1
|
Katz T, Kushnir T, Tomasello M. Children are eager to take credit for prosocial acts, and cost affects this tendency. J Exp Child Psychol 2024; 237:105764. [PMID: 37690347 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2023.105764] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/18/2023] [Revised: 08/07/2023] [Accepted: 08/08/2023] [Indexed: 09/12/2023]
Abstract
We report two experiments on children's tendency to enhance their reputations through communicative acts. In the experiments, 4-year-olds (N = 120) had the opportunity to inform a social partner that they had helped him in his absence. In a first experiment, we pitted a prosocial act ("Let's help clean up for Doggie!") against an instrumental act ("Let's move these out of our way"). Children in the prosocial condition were quicker to inform their partner of the act and more likely to protest when another individual was given credit for it. In a second experiment, we replicated the prosocial condition but with a new manipulation: high-cost versus low-cost helping. We manipulated both the language surrounding cost (i.e., "This will be pretty tough to clean up" vs. "It will be really easy to clean this up") and how difficult the task itself was. As predicted, children in the high-cost condition were quicker to inform their partner of the act and more likely to take back credit for it. These results suggest that even 4-year-old children make active attempts to elicit positive reputational judgments for their prosocial acts, with cost as a moderating factor.
Collapse
|
2
|
Gao Q, Hu J, Hua R, Hong H, Feng Z, Xu H, Yin J. Teenagers' but not young adults' beliefs about intrinsic interpersonal obligations for group members. Psych J 2023; 12:690-703. [PMID: 37434273 DOI: 10.1002/pchj.669] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/13/2022] [Accepted: 05/29/2023] [Indexed: 07/13/2023]
Abstract
Previous research has indicated that children perceive social category members as having intrinsic obligations toward each other, which shape their expectations for social interactions. However, it is unclear whether teenagers (aged 13 to 15) and young adults (aged 19 to 21) continue to hold such beliefs, given their increased experience with group dynamics and external social rules. To explore this question, three experiments were conducted with a total of 360 participants (N = 180 for each age group). Experiment 1 examined negative social interactions using different methods in two sub-experiments, while Experiment 2 focused on positive social interactions to examine whether participants viewed social category members as intrinsically obligated to avoid harming each other and to offer assistance. Results revealed that teenagers evaluated within-group harm and non-help as unacceptable, regardless of external rules, whereas they viewed between-group harm and non-help as both acceptable and unacceptable, depending on the presence of external rules. Conversely, young adults considered both within-group and between-group harm/non-help as more acceptable if an external rule permitted such behavior. These findings suggest that teenagers believe that members of a social category are intrinsically obligated to help and not harm each other, whereas young adults believe that individual social interactions are constrained mainly by external rules. That is, teenagers hold stronger beliefs than young adults about intrinsic interpersonal obligations to group members. Thus, in-group moral obligations and external rules contribute differently to the evaluation and interpretation of social interactions at different developmental stages.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Qiyang Gao
- Center for Brain, Mind and Education, Shaoxing University, Shaoxing, China
| | - Jingjing Hu
- School of Education, Zhejiang International Studies University, Hangzhou, China
| | - Rui Hua
- Lanzhou Resources and Environment Voc-Tech College, Lanzhou, China
| | | | - Zhangwei Feng
- School for Business, Ningbo University, Ningbo, China
| | - Haokui Xu
- Department of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China
| | - Jun Yin
- Department of Psychology, Ningbo University, Ningbo, China
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Sierksma J. Children perpetuate competence-based inequality when they help peers. NPJ SCIENCE OF LEARNING 2023; 8:41. [PMID: 37730707 PMCID: PMC10511518 DOI: 10.1038/s41539-023-00192-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/14/2023] [Accepted: 09/07/2023] [Indexed: 09/22/2023]
Abstract
Exchanges of help between children are common and often have positive consequences. But not all help is equally beneficial, for example because some help does not provide an opportunity to practice and develop skills. Here I examine whether young children might perpetuate competence-based inequality by providing incompetent peers with less opportunity to practice and improve their skills compared to competent peers. Study 1 (N = 253, 6-9 years) shows that young children understand not all help is equally beneficial: Children think that peers who receive empowerment (hints) vs. non-empowerment (correct answers) help can learn more. Study 2 (N = 80) and 3 (N = 41) then assessed children's (7-9 years) actual helping behavior in a lab-based experiment. Through a cover story, participants were introduced to two unknown, same-age children whom they later overheard were either good or not good at solving puzzles (Study 2) or math (Study 3). Subsequently, participants got to help both of them with a puzzle-quiz (Study 2) or a math-quiz (Study 3) by providing either empowerment or non-empowerment when they asked for help. Across both studies, children were more likely to provide empowerment help to competent peers, and non-empowerment help to incompetent peers. This work suggests that when young children perceive differences in competence (e.g., based on stereotypes), they contribute to maintaining the status quo by providing the most vulnerable students, that would profit the most from improving their skills, less opportunity to do so.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jellie Sierksma
