101
|
Tennant SA, Clark H, Hawes I, Tam WK, Hua J, Yang W, Gerlei KZ, Wood ER, Nolan MF. Spatial representation by ramping activity of neurons in the retrohippocampal cortex. Curr Biol 2022; 32:4451-4464.e7. [PMID: 36099915 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2022.08.050] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/14/2022] [Revised: 07/05/2022] [Accepted: 08/17/2022] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
Neurons in the retrohippocampal cortices play crucial roles in spatial memory. Many retrohippocampal neurons have firing fields that are selectively active at specific locations, with memory for rewarded locations associated with reorganization of these firing fields. Whether this is the sole strategy for representing spatial memories is unclear. Here, we demonstrate that during a spatial memory task retrohippocampal neurons encode location through ramping activity that extends across segments of a linear track approaching and following a reward, with the rewarded location represented by offsets or switches in the slope of the ramping activity. Ramping representations could be maintained independently of trial outcome and cues marking the reward location, indicating that they result from recall of the track structure. When recorded in an open arena, neurons that generated ramping activity during the spatial memory task were more numerous than grid or border cells, with a majority showing spatial firing that did not meet criteria for classification as grid or border representations. Encoding of rewarded locations through offsets and switches in the slope of ramping activity also emerged in recurrent neural network models trained to solve a similar spatial memory task. Impaired performance of model networks following disruption of outputs from ramping neurons is consistent with this coding strategy supporting navigation to recalled locations of behavioral significance. Our results suggest that encoding of learned spaces by retrohippocampal networks employs both discrete firing fields and continuous ramping representations. We hypothesize that retrohippocampal ramping activity mediates readout of learned models for goal-directed navigation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah A Tennant
- Centre for Discovery Brain Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
| | - Harry Clark
- Centre for Discovery Brain Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
| | - Ian Hawes
- Centre for Discovery Brain Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
| | - Wing Kin Tam
- Centre for Discovery Brain Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK; Simons Initiative for the Developing Brain, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
| | - Junji Hua
- Centre for Discovery Brain Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
| | - Wannan Yang
- Centre for Discovery Brain Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
| | - Klara Z Gerlei
- Centre for Discovery Brain Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
| | - Emma R Wood
- Centre for Discovery Brain Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK; Simons Initiative for the Developing Brain, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
| | - Matthew F Nolan
- Centre for Discovery Brain Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK; Simons Initiative for the Developing Brain, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK; Centre for Statistics, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
102
|
Yu JH, Kim REY, Park SY, Lee DY, Cho HJ, Kim NH, Yoo HJ, Seo JA, Kim SH, Kim SG, Choi KM, Baik SH, Shin C, Kim NH. Night blood pressure variability, brain atrophy, and cognitive decline. Front Neurol 2022; 13:963648. [PMID: 36119712 PMCID: PMC9474888 DOI: 10.3389/fneur.2022.963648] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/07/2022] [Accepted: 07/29/2022] [Indexed: 12/04/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Although blood pressure variability (BPV) has emerged as a novel risk factor for Alzheimer's disease, few studies have examined the effects of night BPV on brain structure and function. This study investigated the association of night BPV with brain atrophy and cognitive function changes. Methods The analysis included 1,398 participants with valid ambulatory blood pressure (BP) monitoring at baseline and both baseline and 4-year follow-up brain magnetic resonance images who were recruited from the Korean Genome and Epidemiology Study. Participants underwent a comprehensive neuropsychological test battery. BPV was derived from ambulatory BP monitoring and calculated as a standard deviation (SD) of 24-h and daytime and nighttime BP. Results During the median follow-up of 4.3 years, increased SD of night systolic or diastolic BP was an indicator of total brain volume reduction, while daytime BPV or night average BP was not associated with total brain volume changes. High SD of night systolic BP was associated with reduced gray matter (GM) volume, independent of average night BP, and use of antihypertensive drugs. It also was associated with a reduction of temporal GM volume, mostly driven by atrophy in the left entorhinal cortex and the right fusiform gyrus. In cognitive performance, high variability of night systolic BP was associated with a decrease in visual delayed recall memory and verbal fluency for the category. Conclusion Increased night BPV, rather than night mean BP, was associated with reduced brain volume and cognitive decline. High night BPV could be an independent predictor for rapid brain aging in a middle-aged population.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ji Hee Yu
