51
|
Shaw T, Finomore V, Warm J, Matthews G. Effects of regular or irregular event schedules on cerebral hemovelocity during a sustained attention task. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2012; 34:57-66. [DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2011.621890] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/15/2022]
|
52
|
Long B, Anderson V, Jacobs R, Mackay M, Leventer R, Barnes C, Spencer-Smith M. Executive Function Following Child Stroke: The Impact of Lesion Size. Dev Neuropsychol 2011; 36:971-87. [PMID: 22004019 DOI: 10.1080/87565641.2011.581537] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/16/2022]
|
53
|
Nagel IE, Preuschhof C, Li SC, Nyberg L, Bäckman L, Lindenberger U, Heekeren HR. Load Modulation of BOLD Response and Connectivity Predicts Working Memory Performance in Younger and Older Adults. J Cogn Neurosci 2011; 23:2030-45. [DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21560] [Citation(s) in RCA: 124] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/07/2023]
Abstract
Abstract
Individual differences in working memory (WM) performance have rarely been related to individual differences in the functional responsivity of the WM brain network. By neglecting person-to-person variation, comparisons of network activity between younger and older adults using functional imaging techniques often confound differences in activity with age trends in WM performance. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, we investigated the relations among WM performance, neural activity in the WM network, and adult age using a parametric letter n-back task in 30 younger adults (21–31 years) and 30 older adults (60–71 years). Individual differences in the WM network's responsivity to increasing task difficulty were related to WM performance, with a more responsive BOLD signal predicting greater WM proficiency. Furthermore, individuals with higher WM performance showed greater change in connectivity between left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and left premotor cortex across load. We conclude that a more responsive WM network contributes to higher WM performance, regardless of adult age. Our results support the notion that individual differences in WM performance are important to consider when studying the WM network, particularly in age-comparative studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Irene E. Nagel
- 1Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
- 2Freie Universität Berlin, Germany
| | - Claudia Preuschhof
- 1Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
- 2Freie Universität Berlin, Germany
| | - Shu-Chen Li
- 1Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
| | | | - Lars Bäckman
- 1Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
- 4Karolinska Institute, Stockholm, Sweden
| | | | - Hauke R. Heekeren
- 1Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
- 2Freie Universität Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
54
|
Anderson V, Spencer-Smith M, Wood A. Do children really recover better? Neurobehavioural plasticity after early brain insult. Brain 2011; 134:2197-221. [PMID: 21784775 DOI: 10.1093/brain/awr103] [Citation(s) in RCA: 349] [Impact Index Per Article: 26.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/17/2023] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Vicki Anderson
- Department of Psychology, Royal Children's Hospital, Parkville, Victoria 3052, Australia.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
55
|
Daffner KR, Chong H, Sun X, Tarbi EC, Riis JL, McGinnis SM, Holcomb PJ. Mechanisms underlying age- and performance-related differences in working memory. J Cogn Neurosci 2011; 23:1298-314. [PMID: 20617886 PMCID: PMC3076134 DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21540] [Citation(s) in RCA: 104] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
This study took advantage of the subsecond temporal resolution of ERPs to investigate mechanisms underlying age- and performance-related differences in working memory. Young and old subjects participated in a verbal n-back task with three levels of difficulty. Each group was divided into high and low performers based on accuracy under the 2-back condition. Both old subjects and low-performing young subjects exhibited impairments in preliminary mismatch/match detection operations (indexed by the anterior N2 component). This may have undermined the quality of information available for the subsequent decision-making process (indexed by the P3 component), necessitating the appropriation of more resources. Additional anterior and right hemisphere activity was recruited by old subjects. Neural efficiency and the capacity to allocate more resources to decision-making differed between high and low performers in both age groups. Under low demand conditions, high performers executed the task utilizing fewer resources than low performers (indexed by the P3 amplitude). As task requirements increased, high-performing young and old subjects were able to appropriate additional resources to decision-making, whereas their low-performing counterparts allocated fewer resources. Higher task demands increased utilization of processing capacity for operations other than decision-making (e.g., sustained attention) that depend upon a shared pool of limited resources. As demands increased, all groups allocated additional resources to the process of sustaining attention (indexed by the posterior slow wave). Demands appeared to have exceeded capacity in low performers, leading to a reduction of resources available to the decision-making process, which likely contributed to a decline in performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kirk R Daffner
- Harvard Medical School and Brigham and Women's Hospital, Boston, MA 02115, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
56
|
Desco M, Navas-Sanchez FJ, Sanchez-González J, Reig S, Robles O, Franco C, Guzmán-De-Villoria JA, García-Barreno P, Arango C. Mathematically gifted adolescents use more extensive and more bilateral areas of the fronto-parietal network than controls during executive functioning and fluid reasoning tasks. Neuroimage 2011; 57:281-292. [PMID: 21463696 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.03.063] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/13/2010] [Revised: 03/01/2011] [Accepted: 03/22/2011] [Indexed: 10/18/2022] Open
Abstract
The main goal of this study was to investigate the neural substrates of fluid reasoning and visuospatial working memory in adolescents with precocious mathematical ability. The study population comprised two groups of adolescents: 13 math-gifted adolescents and 14 controls with average mathematical skills. Patterns of activation specific to reasoning tasks in math-gifted subjects were examined using functional magnetic resonance images acquired while the subjects were performing Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices (RAPM) and the Tower of London (TOL) tasks. During the tasks, both groups showed significant activations in the frontoparietal network. In the math-gifted group, clusters of activation were always bilateral and more regions were recruited, especially in the right hemisphere. In the TOL task, math-gifted adolescents showed significant hyper-activations relative to controls in the precuneus, superior occipital lobe (BA 19), and medial temporal lobe (BA 39). The maximum differences between the groups were detected during RAPM tasks at the highest level of difficulty, where math-gifted subjects showed significant activations relative to controls in the right inferior parietal lobule (BA 40), anterior cingulated gyrus (BA 32), and frontal (BA 9, and BA 6) areas. Our results support the hypothesis that greater ability for complex mathematical reasoning may be related to more bilateral patterns of activation and that increased activation in the parietal and frontal regions of math-gifted adolescents is associated with enhanced skills in visuospatial processing and logical reasoning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Manuel Desco
- Dept. of Bioengineering and Aerospace Engineering, University Carlos III of Madrid, Spain; Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain; Dept. of Experimental Surgery and Medicine, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, Madrid, Spain
| | - Francisco J Navas-Sanchez
- Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain; Dept. of Experimental Surgery and Medicine, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, Madrid, Spain.
| | - Javier Sanchez-González
- Dept. of Experimental Surgery and Medicine, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, Madrid, Spain; Philips Healthcare, Clinical Science, Madrid, Spain
| | - Santiago Reig
- Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain; Dept. of Experimental Surgery and Medicine, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, Madrid, Spain
| | - Olalla Robles
- Adolescent Psychiatry Unit, Dept. of Psychiatry, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, Madrid, Spain; Centro de Referencia Estatal de Atención al Daño Cerebral (CEADAC), Madrid, Spain
| | - Carolina Franco
- Adolescent Psychiatry Unit, Dept. of Psychiatry, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, Madrid, Spain
| | - Juan A Guzmán-De-Villoria
- Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain; Diagnostic Radiology Dept, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, Madrid, Spain
| | - Pedro García-Barreno
- Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain; Dept. of Experimental Surgery and Medicine, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, Madrid, Spain; Spanish Royal Academy of Mathematical, Physical and Natural Sciences Madrid, Spain
| | - Celso Arango
- Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain; Adolescent Psychiatry Unit, Dept. of Psychiatry, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, Madrid, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
57
|
Long B, Spencer-Smith MM, Jacobs R, Mackay M, Leventer R, Barnes C, Anderson V. Executive function following child stroke: the impact of lesion location. J Child Neurol 2011; 26:279-87. [PMID: 21115744 DOI: 10.1177/0883073810380049] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Child stroke is a major cause of death in children, although limited information exists on neurobehavioral functioning of stroke survivors. Executive function (important for goal-directed behavior) is thought to be vulnerable to early insults such as stroke because of its widespread representation in the immature brain. This study investigated the impact of lesion location on executive skills. Twenty-eight children diagnosed with stroke at least 18 months before assessment were recruited. Lesion characteristics were coded from magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans. Neurobehavioral assessment focused on cognitive and everyday executive skills. Deficits were found in the context of overall normal intellectual functioning (M = 91.60; SD = 19.40). Generally, insults involving frontal and extra-frontal regions impacted equally on cognitive performance. Everyday deficits were marginally more prominent following frontal insult. Subcortical frontal lesions were associated with impairments in everyday executive skills. Results provide further support for the diffuse representation of executive function in the immature brain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brian Long
- University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
58
|
Burzynska AZ, Nagel IE, Preuschhof C, Li SC, Lindenberger U, Bäckman L, Heekeren HR. Microstructure of frontoparietal connections predicts cortical responsivity and working memory performance. Cereb Cortex 2011; 21:2261-71. [PMID: 21350048 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhq293] [Citation(s) in RCA: 59] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
We investigated how the microstructure of relevant white matter connections is associated with cortical responsivity and working memory (WM) performance by collecting diffusion tensor imaging and verbal WM functional magnetic resonance imaging data from 29 young adults. We measured cortical responsivity within the frontoparietal WM network as the difference in blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) signal between 3-back and 1-back conditions. Fractional anisotropy served as an index of the integrity of the superior longitudinal fasciculi (SLF), which connect frontal and posterior regions. We found that SLF integrity is associated with better 3-back performance and greater task-related BOLD responsivity. In addition, BOLD responsivity in right premotor cortex reliably mediated the effects of SLF integrity on 3-back performance but did not uniquely predict 3-back performance after controlling for individual differences in SLF integrity. Our results suggest that task-related adjustments of local gray matter processing are conditioned by the properties of anatomical connections between relevant cortical regions. We suggest that the microarchitecture of white matter tracts influences the speed of signal transduction along axons. This in turn may affect signal summation at neural dendrites, action potential firing, and the resulting BOLD signal change and responsivity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A Z Burzynska
- Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Lentzeallee 94, 14195 Berlin, Germany.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