- Department of Developmental Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Radovanovic M, Chao TWT, Onyshko E, Huynh QDT, Liu YL, Sommerville JA. Not just if, but how much: Children and adults use cost and need to make evaluations about generosity across contexts. Cognition 2023; 238:105533. [PMID: 37390606 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2023.105533] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/04/2022] [Revised: 06/20/2023] [Accepted: 06/22/2023] [Indexed: 07/02/2023]
Abstract
Evaluations of others' generosity are critical for selecting quality social partners, yet the factors which systematically affect these evaluations and whether they vary across development are still relatively unclear. Here, we establish that two key dimensions adults and children (aged 4 to 7 years) consider are the cost associated with a giving action and the need of the recipient, through six pre-registered experiments with Canadian and U.S. American participants. Experiments 1 and 2 demonstrate that adults' and children's third-party evaluations of generosity are sensitive to variations in each factor, across several operationalizations of cost and need in both comparative and standalone contexts, suggesting cost and need can be spontaneously evoked. However, children's responses were more consistent for need scenarios than cost scenarios. In Experiments 3 and 4, we modified our scenarios to evaluate whether variations in cost and need are considered simultaneously in both generosity evaluations and affiliative preferences. Adults' and older children's (ages 6 to 7) evaluations of generosity and affiliative preferences were sensitive to both factors, but younger children did not utilize this information systematically. Importantly, in Experiments 5 and 6, adults' and older children's generosity evaluations were only sensitive to information about cost and need when the giver's actions conferred utility to a recipient, but not when actions were self-serving. Taken together, we establish robust evidence that cost and need are considered in generosity evaluations by demonstrating that Canadian and U.S. American adults and children utilize this information consistently, spontaneously, and simultaneously.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mia Radovanovic
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario M5S 3G3, Canada.
| | - Tim Wei-Ting Chao
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario M5S 3G3, Canada
| | - Emily Onyshko
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario M5S 3G3, Canada; Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario M5S 1V6, Canada
| | | | - Yang Leona Liu
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario M5S 3G3, Canada
| | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Zeng P, Nie J, Geng J, Wang H, Chu X, Qi L, Wang P, Lei L. Self‐compassion and subjective well‐being: A moderated mediation model of online prosocial behavior and gratitude. PSYCHOLOGY IN THE SCHOOLS 2022. [DOI: 10.1002/pits.22849] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/23/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Pan Zeng
- Department of Psychology Renmin University of China Beijing China
| | - Jia Nie
- Institute of Social Psychology, School of Humanities and Social Science Xi'an Jiaotong University Xi'an China
| | - Jingyu Geng
- Department of Psychology Renmin University of China Beijing China
| | - Hongxia Wang
- Department of Psychology Renmin University of China Beijing China
| | - Xiaoyuan Chu
- School of Economics and Management Beijing University of Posts and Telecommunications Beijing China
| | - Lin Qi
- National Innovation Center for Assessment of Basic Education Quality Beijing Normal University Beijing China
| | - Pengcheng Wang
- School of Education Rennin University of China Beijing China
| | - Li Lei
- School of Education Rennin University of China Beijing China
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Lee KJ, Setoh P. Early prosociality is conditional on opportunity cost and familiarity with the target. EVOL HUM BEHAV 2022. [DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2022.10.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
|
7
|
Waddington O, Hepach R, Jackson IR, Köymen B. Preschool children's evaluations of their own unjustified requests. J Exp Child Psychol 2022; 218:105377. [PMID: 35150938 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105377] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/05/2021] [Revised: 01/12/2022] [Accepted: 01/15/2022] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
To make a fair request, requesters should consider the perspective of the requestee and contrast his or her needs with their own needs. Making an unjustified request (e.g., requesting something we do not need but the requestee does need) can induce some negative feelings such as guilt. Here, we investigated whether making unjustified requests resulted in negative emotions in 3- and 5-year-old children. Participants (N = 83; 34 girls) requested resources that they did or did not need from an experimenter who either did or did not need them. Both age groups were slower and more hesitant to make an unjustified request (children did not need the sticker, but the experimenter did) and also showed lowered body posture when making an unjustified request compared with when making a justified request (children needed the sticker, but the experimenter did not). Three-year-olds showed more pronounced changes in their posture, whereas 5-year-olds' emotional expression was overall more blunted. Rather, older children relied more on verbal indirect utterances (e.g., "You've got lovely stickers"), as opposed to direct requests (e.g., "Can I have that sticker?"), when making unjustified requests. These results suggest that preschool children already apply impartial normative standards to their requests for help, account for the fairness of their requests, and consider the needs of others when requesting.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Owen Waddington
- School of Health Sciences, University of Manchester, Manchester M13 9PL, UK.