- Division of Endocrinology and Metabolism, Department of Internal Medicine, Korea University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Regina E. Y. Kim
- Institute of Human Genomic Study, Korea University Ansan Hospital, Korea University College of Medicine, Ansan, South Korea
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, United States
| | - So Young Park
- Division of Endocrinology and Metabolism, Department of Internal Medicine, Korea University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Da Young Lee
- Division of Endocrinology and Metabolism, Department of Internal Medicine, Korea University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Hyun Joo Cho
- Division of Endocrinology and Metabolism, Department of Internal Medicine, Korea University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Nam Hoon Kim
- Division of Endocrinology and Metabolism, Department of Internal Medicine, Korea University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Hye Jin Yoo
- Division of Endocrinology and Metabolism, Department of Internal Medicine, Korea University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Ji A Seo
- Division of Endocrinology and Metabolism, Department of Internal Medicine, Korea University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Seong Hwan Kim
- Division of Cardiology, Department of Internal Medicine, Korea University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Sin Gon Kim
- Division of Endocrinology and Metabolism, Department of Internal Medicine, Korea University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Kyung Mook Choi
- Division of Endocrinology and Metabolism, Department of Internal Medicine, Korea University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Sei Hyun Baik
- Division of Endocrinology and Metabolism, Department of Internal Medicine, Korea University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea
| | - Chol Shin
- Institute of Human Genomic Study, Korea University Ansan Hospital, Korea University College of Medicine, Ansan, South Korea
| | - Nan Hee Kim
- Division of Endocrinology and Metabolism, Department of Internal Medicine, Korea University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea
- *Correspondence: Nan Hee Kim
| |
Collapse
|
103
|
Profile of May-Britt and Edvard Moser. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2022; 119:e2210910119. [PMID: 35878011 PMCID: PMC9371676 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2210910119] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/27/2023] Open
|
104
|
Ning W, Bladon JH, Hasselmo ME. Complementary representations of time in the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus. Hippocampus 2022; 32:577-596. [PMID: 35822589 PMCID: PMC9444055 DOI: 10.1002/hipo.23451] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/17/2021] [Revised: 05/29/2022] [Accepted: 06/08/2022] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
Episodic memory binds the spatial and temporal relationships between the elements of experience. The hippocampus encodes space through place cells that fire at specific spatial locations. Similarly, time cells fire sequentially at specific time points within a temporally organized experience. Recent studies in rodents, monkeys, and humans have identified time cells with discrete firing fields and cells with monotonically changing activity in supporting the temporal organization of events across multiple timescales. Using in vivo electrophysiological tetrode recordings, we simultaneously recorded neurons from the prefrontal cortex and dorsal CA1 of the hippocampus while rats performed a delayed match to sample task. During the treadmill mnemonic delay, hippocampal time cells exhibited sparser firing fields with decreasing resolution over time, consistent with previous results. In comparison, temporally modulated cells in the prefrontal cortex showed more monotonically changing firing rates, ramping up or decaying with the passage of time, and exhibited greater temporal precision for Bayesian decoding of time at long time lags. These time cells show exquisite temporal resolution both in their firing fields and in the fine timing of spikes relative to the phase of theta oscillations. Here, we report evidence of theta phase precession in both the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus during the temporal delay, however, hippocampal cells exhibited steeper phase precession slopes and more punctate time fields. To disentangle whether time cell activity reflects elapsed time or distance traveled, we varied the treadmill running speed on each trial. While many neurons contained multiplexed representations of time and distance, both regions were more strongly influenced by time than distance. Overall, these results demonstrate the flexible integration of spatiotemporal dimensions and reveal complementary representations of time in the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus in supporting memory-guided behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wing Ning
- Center for Systems Neuroscience, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
- Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, University of California, Irvine, Irvine, California, USA
| | - John H. Bladon
- Center for Systems Neuroscience, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
- Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, Waltham, Massachusetts, USA
| | - Michael E. Hasselmo
- Center for Systems Neuroscience, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
| |
Collapse
|
105
|