59
|
Palacios EM, Fernandez-Espejo D, Junque C, Sanchez-Carrion R, Roig T, Tormos JM, Bargallo N, Vendrell P. Diffusion tensor imaging differences relate to memory deficits in diffuse traumatic brain injury. BMC Neurol 2011; 11:24. [PMID: 21345223 PMCID: PMC3050687 DOI: 10.1186/1471-2377-11-24] [Citation(s) in RCA: 60] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/13/2010] [Accepted: 02/23/2011] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Memory is one of the most impaired functions after traumatic brain injury (TBI). We used diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) to determine the structural basis of memory deficit. We correlated fractional anisotropy (FA) of the fasciculi connecting the main cerebral regions that are involved in declarative and working memory functions. Methods Fifteen patients with severe and diffuse TBI and sixteen healthy controls matched by age and years of education were scanned. The neuropsychological assessment included: Letter-number sequencing test (LNS), 2-back task, digit span (forwards and backwards) and the Rivermead profilet. DTI was analyzed by a tract-based spatial statics (TBSS) approach. Results Whole brain DTI analysis showed a global decrease in FA values that correlated with the 2-back d-prime index, but not with the Rivermead profile. ROI analysis revealed positive correlations between working memory performance assessed by 2-back d-prime and superior longitudinal fasciculi, corpus callosum, arcuate fasciculi and fornix. Declarative memory assessed by the Rivermead profile scores correlated with the fornix and the corpus callosum. Conclusions Diffuse TBI is associated with a general decrease of white matter integrity. Nevertheless deficits in specific memory domains are related to different patterns of white matter damage.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eva M Palacios
- Department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychobiology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
60
|
Roussotte FF, Bramen JE, Nunez SC, Quandt LC, Smith L, O'Connor MJ, Bookheimer SY, Sowell ER. Abnormal brain activation during working memory in children with prenatal exposure to drugs of abuse: the effects of methamphetamine, alcohol, and polydrug exposure. Neuroimage 2010; 54:3067-75. [PMID: 21040792 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.072] [Citation(s) in RCA: 53] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/14/2010] [Revised: 10/20/2010] [Accepted: 10/24/2010] [Indexed: 10/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Structural and metabolic abnormalities in fronto-striatal structures have been reported in children with prenatal methamphetamine (MA) exposure. The current study was designed to quantify functional alterations to the fronto-striatal circuit in children with prenatal MA exposure using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Because many women who use MA during pregnancy also use alcohol, a known teratogen, we examined 50 children (age range 7-15), 19 with prenatal MA exposure, 15 of whom had concomitant prenatal alcohol exposure (the MAA group), 13 with heavy prenatal alcohol but no MA exposure (ALC group), and 18 unexposed controls (CON group). We hypothesized that MA exposed children would demonstrate abnormal brain activation during a visuospatial working memory (WM) "N-Back" task. As predicted, the MAA group showed less activation than the CON group in many brain areas, including the striatum and frontal lobe in the left hemisphere. The ALC group showed less activation than the MAA group in several regions, including the right striatum. We found an inverse correlation between performance and activity in the striatum in both the CON and MAA groups. However, this relationship was significant in the caudate of the CON group but not the MAA group, and in the putamen of the MAA group but not the CON group. These findings suggest that structural damage in the fronto-striatal circuit after prenatal MA exposure leads to decreased recruitment of this circuit during a WM challenge, and raise the possibility that a rewiring of cortico-striatal networks may occur in children with prenatal MA exposure.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Florence F Roussotte
- Department of Neurology, David Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, University of California, Los Angeles, CA 90095-7332, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
61
|
Bendlin BB, Fitzgerald ME, Ries ML, Xu G, Kastman EK, Thiel BW, Rowley HA, Lazar M, Alexander AL, Johnson SC. White matter in aging and cognition: a cross-sectional study of microstructure in adults aged eighteen to eighty-three. Dev Neuropsychol 2010; 35:257-77. [PMID: 20446132 DOI: 10.1080/87565641003696775] [Citation(s) in RCA: 124] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
Structural brain change and concomitant cognitive decline are the seemingly unavoidable escorts of aging. Despite accumulating studies detailing the effects of age on the brain and cognition, the relationship between white matter features and cognitive function in aging have only recently received attention and remain incompletely understood. White matter microstructure can be measured with diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), but whether DTI can provide unique information on brain aging that is not explained by white matter volume is not known. In the current study, the relationship between white matter microstructure, age, and neuropsychological function was assessed using DTI in a statistical framework that employed white matter volume as a voxel-wise covariate in a sample of 120 healthy adults across a broad age range (18-83). Memory function and executive function were modestly correlated with the DTI measures while processing speed showed the greatest extent of correlation. The results suggest that age-related white matter alterations underlie age-related declines in cognitive function. Mean diffusivity and fractional anisotropy in several white matter brain regions exhibited a nonlinear relationship with age, while white matter volume showed a primarily linear relationship with age. The complex relationships between cognition, white matter microstructure, and white matter volume still require further investigation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Barbara B Bendlin
- Geriatric Research Educational and Clinical Center, William S. Middleton Memorial Veterans Hospital, Madison, Wisconsin 53705, USA
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
62
|
Duarte A, Henson RN, Knight RT, Emery T, Graham KS. Orbito-frontal cortex is necessary for temporal context memory. J Cogn Neurosci 2010; 22:1819-31. [PMID: 19642880 DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21316] [Citation(s) in RCA: 54] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Lesion and neuroimaging studies suggest that orbito-frontal cortex (OFC) supports temporal aspects of episodic memory. However, it is unclear whether OFC contributes to the encoding and/or retrieval of temporal context and whether it is selective for temporal relative to nontemporal (spatial) context memory. We addressed this issue with two complimentary studies: functional magnetic resonance imaging to measure OFC activity associated with successful temporal and spatial context memory during encoding and retrieval in healthy young participants, and a neuropsychological investigation to measure changes in spatial and temporal context memory in OFC lesion patients. Imaging results revealed that OFC contributed to encoding and retrieval of associations between objects and their temporal but not their spatial contexts. Consistent with this, OFC patients exhibited impairments in temporal but not spatial source memory accuracy. These results suggest that OFC plays a critical role in the formation and subsequent retrieval of temporal context.
Collapse
|
63
|
Jacobs R, Harvey AS, Anderson V. Are executive skills primarily mediated by the prefrontal cortex in childhood? Examination of focal brain lesions in childhood. Cortex 2010; 47:808-24. [PMID: 20673886 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.06.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/21/2007] [Revised: 08/15/2008] [Accepted: 03/26/2010] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Based on the study of adults with brain insult, traditional localizationist views have argued that executive skills are primarily mediated by prefrontal cortex. It remains unclear whether a similar pattern of localization exists in childhood. METHODS To investigate this hypothesis, we compared the performance of children, aged 7-16 years, with radiological evidence of brain pathology. The sample was divided according to lesion location as follows: frontal pathology (n=38), extra-frontal pathology (n=20), generalized pathology (n=21) and healthy controls (n=40). Using a multidimensional model of executive function described by Anderson (2002) and Anderson (1998; Anderson et al., 2001c, 2001d), these groups were compared on a range of executive function domains including: attentional control, goal setting, cognitive flexibility, and information processing. Additional, non-executive measures were also administered. RESULTS Contrary to adult lesion-based studies, there was little differentiation in executive processes between frontal and extra-frontal groups. CONCLUSIONS These results provide support for contemporary models which propose a distributed, but integrated neural network for executive skills, suggesting that the integrity of the entire brain is necessary for adequate executive functions in childhood. Further, focal lesions to any brain region during development may render children vulnerable to a range of executive deficits that would not normally be expected following similar pathology in adulthood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rani Jacobs
- Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, Royal Children's Hospital, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
64
|
Hosseini SMH, Rostami M, Yomogida Y, Takahashi M, Tsukiura T, Kawashima R. Aging and decision making under uncertainty: behavioral and neural evidence for the preservation of decision making in the absence of learning in old age. Neuroimage 2010; 52:1514-20. [PMID: 20472072 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/27/2010] [Revised: 04/22/2010] [Accepted: 05/04/2010] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
Abstract
Decision making under uncertainty is an essential component of everyday life. Recent psychological studies suggest that older adults, despite age-related neurological decline, can make advantageous decisions when information about the contingencies of the outcomes is available. In this study, a two-choice prediction paradigm has been used, in conjunction with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), to investigate the effects of normal aging on neural substrates underlying uncertain decision making in the absence of learning that have not been addressed in previous neuroimaging studies. Neuroimaging results showed that both the healthy older and young adults recruited a network of brain regions comprising the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, bilateral inferior parietal lobule, medial frontal cortex, and right lateral orbitofrontal cortex during the prediction task. As was hypothesized, the performance of older adults in the prediction task was not impaired compared to young adults. Although no significant age-related increases in brain activity have been found, we observed an age-related decrease in activity in the right inferior parietal lobule. We speculate that the observed age-related decrease in parietal activity could be explained by age-related differences in decision making behavior revealed by questionnaire results and maximizing scores. Together, this study demonstrates behavioral and neural evidence for the preservation of decision making in older adults when information about the contingencies of the outcome is available.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S M Hadi Hosseini
- Department of Management Science and Technology, Graduate School of Engineering, Tohoku University, Aoba-ku, Sendai, Japan.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
65
|
Cerebral lateralization of vigilance: a function of task difficulty. Neuropsychologia 2010; 48:1683-8. [PMID: 20171235 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 85] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/06/2009] [Revised: 02/10/2010] [Accepted: 02/12/2010] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
Functional near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) measures of cerebral oxygenation levels were collected from participants performing difficult and easy versions of a 12 min vigilance task and for controls who merely watched the displays without a work imperative. For the active participants, the fNIRS measurements in both vigilance tasks showed higher levels of cerebral activity than was present in the case of the no-work controls. In the easier task, greater activation was found in the right than in the left cerebral hemisphere, matching previous results indicating right hemisphere dominance for vigilance. However, for the more difficult task, this laterality difference was not found, instead activation was bilateral. Unilateral hemispheric activation in vigilance may be a result of employing relatively easy/simple tasks, not vigilance per se.