| | - Robert Hepach
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX2 6GG, UK
| | - Iain R Jackson
- School of Health Sciences, University of Manchester, Manchester M13 9PL, UK
| | - Bahar Köymen
- School of Health Sciences, University of Manchester, Manchester M13 9PL, UK
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Competence-based helping: Children's consideration of need when providing others with help. J Exp Child Psychol 2021; 210:105206. [PMID: 34134018 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105206] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/12/2021] [Revised: 05/21/2021] [Accepted: 05/21/2021] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
When and how other people's needs influence children's helping is poorly understood. Here we focused on whether children use information about other people's competence in their helping. In Study 1 (N = 128 4- to 8-year-old children), children could provide help to both an incompetent target and a competent target by pushing levers. Whereas older children helped incompetent targets more than competent targets, younger children (<5 years) helped both targets equally. Two further experiments (N = 20 and N = 28) revealed that 4-year-olds understood that the incompetent person needed more help and also understood how they could help. Thus, young children do not, like older children, give more help to those who need it the most. We discuss potential developmental changes toward competence-based helping.
Collapse
|
9
|
Hao J, Li W, Li J, Liu Y. Why are we unwilling to help sometimes? Reconsideration and integration of the attribution-affect model and the arousal: cost-reward model. CURRENT PSYCHOLOGY 2021. [DOI: 10.1007/s12144-021-01634-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
10
|
Marshall J, Wynn K, Bloom P. Do Children and Adults Take Social Relationship Into Account When Evaluating People’s Actions? Child Dev 2020; 91:e1082-e1100. [DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13390] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/14/2022]
|
11
|
Moral perfectionism and online prosocial behavior: The mediating role of moral identity and the moderating role of online interpersonal trust. PERSONALITY AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2020. [DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
|
12
|
Jungert T, Perrin S. Trait anxiety and bystander motivation to defend victims of school bullying. J Adolesc 2019; 77:1-10. [PMID: 31593855 DOI: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2019.10.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/09/2019] [Revised: 09/29/2019] [Accepted: 10/01/2019] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION School-based bullying is an omnipresent problem, but is less frequent when bystanders are inclined to defend victims. This makes it important to focus on motivation to intervene in bullying. METHODS 202 students (Mage = 16.44 years, 52% boys) from public Swedish high schools participated in a vignette experiment. Students were randomized to one of two vignettes (victim belonging to/not belonging to ingroup). Self-report measures of motivation to defend and trait anxiety were used. RESULTS Participants reported more autonomous motivation when the victim belonged to the ingroup and more extrinsic motivation when the victim did not belong to the ingroup. Trait anxiety interacted with the manipulation: bystanders high in anxiety reported low levels of autonomous motivation when the victim did not belong to the ingroup and low levels of extrinsic motivation when the victim belonged to the ingroup. CONCLUSIONS Findings suggest that anti-bullying-programs should focus on how defender motivation is influenced by the way in which victim ingroup status is perceived and address the bystander's level of anxiety as this interacts with such perceptions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Sean Perrin
- Lund University, Box 213, SE-221 00, Lund, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Thijs J, Zee M. Further evidence for social projection in the classroom: Predicting perceived ethnic norms. JOURNAL OF APPLIED DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2019. [DOI: 10.1016/j.appdev.2019.03.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
|
14
|
Sabato H, Kogut T. Feel good, do good? Subjective well-being and sharing behavior among children. J Exp Child Psychol 2018; 177:335-350. [PMID: 30249367 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.08.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/02/2017] [Revised: 08/16/2018] [Accepted: 08/17/2018] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
Abstract
The current study examined the association between children's subjective well-being (SWB) and their sharing behavior. School children (second and fifth graders) were interviewed in private and had an opportunity to share candy with a recipient under one of two between-participants conditions: Perceived-High Obligation (a recipient in poverty) and Perceived-Low Obligation (a temporarily needy recipient). Results provide initial evidence of an increased association between SWB and sharing decisions with age; whereas SWB was not significantly correlated with the incidence of sharing by younger children (second graders), it was a positive predictor of sharing behavior among fifth graders. Manipulating the perceived obligation to share (by emphasizing the causes beyond the recipient's need), we found that higher levels of SWB were linked to sharing only in the Perceived-Low Obligation condition. Children with lower SWB behaved as expected by the norm and shared to a similar degree as children with higher SWB when sharing felt obligatory. However, when sharing was less obligatory, higher levels of SWB were linked to higher levels of sharing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hagit Sabato
- Department of Education, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva 84105, Israel
| | - Tehila Kogut
- Department of Education, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva 84105, Israel; Decision Making and Economic Psychology Center, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva 84105, Israel.
