1
|
Fan R, Shi X, Wang K, Ma Q, Wang L. Scene-aware Foveated Rendering. IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON VISUALIZATION AND COMPUTER GRAPHICS 2024; 30:7097-7106. [PMID: 39255108 DOI: 10.1109/tvcg.2024.3456157] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 09/12/2024]
Abstract
We propose a new scene-aware foveated rendering method, which incorporates the scene awareness and characteristics of the human visual system into the mapping-based foveated rendering framework. First, we generate the conservative visual importance map that encodes the visual features of the scene, visual acuity, and gaze motion. Second, we construct the pixel size control map using a convolution kernel method. Third, we utilize the pixel size control map to guide the foveated rendering. At last, a temporal coherent refinement strategy is used to maintain the smooth foveated rendering for the adjacent frames. Compared to the state-of-the-art mapping-based foveated rendering methods using the same compression ratio, our method achieves smaller MSE, higher PSNR, and SSIM in the fovea, periphery, salient regions, and the whole image. We also conducted user studies, and the results proved that the perceptual quality of our method has a high visual similarity with the around truth rendered with the full resolution.
Collapse
|
2
|
Wang Z, Wu J, Fan R, Ke W, Wang L. VPRF: Visual Perceptual Radiance Fields for Foveated Image Synthesis. IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON VISUALIZATION AND COMPUTER GRAPHICS 2024; 30:7183-7192. [PMID: 39259633 DOI: 10.1109/tvcg.2024.3456184] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 09/13/2024]
Abstract
Neural radiance fields (NeRF) has achieved revolutionary breakthrough in the novel view synthesis task for complex 3D scenes. However, this new paradigm struggles to meet the requirements for real-time rendering and high perceptual quality in virtual reality. In this paper, we propose VPRF, a novel visual perceptual based radiance fields representation method, which for the first time integrates the visual acuity and contrast sensitivity models of human visual system (HVS) into the radiance field rendering framework. Initially, we encode both the appearance and visual sensitivity information of the scene into our radiance field representation. Then, we propose a visual perceptual sampling strategy, allocating computational resources according to the HVS sensitivity of different regions. Finally, we propose a sampling weight-constrained training scheme to ensure the effectiveness of our sampling strategy and improve the representation of the radiance field based on the scene content. Experimental results demonstrate that our method renders more efficiently, with higher PSNR and SSIM in the foveal and salient regions compared to the state-of-the-art FoV-NeRF. The results of the user study confirm that our rendering results exhibit high-fidelity visual perception.
Collapse
|
3
|
Pruitt J, Knotts JD, Odegaard B. Consistent metacognitive efficiency and variable response biases in peripheral vision. J Vis 2024; 24:4. [PMID: 39110584 PMCID: PMC11314628 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.8.4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/22/2023] [Accepted: 06/17/2024] [Indexed: 08/11/2024] Open
Abstract
Across the visual periphery, perceptual and metacognitive abilities differ depending on the locus of visual attention, the location of peripheral stimulus presentation, the task design, and many other factors. In this investigation, we aimed to illuminate the relationship between attention and eccentricity in the visual periphery by estimating perceptual sensitivity, metacognitive sensitivity, and response biases across the visual field. In a 2AFC detection task, participants were asked to determine whether a signal was present or absent at one of eight peripheral locations (±10°, 20°, 30°, and 40°), using either a valid or invalid attentional cue. As expected, results revealed that perceptual sensitivity declined with eccentricity and was modulated by attention, with higher sensitivity on validly cued trials. Furthermore, a significant main effect of eccentricity on response bias emerged, with variable (but relatively unbiased) c'a values from 10° to 30°, and conservative c'a values at 40°. Regarding metacognitive sensitivity, significant main effects of attention and eccentricity were found, with metacognitive sensitivity decreasing with eccentricity, and decreasing in the invalid cue condition. Interestingly, metacognitive efficiency, as measured by the ratio of meta-d'a/d'a, was not modulated by attention or eccentricity. Overall, these findings demonstrate (1) that in some circumstances, observers have surprisingly robust metacognitive insights into how performance changes across the visual field and (2) that the periphery may be subject to variable detection biases that are contingent on the exact location in peripheral space.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joseph Pruitt
- University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
- https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4887-6090
| | | | - Brian Odegaard
- University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
- https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5459-1884
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Ghiani A, Amelink D, Brenner E, Hooge ITC, Hessels RS. When knowing the activity is not enough to predict gaze. J Vis 2024; 24:6. [PMID: 38984899 PMCID: PMC11238878 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.7.6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/30/2023] [Accepted: 05/31/2024] [Indexed: 07/11/2024] Open
Abstract
It is reasonable to assume that where people look in the world is largely determined by what they are doing. The reasoning is that the activity determines where it is useful to look at each moment in time. Assuming that it is vital to accurately judge the positions of the steps when navigating a staircase, it is surprising that people differ a lot in the extent to which they look at the steps. Apparently, some people consider the accuracy of peripheral vision, predictability of the step size, and feeling the edges of the steps with their feet to be good enough. If so, occluding part of the view of the staircase and making it more important to place one's feet gently might make it more beneficial to look directly at the steps before stepping onto them, so that people will more consistently look at many steps. We tested this idea by asking people to walk on staircases, either with or without a tray with two cups of water on it. When carrying the tray, people walked more slowly, but they shifted their gaze across steps in much the same way as they did when walking without the tray. They did not look at more steps. There was a clear positive correlation between the fraction of steps that people looked at when walking with and without the tray. Thus, the variability in the extent to which people look at the steps persists when one makes walking on the staircase more challenging.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrea Ghiani
- Department of Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Daan Amelink
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Eli Brenner
- Department of Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Ignace T C Hooge
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Roy S Hessels
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Chen C, Lee VG. Peripheral vision contributes to implicit attentional learning: Findings from the "mouse-eye" paradigm. Atten Percept Psychophys 2024:10.3758/s13414-024-02907-5. [PMID: 38839714 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-024-02907-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 05/15/2024] [Indexed: 06/07/2024]
Abstract
The central visual field is essential for activities like reading and face recognition. However, the impact of peripheral vision loss on daily activities is profound. While the importance of central vision is well established, the contribution of peripheral vision to spatial attention is less clear. In this study, we introduced a "mouse-eye" method as an alternative to traditional gaze-contingent eye tracking. We found that even in tasks requiring central vision, peripheral vision contributes to implicit attentional learning. Participants searched for a T among Ls, with the T appearing more often in one visual quadrant. Earlier studies showed that participants' awareness of the T location probability was not essential for their ability to learn. When we limited the visible area around the mouse cursor, only participants aware of the target's location probability showed learning; those unaware did not. Adding placeholders in the periphery did not restore implicit attentional learning. A control experiment showed that when participants were allowed to see all items while searching and moving the mouse to reveal the target's color, both aware and unaware participants acquired location probability learning. Our results underscore the importance of peripheral vision in implicitly guided attention. Without peripheral vision, only explicit, but not implicit, attentional learning prevails.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chen Chen
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, S504 Elliott Hall, 75 East River Road, Minneapolis, MN, 55455, USA.