Rolls ET. The hippocampus, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, and episodic and semantic memory. Prog Neurobiol 2022; 217:102334. [PMID: 35870682 DOI: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2022.102334] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 19.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/21/2022] [Revised: 06/07/2022] [Accepted: 07/19/2022] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
The human ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC)/anterior cingulate cortex is implicated in reward and emotion, but also in memory. It is shown how the human orbitofrontal cortex connecting with the vmPFC and anterior cingulate cortex provide a route to the hippocampus for reward and emotional value to be incorporated into episodic memory, enabling memory of where a reward was seen. It is proposed that this value component results in primarily episodic memories with some value component to be repeatedly recalled from the hippocampus so that they are more likely to become incorporated into neocortical semantic and autobiographical memories. The same orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate regions also connect in humans to the septal and basal forebrain cholinergic nuclei, thereby helping to consolidate memory, and helping to account for why damage to the vMPFC impairs memory. The human hippocampus and vmPFC thus contribute in complementary ways to forming episodic and semantic memories.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Edmund T Rolls
- Oxford Centre for Computational Neuroscience, Oxford, UK; University of Warwick, Department of Computer Science, Coventry, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
106
|
Yoo HB, Umbach G, Lega B. Episodic boundary cells in human medial temporal lobe during the free recall task. Hippocampus 2022; 32:481-487. [PMID: 35579307 PMCID: PMC10682840 DOI: 10.1002/hipo.23421] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/02/2021] [Revised: 03/31/2022] [Accepted: 04/25/2022] [Indexed: 11/11/2022]
Abstract
A necessary condition for forming episodic memories is the construction of specific episodes demarcated from other episodes in space and time. Evidence from studies of episodic memory formation using rodent models suggest that the medial temporal lobe (MTL) supports the representation of boundary information. Building on recent work using human microelectrode recordings as well, we hypothesized of human MTL neurons with firing rates sensitive to episodic boundary information. We identified 27 episodic boundary neurons out of 736 single neurons recorded across 27 subjects. Firing of these neurons increased at the beginning and end of mnemonically relevant episodes in the free recall task. We distinguish episodic boundary neurons from a population of ramping neurons (n = 58), which are time-sensitive neurons whose activity provides complementary information during episodic representation. Episodic boundary neurons exhibited a U-shaped activity pattern demonstrating increased activity after both beginning and end boundaries of encoding and retrieval epochs. We also describe evidence that the firing of boundary neurons within episodic boundaries is organized by hippocampal theta oscillations, using spike-field coherence metrics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hye Bin Yoo
- Department of Neurological Surgery, University of Texas Southwestern, Dallas, Texas, USA
| | - Gray Umbach
- Department of Neurological Surgery, University of Texas Southwestern, Dallas, Texas, USA
| | - Bradley Lega
- Department of Neurological Surgery, University of Texas Southwestern, Dallas, Texas, USA
| |
Collapse
|
107
|
Bellmund JLS, Deuker L, Montijn ND, Doeller CF. Mnemonic construction and representation of temporal structure in the hippocampal formation. Nat Commun 2022; 13:3395. [PMID: 35739096 PMCID: PMC9226117 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-022-30984-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/15/2021] [Accepted: 05/20/2022] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
The hippocampal-entorhinal region supports memory for episodic details, such as temporal relations of sequential events, and mnemonic constructions combining experiences for inferential reasoning. However, it is unclear whether hippocampal event memories reflect temporal relations derived from mnemonic constructions, event order, or elapsing time, and whether these sequence representations generalize temporal relations across similar sequences. Here, participants mnemonically constructed times of events from multiple sequences using infrequent cues and their experience of passing time. After learning, event representations in the anterior hippocampus reflected temporal relations based on constructed times. Temporal relations were generalized across sequences, revealing distinct representational formats for events from the same or different sequences. Structural knowledge about time patterns, abstracted from different sequences, biased the construction of specific event times. These findings demonstrate that mnemonic construction and the generalization of relational knowledge combine in the hippocampus, consistent with the simulation of scenarios from episodic details and structural knowledge.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jacob L S Bellmund
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany.
| | - Lorena Deuker
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Nicole D Montijn
- Department of Clinical Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Christian F Doeller
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany.