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Sierksma J. Costs of helping only influence children’s intention to help ethnic out-group peers. J Exp Child Psychol 2018; 173:85-99. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.03.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/30/2017] [Revised: 03/25/2018] [Accepted: 03/27/2018] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
16
|
Tamnes CK, Overbye K, Ferschmann L, Fjell AM, Walhovd KB, Blakemore SJ, Dumontheil I. Social perspective taking is associated with self-reported prosocial behavior and regional cortical thickness across adolescence. Dev Psychol 2018; 54:1745-1757. [PMID: 30058815 PMCID: PMC6110335 DOI: 10.1037/dev0000541] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/21/2022]
Abstract
Basic perspective taking and mentalizing abilities develop in childhood, but recent studies indicate that the use of social perspective taking to guide decisions and actions has a prolonged development that continues throughout adolescence. Here, we aimed to replicate this research and investigate the hypotheses that individual differences in social perspective taking in adolescence are associated with real-life prosocial and antisocial behavior and differences in brain structure. We used an experimental approach and a large cross-sectional sample (n = 293) of participants aged 7–26 years old to assess age-related improvement in social perspective taking usage during performance of a version of the director task. In subsamples, we then tested how individual differences in social perspective taking were related to self-reported prosocial behavior and peer relationship problems on the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (n = 184) and to MRI measures of regional cortical thickness and surface area (n = 226). The pattern of results in the director task replicated previous findings by demonstrating continued improvement in use of social perspective taking across adolescence. The study also showed that better social perspective taking usage is associated with more self-reported prosocial behavior, as well as to thinner cerebral cortex in regions in the left hemisphere encompassing parts of the caudal middle frontal and precentral gyri and lateral parietal regions. These associations were observed independently of age and might partly reflect individual developmental variability. The relevance of cortical development was additionally supported by indirect effects of age on social perspective taking usage via cortical thickness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Knut Overbye
- Center for Lifespan Changes in Brain and Cognition
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Palmer SB, Abbott N. Bystander Responses to Bias-Based Bullying in Schools: A Developmental Intergroup Approach. CHILD DEVELOPMENT PERSPECTIVES 2017. [DOI: 10.1111/cdep.12253] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
18
|
Palmer SB, Rutland A, Cameron L. The development of bystander intentions and social-moral reasoning about intergroup verbal aggression. BRITISH JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2015; 33:419-33. [DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12092] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/05/2014] [Revised: 04/21/2015] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
19
|
Killen M, Malti T. Moral judgments and emotions in contexts of peer exclusion and victimization. ADVANCES IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT AND BEHAVIOR 2015; 48:249-76. [PMID: 25735947 DOI: 10.1016/bs.acdb.2014.11.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Morality is at the core of social development. How individuals treat one another, develop a sense of obligation toward others regarding equality and equity, and understand the emotions experienced by victims and victimizers, are essential ingredients for healthy development, and for creating a just and civil society. In this chapter, we review research on two forms of social exclusion, intergroup exclusion and interpersonal victimization, from a moral development perspective, identifying distinctions as well as areas of overlap and intersections. Intergroup exclusion (defined as exclusion based on group membership, such as gender, race, ethnicity, and nationality) is most often analyzed at the group level in contrast to interpersonal victimization (defined as the repeated infliction of physical and psychological harm on another) which is most often analyzed at the individual level. In this chapter, we assert that research needs to examine both group-level and individual-level factors for intergroup and interpersonal exclusion and that moral development provides an important framework for investigating these phenomena.