| | - Vanessa G Lee
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, S504 Elliott Hall, 75 East River Road, Minneapolis, MN, 55455, USA
- Center for Cognitive Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
García-Pedreño C, Tabernero J, Benito A, Artal P. Impact of Peripheral Refractive Errors in Mobility Performance. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2024; 65:42. [PMID: 38940761 PMCID: PMC11216271 DOI: 10.1167/iovs.65.6.42] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/01/2024] [Accepted: 06/11/2024] [Indexed: 06/29/2024] Open
Abstract
Purpose The purpose of this study was to investigate the functional effects of peripheral refractive errors on mobility performance through a stair negotiation task. Methods Twenty-one young, normal sighted subjects navigated through an obstacle with steps, wearing spectacles that altered only their peripheral refraction. Lenses were used to induce positive defocus (+2 diopters [D] and +4 D), negative defocus (-2 D and -4 D), or astigmatism (+1.75 D and -3.75 D, axis 45 degrees) in the periphery. Feet trajectories were analyzed, and several gait assessment parameters were obtained. Statistical tests were conducted to determine significant performance differences between the lenses. Peripheral refraction in each subject was measured using a scanning Hartmann-Shack wavefront sensor to assess the impact of intrinsic peripheral refraction on the experiment. Results Statistically significant differences in performance appeared when peripheral errors were superimposed. Crossing time with respect to plano lenses increased by 6.2%, 7.6%, 19.2%, and 29.6% for the -2 D, +2 D, -4 D, and +4 D lenses, respectively (P < 0.05 in the last 3 cases). Subjects exhibited slower walking speeds, increased step count, and adopted precautionary measures. High-power positive defocus lenses had the biggest impact on performance, and differences were observed in distance to steps between induced positive and negative defocus. Conclusions In this laboratory-based study without an adaptation period, peripheral refractive errors affected stair negotiation, causing cautious behavior in subjects. Performance differences among types of peripheral defocus may result from magnification effects and intrinsic peripheral refraction. These results highlight the importance of understanding the effects of induced peripheral errors by myopia control and intraocular lenses.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Clara García-Pedreño
- Departamento de Electromagnetismo y Electrónica, Universidad de Murcia, Murcia, Spain
- Laboratorio de Óptica, Universidad de Murcia, Murcia, Spain
| | - Juan Tabernero
- Departamento de Electromagnetismo y Electrónica, Universidad de Murcia, Murcia, Spain
- Laboratorio de Óptica, Universidad de Murcia, Murcia, Spain
| | - Antonio Benito
- Laboratorio de Óptica, Universidad de Murcia, Murcia, Spain
| | - Pablo Artal
- Laboratorio de Óptica, Universidad de Murcia, Murcia, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Cho H, Rietdyk S. The effect of visual sensory interference during multitask obstacle crossing in younger and older adults. PLoS One 2024; 19:e0302838. [PMID: 38753863 PMCID: PMC11098502 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0302838] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/05/2023] [Accepted: 04/12/2024] [Indexed: 05/18/2024] Open
Abstract
When older adults step over obstacles during multitasking, their performance is impaired; the impairment results from central and/or sensory interference. The purpose was to determine if sensory interference alters performance under low levels of cognitive, temporal, and gait demand, and if the change in performance is different for younger versus older adults. Participants included 17 younger adults (20.9±1.9 years) and 14 older adults (69.7±5.4 years). The concurrent task was a single, simple reaction time (RT) task: depress button in response to light cue. The gait task was stepping over an obstacle (8 m walkway) in three conditions: (1) no sensory interference (no RT task), (2) low sensory interference (light cue on obstacle, allowed concurrent foveation of cue and obstacle), or (3) high sensory interference (light cue away from obstacle, prevented concurrent foveation of cue and obstacle). When standing, the light cue location was not relevant (no sensory interference). An interaction (sensory interference by task, p<0.01) indicated that RT was longer for high sensory interference during walking, but RT was not altered for standing, confirming that sensory interference increased RT during obstacle approach. An interaction (sensory interference by age, p<0.01) was observed for foot placement before the obstacle: With high sensory interference, younger adults placed the trail foot closer to the obstacle while older adults placed it farther back from the obstacle. The change increases the likelihood of tripping with the trail foot for younger adults, but with the lead limb for older adults. Recovery from a lead limb trip is more difficult due to shorter time for corrective actions. Overall, visual sensory interference impaired both RT and gait behavior with low levels of multitask demand. Changes in foot placement increased trip risk for both ages, but for different limbs, reducing the likelihood of balance recovery in older adults.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- HyeYoung Cho
- Department of Health and Kinesiology, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, United States of America
- Center for Aging and the Life Course, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, United States of America
- Department of Kinesiology, University of Northern Iowa, Cedar Falls, IA, United States of America
| | - Shirley Rietdyk
- Department of Health and Kinesiology, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, United States of America
- Center for Aging and the Life Course, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Eddine RJ, Mulatti C, Biondi FN. On investigating drivers' attention allocation during partially-automated driving. Cogn Res Princ Implic 2024; 9:21. [PMID: 38598036 PMCID: PMC11006638 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-024-00549-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/01/2023] [Accepted: 03/31/2024] [Indexed: 04/11/2024] Open
Abstract
The use of partially-automated systems require drivers to supervise the system functioning and resume manual control whenever necessary. Yet literature on vehicle automation show that drivers may spend more time looking away from the road when the partially-automated system is operational. In this study we answer the question of whether this pattern is a manifestation of inattentional blindness or, more dangerously, it is also accompanied by a greater attentional processing of the driving scene. Participants drove a simulated vehicle in manual or partially-automated mode. Fixations were recorded by means of a head-mounted eye-tracker. A surprise two-alternative forced-choice recognition task was administered at the end of the data collection whereby participants were quizzed on the presence of roadside billboards that they encountered during the two drives. Data showed that participants were more likely to fixate and recognize billboards when the automated system was operational. Furthermore, whereas fixations toward billboards decreased toward the end of the automated drive, the performance in the recognition task did not suffer. Based on these findings, we hypothesize that the use of the partially-automated driving system may result in an increase in attention allocation toward peripheral objects in the road scene which is detrimental to the drivers' ability to supervise the automated system and resume manual control of the vehicle.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Claudio Mulatti
- Dipartimento di Psicologia e Scienze Cognitive, Universita' di Trento, Rovereto, Italia
| | - Francesco N Biondi
- Human Systems Lab, University of Windsor, Windsor, ON, Canada.