Collapse
|
66
|
Reduced neural selectivity increases fMRI adaptation with age during face discrimination. Neuroimage 2010; 51:336-44. [PMID: 20139012 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.107] [Citation(s) in RCA: 108] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/02/2009] [Revised: 01/28/2010] [Accepted: 01/30/2010] [Indexed: 12/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Ventral-visual activity in older adults has been characterized by dedifferentiation, or reduced distinctiveness, of responses to different categories of visual stimuli such as faces and houses, that typically elicit highly specialized responses in the fusiform and parahippocampal brain regions respectively in young adults (Park et al., 2004). In the present study, we demonstrate that age-related neural dedifferentiation applies to within-category stimuli (different types of faces) as well, such that older adults process less distinctive representations for individual faces than young adults. We performed a functional magnetic resonance imaging adaptation experiment while young and older participants made same-different judgments to serially presented face-pairs that were Identical, Moderate in similarity through morphing, or Different. As expected, older adults showed adaptation in the fusiform face area (FFA), during the Identical as well as the Moderate conditions relative to the Different condition. Young adults showed adaptation during the Identical condition, but minimal adaptation to the Moderate condition. These results indicate that older adults' FFA treated the morphed faces as Identical faces, reflecting decreased fidelity of neural representation of faces with age.
Collapse
|
67
|
Preuschhof C, Heekeren HR, Li SC, Sander T, Lindenberger U, Bäckman L. KIBRA and CLSTN2 polymorphisms exert interactive effects on human episodic memory. Neuropsychologia 2009; 48:402-8. [PMID: 19804789 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.031] [Citation(s) in RCA: 59] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/24/2009] [Revised: 09/23/2009] [Accepted: 09/28/2009] [Indexed: 01/10/2023]
Abstract
Individual differences in episodic memory are highly heritable. Several studies have linked a polymorphism in the gene encoding the KIBRA protein to episodic memory performance. Results regarding CLSTN2, the gene encoding the synaptic protein calsyntenin 2, have been less consistent, possibly pointing to interactions with other genes. Given that both KIBRA and CLSTN2 are expressed in the medial temporal lobe and have been linked to synaptic plasticity, we investigated whether KIBRA and CLSTN2 interactively modulate episodic memory performance (n=383). We replicated the beneficial effect of the KIBRA T-allele on episodic memory, and discovered that this effect increases with the associative demands of the memory task. Importantly, the memory-enhancing effect of the KIBRA T-allele was boosted by the presence of the CLSTN2 C-allele, which positively affected memory performance in some previous studies. In contrast, the presence of CLSTN2 C-allele led to reduced performance in subjects homozygous for the KIBRA C-allele. Overall, these findings suggest that KIBRA and CLSTN2 interactively modulate episodic memory performance, and underscore the need for delineating the interactive effects of multiple genes on brain and behavior.
Collapse
|
68
|
Montero-Odasso M, Casas A, Hansen KT, Bilski P, Gutmanis I, Wells JL, Borrie MJ. Quantitative gait analysis under dual-task in older people with mild cognitive impairment: a reliability study. J Neuroeng Rehabil 2009; 6:35. [PMID: 19772593 PMCID: PMC2754991 DOI: 10.1186/1743-0003-6-35] [Citation(s) in RCA: 132] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/23/2009] [Accepted: 09/21/2009] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Reliability of quantitative gait assessment while dual-tasking (walking while doing a secondary task such as talking) in people with cognitive impairment is unknown. Dual-tasking gait assessment is becoming highly important for mobility research with older adults since better reflects their performance in the basic activities of daily living. Our purpose was to establish the test-retest reliability of assessing quantitative gait variables using an electronic walkway in older adults with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) under single and dual-task conditions. METHODS The gait performance of 11 elderly individuals with MCI was evaluated using an electronic walkway (GAITRite System) in two sessions, one week apart. Six gait parameters (gait velocity, step length, stride length, step time, stride time, and double support time) were assessed under two conditions: single-task (sG: usual walking) and dual-task (dG: counting backwards from 100 while walking). Test-retest reliability was determined using intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC). Gait variability was measured using coefficient of variation (CoV). RESULTS Eleven participants (average age = 76.6 years, SD = 7.3) were assessed. They were high functioning (Clinical Dementia Rating Score = 0.5) with a mean Mini-Mental Status Exam (MMSE) score of 28 (SD = 1.56), and a mean Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) score of 22.8 (SD = 1.23). Under dual-task conditions, mean gait velocity (GV) decreased significantly (sGV = 119.11 +/- 20.20 cm/s; dGV = 110.88 +/- 19.76 cm/s; p = 0.005). Additionally, under dual-task conditions, higher gait variability was found on stride time, step time, and double support time. Test-retest reliability was high (ICC>0.85) for the six parameters evaluated under both conditions. CONCLUSION In older people with MCI, variability of time-related gait parameters increased with dual-tasking suggesting cognitive control of gait performance. Assessment of quantitative gait variables using an electronic walkway is highly reliable under single and dual-task conditions. The presence of cognitive impairment did not preclude performance of dual-tasking in our sample supporting that this methodology can be reliably used in cognitive impaired older individuals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Manuel Montero-Odasso
- Department of Medicine, Division of Geriatric Medicine, Parkwood Hospital, University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
69
|
Charlton RA, Barrick TR, Lawes INC, Markus HS, Morris RG. White matter pathways associated with working memory in normal aging. Cortex 2009; 46:474-89. [PMID: 19666169 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.07.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 126] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/28/2008] [Revised: 05/21/2009] [Accepted: 07/11/2009] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Previous studies by our group have found that white matter integrity as determined by Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI) is associated with working memory decline. It has been proposed that subtle white matter integrity loss may lead to the disruption of working memory in particular because it relies on the dynamic and reiterative activity of cortico-cortical pathways. METHODS DTI and working memory measurement were acquired for 99 adults from our GENIE study of healthy middle aged and elderly individuals. Voxel-based statistics were used to identify clusters of voxels in mean diffusivity images specifically associated with variations in working memory performance. Tractography then identified the cortico-cortical white matter pathways passing through these clusters, between the temporal, parietal and frontal cortices. RESULTS Significant clusters were identified which were associated with working memory in the white matter of the temporal and frontal lobes, the cingulate gyrus, and in the thalamus. The tracts that passed through these clusters included the superior parietal lobule pathway, the medial temporo-frontal pathway, the uncinate fasciculus, the fronto-parietal fasciculus, and the cingulum. CONCLUSIONS Significant clusters were identified in the white matter that were associated with working memory performance. Tractography performed through these clusters identified white matter fiber tracts which pass between grey matter regions known to be activated by working memory tasks and also mirror working memory pathways suggested by previous functional connectivity imaging.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rebecca A Charlton
- Centre for Clinical Neuroscience, Division of Cardiac and Vascular Sciences, St George's University of London, Cranmer Terrace, UK.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
70
|
Astley SJ, Aylward EH, Olson HC, Kerns K, Brooks A, Coggins TE, Davies J, Dorn S, Gendler B, Jirikowic T, Kraegel P, Maravilla K, Richards T. Functional magnetic resonance imaging outcomes from a comprehensive magnetic resonance study of children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. J Neurodev Disord 2009; 1:61-80. [PMID: 21547622 PMCID: PMC3164017 DOI: 10.1007/s11689-009-9004-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/20/2008] [Accepted: 01/23/2009] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
Abstract
A comprehensive neuropsychological/psychiatric, MR imaging, (MRI), MR spectroscopy (MRS), and functional MRI (fMRI) assessment was administered to children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders (FASD) to determine if global and/or focal abnormalities could be identified, and distinguish diagnostic subclassifications across the spectrum. The four study groups included: 1. FAS/Partial FAS; 2. Static Encephalopathy/Alcohol Exposed (SE/AE); 3. Neurobehavioral Disorder/Alcohol Exposed (ND/AE); and 4. healthy peers with no prenatal alcohol exposure. fMRI outcomes are reported here. The neuropsychological/psychiatric, MRI, and MRS outcomes are reported separately. fMRI was used to assess activation in seven brain regions during performance of N-back working memory tasks. Children across the full spectrum of FASD exhibited significant working memory deficits and altered activation patterns in brain regions that are known to be involved in working memory. These results demonstrate the potential research and diagnostic value of this non-invasive MR tool in the field of FASD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Susan J Astley