Collapse
|
20
|
Nielsen M, Gigante J, Collier-Baker E. Direct cost does not impact on young children's spontaneous helping behavior. Front Psychol 2014; 5:1509. [PMID: 25566167 PMCID: PMC4274792 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01509] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/22/2014] [Accepted: 12/08/2014] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
The propensity of humans to engage in prosocial behavior is unlike that of any other species. Individuals will help others even when it comes at a cost to themselves, and even when the others are complete strangers. However, to date, scant empirical evidence has been forthcoming on young children's altruistic tendencies. To investigate this 45 4-year-olds were presented with a task in which they had opportunity to help an adult confederate retrieve a reward from a novel box. In a control condition children were given no information about the effect of potential helping behavior. Alternatively they were informed that helping would either cost them (i.e., they would miss out on getting the reward) or benefit them (i.e., they would get the reward). It was hypothesized that children would be less likely, and slower, to help in the cost condition, compared to the other two conditions. This hypothesis was not supported: children across all conditions provided help at near ceiling levels.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mark Nielsen
- Early Cognitive Development Centre, School of Psychology, The University of QueenslandBrisbane, QLD, Australia
- School of Applied Human Sciences, University of KwaZulu-NatalDurban, South Africa
| | - Julia Gigante
- Early Cognitive Development Centre, School of Psychology, The University of QueenslandBrisbane, QLD, Australia
| | - Emma Collier-Baker
- Early Cognitive Development Centre, School of Psychology, The University of QueenslandBrisbane, QLD, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Sierksma J, Thijs J, Verkuyten M. Children’s intergroup helping: The role of empathy and peer group norms. J Exp Child Psychol 2014; 126:369-83. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.06.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/02/2013] [Revised: 06/06/2014] [Accepted: 06/06/2014] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
22
|
Williams A, Moore C. Exploring disadvantageous inequality aversion in children: how cost and discrepancy influence decision-making. Front Psychol 2014; 5:1088. [PMID: 25309496 PMCID: PMC4176036 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01088] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/20/2014] [Accepted: 09/09/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
This research examined disadvantageous inequality aversion in 4- and 6-year-old children. Using the resource allocation paradigm, we explored how inequality aversion was influenced by whether a cost was associated with the equitable choice. We also investigated whether preferences for equality differed depending on whether the inequitable choice presented a small or large discrepancy between the payoff of the participant and their partner. The results demonstrated that cost plays a large role in decision-making, as children preferred equality more when there was no cost associated with it compared to when there was a cost. Interestingly, the effect of cost also affected discrepancy, with children more likely to choose equality when the discrepancy was large as opposed to small, in cost trials but not in no cost trials. Finally, the effect of discrepancy also interacted with age, with older children being more sensitive to the discrepancy between themselves and their partner. Together, these results suggest that children's behavior is not indiscriminately guided by a generalized aversion to inequality or established fairness norms. Alternate motives for inequality aversion are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Amanda Williams
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Dalhousie University Halifax, NS, Canada
| | - Chris Moore
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Dalhousie University Halifax, NS, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Sierksma J, Thijs J, Verkuyten M. In-group bias in children's intention to help can be overpowered by inducing empathy. BRITISH JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2014; 33:45-56. [PMID: 25252035 DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12065] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/20/2014] [Revised: 08/29/2014] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
24
|
Kim S, Sodian B, Paulus M. "Does he need help or can he help himself?" Preschool children's expectations about others' instrumental helping versus self-helping. Front Psychol 2014; 5:430. [PMID: 24860540 PMCID: PMC4026750 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00430] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/28/2014] [Accepted: 04/23/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
In the present study, we investigated a total of fifty-one 3.5-, 4.5-, and 5.5-year-old children's expectations about another person's helping behaviors. We asked children to complete a story in which one person failed to complete his goal (e.g., because an object was misplaced or put out of his reach) while the other person observed the event. We asked whether the children expected the other person to help the protagonist or whether they expected the protagonist to help himself. Children of 3.5 years expected the other person to provide help in the majority of trials. In contrast, the older children were equally likely to predict that the other person would help the protagonist or the protagonist would help himself.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Markus Paulus
- Department of Psychology, Ludwig Maximilian University of MunichMunich, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Sierksma J, Thijs J, Verkuyten M. Ethnic Helping and Group Identity: A Study among Majority Group Children. SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT 2014. [DOI: 10.1111/sode.12077] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|