- Applied Cognition Lab, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, Utah, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Pusti D, Patel NB, Ostrin LA, Nti AN, Das S, Yoon G. Peripheral Choroidal Response to Localized Defocus Blur: Influence of Native Peripheral Aberrations. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2024; 65:14. [PMID: 38578621 PMCID: PMC11005066 DOI: 10.1167/iovs.65.4.14] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/21/2023] [Accepted: 02/07/2024] [Indexed: 04/06/2024] Open
Abstract
Purpose This study aims to examine the short-term peripheral choroidal thickness (PChT) response to signed defocus blur, both with and without native peripheral aberrations. This examination will provide insights into the role of peripheral aberration in detecting signs of defocus. Methods The peripheral retina (temporal 15°) of the right eye was exposed to a localized video stimulus in 11 young adults. An adaptive optics system induced 2D myopic or hyperopic defocus onto the stimulus, with or without correcting native peripheral ocular aberrations (adaptive optics [AO] or NoAO defocus conditions). Choroidal scans were captured using Heidelberg Spectralis OCT at baseline, exposure (10, 20, and 30 minutes), and recovery phases (4, 8, and 15 minutes). Neural network-based automated MATLAB segmentation program measured PChT changes from OCT scans, and statistical analysis evaluated the effects of different optical conditions over time. Results During the exposure phase, NoAO myopic and hyperopic defocus conditions exhibited distinct bidirectional PChT alterations, showing average thickening (10.0 ± 5.3 µm) and thinning (-9.1 ± 5.5 µm), respectively. In contrast, induced AO defocus conditions did not demonstrate a significant change from baseline. PChT recovery to baseline occurred for all conditions. The unexposed fovea did not show any significant ChT change, indicating a localized ChT response to retinal blur. Conclusions We discovered that the PChT response serves as a marker for detecting peripheral retinal myopic and hyperopic defocus blur, especially in the presence of peripheral aberrations. These findings highlight the significant role of peripheral oriented blur in cueing peripheral defocus sign detection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dibyendu Pusti
- College of Optometry, University of Houston, Houston, Texas, United States
| | - Nimesh B. Patel
- College of Optometry, University of Houston, Houston, Texas, United States
| | - Lisa A. Ostrin
- College of Optometry, University of Houston, Houston, Texas, United States
| | - Augustine N. Nti
- College of Optometry, University of Houston, Houston, Texas, United States
| | - Siddarth Das
- College of Optometry, University of Houston, Houston, Texas, United States
| | - Geunyoung Yoon
- College of Optometry, University of Houston, Houston, Texas, United States
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Zhaoping L. Peripheral vision is mainly for looking rather than seeing. Neurosci Res 2024; 201:18-26. [PMID: 38000447 DOI: 10.1016/j.neures.2023.11.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/08/2023] [Accepted: 11/14/2023] [Indexed: 11/26/2023]
Abstract
Vision includes looking and seeing. Looking, mainly via gaze shifts, selects a fraction of visual input information for passage through the brain's information bottleneck. The selected input is placed within the attentional spotlight, typically in the central visual field. Seeing decodes, i.e., recognizes and discriminates, the selected inputs. Hence, peripheral vision should be mainly devoted to looking, in particular, deciding where to shift the gaze. Looking is often guided exogenously by a saliency map created by the primary visual cortex (V1), and can be effective with no seeing and limited awareness. In seeing, peripheral vision not only suffers from poor spatial resolution, but is also subject to crowding and is more vulnerable to illusions by misleading, ambiguous, and impoverished visual inputs. Central vision, mainly for seeing, enjoys the top-down feedback that aids seeing in light of the bottleneck which is hypothesized to starts from V1 to higher areas. This feedback queries for additional information from lower visual cortical areas such as V1 for ongoing recognition. Peripheral vision is deficient in this feedback according to the Central-peripheral Dichotomy (CPD) theory. The saccades engendered by peripheral vision allows looking to combine with seeing to give human observers the impression of seeing the whole scene clearly despite inattentional blindness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Li Zhaoping
- University of Tübingen, Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, Tübingen, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Vater C. Viewing angle, skill level and task representativeness affect response times in basketball defence. Sci Rep 2024; 14:3337. [PMID: 38336961 PMCID: PMC10858043 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-53706-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/18/2023] [Accepted: 02/04/2024] [Indexed: 02/12/2024] Open
Abstract
In basketball defence, it is impossible to keep track of all players without peripheral vision. This is the first study to investigate peripheral vision usage in an experimentally controlled setup, with sport-specific basketball stimuli from a first-person perspective, large viewing eccentricities (up to 90° to the left and right), and natural action responses. A CAVE and a motion-tracking system was used to project the scenarios and capture movement responses of high- and low-skilled basketball players, respectively. Four video conditions were created: (1) a simple reaction time task without crowding (only attackers), (2) a simple reaction time task with crowding (with attackers and defenders), (3) a choice-reaction time task where the player cutting to the basket eventually passed the ball to another player and (4) a game simulation. The results indicated eccentricity effects in all tests, a crowding effect in condition 2, and expertise differences in conditions 3 and 4 only. These findings suggest that viewing eccentricity has an impact on response times, that crowding is a limiting factor for peripheral perception in sports games, and that high-skilled but not low-skilled players can compensate for eccentricity effects in real game situations, indicating their superior positioning and perceptual strategies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christian Vater
- Institute of Sport Science, University of Bern, Bremgartenstrasse 145, 3012, Bern, Switzerland.