- Department of Epidemiology, Center on Human Development and Disability, University of Washington, Box 357920, Seattle, WA, 98195-7920, USA,
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
71
|
Bearden CE, van Erp TGM, Dutton RA, Lee AD, Simon TJ, Cannon TD, Emanuel BS, McDonald-McGinn D, Zackai EH, Thompson PM. Alterations in midline cortical thickness and gyrification patterns mapped in children with 22q11.2 deletions. Cereb Cortex 2009; 19:115-26. [PMID: 18483006 PMCID: PMC2733329 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhn064] [Citation(s) in RCA: 71] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/17/2022] Open
Abstract
The 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (velocardiofacial/DiGeorge syndrome) is a neurogenetic condition associated with visuospatial deficits, as well as elevated rates of attentional disturbance, mood disorder, and psychosis. Previously, we detected pronounced cortical thinning in superior parietal and right parieto-occipital cortices in patients with this syndrome, regions critical for visuospatial processing. Here we applied cortical pattern-matching algorithms to structural magnetic resonance images obtained from 21 children with confirmed 22q11.2 deletions (ages 8-17) and 13 demographically matched comparison subjects, in order to map cortical thickness across the medial hemispheric surfaces. In addition, cortical models were remeshed in frequency space to compute their surface complexity. Cortical maps revealed a pattern of localized thinning in the ventromedial occipital-temporal cortex, critical for visuospatial representation, and the anterior cingulate, a key area for attentional control. However, children with 22q11.2DS showed significantly increased gyral complexity bilaterally in occipital cortex. Regional gray matter volumes, particularly in medial frontal cortex, were strongly correlated with both verbal and nonverbal cognitive functions. These findings suggest that aberrant parieto-occipital brain development, as evidenced by both increased complexity and cortical thinning in these regions, may be a neural substrate for the deficits in visuospatial and numerical understanding characteristic of this syndrome.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carrie E Bearden
- Department of Psychiatry & Biobehavioral Sciences, Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior, University of California-Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90024, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
72
|
Drapier D, Surguladze S, Marshall N, Schulze K, Fern A, Hall MH, Walshe M, Murray RM, McDonald C. Genetic liability for bipolar disorder is characterized by excess frontal activation in response to a working memory task. Biol Psychiatry 2008; 64:513-20. [PMID: 18571627 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.04.038] [Citation(s) in RCA: 69] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/09/2007] [Revised: 04/11/2008] [Accepted: 04/15/2008] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND There is evidence that patients with bipolar disorder have working memory deficits even during periods of euthymia. The neural basis of such deficits and its relationship with genetic risk remain unclear. We utilized functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate neural activity in samples of bipolar disorder patients and their unaffected first-degree relatives while performing working memory tasks of increasing difficulty. METHODS Twenty remitted bipolar I disorder patients, 20 of their unaffected first-degree relatives, and 20 healthy volunteers were recruited and successfully completed scanning. Subjects participated in fMRI scans consisting of an n-back working memory task with three stages of increasing difficulty (1-back, 2-back, and 3-back), alternating with a baseline attention task. Groups were analyzed separately to produce brain activation maps, and a group-by-task analysis of variance (ANOVA) with post hoc comparisons was completed. RESULTS Patients performed more poorly online than control subjects and relatives on the 2-back and 3-back tasks. The group-by-task ANOVA demonstrated a significantly altered region of neural activity involving a cluster located in the left frontal pole/ventrolateral gyrus. Post hoc analyses demonstrated that this cluster was accounted for by significantly greater activation in relatives compared with control subjects for the 2-back task. Patients demonstrated a trend to significantly greater activation than control subjects in the same cluster during 1-back performance. CONCLUSIONS Left prefrontal hyperactivation during working memory is associated with genetic liability for bipolar disorder and represents a potential neurobiological endophenotype for the illness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dominique Drapier
- Section of Neuroscience and Emotion, Psychological Medicine and Psychiatry, King's College London Institute of Psychiatry, London, United Kingdom.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
73
|
Nagel IE, Chicherio C, Li SC, von Oertzen T, Sander T, Villringer A, Heekeren HR, Bäckman L, Lindenberger U. Human aging magnifies genetic effects on executive functioning and working memory. Front Hum Neurosci 2008; 2:1. [PMID: 18958202 PMCID: PMC2525971 DOI: 10.3389/neuro.09.001.2008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 196] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/03/2008] [Accepted: 04/19/2008] [Indexed: 12/11/2022] Open
Abstract
We demonstrate that common genetic polymorphisms contribute to the increasing heterogeneity of cognitive functioning in old age. We assess two common Val/Met polymorphisms, one affecting the Catechol-O-Methyltransferase (COMT) enzyme, which degrades dopamine (DA) in prefrontal cortex (PFC), and the other influencing the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) protein. In two tasks (Wisconsin Card Sorting and spatial working memory), we find that effects of COMT genotype on cognitive performance are magnified in old age and modulated by BDNF genotype. Older COMT Val homozygotes showed particularly low levels of performance if they were also BDNF Met carriers. The age-associated magnification of COMT gene effects provides novel information on the inverted U-shaped relation linking dopaminergic neuromodulation in PFC to cognitive performance. The modulation of COMT effects by BDNF extends recent evidence of close interactions between frontal and medial-temporal circuitries in executive functioning and working memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Irene E Nagel
- Max Planck Institute for Human Development Berlin, Germany
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
74
|
Strand F, Forssberg H, Klingberg T, Norrelgen F. Phonological working memory with auditory presentation of pseudo-words — An event related fMRI Study. Brain Res 2008; 1212:48-54. [DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.02.097] [Citation(s) in RCA: 55] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/19/2007] [Revised: 02/26/2008] [Accepted: 02/27/2008] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
75
|
Origins of spatial working memory deficits in schizophrenia: an event-related FMRI and near-infrared spectroscopy study. PLoS One 2008; 3:e1760. [PMID: 18335036 PMCID: PMC2257999 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001760] [Citation(s) in RCA: 84] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/15/2007] [Accepted: 02/05/2008] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Abnormal prefrontal functioning plays a central role in the working memory (WM) deficits of schizophrenic patients, but the nature of the relationship between WM and prefrontal activation remains undetermined. Using two functional neuroimaging methods, we investigated the neural correlates of remembering and forgetting in schizophrenic and healthy participants. We focused on the brain activation during WM maintenance phase with event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). We also examined oxygenated hemoglobin changes in relation to memory performance with the near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) using the same spatial WM task. Distinct types of correct and error trials were segregated for analysis. fMRI data indicated that prefrontal activation was increased during WM maintenance on correct trials in both schizophrenic and healthy subjects. However, a significant difference was observed in the functional asymmetry of frontal activation pattern. Healthy subjects showed increased activation in the right frontal, temporal and cingulate regions. Schizophrenic patients showed greater activation compared with control subjects in left frontal, temporal and parietal regions as well as in right frontal regions. We also observed increased ‘false memory’ errors in schizophrenic patients, associated with increased prefrontal activation and resembling the activation pattern observed on the correct trials. NIRS data replicated the fMRI results. Thus, increased frontal activity was correlated with the accuracy of WM in both healthy control and schizophrenic participants. The major difference between the two groups concerned functional asymmetry; healthy subjects recruited right frontal regions during spatial WM maintenance whereas schizophrenic subjects recruited a wider network in both hemispheres to achieve the same level of memory performance. Increased “false memory” errors and accompanying bilateral prefrontal activation in schizophrenia suggest that the etiology of memory errors must be considered when comparing group performances. Finally, the concordance of fMRI and NIRS data supports NIRS as an alternative functional neuroimaging method for psychiatric research.