- Kavli Institute for Systems Neuroscience, Centre for Neural Computation, The Egil and Pauline Braathen and Fred Kavli Centre for Cortical Microcircuits, Jebsen Centre for Alzheimer's Disease, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway.
- Wilhelm Wundt Institute of Psychology, Leipzig University, Leipzig, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
108
|
Lee H, Wang Z, Tillekeratne A, Lukish N, Puliyadi V, Zeger S, Gallagher M, Knierim JJ. Loss of functional heterogeneity along the CA3 transverse axis in aging. Curr Biol 2022; 32:2681-2693.e4. [PMID: 35597233 PMCID: PMC9233142 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2022.04.077] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/09/2021] [Revised: 04/18/2022] [Accepted: 04/26/2022] [Indexed: 01/05/2023]
Abstract
Age-related deficits in pattern separation have been postulated to bias the output of hippocampal memory processing toward pattern completion, which can cause deficits in accurate memory retrieval. Although the CA3 region of the hippocampus is often conceptualized as a homogeneous network involved in pattern completion, growing evidence demonstrates a functional gradient in CA3 along the transverse axis, as pattern-separated outputs (dominant in the more proximal CA3) transition to pattern-completed outputs (dominant in the more distal CA3). We examined the neural representations along the CA3 transverse axis in young (Y), aged memory-unimpaired (AU), and aged memory-impaired (AI) rats when different changes were made to the environment. Functional heterogeneity in CA3 was observed in Y and AU rats when the environmental similarity was high (altered cues or altered environment shapes in the same room), with more orthogonalized representations in proximal CA3 than in distal CA3. In contrast, AI rats showed reduced orthogonalization in proximal CA3 but showed normal (i.e., generalized) representations in distal CA3, with little evidence of a functional gradient. Under experimental conditions when the environmental similarity was low (different rooms), representations in proximal and distal CA3 remapped in all rats, showing that CA3 of AI rats is able to encode distinctive representations for inputs with greater dissimilarity. These experiments support the hypotheses that the age-related bias toward hippocampal pattern completion is due to the loss in AI rats of the normal transition from pattern separation to pattern completion along the CA3 transverse axis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Heekyung Lee
- Krieger Mind/Brain Institute, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, 21218,Correspondence: ;
| | - Zitong Wang
- Department of Biostatistics, Johns Hopkins University Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, 21205
| | - Arjuna Tillekeratne
- Krieger Mind/Brain Institute, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, 21218
| | - Nick Lukish
- Krieger Mind/Brain Institute, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, 21218
| | - Vyash Puliyadi
- Krieger Mind/Brain Institute, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, 21218,Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD
| | - Scott Zeger
- Department of Biostatistics, Johns Hopkins University Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, 21205
| | - Michela Gallagher
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD,Kavli Neuroscience Discovery Institute, Johns Hopkins University
| | - James J. Knierim
- Krieger Mind/Brain Institute, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, 21218,Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD,Kavli Neuroscience Discovery Institute, Johns Hopkins University,Solomon H. Snyder Department of Neuroscience, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, 21205,Lead Contact,Correspondence: ;
| |
Collapse
|
109
|
Fountas Z, Sylaidi A, Nikiforou K, Seth AK, Shanahan M, Roseboom W. A Predictive Processing Model of Episodic Memory and Time Perception. Neural Comput 2022; 34:1501-1544. [PMID: 35671462 DOI: 10.1162/neco_a_01514] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/02/2021] [Accepted: 03/06/2022] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Human perception and experience of time are strongly influenced by ongoing stimulation, memory of past experiences, and required task context. When paying attention to time, time experience seems to expand; when distracted, it seems to contract. When considering time based on memory, the experience may be different than what is in the moment, exemplified by sayings like "time flies when you're having fun." Experience of time also depends on the content of perceptual experience-rapidly changing or complex perceptual scenes seem longer in duration than less dynamic ones. The complexity of interactions among attention, memory, and perceptual stimulation is a likely reason that an overarching theory of time perception has been difficult to achieve. Here, we introduce a model of perceptual processing and episodic memory that makes use of hierarchical predictive coding, short-term plasticity, spatiotemporal attention, and episodic memory formation and recall, and apply this model to the problem of human time perception. In an experiment with approximately 13,000 human participants, we investigated the effects of memory, cognitive load, and stimulus content on duration reports of dynamic natural scenes up to about 1 minute long. Using our model to generate duration estimates, we compared human and model performance. Model-based estimates replicated key qualitative biases, including differences by cognitive load (attention), scene type (stimulation), and whether the judgment was made based on current or remembered experience (memory). Our work provides a comprehensive model of human time perception and a foundation for exploring the computational basis of episodic memory within a hierarchical predictive coding framework.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zafeirios Fountas
- Emotech Labs, London, N1 7EU U.K.,Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, Institute of Neurology, University College London, London WC1N 3AR, U.K.