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Moore CM, Zheng Q. Limited midlevel mediation of visual crowding: Surface completion fails to support uncrowding. J Vis 2024; 24:11. [PMID: 38294775 PMCID: PMC10839818 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.1.11] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/28/2023] [Accepted: 12/10/2023] [Indexed: 02/01/2024] Open
Abstract
Visual crowding refers to impaired object recognition that is caused by nearby stimuli. It increases with eccentricity. Image-level explanations of crowding maintain that it is caused by information loss within early encoding processes that vary in functionality with eccentricity. Alternative explanations maintain that the interference is not limited to two-dimensional image-level interactions but that it is mediated within representations that reflect three-dimensional scene structure. Uncrowding refers to when adding stimulus information to a display, which increases the noise at an image level, nonetheless decreasing the amount of crowding that occurs. Uncrowding has been interpreted as evidence of midlevel mediation of crowding because the additional information tends to provide an opportunity for perceptually organizing stimuli into distinct and therefore protected representations. It is difficult, however, to rule out image-level explanations of crowding and uncrowding when stimulus differences exist between conditions. We adapted displays of a specific form of uncrowding to minimize stimulus differences across conditions, while retaining the potential for perceptual organization, specifically perceptual surface completion. Uncrowding under these conditions would provide strong support for midlevel mediation of crowding. In five experiments, however, we found no evidence of midlevel mediation of crowding, indicating that at least for this version of uncrowding, image-level explanations cannot be ruled out.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cathleen M Moore
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, USA
| | - Qingzi Zheng
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Pinto JDG, Papesh MH. High target prevalence may reduce the spread of attention during search tasks. Atten Percept Psychophys 2024; 86:62-83. [PMID: 38036870 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-023-02821-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 11/15/2023] [Indexed: 12/02/2023]
Abstract
Target prevalence influences many cognitive processes during visual search, including target detection, search efficiency, and item processing. The present research investigated whether target prevalence may also impact the spread of attention during search. Relative to low-prevalence searches, high-prevalence searches typically yield higher fixation counts, particularly during target-absent trials. This may emerge because the attention spread around each fixation may be smaller for high than low prevalence searches. To test this, observers searched for targets within object arrays in Experiments 1 (free-viewing) and 2 (gaze-contingent viewing). In Experiment 3, observers searched for targets in a Rapid Serial Visual Presentation (RSVP) stream at the center of the display while simultaneously processing occasional peripheral objects. Experiment 1 used fixation patterns to estimate attentional spread, and revealed that attention was narrowed during high, relative to low, prevalence searches. This effect was weakened during gaze-contingent search (Experiment 2) but emerged again when eye movements were unnecessary in RSVP search (Experiment 3). These results suggest that, although task demands impact how attention is allocated across displays, attention may also narrow when searching for frequent targets.
Collapse
|
14
|
Bao X, Gu Z, Yang J, Li Y, Wang D, Tian Y. Duration perception in peripheral vision: Underestimation increases with greater stimuli eccentricity. Atten Percept Psychophys 2024; 86:237-247. [PMID: 38087157 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-023-02822-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 11/17/2023] [Indexed: 01/06/2024]
Abstract
Duration perception plays a fundamental role in our daily visual activities; however, it can be easily distorted, even in the retinal location. While this topic has been extensively investigated in central vision, similar exploration in peripheral vision is still at an early stage. To investigate the influence of eccentricity, a commonly used indicator for quantifying retinal location, on duration perception in peripheral vision, we conducted two psychophysical experiments. In Experiment 1, we observed that the retinal location influenced the Point of Subjective Equality (PSE) but not the Weber Fraction (WF) of stimuli appearing at eccentricities ranging from 30° to 70°. Except at 30°, the PSEs were significantly longer than 416.7 ms (25 frames), which was the duration of standard stimuli. This suggested that participants underestimated duration, and this underestimation increased with greater distance from the central fixation point on the retina. To eliminate the potential interference of the central task used in Experiment 1, we conducted a supplementary experiment (Experiment 2) that demonstrated that this central task did not change the underestimation (PSE) but did influence the sensitivity (WF) at an eccentricity of 50°. In summary, our findings revealed a compressive effect of eccentricity on duration perception in peripheral vision: as stimuli appeared more peripheral on the retina, there was an increasing underestimation of subjective duration. Reasons and survival advantages of this underestimation are discussed. Findings provide new insight on duration perception in peripheral vision, highlighting an expanding compressive underestimation effect with greater eccentricity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xinle Bao
- Department of Psychology, Zhejiang Sci-Tech University, Zhejiang, 310018, Hangzhou, China
| | - Zhengyin Gu
- Department of Psychology, Zhejiang Sci-Tech University, Zhejiang, 310018, Hangzhou, China
| | - Jinxing Yang
- Department of Psychology, Zhejiang Sci-Tech University, Zhejiang, 310018, Hangzhou, China
| | - You Li
- National Key Laboratory of Human Factors Engineering, China Astronaut Research and Training Center, Beijing, 100094, China
| | - Duming Wang
- Department of Psychology, Zhejiang Sci-Tech University, Zhejiang, 310018, Hangzhou, China.
| | - Yu Tian
- National Key Laboratory of Human Factors Engineering, China Astronaut Research and Training Center, Beijing, 100094, China
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Lai X, Lee YC, Hong X, Rau PLP. Watch your step: A pilot study of smartphone use effect on young females' gait performance while walking up and down stairs and escalators. APPLIED ERGONOMICS 2024; 114:104130. [PMID: 37657240 DOI: 10.1016/j.apergo.2023.104130] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/24/2023] [Revised: 08/15/2023] [Accepted: 08/25/2023] [Indexed: 09/03/2023]
Abstract
The increasingly ubiquitous use of smartphones has made distracted walking common, not only on flat ground, but also on stairs. Available information regarding changes in gait performance while walking and using a smartphone in different environments is still lacking. We aimed to investigate the differences in gait behavior and subjective walking confidence while walking up and down stairs and escalators, with and without smartphone use. A field experiment involving 32 female adults was conducted at a subway station. Gait parameters collected included step frequency, acceleration root mean square, step variability, step regularity, and step symmetry. The results showed that walking task, walking environment, and walking direction significantly affected gait performance and walking confidence. Overall, playing games or texting while walking down escalators resulted in the lowest walking confidence and the largest gait performance decrement: slower step frequency; reduced root mean square; decreased step regularity and step symmetry; and increased step variability. Step frequency, step variability, and step regularity significantly correlated with walking confidence. Smartphone use while walking on stairs and escalators significantly affects gait behavior and might increase the risk of falls. Interventions and prevention are needed to increase safety education and hazard warnings for the general population.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xiaojun Lai
- Department of Industrial Engineering, Tsinghua University, Beijing, China
| | - Yu-Chi Lee
- Department of Industrial Engineering and Management, National Taipei University of Technology, Taiwan.