Collapse
|
76
|
Welcome SE, Chiarello C. How dynamic is interhemispheric interaction? Effects of task switching on the across-hemisphere advantage. Brain Cogn 2008; 67:69-75. [PMID: 18206285 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2007.11.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/20/2007] [Revised: 10/24/2007] [Accepted: 11/27/2007] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Interaction between the cerebral hemispheres may allow both hemispheres to contribute their processing resources in order to cope efficiently with complex tasks [Banich, M. (1998). The missing link: the role of interhemispheric interaction in attentional processing. Brain and Cognition, 36, 128-157]. The current study investigated whether the benefits of interhemispheric interaction arise because of top-down knowledge about the task built up over the course of a block of trials or because of the processing demands present in a single trial. Participants performed a less computationally complex physical identity task and a more complex adding task on within-visual field and across-visual field trials. Task differences in interhemispheric interaction were compared between the blocked and mixed conditions to investigate whether frequent task switches altered the pattern of interhemispheric resource recruitment. A similar interaction between task difficulty and trial type (across- or within-visual field presentation) was obtained for both the blocked and mixed conditions. The degree of task-dependency of interhemispheric interaction was not altered in the mixed condition. This finding supports the view that interhemispheric interaction becomes beneficial in response to the processing demands of an individual trial rather than as a result of top-down task knowledge.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Suzanne E Welcome
- Department of Neuroscience, University of California, Riverside, 800 University Avenue, Riverside, CA 92521, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
77
|
Picchioni M, Matthiasson P, Broome M, Giampietro V, Brammer M, Mathes B, Fletcher P, Williams S, McGuire P. Medial temporal lobe activity at recognition increases with the duration of mnemonic delay during an object working memory task. Hum Brain Mapp 2008; 28:1235-50. [PMID: 17358019 PMCID: PMC6871489 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.20357] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/11/2022] Open
Abstract
Object working memory (WM) engages a disseminated neural network, although the extent to which the length of time that data is held in WM influences regional activity within this network is unclear. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging to study a delayed matching to sample task in 14 healthy subjects, manipulating the duration of mnemonic delay. Across all lengths of delay, successful recognition was associated with the bilateral engagement of the inferior and middle frontal gyri and insula, the medial and inferior temporal, dorsal anterior cingulate and the posterior parietal cortices. As the length of time that data was held in WM increased, activation at recognition increased in the medial temporal, medial occipito-temporal, anterior cingulate and posterior parietal cortices. These results confirm the components of an object WM network required for successful recognition, and suggest that parts of this network, including the medial temporal cortex, are sensitive to the duration of mnemonic delay.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marco Picchioni
- King's College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Section of Neuroimaging, London, United Kingdom.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
78
|
Jacobsen LK, Mencl WE, Constable RT, Westerveld M, Pugh KR. Impact of smoking abstinence on working memory neurocircuitry in adolescent daily tobacco smokers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007; 193:557-66. [PMID: 17505817 DOI: 10.1007/s00213-007-0797-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 75] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/09/2007] [Accepted: 04/02/2007] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
RATIONALE Efficient function of neurocircuitry that supports working memory occurs within a narrow range of dopamine neurotransmission. Work in rodents has shown that exposure to nicotine during adolescence leads to nicotine withdrawal emergent alterations in cortical and subcortical dopamine neurotransmission. OBJECTIVES To test for evidence that the efficiency of neurocircuitry supporting working memory is altered during acute smoking abstinence in adolescent daily tobacco smokers. MATERIALS AND METHODS Fifty-five adolescent daily tobacco smokers were compared with 38 nonsmokers using functional magnetic resonance imaging while subjects performed a verbal working memory task. Smokers were studied during smoking and after 24 h of abstinence from tobacco use. RESULTS Performance of a task with high working memory load in the context of smoking abstinence was associated with greater activation of components of the verbal working memory neurocircuit, including left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and left inferior parietal lobe, among smokers relative to nonsmokers. During smoking abstinence, smokers failed to exhibit increases in functional connectivity between components of the working memory neurocircuit with increasing working memory load observed in nonsmoking adolescents and in prior studies of adults. CONCLUSIONS Smoking abstinence in adolescent smokers is associated with reductions in the efficiency of working memory neurocircuitry and alterations in the functional coordination between components of the working memory neurocircuit. These alterations may stem from effects of nicotine exposure on catecholaminergic systems during adolescent development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Leslie K Jacobsen
- Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, 2 Church Street South, Suite 207, New Haven, CT 06519, USA.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
79
|
Sung EJ, Yoo SS, Yoon HW, Oh SS, Han Y, Park HW. Brain activation related to affective dimension during thermal stimulation in humans: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Int J Neurosci 2007; 117:1011-27. [PMID: 17613111 DOI: 10.1080/00207450600934432] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
The aim of this study was to identify the activated brain region that is involved with the affective dimension of thermal stimulation (not pain, but innocuous warming) using functional MR imaging. Twelve healthy, right-handed male subjects participated in the study. Thermal stimulation with two different temperatures of 41 degrees C and 34 degrees C was applied to the subjects using a fomentation pack, wrapped around the right lower leg of each subject. On the basis of the subjects' responses after the scanning sessions, the authors were able to observe that the subjects felt "warm" and "slightly pleasant and comfortable" under the 41 degrees C condition. The experimental results indicated that warm stimulation produced a significant increase of activation compared to thermal neutral stimulation in various regions such as contralateral insular, ipsilateral cerebellum, ipsilateral putamen, contralateral middle frontal gyrus, ipsilateral inferior frontal gyrus, contralateral postcentral gyrus, and contralateral paracentral lobule. The activated regions are known to be related to thermal sensory, affective/emotional awareness, cognitive functions, sensory-discrimination, and emotion/affective processing, and so on. These results suggest that an appropriate thermal stimulation can produce a positive emotion and activate emotion/affect related regions of the brain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eun-Jung Sung
- Office of Policy, Korea Research Institute of Standards and Science, Daejeon, Republic of Korea
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
80
|
Gontier E, Le Dantec C, Leleu A, Paul I, Charvin H, Bernard C, Lalonde R, Rebaï M. Frontal and parietal ERPs associated with duration discriminations with or without task interference. Brain Res 2007; 1170:79-89. [PMID: 17706619 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.07.022] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/29/2007] [Revised: 07/10/2007] [Accepted: 07/11/2007] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
The main objective of this study was to examine fronto-parietal networks underlying visual duration discriminations. Two types of interference tasks were used to augment cognitive load: line orientation associated with the right hemisphere and multiplication with the left. Both subtasks deteriorated duration discriminations, more severely for line orientation. Relative to the condition without interference, the dual task paradigm decreased amplitudes of the contingent negative variation (CNV) wave, predominant at frontal sites, and the P300 wave, predominant at parietal sites. Inversely, amplitudes of a later appearing positive component (LPC) and its parietal counterpart of opposite polarity (LNC) increased with spatial or numeric task interference. These results are concordant with the view that fronto-parietal networks underlying duration discriminations act in a concerted fashion, with the LPC/LNC waves acting as a warning signal to mitigate errors during high cognitive load.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emilie Gontier
- Université de Rouen, Faculté des Sciences, Laboratoire de Psychologie et Neurosciences de la Cognition (EA1780), 76821 Mont-Saint-Aignan Rouen Cedex, France
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
81
|
Westerberg H, Klingberg T. Changes in cortical activity after training of working memory--a single-subject analysis. Physiol Behav 2007; 92:186-92. [PMID: 17597168 DOI: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.05.041] [Citation(s) in RCA: 131] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
Working memory (WM) capacity is an important factor for a wide range of cognitive skills. This capacity has generally been assumed to be fixed. However, recent studies have suggested that WM can be improved by intensive, computerized training [Klingberg T, Fernell E, Olesen P, Johnson M, Gustafsson P, Dahlström K, et al. Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD--a randomized, controlled trial. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psych 2005;44:177--86]. A recent study by Olesen, Westerberg and Klingberg [Olesen P, Westerberg H, Klingberg T. Increased prefrontal and parietal brain activity after training of working memory. Nat Neurosci 2004;7:75--9] showed that group analysis of brain activity data show increases in prefrontal and parietal cortices after WM training. In the present study we performed single-subject analysis of the changes in brain activity after five weeks of training. Three young, healthy adults participated in the study. On two separate days before practice and during one day after practice, brain activity was measured with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) during performance of a WM and a baseline task. Practice on the WM tasks gradually improved performance and this effect lasted several months. The effect of practice also generalized to improve performance on a non-trained WM task and a reasoning task. After training, WM-related brain activity was significantly increased in the middle and inferior frontal gyrus. The changes in activity were not due to activations of any additional area that was not activated before training. Instead, the changes could best be described by small increases in the extent of the area of activated cortex. The effect of training of WM is thus in several respects similar to the changes in the functional map observed in primate studies of skill learning, although the physiological effect in WM training is located in the prefrontal association cortex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Helena Westerberg
- Karolinska Institutet, Department of Neuropediatrics, Stockholm, Sweden
| | | |
Collapse
|
82
|
Zhou X, Chen C, Zang Y, Dong Q, Chen C, Qiao S, Gong Q. Dissociated brain organization for single-digit addition and multiplication. Neuroimage 2007; 35:871-80. [PMID: 17292628 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.12.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 82] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/03/2006] [Revised: 12/04/2006] [Accepted: 12/08/2006] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
This study compared the patterns of brain activation elicited by single-digit addition and multiplication problems. 20 Chinese undergraduates were asked to verify whether arithmetic equations were true or false during functional magnetic resonance imaging. Results showed that both addition and multiplication were supported by a broad neural system that involved regions within SMA, precentral gyrus, intraparietal sulcus, occipital gyri, superior temporal gyrus, and middle frontal gyrus, as well as some subcortical structures. Nevertheless, addition problems elicited more activation in the intraparietal sulcus and middle occipital gyri at the right hemisphere, and superior occipital gyri at both hemispheres, whereas multiplication had more activation in precentral gyrus, supplementary motor areas, and posterior and anterior superior temporal gyrus at the left hemisphere. This pattern of dissociated activation supports our hypothesis that addition has greater reliance on visuospatial processing and multiplication on verbal processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xinlin Zhou