| | | | | | - Anil K Seth
- Department of Informatics and Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, University of Sussex, Brighton, BN1 9RH, U.K.,Canadian Institute for Advanced Research Program on Brain, Mind, and Consciousness, Toronto, ON M5G 1M1, Canada
| | - Murray Shanahan
- Department of Computing, Imperial College London, London, SW7 2RH, U.K.
| | - Warrick Roseboom
- Department of Informatics and Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, University of Sussex, Brighton BN1 9RH, U.K.
| |
Collapse
|
110
|
|
111
|
Nadasdy Z, Howell DHP, Török Á, Nguyen TP, Shen JY, Briggs DE, Modur PN, Buchanan RJ. Phase coding of spatial representations in the human entorhinal cortex. SCIENCE ADVANCES 2022; 8:eabm6081. [PMID: 35507662 PMCID: PMC9067922 DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.abm6081] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/28/2021] [Accepted: 03/15/2022] [Indexed: 06/14/2023]
Abstract
The grid-like activity pattern of cells in the mammalian entorhinal cortex provides an internal reference frame for allocentric self-localization. The same neurons maintain robust phase couplings with local field oscillations. We found that neurons of the human entorhinal cortex display consistent spatial and temporal phase locking between spikes and slow gamma band local field potentials (LFPs) during virtual navigation. The phase locking maintained an environment-specific map over time. The phase tuning of spikes to the slow gamma band LFP revealed spatially periodic phase grids with environment-dependent scaling and consistent alignment with the environment. Using a Bayesian decoding model, we could predict the avatar's position with near perfect accuracy and, to a lesser extent, that of heading direction as well. These results imply that the phase of spikes relative to spatially modulated gamma oscillations encode allocentric spatial positions. We posit that a joint spatiotemporal phase code can implement the combined neural representation of space and time in the human entorhinal cortex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zoltan Nadasdy
- Zeto Inc., Santa Clara, CA 95054, USA
- Department of Psychology, The University of Texas at Austin at Austin, Austin, TX 78712, USA
- Department of Cognitive Psychology, Eötvös Loránd University, 1064 Budapest, Hungary
| | - Daniel H. P. Howell
- Department of Psychology, The University of Texas at Austin at Austin, Austin, TX 78712, USA
- Department of Neuroscience, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX 78712, USA
| | - Ágoston Török
- Systems and Control Laboratory, Institute for Computer Science and Control, Hungarian Academy of Sciences, 1111 Budapest, Hungary
| | - T. Peter Nguyen
- School of Medicine, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX 77030, USA
| | - Jason Y. Shen
- Seton Brain and Spine Institute, Austin, TX 78701, USA
- Department of Neurology, Dell Medical School, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX 78712, USA
| | - Deborah E. Briggs
- Seton Brain and Spine Institute, Austin, TX 78701, USA
- Department of Neurology, Dell Medical School, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX 78712, USA
| | - Pradeep N. Modur
- Seton Brain and Spine Institute, Austin, TX 78701, USA
- Department of Neurology, Dell Medical School, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX 78712, USA
| | - Robert J. Buchanan
- Department of Psychology, The University of Texas at Austin at Austin, Austin, TX 78712, USA
- Seton Brain and Spine Institute, Austin, TX 78701, USA
- Department of Surgery, Dell Medical School, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX 78712, USA
- Department of Psychiatry, Dell Medical School, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX 78712, USA
| |
Collapse
|
112
|
Takehara-Nishiuchi K. Neuronal Code for Episodic Time in the Lateral Entorhinal Cortex. Front Integr Neurosci 2022; 16:899412. [PMID: 35573446 PMCID: PMC9099416 DOI: 10.3389/fnint.2022.899412] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/18/2022] [Accepted: 04/11/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Kaori Takehara-Nishiuchi
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
- Department of Cell and Systems Biology, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
- Neuroscience Program, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
- *Correspondence: Kaori Takehara-Nishiuchi