| | - Xinye Hong
- School of Design, South China University of Technology, Guangzhou, China
| | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Do T, Wang JK, Steele T, Strong EB, Shahlaie K, Liu YA. Neuro-ophthalmic features of patients with spontaneous cerebrospinal fluid leaks. MEDICAL HYPOTHESIS, DISCOVERY & INNOVATION OPHTHALMOLOGY JOURNAL 2023; 12:106-114. [PMID: 38476573 PMCID: PMC10926311 DOI: 10.51329/mehdiophthal1476] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/29/2023] [Accepted: 12/29/2023] [Indexed: 03/14/2024]
Abstract
Background Increased intracranial pressure is a potential cause of spontaneous cerebrospinal fluid (sCSF) leak. Associated neuro-ophthalmic features have not been well studied, particularly relationships with idiopathic intracranial hypertension (IIH). We hypothesized that neuro-ophthalmic features routinely used in evaluations for IIH can be useful in the investigation of a causal relationship between IIH and sCSF leak. We reviewed the neuro-ophthalmic examination and office-based ophthalmic imaging data of all consecutive patients with sCSF leaks and at least one repair to investigate the clinical and neuro-ophthalmic features of increased intracranial pressure. Methods We conducted a retrospective longitudinal study at a single institution by querying the electronic medical record system for CSF leak Current Procedural Terminology (CPT) codes (G96.00 and G96.01) from June 1, 2019, to July 31, 2022. For patients with a confirmed diagnosis of sCSF leak, demographic information, eye examination results, and ophthalmic imaging details for both eyes were collected. Results A total of 189 patients with CSF leaks were identified through CPT coding; 159 had iatrogenic or traumatic CSF leaks, and 30 individuals (3 male, 27 female) had confirmed sCSF leaks. The mean age of patients with sCSF leaks was 46 years (range: 29 - 81), with a mean body mass index of 35.2 kg/m2 (range: 18.2 - 54.1). Only 11 of 30 underwent eye examinations (8 before surgical repair and 10 after). The mean pre-repair and post-repair best-corrected visual acuity were 20/30 (range: 20/20 - 20/55) and 20/25 (range: 20/20 - 20/40), respectively (P = 0.188). The mean retinal nerve fiber layer thickness was 99 µm (range: 96 - 104) pre-repair and 97 µm (range: 84 - 103) post-repair (P = 0.195). The mean ganglion cell complex thickness was 84 µm (range: 72 - 94) pre-repair and 82 µm (range: 71 - 94) post-repair (P = 0.500). Humphrey visual field average mean deviation was -5.1 (range: -12.4 - -1.8) pre-repair and -1.0 (range: -10.1 - 2.1) post-repair (P = 0.063). Conclusions Serial neuro-ophthalmic examinations are recommended for patients with sCSF leaks to screen for signs of current or prior increased intracranial pressure. Larger studies are required to clarify the longitudinal changes in neuro-ophthalmic features, to investigate the incidence of IIH in cases of sCSF leak development or recurrence after surgical repair, and to explore potential causal relationships to guide post-repair management and prevent recurrent leaks. A multicenter consortium is also suggested to develop a standard clinical protocol for comprehensive management of sCSF leaks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Timothy Do
- School of Medicine, University of California, Davis, USA
| | - Jui-Kai Wang
- Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, University of Iowa, Iowa, USA
- Center for the Prevention and Treatment of Visual Loss, Iowa City VA Health System, Iowa, USA
| | - Toby Steele
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology, University of California, Davis, USA
| | - E. Bradley Strong
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology, University of California, Davis, USA
| | - Kiarash Shahlaie
- Department of Neurological Surgery, University of California, Davis, USA
| | - Yin Allison Liu
- Department of Neurological Surgery, University of California, Davis, USA
- Department of Ophthalmology, University of California, Davis, USA
- Department of Neurology, University of California, Davis, USA
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Uno T, Matsuo T, Asano M, Loh PY. Effects of Simulated Visual Impairment Conditions on Movement and Anxiety during Gap Crossing. Healthcare (Basel) 2023; 12:42. [PMID: 38200948 PMCID: PMC10779388 DOI: 10.3390/healthcare12010042] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/10/2023] [Revised: 12/15/2023] [Accepted: 12/18/2023] [Indexed: 01/12/2024] Open
Abstract
This study investigated the effects of visual conditions associated with progressive eye disease on movement patterns and anxiety levels during gap-crossing tasks. Notably, 15 healthy young adults performed crossover platforms with a 10 cm gap at three different heights, namely equal (0 cm), raised (+15 cm), and lowered (-15 cm) levels, under four vision conditions, namely normal or corrected eyesight, 10° tunnel vision, 5° tunnel vision, and 5° tunnel vision with 0.04 occlusion. Leg movements during gap crossing were analyzed using three-dimensional motion analysis. The results highlighted a distinct motion pattern in the trajectories of participants' legs under the different visual conditions. Specifically, at the point where the gap-crossing movement began (D1), the normal or corrected eyesight conditions resulted in further separation between the steps compared with the other visual conditions. The highest point of the foot during movement (D2) did not differ between the visual conditions, except for the 0 cm step. Furthermore, anxiety levels, as quantified by the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-S) questionnaire, were exacerbated under conditions of restricted visual information. In conclusion, visual impairments associated with progressive ocular diseases may perturb complex motor movement patterns, including those involved in gap-crossing tasks, with heightened anxiety potentially amplifying these disturbances.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tadashi Uno
- Center of Liberal Arts and Science, Sanyo-Onoda City University, Yamaguchi 756-0884, Japan
| | - Taihei Matsuo
- Graduate School of Design, Kyushu University, Fukuoka 815-8540, Japan;
| | - Masanari Asano
- Faculty of Humanity-Oriented Science and Engineering, Kindai University, Fukuoka 820-8555, Japan;
| | - Ping Yeap Loh
- Department of Life Design and Science, Faculty of Design, Kyushu University, Fukuoka 815-8540, Japan;
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Richter Y, Gordon C, Vainstein G, Bublil-Mor C, Geisinger D, Meital-Kfir N, Elyoseph Z. A novel intervention for treating adults with ADHD using peripheral visual stimulation. Front Psychiatry 2023; 14:1280440. [PMID: 37928920 PMCID: PMC10623343 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1280440] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/20/2023] [Accepted: 10/03/2023] [Indexed: 11/07/2023] Open
Abstract