- State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, China
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
83
|
Ahmadi-Pajouh MA, Towhidkhah F, Gharibzadeh S, Mashhadimalek M. Path planning in the hippocampo-prefrontal cortex pathway: an adaptive model based receding horizon planner. Med Hypotheses 2007; 68:1411-5. [PMID: 17337125 DOI: 10.1016/j.mehy.2006.06.060] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/21/2006] [Accepted: 06/21/2006] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
Four characteristic properties of human path planning strategy are course and fine planning, supervised planning, adaptation and robustness, and complexity reduction. These four characteristics are also observed in "model predictive controller" and its modified version, "receding horizon planner". We hypothesize that the human brain performs path planning tasks, literally like a receding horizon planner. The similarities between human brain and a receding horizon planner are: (1) hippocampus contains the course model and the parietal cortex is responsible for the fine model. (2) Replanning and trajectory tuning using the visual data in parietal cortex and prefrontal cortex is exploited in an adaptive restricted receding horizon. Prefrontal cortex plays the role of the supervisor. (3) Adjusting the sampling time of the planner is implemented based on changes in the complexity of the environment and tasks. This is in fact, the adaptation, which exists both in human behavior and in receding horizon planner. (4) The brain simplifies path-finding problems to reduce computational loads, exactly similar to what engineering controllers intend to do. The visual data is smoothed by clustering of obstacles, before performing any computational task. Finally, we have discussed the consequence of our hypothesis in Alzheimer disease as an optimal planning disorder. Based on some experimental data, Alzheimer patients have a reduced predictive horizon, making the system less robust and exposed to hazardous conditions in sophisticated environments. Patients with mild Alzheimer disease have little trouble with simple optimization problems; working memory of the prefrontal cortex is sufficient for this purpose. However, in complicated tasks, the brain needs huge extended memory. This memory is available through hippocampo-prefrontal pathway, which is to some extent disturbed in Alzheimer patients. We suggest that this fact may be a basis for future experimental diagnosis tests. We predict that Alzheimer patients should have problems with planning for far future; because they have a weak memory, insufficient for heavy optimization tasks, such as moving through moving obstacles in a dynamic environment. Alzheimer disease could be early detected by designing new tests in which the ability of patients to predict the future events is checked. These tests could be accompanied with a multi-step optimization problem. We believe that paying attention to this opinion may provide a good help in diagnosing Alzheimer disease in earlier stages. Surely, experimental studies are needed to validate our hypothesis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mohammad Ali Ahmadi-Pajouh
- Biological Systems Modeling Laboratory, Faculty of Biomedical Engineering, Amirkabir University of Technology, Somayyeh, Hafez, Tehran 15875-4413, Iran
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
84
|
Hester R, D'Esposito M, Cole MW, Garavan H. Neural mechanisms for response selection: comparing selection of responses and items from working memory. Neuroimage 2007; 34:446-54. [PMID: 17071112 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/03/2006] [Revised: 07/31/2006] [Accepted: 08/03/2006] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Recent functional imaging studies of working memory (WM) have suggested a relationship between the requirement for response selection and activity in dorsolateral prefrontal (DLPFC) and parietal regions. Although a number of WM operations are likely to occur during response selection, the current study was particularly interested in the contribution of this neural network to WM-based response selection when compared to the selection of an item from a list being maintained in memory, during a verbal learning task. The design manipulated stimulus-response mappings so that selecting an item from memory was not always accompanied with selecting a motor response. Functional activation during selection supported previous findings of fronto-parietal involvement, although in contrast to previous findings left, rather than right, DLPFC activity was significantly more active for selecting a memory-guided motor response, when compared to selecting an item currently maintained in memory or executing a memory-guided response. Our results contribute to the debate over the role of fronto-parietal activity during WM tasks, suggesting that this activity appears particularly related to response selection, potentially supporting the hypothesized role of prefrontal activity in biasing attention toward task-relevant material in more posterior regions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robert Hester
- Department of Psychology, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
85
|
Jacobsen LK, Pugh KR, Constable RT, Westerveld M, Mencl WE. Functional correlates of verbal memory deficits emerging during nicotine withdrawal in abstinent adolescent cannabis users. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 61:31-40. [PMID: 16631130 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.02.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 111] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/27/2005] [Revised: 12/07/2005] [Accepted: 02/22/2006] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Cannabis remains the most widely used illicit substance by adolescents and is typically consumed by this population in the context of ongoing tobacco use. Human studies have shown that both cannabis and tobacco exert effects on cognitive function; however, little is known about possible interacting effects of these drugs on brain function and cognition during adolescent development. METHODS Verbal learning and memory were assessed in 20 adolescent users of tobacco and cannabis and 25 adolescent tobacco users with minimal history of cannabis use. Functional magnetic resonance imaging was used to examine brain function and functional connectivity while a subset of these subjects performed a verbal working memory task. RESULTS Delayed recall of verbal stimuli deteriorated during nicotine withdrawal among cannabis users but not among comparison subjects. During high verbal working memory load, nicotine withdrawal selectively increased task-related activation of posterior cortical regions and was associated with disruption of frontoparietal connectivity in adolescent cannabis users relative to comparison subjects. CONCLUSIONS These observations suggest that cannabis use during adolescent development may disrupt neurocircuitry supporting verbal memory formation and that deficits associated with disruption of these neurocircuits are unmasked during nicotine withdrawal.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Leslie K Jacobsen
- Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, Connecticut 06519, USA.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
86
|
Nielson KA, Douville KL, Seidenberg M, Woodard JL, Miller SK, Franczak M, Antuono P, Rao SM. Age-related functional recruitment for famous name recognition: an event-related fMRI study. Neurobiol Aging 2006; 27:1494-504. [PMID: 16225965 PMCID: PMC2078241 DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.08.022] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/02/2005] [Revised: 08/19/2005] [Accepted: 08/19/2005] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
Recent neuroimaging research shows that older adults exhibit recruitment, or increased activation on various cognitive tasks. The current study evaluated whether a similar pattern also occurs in semantic memory by evaluating age-related differences during recognition of Recent (since the 1990s) and Enduring (1950s to present) famous names. Fifteen healthy older and 15 healthy younger adults performed the name recognition task with a high and comparable degree of accuracy, although older adults had slower reaction time in response to Recent famous names. Event-related functional MRI showed extensive networks of activation in the two groups including posterior cingulate, right hippocampus, temporal lobe and left prefrontal regions. The Recent condition produced more extensive activation than the Enduring condition. Older adults had more extensive and greater magnitude of activation in 15 of 20 regions, particularly for the Recent condition (15 of 15; 7 of 15 also differed for Enduring); young adults did not show greater activation magnitude in any region. There were no group differences for non-famous names, indicating that age differences are task-specific. The results support and extend the existing literature to semantic memory tasks, indicating that older adult brains use functional recruitment to support task performance, even when task performance accuracy is high.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kristy A Nielson
- Department of Psychology and the Integrative Neuroscience Research Center, Marquette University, Milwaukee, WI 53201-1881, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
87
|
Zhou X, Chen C, Zhang H, Xue G, Dong Q, Jin Z, Zhang L, Peng C, Zhao H, Guo Y, Jiang T, Chen C. Neural substrates for forward and backward recitation of numbers and the alphabet: a close examination of the role of intraparietal sulcus and perisylvian areas. Brain Res 2006; 1099:109-20. [PMID: 16784724 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.01.133] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/12/2005] [Revised: 01/14/2006] [Accepted: 01/17/2006] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Despite numerous studies on the neural basis of numerical processing, few studies have examined the neural substrates of one of the most basic numerical processing-number sequence recitation. The present study used fMRI to investigate neural substrates of number sequence recitation, focusing on the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) and perisylvian areas. This study used a 2 (number versus alphabet) x 2 (forward versus backward recitation) design. 12 Chinese undergraduates were asked to recite overtly but gently numerical and alphabetical sequences forward and backward. Results showed that, for both numerical and alphabetic sequences, the left IPS was activated when performing backward recitation, but not when performing forward recitation. In terms of perisylvian areas, all four tasks elicited activation in bilateral superior temporal gyrus and inferior frontal gyrus, but forward recitation elicited greater activation in the left posterior superior temporal gyrus than did backward recitation, whereas backward recitation elicited greater activation in the left inferior frontal gyrus than did forward recitation. These results suggest that forward recitation of numbers and the alphabet is typically based on verbal processing of numbers implemented in the perisylvian area, whereas backward recitation would likely require additional neural resources in the IPS.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xinlin Zhou
- State Key Lab of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University, China
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
88
|
Weissman DH, Roberts KC, Visscher KM, Woldorff MG. The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. Nat Neurosci 2006; 9:971-8. [PMID: 16767087 DOI: 10.1038/nn1727] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1159] [Impact Index Per Article: 64.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/08/2006] [Accepted: 05/24/2006] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Momentary lapses in attention frequently impair goal-directed behavior, sometimes with serious consequences. Nevertheless, we lack an integrated view of the brain mechanisms underlying such lapses. By investigating trial-by-trial relationships between brain activity and response time in humans, we determined that attentional lapses begin with reduced prestimulus activity in anterior cingulate and right prefrontal regions involved in controlling attention. Less efficient stimulus processing during attentional lapses was also characterized by less deactivation of a 'default-mode' network, reduced stimulus-evoked sensory activity, and increased activity in widespread regions of frontal and parietal cortex. Finally, consistent with a mechanism for recovering from attentional lapses, increased stimulus-evoked activity in the right inferior frontal gyrus and the right temporal-parietal junction predicted better performance on the next trial. Our findings provide a new, system-wide understanding of the patterns of brain activity that are associated with brief attentional lapses, which informs both theoretical and clinical models of goal-directed behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D H Weissman
- Center for Cognitive Neuroscience and Department of Psychiatry, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina 27708, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
89
|