| |
Collapse
|
113
|
Ye Z, Heldmann M, Herrmann L, Brüggemann N, Münte TF. Altered alpha and theta oscillations correlate with sequential working memory in Parkinson's disease. Brain Commun 2022; 4:fcac096. [PMID: 35755636 PMCID: PMC9214782 DOI: 10.1093/braincomms/fcac096] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/22/2021] [Revised: 12/02/2021] [Accepted: 04/09/2022] [Indexed: 12/02/2022] Open
Abstract
Daily activities such as preparing a meal rely on the ability to arrange thoughts and actions in the right order. Patients with Parkinson's disease have difficulties in sequencing tasks. Their deficits in sequential working memory have been associated with basal ganglia dysfunction. Here we demonstrate that altered parietal alpha and theta oscillations correlate with sequential working memory in Parkinson's disease. We included 15 patients with Parkinson's disease (6 women, mean age: 66.0 years), 24 healthy young (14 women, mean age: 24.1 years), and 16 older participants (7 women, mean age: 68.6 years). All participants completed a picture ordering task with scalp electroencephalogram (EEG) recording, where they arranged five pictures in a specific order and memorized them over a delay. When encoding and maintaining picture sequences, patients with Parkinson's disease showed a lower baseline alpha peak frequency with higher alpha power than healthy young and older participants. Patients with a higher baseline alpha power responded more slowly for ordered trials. When manipulating picture sequences, patients with Parkinson's disease showed a lower frequency of maximal power change for random versus ordered trials than healthy young and older participants. Healthy older participants showed a higher frequency of maximal power change than healthy young participants. Compared with patients with frequency of maximal power change in the alpha band (8-15 Hz), patients with frequency of maximal power change in the theta band (4-7 Hz) showed a higher ordering-related accuracy cost (random versus ordered) in the main task and tended to respond more slowly and less accurately in an independent working memory test. In conclusion, altered baseline alpha oscillations and task-dependent modulation of alpha and theta oscillations may be neural markers of poor sequential working memory in Parkinson's disease.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zheng Ye
- Institute of Neuroscience, Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shanghai 200031, China
| | - Marcus Heldmann
- Department of Neurology, University of Lübeck, Lübeck 23538, Germany
- Institute of Psychologie II, University of Lübeck, Lübeck 23538, Germany
| | - Lisa Herrmann
- Department of Neurology, University of Lübeck, Lübeck 23538, Germany
| | - Norbert Brüggemann
- Department of Neurology, University of Lübeck, Lübeck 23538, Germany
- Institute of Neurogenetics, University of Lübeck, Lübeck 23538, Germany
| | - Thomas F Münte
- Department of Neurology, University of Lübeck, Lübeck 23538, Germany
- Institute of Psychologie II, University of Lübeck, Lübeck 23538, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
114
|
Persson BM, Ambrozova V, Duncan S, Wood ER, O’Connor AR, Ainge JA. Lateral entorhinal cortex lesions impair odor-context associative memory in male rats. J Neurosci Res 2022; 100:1030-1046. [PMID: 35187710 PMCID: PMC9302644 DOI: 10.1002/jnr.25027] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/27/2021] [Revised: 01/10/2022] [Accepted: 01/12/2022] [Indexed: 01/14/2023]
Abstract
Lateral entorhinal cortex (LEC) has been hypothesized to process nonspatial, item information that is combined with spatial information from medial entorhinal cortex to form episodic memories within the hippocampus. Recent studies, however, have demonstrated that LEC has a role in integrating features of episodic memory prior to the hippocampus. While the precise role of LEC is still unclear, anatomical studies show that LEC is ideally placed to be a hub integrating multisensory information. The current study tests whether the role of LEC in integrating information extends to long-term multimodal item-context associations. In Experiment 1, male rats were trained on a context-dependent odor discrimination task, where two different contexts served as the cue to the correct odor. Rats were pretrained on the task and then received either bilateral excitotoxic LEC or sham