Objective Stimulation of the peripheral visual field has been previously reported as beneficial for cognitive performance in ADHD. This study assesses the safety and efficacy of a novel intervention involving peripheral visual stimuli in managing attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Methods One hundred and eight adults, 18-40 years old, with ADHD, were enrolled in a two-month open-label study. The intervention (i.e., Neuro-glasses) consisted of standard eyeglasses with personalized peripheral visual stimuli embedded on the lenses. Participants were assessed at baseline and at the end of the study with self-report measures of ADHD symptoms (the Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale; ASRS), and executive functions (The Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function Adult Version; BRIEF-A). A computerized test of continuous performance (The Conners' Continuous Performance Test-3; CPT-3) was tested at baseline with standard eyeglasses and at the end of study using Neuro-glasses. The Clinical Global Impression-Improvement scale (CGI-I) was assessed at the intervention endpoint. Safety was monitored by documentation of adverse events. Results The efficacy analysis included 97 participants. Significant improvements were demonstrated in self-reported measures of inattentive symptoms (ASRS inattentive index; p = 0.037) and metacognitive functions concerning self-management and performance monitoring (BRIEF-A; p = 0.029). A continuous-performance test (CPT-3) indicated significant improvement in detectability (d'; p = 0.027) and reduced commission errors (p = 0.004), suggesting that the Neuro-glasses have positive effects on response inhibition. Sixty-two percent of the participants met the response criteria assessed by a clinician (CGI-I). No major adverse events were reported. Conclusion Neuro-glasses may offer a safe and effective approach to managing adult ADHD. Results encourage future controlled efficacy studies to confirm current findings in adults and possibly children with ADHD.Clinical trial registration: https://www.clinicaltrials.gov/, Identifier NCT05777785.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Gabriel Vainstein
- Kahan-Sagol-Maccabi Research and Innovation Institute, Tel Aviv, Israel
| | | | | | | | - Zohar Elyoseph
- The Department of Educational Psychology and Educational Counseling, Max Stern Yezreel Valley College, Emek Yezreel, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
DeCouto BS, Fawver B, Thomas JL, Williams AM, Vater C. The role of peripheral vision during decision-making in dynamic viewing sequences. J Sports Sci 2023; 41:1852-1867. [PMID: 38234241 DOI: 10.1080/02640414.2023.2301143] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/21/2022] [Accepted: 12/21/2023] [Indexed: 01/19/2024]
Abstract
Decision-making in team sports necessitates monitoring multiple performers located at different distances (i.e., viewing eccentricities) from a critical information source. The processing of peripheral information is generally impaired under anxiety and when responding to stimuli located at larger eccentricities. These hypotheses have not been sufficiently tested in dynamic performance environments. We examined how pressure and eccentricities affect decision-making and visual behaviour in 4v4 basketball defensive scenarios using a head mounted display. Experienced players monitored plays from the first-person perspective (centre position) and made defensive steps towards opponents threatening the basket from different eccentricities under low- and high-pressure. To tax working memory, participants simultaneously performed a backward counting task. Players responded slower and with lower accuracy to opponents at larger eccentricities. Players mostly fixated on the ball-carrier, but over 50% of fixations were located on peripheral players, indicating that information in the periphery must be frequently updated with foveal vision (i.e., pivot strategy). When pressured, participants increased mental effort and improved counting performance; however, gaze behaviour and decision-making were relatively unaffected. Findings suggest that basketball players respond more quickly to opponents positioned at lower compared to higher eccentricities at the cost of impaired responses to opponents in the periphery.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- B S DeCouto
- Human Health, Resilience & Performance, Institute for Human and Machine Cognition, Pensacola, FL, USA
| | - B Fawver
- US Army Medical Research Directorate-West, Walter Reed Army Institute of Research, WA, Joint Base Lewis-McChord, USA
| | | | - A M Williams
- Human Health, Resilience & Performance, Institute for Human and Machine Cognition, Pensacola, FL, USA
| | - C Vater
- Department of Movement and Training, Institute of Sport Science, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
- Department of Humanities, Social and Political Sciences, Chair of Cognitive Science, ETH Zurich Chair of Cognitive Science, Zürich, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Cooper EA. The Perceptual Science of Augmented Reality. Annu Rev Vis Sci 2023; 9:455-478. [PMID: 36944311 DOI: 10.1146/annurev-vision-111022-123758] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 03/23/2023]
Abstract
Augmented reality (AR) systems aim to alter our view of the world and enable us to see things that are not actually there. The resulting discrepancy between perception and reality can create compelling entertainment and can support innovative approaches to education, guidance, and assistive tools. However, building an AR system that effectively integrates with our natural visual experience is hard. AR systems often suffer from visual limitations and artifacts, and addressing these flaws requires basic knowledge of perception. At the same time, AR system development can serve as a catalyst that drives innovative new research in perceptual science. This review describes recent perceptual research pertinent to and driven by modern AR systems, with the goal of highlighting thought-provoking areas of inquiry and open questions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emily A Cooper
- Herbert Wertheim School of Optometry & Vision Science, Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, California, USA;
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Pusti D, Degre Kendrick C, Wu Y, Ji Q, Jung HW, Yoon G. Widefield wavefront sensor for multidirectional peripheral retinal scanning. BIOMEDICAL OPTICS EXPRESS 2023; 14:4190-4204. [PMID: 37799675 PMCID: PMC10549753 DOI: 10.1364/boe.491412] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/24/2023] [Revised: 06/30/2023] [Accepted: 07/01/2023] [Indexed: 10/07/2023]
Abstract
The quantitative evaluation of peripheral ocular optics is essential in both myopia research and the investigation of visual performance in people with normal and compromised central vision. We have developed a widefield scanning wavefront sensor (WSWS) capable of multidirectional scanning while maintaining natural central fixation at the primary gaze. This Shack-Hartmann-based WSWS scans along any retinal meridian by using a unique scanning method that involves the concurrent operation of a motorized rotary stage (horizontal scan) and a goniometer (vertical scan). To showcase the capability of the WSWS, we tested scanning along four meridians including a 60° horizontal, 36° vertical, and two 36° diagonal scans, each completed within a time frame of 5 seconds.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dibyendu Pusti
- College of Optometry,
University of Houston, Houston, TX 77204, USA
- Flaum Eye Institute, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY 14623, USA
| | - Chloe Degre Kendrick
- College of Optometry,
University of Houston, Houston, TX 77204, USA
- Flaum Eye Institute, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY 14623, USA
| | - Yifei Wu
- Flaum Eye Institute, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY 14623, USA
| | - Qiuzhi Ji
- Institute of Optics, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY 14627, USA
- School of Optometry, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405, USA
| | - Hae Won Jung
- College of Optometry,
University of Houston, Houston, TX 77204, USA
- Institute of Optics, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY 14627, USA
| | - Geunyoung Yoon
- College of Optometry,
University of Houston, Houston, TX 77204, USA
- Flaum Eye Institute, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY 14623, USA
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Badau D, Stoica AM, Litoi MF, Badau A, Duta D, Hantau CG, Sabau AM, Oancea BM, Ciocan CV, Fleancu JL, Gozu B. The Impact of Peripheral Vision on Manual Reaction Time Using Fitlight Technology for Handball, Basketball and Volleyball Players. Bioengineering (Basel) 2023; 10:697. [PMID: 37370628 DOI: 10.3390/bioengineering10060697] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/06/2023] [Revised: 05/26/2023] [Accepted: 06/06/2023] [Indexed: 06/29/2023] Open
Abstract