Leiberg S, Kaiser J, Lutzenberger W. Gamma-band activity dissociates between matching and nonmatching stimulus pairs in an auditory delayed matching-to-sample task. Neuroimage 2006; 30:1357-64. [PMID: 16469508 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/14/2005] [Revised: 09/12/2005] [Accepted: 11/09/2005] [Indexed: 12/01/2022] Open
Abstract
Electro- and magnetoencephalography studies have suggested that increased gamma-band activity (GBA) is a correlate of activated neural stimulus representations. In this study, a delayed matching-to-sample paradigm for auditory spatial information was employed to investigate the role of magnetoencephalographic gamma-band activity in the differentiation between matching and nonmatching stimulus pairs. Twelve subjects made same-different judgments about the lateralization angle of pairs of filtered noise stimuli (S1 and S2) presented with 0.8-s delays. One half of the subjects had to respond to matching stimulus pairs, the other half to nonmatching stimulus pairs. Cortical oscillatory activity in the memory task was compared to a control task requiring the detection of background noise intensity changes. Memory-related GBA increases were revealed over midline parietal areas in the middle of the delay phase and during the presentation of S2 and over frontocentral areas at the end of the delay phase. This replicated previous findings. In addition, nonmatching trials were associated with increased GBA over right parietal areas in response to S2. The midline parietal GBA increase during S2 in the memory condition may have reflected the representation of S1 needed for a comparison between S1 and S2. When S1 and S2 were identical, no further representation was required. In contrast, for nonmatching pairs, a second representation was activated over right parietal areas.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Susanne Leiberg
- Institute of Medical Psychology and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Tübingen, MEG Center, Otfried-Müller-Str. 47, 72076 Tübingen, Germany.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
90
|
Kim J, Whyte J, Wang J, Rao H, Tang KZ, Detre JA. Continuous ASL perfusion fMRI investigation of higher cognition: quantification of tonic CBF changes during sustained attention and working memory tasks. Neuroimage 2006; 31:376-85. [PMID: 16427324 PMCID: PMC2362398 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.035] [Citation(s) in RCA: 68] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/25/2005] [Revised: 11/11/2005] [Accepted: 11/18/2005] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Arterial spin labeling (ASL) perfusion fMRI is an emerging method in clinical neuroimaging. Its non-invasiveness, absence of low frequency noise, and ability to quantify the absolute level of cerebral blood flow (CBF) make the method ideal for longitudinal designs or low frequency paradigms. Despite the usefulness in the study of cognitive dysfunctions in clinical populations, perfusion activation studies to date have been conducted for simple sensorimotor paradigms or with single-slice acquisition, mainly due to technical challenges. Using our recently developed amplitude-modulated continuous ASL (CASL) perfusion fMRI protocol, we assessed the feasibility of a higher level cognitive activation study in twelve healthy subjects. Taking advantage of the ASL noise properties, we were able to study tonic CBF changes during uninterrupted 6-min continuous performance of working memory and sustained attention tasks. For the visual sustained attention task, regional CBF increases (6-12 ml/100 g/min) were detected in the right middle frontal gyrus, the bilateral occipital gyri, and the anterior cingulate/medial frontal gyri. During the 2-back working memory task, significantly increased activations (7-11 ml/100 g/min) were found in the left inferior frontal/precentral gyri, the left inferior parietal lobule, the anterior cingulate/medial frontal gyri, and the left occipital gyrus. Locations of activated and deactivated areas largely concur with previous PET and BOLD fMRI studies utilizing similar paradigms. These results demonstrate that CASL perfusion fMRI can be successfully utilized for the investigation of the tonic CBF changes associated with high level cognitive operations. Increased applications of the method to the investigation of cognitively impaired populations are expected to follow.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Junghoon Kim
- Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, Albert Einstein Healthcare Network, 1200 W. Tabor Road, Philadelphia, PA 19141, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
91
|
Bokde ALW, Dong W, Born C, Leinsinger G, Meindl T, Teipel SJ, Reiser M, Hampel H. Task difficulty in a simultaneous face matching task modulates activity in face fusiform area. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2005; 25:701-10. [PMID: 16325382 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/27/2004] [Revised: 08/31/2005] [Accepted: 09/01/2005] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
The level of difficulty of a task can alter the neural network that activates for performance of the task. Previous studies have shown increased activation with task difficulty in the frontal lobes while the effects in the extrastriate visual areas have been unclear. We hypothesized that the face fusiform area (FFA), an area specialized for face processing, would increase activation as task difficulty increased in a face matching task. The difficulty level was increased by degrading the quality of the images. The degradation levels were 10%, 20%, 40% and 60%. Based on the correct response rate, the data were divided into a baseline level (composed of non-degraded and 10% degraded images) and a difficult level (composed of the 20%, 40% and 60% degraded images). Brain activation was measured using functional magnetic resonance imaging. The baseline face matching task activated a wide network of regions that included bilaterally the occipital, temporal and parietal lobes and the right frontal lobe. A novel behavioral finding was that task difficulty did not linearly increase with image degradation. The novel brain imaging finding was that the FFA is modulated by task difficulty and performance in the task was linearly correlated to activation in FFA. In addition, we found that activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) had increased activation as task difficulty increased. When adding the response time as a covariate, the differences in the DLPFC did not remain statistically significant. Increased task difficulty also led to a decrease in activation of visual areas in the extrastriate cortex. Task difficulty increased activation in the FFA to enhance the face processing and suppressed activation in visual extrastriate areas that processed low level properties of the stimuli. Task difficulty led to enhanced response in the FFA and suppressed response in other visual areas.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A L W Bokde
- Alzheimer Memorial Center and Geriatric Psychiatry Branch, Department of Psychiatry, Ludwig-Maximilian University, Nussbaumstr. 7, 80336 Munich, Germany.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
92
|
Goldstein JM, Jerram M, Poldrack R, Anagnoson R, Breiter HC, Makris N, Goodman JM, Tsuang MT, Seidman LJ. Sex differences in prefrontal cortical brain activity during fMRI of auditory verbal working memory. Neuropsychology 2005; 19:509-19. [PMID: 16060826 DOI: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.4.509] [Citation(s) in RCA: 118] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
Functional imaging studies of sex effects in working memory (WMEM) are few, despite significant normal sex differences in brain regions implicated in WMEM. This functional MRI (fMRI) study tested for sex effects in an auditory verbal WMEM task in prefrontal, parietal, cingulate, and insula regions. Fourteen healthy, right-handed community subjects were comparable between the sexes, including on WMEM performance. Per statistical parametric mapping, women exhibited greater signal intensity changes in middle, inferior, and orbital prefrontal cortices than men (corrected for multiple comparisons). A test of mixed-sex groups, comparable on performance, showed no significant differences in the hypothesized regions, providing evidence for discriminant validity for significant sex differences. The findings suggest that combining men and women in fMRI studies of cognition may obscure or bias results.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jill M Goldstein
- Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02120, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
93
|
Lee KH, Choi YY, Gray JR, Cho SH, Chae JH, Lee S, Kim K. Neural correlates of superior intelligence: stronger recruitment of posterior parietal cortex. Neuroimage 2005; 29:578-86. [PMID: 16122946 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.036] [Citation(s) in RCA: 156] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/20/2005] [Revised: 07/21/2005] [Accepted: 07/22/2005] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
General intelligence (g) is a common factor in diverse cognitive abilities and a major influence on life outcomes. Neuroimaging studies in adults suggest that the lateral prefrontal and parietal cortices play a crucial role in related cognitive activities including fluid reasoning, the control of attention, and working memory. Here, we investigated the neural bases for intellectual giftedness (superior-g) in adolescents, using fMRI. The participants consisted of a superior-g group (n = 18, mean RAPM = 33.9 +/- 0.8, >99%) from the national academy for gifted adolescents and the control group (n = 18, mean RAPM = 22.8 +/- 1.6, 60%) from local high schools in Korea (mean age = 16.5 +/- 0.8). fMRI data were acquired while they performed two reasoning tasks with high and low g-loadings. In both groups, the high g-loaded tasks specifically increased regional activity in the bilateral fronto-parietal network including the lateral prefrontal, anterior cingulate, and posterior parietal cortices. However, the regional activations of the superior-g group were significantly stronger than those of the control group, especially in the posterior parietal cortex. Moreover, regression analysis revealed that activity of the superior and intraparietal cortices (BA 7/40) strongly covaried with individual differences in g (r = 0.71 to 0.81). A correlated vectors analysis implicated bilateral posterior parietal areas in g. These results suggest that superior-g may not be due to the recruitment of additional brain regions but to the functional facilitation of the fronto-parietal network particularly driven by the posterior parietal activation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kun Ho Lee
- School of Biological Sciences, Seoul National University, Seoul 151-742, South Korea.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
94
|
Foxe JJ, Simpson GV, Ahlfors SP, Saron CD. Biasing the brain's attentional set: I. cue driven deployments of intersensory selective attention. Exp Brain Res 2005; 166:370-92. [PMID: 16086144 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-005-2378-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/09/2004] [Accepted: 02/24/2005] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
Brain activity associated with directing attention to one of two possible sensory modalities was examined using high-density mapping of human event-related potentials. The deployment of selective attention was based on visually presented symbolic cue-words instructing subjects on a trial-by-trial basis, which sensory modality to attend. We measured the spatio-temporal pattern of activation in the approximately 1 second period between the cue-instruction and a subsequent compound auditory-visual imperative stimulus. This allowed us to assess the flow of processing across brain regions involved in deploying and sustaining inter-sensory selective attention, prior to the actual selective processing of the compound audio-visual target stimulus. Activity over frontal and parietal areas showed sensory specific increases in activation during the early part of the anticipatory period (~230 ms), probably representing the activation of fronto-parietal attentional deployment systems for top-down control of attention. In the later period preceding the arrival of the "to-be-attended" stimulus, sustained differential activity was seen over fronto-central regions and parieto-occipital regions, suggesting the maintenance of sensory-specific biased attentional states that would allow for subsequent selective processing. Although there was clear sensory biasing in this late sustained period, it was also clear that both sensory systems were being prepared during the cue-target period. These late sensory-specific biasing effects were also accompanied by sustained activations over frontal cortices that also showed both common and sensory specific activation patterns, suggesting that maintenance of the biased state includes top-down inputs from generators in frontal cortices, some of which are sensory-specific regions. These data support extensive interactions between sensory, parietal and frontal regions during processing of cue information, deployment of attention, and maintenance of the focus of attention in anticipation of impending attentionally relevant input.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- John J Foxe
