2015] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Schizophrenia patients have been shown to exhibit subnormal levels of electrophysiological suppression to self-initiated, button press elicited sounds. These self-suppression deficits have been shown to improve following the imposition of a subsecond delay between the button press and the evoked sound. The current study aimed to investigate whether nonclinical individuals who scored highly on the personality dimension of schizotypy would exhibit similar patterns of self-suppression abnormalities to those exhibited in schizophrenia. Thirty-nine nonclinical individuals scoring above the median (High Schizotypy) and 41 individuals scoring below the median (Low Schizotypy) on the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ) underwent electroencephalographic recording. The amplitude of the N1-component was calculated while participants (1) listened to tones initiated by a willed button press and played back with varying delay periods between the button press and the tone (Active conditions) and (2) passively listened to a series of tones (Listen condition). N1-suppression was calculated by subtracting the amplitude of the N1-component of the auditory evoked potential in the Active condition from that of the Listen condition, while controlling for the activity evoked by the button press per se. The Low Schizotypy group exhibited significantly higher levels of N1-suppression to undelayed tones compared to the High Schizotypy group. Furthermore, while N1-suppression was found to decrease linearly with increasing delays between the button press and the tone in the Low Schizotypy group, this was not the case in the High Schizotypy group. The findings of this study suggest that nonclinical, highly schizotypal individuals exhibit subnormal levels of N1-suppression to undelayed self-initiated tones and an abnormal pattern of N1-suppression to delayed self-initiated tones. To the extent that these results are similar to those previously reported in patients with schizophrenia, these findings provide support for the existence of a neurophysiological "continuum of psychosis".
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lena K L Oestreich
- School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
| | - Nathan G Mifsud
- School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
| | - Judith M Ford
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Brian J Roach
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Daniel H Mathalon
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Thomas J Whitford
- School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
50
|
Isingrini M, Angel L, Fay S, Taconnat L, Lemaire P, Bouazzaoui B. Age-Related Differences in the Reliance on Executive Control in Working Memory: Role of Task Demand. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0145361. [PMID: 26700019 PMCID: PMC4689563 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0145361] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/09/2015] [Accepted: 12/01/2015] [Indexed: 01/03/2023] Open
Abstract
We examined the hypothesis that age-related differences in the reliance on executive control may be better explained by variations of task demand than by a mechanism specifically linked to aging. To this end, we compared the relationship between the performance of young and older adults on two executive functioning tests and an updating working-memory task with different load levels. The results revealed a significant interaction between age, task demand, and individual executive capacities, indicating that executive resources were only involved at lower loads in older adults, and only at higher loads in young adults. Overall, the results are not consistent with the proposition that cognition places greater demand on executive control in older adults. However, they support the view that how much young and older adults rely on executive control to accomplish cognitive tasks depends on task demand. Finally, interestingly these results are consistent with the CRUNCH model accounting for age-related differences in brain activations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michel Isingrini
- Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition et l’Apprentissage, François Rabelais University of Tours, Tours, France
- Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Poitiers & Tours, France
- * E-mail:
| | - Lucie Angel
- Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition et l’Apprentissage, François Rabelais University of Tours, Tours, France
- Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Poitiers & Tours, France
| | - Séverine Fay
- Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition et l’Apprentissage, François Rabelais University of Tours, Tours, France
- Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Poitiers & Tours, France
| | - Laurence Taconnat
- Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition et l’Apprentissage, François Rabelais University of Tours, Tours, France
- Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Poitiers & Tours, France
| | - Patrick Lemaire
- Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Poitiers & Tours, France
- Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive, Aix-Marseille University, Marseille, France
- Institut Universitaire de France, Paris, France
| | - Badiâa Bouazzaoui
- Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition et l’Apprentissage, François Rabelais University of Tours, Tours, France
- Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Poitiers & Tours, France
| |
Collapse
|