lesions. Following surgery, rats were tested on the previously learned odor-context associations. Control rats showed good memory for the previously learned association but rats with LEC lesions showed significantly impaired performance relative to both their own presurgery performance and to control rats. Experiment 2 went on to test whether impairments in Experiment 1 were the result of LEC lesions impairing either odor or context memory retention alone. Male rats were trained on simple odor and context discrimination tasks that did not require integration of features to solve. Following surgery, both LEC and control rats showed good memory for previously learned odors and contexts. These data show that LEC is critical for long-term odor-context associative memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bjorn M. Persson
- School of Psychology & NeuroscienceUniversity of St AndrewsSt AndrewsUK
| | | | - Stephen Duncan
- School of Psychology & NeuroscienceUniversity of St AndrewsSt AndrewsUK
| | - Emma R. Wood
- Centre for Discovery Brain SciencesUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
| | - Akira R. O’Connor
- School of Psychology & NeuroscienceUniversity of St AndrewsSt AndrewsUK
| | - James A. Ainge
- School of Psychology & NeuroscienceUniversity of St AndrewsSt AndrewsUK
| |
Collapse
|
115
|
Oltmer J, Slepneva N, Llamas Rodriguez J, Greve DN, Williams EM, Wang R, Champion SN, Lang-Orsini M, Nestor K, Fernandez-Ros N, Fischl B, Frosch MP, Magnain C, van der Kouwe AJW, Augustinack JC. Quantitative and histologically validated measures of the entorhinal subfields in ex vivo MRI. Brain Commun 2022; 4:fcac074. [PMID: 35620167 PMCID: PMC9128374 DOI: 10.1093/braincomms/fcac074] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/11/2021] [Revised: 01/04/2022] [Accepted: 03/17/2022] [Indexed: 12/15/2022] Open
Abstract
Neuroimaging studies have routinely used hippocampal volume as a measure of Alzheimer's disease severity, but hippocampal changes occur too late in the disease process for potential therapies to be effective. The entorhinal cortex is one of the first cortical areas affected by Alzheimer's disease; its neurons are especially vulnerable to neurofibrillary tangles. Entorhinal atrophy also relates to the conversion from non-clinical to clinical Alzheimer's disease. In neuroimaging, the human entorhinal cortex has so far mostly been considered in its entirety or divided into a medial and a lateral region. Cytoarchitectonic differences provide the opportunity for subfield parcellation. We investigated the entorhinal cortex on a subfield-specific level-at a critical time point of Alzheimer's disease progression. While MRI allows multidimensional quantitative measurements, only histology provides enough accuracy to determine subfield boundaries-the pre-requisite for quantitative measurements within the entorhinal cortex. This study used histological data to validate ultra-high-resolution 7 Tesla ex vivo MRI and create entorhinal subfield parcellations in a total of 10 pre-clinical Alzheimer's disease and normal control cases. Using ex vivo MRI, eight entorhinal subfields (olfactory, rostral, medial intermediate, intermediate, lateral rostral, lateral caudal, caudal, and caudal limiting) were characterized for cortical thickness, volume, and pial surface area. Our data indicated no influence of sex, or Braak and Braak staging on volume, cortical thickness, or pial surface area. The volume and pial surface area for mean whole entorhinal cortex were 1131 ± 55.72 mm3 and 429 ± 22.6 mm2 (mean ± SEM), respectively. The subfield volume percentages relative to the entire entorhinal cortex were olfactory: 18.73 ± 1.82%, rostral: 14.06 ± 0.63%, lateral rostral: 14.81 ± 1.22%, medial intermediate: 6.72 ± 0.72%, intermediate: 23.36 ± 1.85%, lateral caudal: 5.42 ± 0.33%, caudal: 10.99 ± 1.02%, and caudal limiting: 5.91 ± 0.40% (all mean ± SEM). Olfactory and intermediate subfield revealed the most extensive intra-individual variability (cross-subject variance) in volume and pial surface area. This study provides validated measures. It maps individuality and demonstrates human variability in the entorhinal cortex, providing a baseline for approaches in individualized medicine. Taken together, this study serves as a ground-truth validation study for future in vivo comparisons and treatments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jan Oltmer