- Department of Psychology, Program in Cognitive Neuroscience, The City College of the City University of New York, North Academic Complex, 138th Street and Convent Avenue, NY 10031, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
95
|
Prohovnik I, Skudlarski P, Fulbright RK, Gore JC, Wexler BE. Functional MRI changes before and after onset of reported emotions. Psychiatry Res 2004; 132:239-50. [PMID: 15664795 DOI: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2004.03.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/05/2002] [Revised: 05/15/2003] [Accepted: 03/25/2004] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
The social nature of emotion is evident in the importance of facial and vocal displays in emotion-related behavior. This is the first brain-imaging study to use simulated face-to-face social interactions to evoke emotional responses and to compare valence-related activations before and after subjective onset of emotional response. Videotapes were prepared with actresses who described happy or unhappy experiences. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) at 1.5 T was used to acquire BOLD images in 21 healthy young adults before, after, and during viewing of the happy and sad tapes. Subjects pushed buttons to indicate the onset of subjective emotional responses. Group data were analyzed by a bootstrap randomization method after anatomical normalization. Significant activation was detected in frontal and sensory regions prior to the reported onset of emotional response, and this activity showed a marked decrease after the report of conscious emotional experience. Significant differences between happy and sad conditions were evident in multiple brain regions both before and after the reported onset of emotional response, including the middle and superior temporal gyri, the middle frontal gyrus, the caudate, and the hippocampus. Socially relevant emotional stimulation is feasible and evokes robust responses. The neural correlates of the evoked emotion are multiple, widely distributed, and inclusive of areas important in many cognitive tasks. Positive and negative emotional responses include activation of common and distinctive brain regions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Isak Prohovnik
- Department of Diagnostic Radiology, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
96
|
DelBello MP, Adler CM, Amicone J, Mills NP, Shear PK, Warner J, Strakowski SM. Parametric neurocognitive task design: a pilot study of sustained attention in adolescents with bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord 2004; 82 Suppl 1:S79-88. [PMID: 15571793 DOI: 10.1016/j.jad.2004.05.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/08/2003] [Accepted: 05/19/2004] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Disruption in attention is one of the core features of bipolar disorder (BP). Therefore, neurocognitive paradigms assessing brain function in response to tasks of increasing attentional difficulty may be useful to clarify the neurophysiology of bipolar disorder. The aim of this study was to obtain pilot performance data using a parametric task of sustained attention that might be useful as an experimental paradigm for future functional neuroimaging studies. We hypothesized that task performance would worsen as task difficulty increased in manic and euthymic bipolar and healthy subjects. Additionally, we hypothesized that the groups would exhibit a similar decline in task performance as level of task difficulty increased and that within each level of task difficulty there would be similar performance among groups. METHODS A novel parametric Continuous Performance Task-Identical Pairs (CPT-IP) version was administered to manic (N=10) and euthymic (N=10) adolescents with bipolar disorder and healthy controls (N=10). RESULTS There were no statistically significant group differences in task performance as measured by discriminability, percent correct, false positive hits, and reaction time. However, within each group, performance on all measures worsened with increased attentional difficulty (p<0.0001). There were no statistically significant task difficulty by group interactions. Furthermore, medication exposure and comorbid attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder were not associated with most measures of task performance. However, BP subjects who were treated with medications had slower task performance compared with BP subjects who were unmedicated. LIMITATIONS Larger studies examining the effects of specific medication classes on task performance are necessary. CONCLUSIONS The results of this pilot study suggest that manic and euthymic BP patients do not exhibit attentional dysfunction as compared to healthy adolescents using a novel parametric version of the CPT-IP. Furthermore, our parametric CPT-IP version may be useful as a novel parametric neurocognitive paradigm for future functional neuroimaging studies of bipolar adolescents.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Melissa P DelBello
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Cincinnati College of Medicine, 231 Bethesda Ave., P.O. Box 670559, Cincinnati, OH 45267-0559, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
97
|
Hester RL, Kinsella GJ, Ong B. Effect of age on forward and backward span tasks. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2004; 10:475-81. [PMID: 15327726 DOI: 10.1017/s1355617704104037] [Citation(s) in RCA: 120] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/24/2003] [Revised: 09/30/2003] [Indexed: 12/22/2022]
Abstract
The central executive component of working memory has been argued to play an important role in the performance of span tasks, particularly backward span. Age-related decline in central executive function has also been reported, and yet there have been inconsistent findings to indicate that with increasing age, the discrepancy between forward and backward span increases. A secondary analysis of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Third Edition standardization sample (N = 1030) was performed to investigate this relationship. It was hypothesized on the basis of past research indicating an age-related decline in central executive performance, that backward digit and spatial span performance would decrease at a greater rate than forward span performance. However, the results indicated that the rate of age-related performance decline was equivalent for both measures. It is proposed that both forward and backward span tasks recruit central executive resources for successful task performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robert L Hester
- School of Psychological Science, La Trobe University, Bundoora, Victoria, Australia
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
98
|
Boyd LA, Winstein CJ. Providing explicit information disrupts implicit motor learning after basal ganglia stroke. Learn Mem 2004; 11:388-96. [PMID: 15286181 PMCID: PMC498316 DOI: 10.1101/lm.80104] [Citation(s) in RCA: 99] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/12/2004] [Accepted: 06/09/2004] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
Despite their purported neuroanatomic and functional isolation, empirical evidence suggests that sometimes conscious explicit processes can influence implicit motor skill learning. Our goal was to determine if the provision of explicit information affected implicit motor-sequence learning after damage to the basal ganglia. Individuals with stroke affecting the basal ganglia (BG) and healthy controls (HC) practiced a continuous implicit motor-sequencing task; half were provided with explicit information (EI) and half were not (No-EI). The focus of brain damage for both BG groups was in the putamen. All of the EI participants were at least explicitly aware of the repeating sequence. Across three days of practice, explicit information had a differential effect on the groups. Explicit information disrupted acquisition performance in participants with basal ganglia stroke but not healthy controls. By retention (day 4), a dissociation was apparent--explicit information hindered implicit learning in participants with basal ganglia lesions but aided healthy controls. It appears that after basal ganglia stroke explicit information is less helpful in the development of the motor plan than is discovering a motor solution using the implicit system alone. This may be due to the increased demand placed on working memory by explicit information. Thus, basal ganglia integrity may be a crucial factor in determining the efficacy of explicit information for implicit motor-sequence learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lara A Boyd
- Department of Physical Therapy and Rehabilitation Sciences, Department of Neurology, University of Kansas Medical Center, Kansas City, Kansas 66160, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
99
|
Xue G, Dong Q, Jin Z, Chen C. Mapping of verbal working memory in nonfluent Chinese-English bilinguals with functional MRI. Neuroimage 2004; 22:1-10. [PMID: 15109992 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 57] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/30/2003] [Revised: 01/10/2004] [Accepted: 01/12/2004] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Existing cognitive and neural imaging studies have suggested a frontoparietal network of multiple, cooperative components for verbal working memory (WM). We used functional MRI to investigate whether this neural network is also involved in the processing of second language by nonfluent bilinguals. Twelve (five males, seven females) native Chinese speakers who had limited English proficiency were scanned while performing working memory tasks in Chinese and English. They were asked to make judgment continuously whether the word presented on the screen was semantically related to (i.e., the semantic tasks) another word presented two words earlier. On a different task (i.e., the phonological tasks), they were asked to make judgment whether the target word rhymed with the other word. A naming and judgment task in each language was adopted to control for the visual process, initial lexical process, and motor responses. Behavioral data showed that subjects performed better at tasks in their native language (Chinese, L1) than in English (L2). Imaging results showed that all working memory tasks in both L1 and L2 elicited a very similar pattern of left-hemisphere-dominated activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, pars opercularis region, pars triangularis region, precentral cortex, and parietal lobule. Consistent with the behavioral data, the volume of activation in the left opercularis region, left parietal lobule, and right precentral region was greater for L2 than for L1. These results suggest that working memory in L1 and L2 is mediated by a unitary neural system (i.e., frontoparietal region), which is capable of recruiting surrounding cortical resources to meet the increased computational demand caused by low L2 proficiency.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gui Xue
- Institute of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, School of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, PR China
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
100
|
Pastor MA, Day BL, Macaluso E, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RSJ. The functional neuroanatomy of temporal discrimination. J Neurosci 2004; 24:2585-91. [PMID: 15014134 PMCID: PMC6729480 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.4210-03.2004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 152] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Two identical stimuli, such as a pair of electrical shocks to the skin, are readily perceived as two separate events in time provided the interval between them is sufficiently long. However, as they are presented progressively closer together, there comes a point when the two separate stimuli are perceived as a single stimulus. Damage to posterior parietal cortex, peri-supplementary motor area (peri-SMA), and basal ganglia can disturb this form of temporal discrimination. Our aim was to establish, in healthy subjects, the brain areas that are involved in this process. During functional magnetic resonance imaging scanning, paired electrical pulses, separated by variable inter-stimulus intervals (5-110 msec), were delivered to different sites on one forearm (8-64 mm from the midline). Subjects were required to simply detect the stimulus (control task) or to identify a stimulus property. For temporal discrimination (TD), subjects reported whether they felt one or two stimuli. For spatial discrimination, they reported whether the stimuli were located on the right or left side of the forearm. Subjects reported their choice by pressing a button with the opposite hand. Our results showed that discrimination, as opposed to simply detection, activated several brain areas. Most were common to both discrimination tasks. These included regions of prefrontal cortex, right postcentral gyrus and inferior parietal lobule, basal ganglia, and cerebellum. However, activation of pre-SMA and anterior cingulate was found to be specific to the TD task. This suggests that these two frontal regions may play a role in the temporal processing of somatosensory events.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maria A Pastor
- Department of Neurology, University of Navarre School of Medicine, Clinica Universitaria, 31080 Pamplona, Spain.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|