The purpose of the research was to identify the impact of peripheral (unilateral and bilateral) vision on manual reaction time to visual stimuli in handball, basketball and volleyball players by implementing a 6-week experimental program of specific exercises and some adapted tests using Fitlight technology. The research included 412 players (212 male-51.5%; 200 female-48.5%) from three team sports: basketball-146 (35.4%), handball-140 (40%) and volleyball-126 (30.6%). The experimental program carried out over 6 weeks was identical for all handball, basketball and volleyball players participating in the study; two training sessions per week were performed, with each session lasting 30 min; 15 exercises were used for the improvement of manual reaction time to visual stimuli involving peripheral vision. Through the Analysis of Variance (ANOVA), we identified statistically significant differences between the arithmetic means of the samples of handball, basketball and volleyball players, as well as according to general samples also of gender (male and female), p = 0.000. Male and female handball samples achieved the greatest progress in manual reaction time to visual stimuli involving peripheral vision for the Reaction time test with a unilateral right visual stimulus (30 s) and the Reaction time test with a unilateral left visual stimulus (30 s), while general sample also of male and female basketball samples, for the Reaction time test with bilateral visual stimuli (30 s) and the Reaction time test with six Fitlights (1 min); male and female volleyball samples recorded the lowest progress in all tests compared to handball and basketball groups. According to our results, female samples made greater progress in reaction time than male groups for all four tests of the present study. The implemented experimental program led to an improvement in manual reaction time to visual stimuli due to the use of Fitlight technology and the involvement of peripheral vision for all research samples.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dana Badau
- Petru Maior Faculty of Sciences and Letters, George Emil Palade University of Medicine, Pharmacy, Sciences and Technology, 540142 Targu Mures, Romania
- Interdisciplinary Doctoral School, Transylvania University of Brasov, 500036 Brasov, Romania
| | - Alina Mihaela Stoica
- Department of Physical Education and Sports, University of Bucharest, 050107 Bucharest, Romania
| | - Marin Florin Litoi
- Department of Physical Education and Sports, University of Bucharest, 050107 Bucharest, Romania
| | - Adela Badau
- Petru Maior Faculty of Sciences and Letters, George Emil Palade University of Medicine, Pharmacy, Sciences and Technology, 540142 Targu Mures, Romania
| | - Daniel Duta
- Faculty of Physical Education and Sport, Ovidius University of Constanta, 900470 Constanta, Romania
| | - Cezar Gheorghe Hantau
- Faculty of Physical Education and Sport, National University of Physical Education and Sport, 060057 Bucharest, Romania
| | - Anca Maria Sabau
- Faculty of Geography, Tourism and Sports, University of Oradea, 410081 Oradea, Romania
| | - Bogdan Marian Oancea
- Faculty of Physical Education and Mountain Sports, Transylvania University of Brasov, 500036 Brasov, Romania
| | - Catalin Vasile Ciocan
- Faculty of Physical Education and Sports, Vasile Alecsandri University of Bacau, 600115 Bacau, Romania
| | - Julien Leonard Fleancu
- Faculty of Sciences, Physical Education and Informatics, University of Pitesti, 110040 Pitesti, Romania
| | - Bogdan Gozu
- Department of Physical Education and Sports, University of Bucharest, 050107 Bucharest, Romania
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Yu X, Zhou Z, Becker SI, Boettcher SEP, Geng JJ. Good-enough attentional guidance. Trends Cogn Sci 2023; 27:391-403. [PMID: 36841692 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2023.01.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 15.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/02/2022] [Revised: 01/24/2023] [Accepted: 01/25/2023] [Indexed: 02/27/2023]
Abstract
Theories of attention posit that attentional guidance operates on information held in a target template within memory. The template is often thought to contain veridical target features, akin to a photograph, and to guide attention to objects that match the exact target features. However, recent evidence suggests that attentional guidance is highly flexible and often guided by non-veridical features, a subset of features, or only associated features. We integrate these findings and propose that attentional guidance maximizes search efficiency based on a 'good-enough' principle to rapidly localize candidate target objects. Candidates are then serially interrogated to make target-match decisions using more precise information. We suggest that good-enough guidance optimizes the speed-accuracy-effort trade-offs inherent in each stage of visual search.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xinger Yu
- Center for Mind and Brain, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, USA; Department of Psychology, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, USA
| | - Zhiheng Zhou
- Center for Mind and Brain, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, USA
| | - Stefanie I Becker
- School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
| | | | - Joy J Geng
- Center for Mind and Brain, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, USA; Department of Psychology, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Grahn H, Kujala T, Taipalus T, Lee J, Lee JD. On the relationship between occlusion times and in-car glance durations in simulated driving. ACCIDENT; ANALYSIS AND PREVENTION 2023; 182:106955. [PMID: 36630858 DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2023.106955] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/31/2022] [Revised: 12/21/2022] [Accepted: 01/05/2023] [Indexed: 06/17/2023]
Abstract
Drivers have spare visual capacity in driving, and often this capacity is used for engaging in secondary in-car tasks. Previous research has suggested that the spare visual capacity could be estimated with the occlusion method. However, the relationship between drivers' occlusion times and in-car glance duration preferences has not been sufficiently investigated for granting occlusion times the role of an estimate of spare visual capacity. We conducted a driving simulator experiment (N = 30) and investigated if there is an association between drivers' occlusion times and in-car glance durations in a given driving scenario. Furthermore, we explored which factors and variables could explain the strength of the association. The findings suggest an association between occlusion time preferences and in-car glance durations in visually and cognitively low demanding unstructured tasks but that this association is lost if the in-car task is more demanding. The findings might be explained by the inability to utilize peripheral vision for lane-keeping when conducting in-car tasks and/or by in-car task structures that override drivers' preferences for the in-car glance durations. It seems that the occlusion technique could be utilized as an estimate of drivers' spare visual capacity in research - but with caution. It is strongly recommended to use occlusion times in combination with driving performance metrics. There is less spare visual capacity if this capacity is used for secondary tasks that interfere with the driver's ability to utilize peripheral vision for driving or preferences for the in-car glance durations. However, we suggest that the occlusion method can be a valid method to control for inter-individual differences in in-car glance duration preferences when investigating the visual distraction potential of, for instance, in-vehicle infotainment systems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hilkka Grahn
- University of Jyväskylä, Faculty of Information Technology, P.O. Box 35, FI-40014, University of Jyväskylä, Finland.
| | - Tuomo Kujala
- University of Jyväskylä, Faculty of Information Technology, P.O. Box 35, FI-40014, University of Jyväskylä, Finland.
| | - Toni Taipalus
- University of Jyväskylä, Faculty of Information Technology, P.O. Box 35, FI-40014, University of Jyväskylä, Finland.
| | - Joonbum Lee
- University of Wisconsin-Madison, Department of Industrial and Systems Engineering, 1513 University Avenue, Madison, WI 53706, USA.
| | - John D Lee
- University of Wisconsin-Madison, Department of Industrial and Systems Engineering, 1513 University Avenue, Madison, WI 53706, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Khanganba SP, Najar SA. In-Vehicle Interaction Systems, Eye Movements, and Safety. Percept Mot Skills 2023; 130:127-148. [PMID: 36214664 DOI: 10.1177/00315125221132086] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
In-vehicle interactive systems are widely used in modern vehicles. While these systems serve various purposes, they are also potential distracters for drivers. In this study we investigated the effect of in-vehicle spatial distractions on driving errors and driver gaze behavior by developing an experimental paradigm named, "Direction Following in Distracted Driving - Spatial: Simultaneous and Sequential (D3-SSS)." Twenty-seven drivers voluntarily participated and were required to drive an instrumented vehicle on a two-lane test-track, following directions shown on direction signboards. While driving, drivers were required to respond to in-vehicle spatial distracting stimuli presented to them through a monitor attached to the dashboard. The drivers were randomly assigned to in-vehicle spatial-simultaneous and spatial-sequential distraction conditions. Our results demonstrated that, in comparison with spatial-sequential distractions, there were shorter eye fixation durations and fewer eye fixation counts on areas of interest (AOIs) during spatial-simultaneous distractions. We also found that drivers committed more driving errors during in-vehicle spatial-simultaneous distractions than when these were not occurring. In conclusion, drivers committed more slips than lapses, and spatial-simultaneous distraction was more detrimental to driving performance than was spatial-sequential distraction.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sanjram Premjit Khanganba
- Human Factors & Applied Cognition Lab, 226957Indian Institute of Technology Indore, Indore, India.,Discipline of Psychology, 226957Indian Institute of Technology Indore, Indore, India.,Center for Electric Vehicles and Intelligent Transport Systems, 226957Indian Institute of Technology Indore, Indore, India.,Department of Biosciences and Biomedical Engineering, 226957Indian Institute of Technology Indore, Indore, India.,Centre of Futuristic Defense and Space Technologies, 226957Indian Institute of Technology Indore, Indore, India
| | - Sajad Ahmad Najar
- Department of Psychology, 231505Central University of Punjab, Bathinda, India
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Ortiz-Peregrina S, Casares-López M, Castro-Torres JJ, Anera RG, Artal P. Effect of peripheral refractive errors on driving performance. BIOMEDICAL OPTICS EXPRESS 2022; 13:5533-5550. [PMID: 36425634 PMCID: PMC9664894 DOI: 10.1364/boe.468032] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/15/2022] [Revised: 09/13/2022] [Accepted: 09/15/2022] [Indexed: 05/30/2023]
Abstract
The effect of peripheral refractive errors on driving while performing secondary tasks at 40° of eccentricity was studied in thirty-one young drivers. They drove a driving simulator under 7 different induced peripheral refractive errors (baseline (0D), spherical lenses of +/- 2D, +/- 4D and cylindrical lenses of +2D and +4D). Peripheral visual acuity and contrast sensitivity were also evaluated at 40°. Driving performance was significantly impaired by the addition of myopic defocus (4D) and astigmatism (4D). Worse driving significantly correlated with worse contrast sensitivity for the route in general, but also with worse visual acuity when participants interacted with the secondary task. Induced peripheral refractive errors may negatively impact driving when performing secondary tasks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sonia Ortiz-Peregrina
- Department of Optics, Laboratory of Vision Sciences and Applications, University of Granada, Granada 18071, Spain
| | - Miriam Casares-López
- Department of Optics, Laboratory of Vision Sciences and Applications, University of Granada, Granada 18071, Spain
| | - José J. Castro-Torres
- Department of Optics, Laboratory of Vision Sciences and Applications, University of Granada, Granada 18071, Spain
| | - Rosario G. Anera
- Department of Optics, Laboratory of Vision Sciences and Applications, University of Granada, Granada 18071, Spain
| | - Pablo Artal
- Laboratorio de Óptica, Universidad de Murcia, Campus de Espinardo, Murcia 30100, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Vater C, Mann DL. Are predictive saccades linked to the processing of peripheral information? PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2022; 87:1501-1519. [PMID: 36167931 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-022-01743-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/23/2022] [Accepted: 09/15/2022] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
High-level athletes can predict the actions of an opposing player. Interestingly, such predictions are also reflected by the athlete's gaze behavior. In cricket, for example, players first pursue the ball with their eyes before they very often initiate two predictive saccades: one to the predicted ball-bounce point and a second to the predicted ball-bat-contact point. That means, they move their eyes ahead of the ball and "wait" for the ball at the new fixation location, potentially using their peripheral vision to update information about the ball's trajectory. In this study, we investigated whether predictive saccades are linked to the processing of information in peripheral vision and if predictive saccades are superior to continuously following the ball with foveal vision using smooth-pursuit eye-movements (SPEMs). In the first two experiments, we evoked the typical eye-movements observed in cricket and showed that the information gathered during SPEMs is sufficient to predict when the moving object will hit the target location and that (additional) peripheral monitoring of the object does not help to improve performance. In a third experiment, we show that it could actually be beneficial to use SPEMs rather than predictive saccades to improve performance. Thus, predictive saccades ahead of a target are unlikely to be performed to enhance the peripheral monitoring of target.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christian Vater
- Institute of Sport Science, University of Bern, Bremgartenstrasse 145, 3012, Bern, Switzerland.
| | - David L Mann
- Faculty of Behavioural and Movement Sciences, Motor Learning and Performance, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|