- Department of Radiology, Athinoula A. Martinos
Center, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA,Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA,
USA
| | - Natalya Slepneva
- Department of Radiology, Athinoula A. Martinos
Center, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA
| | - Josue Llamas Rodriguez
- Department of Radiology, Athinoula A. Martinos
Center, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA
| | - Douglas N. Greve
- Department of Radiology, Athinoula A. Martinos
Center, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA,Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA,
USA
| | - Emily M. Williams
- Department of Radiology, Athinoula A. Martinos
Center, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA
| | - Ruopeng Wang
- Department of Radiology, Athinoula A. Martinos
Center, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA
| | | | - Melanie Lang-Orsini
- Department of Neuropathology, Massachusetts General
Hospital, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Kimberly Nestor
- Department of Radiology, Athinoula A. Martinos
Center, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA
| | - Nídia Fernandez-Ros
- Department of Radiology, Athinoula A. Martinos
Center, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA
| | - Bruce Fischl
- Department of Radiology, Athinoula A. Martinos
Center, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA,Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA,
USA,CSAIL, Cambridge, MA, USA
| | - Matthew P. Frosch
- Department of Neuropathology, Massachusetts General
Hospital, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Caroline Magnain
- Department of Radiology, Athinoula A. Martinos
Center, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA,Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA,
USA
| | - Andre J. W. van der Kouwe
- Department of Radiology, Athinoula A. Martinos
Center, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA,Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA,
USA
| | - Jean C. Augustinack
- Department of Radiology, Athinoula A. Martinos
Center, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA,Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA,
USA,Correspondence to: Jean C. Augustinack Department of
Radiology Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging Massachusetts
General Hospital Building 149, 13th St Room 2301 Charlestown, MA 02129, USA
E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
116
|
Assouline A, Mendelsohn A, Reshef A. Memory-directed acupuncture as a neuromodulatory treatment for PTSD: Theory, clinical model and case studies. Transl Psychiatry 2022; 12:110. [PMID: 35296636 PMCID: PMC8927413 DOI: 10.1038/s41398-022-01876-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/19/2021] [Revised: 02/21/2022] [Accepted: 02/24/2022] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) poses an ongoing challenge to society, to health systems, and to the trauma victims themselves. Today PTSD is often considered an incurable chronic problem that lacks effective treatment. While PTSD is closely related to memory, it also affects many physiological systems. PTSD is usually treated with medications and psychotherapy with moderate success, leaving a substantial proportion of patients with enduring distress and disability. Therefore, a search for better treatment options is vital. In this paper, we propose a model in which a conversation-based technique is integrated with bodily manipulation through acupuncture. This approach first emerged in clinical experience showing intriguing results from treating PTSD patients using acupuncture as a main strategy. Its theoretical foundations derive from the clinic and rely on contemporary neuroscience's understanding of memory consolidation and reconsolidation processes. Research shows that acupuncture can have potentially positive effects at three levels: (a) achieving a balance between sympathetic and parasympathetic neural activity; (b) reducing activation in the limbic system, hence inducing a calming effect; (c) reshaping the functional connectivity map within important and relevant cortical regions that encompass the default-mode network. We suggest that coupling traumatic memory retrieval leading to reconsolidation, combined with acupuncture, offers considerable potential for positive clinical improvement in patients with PTSD. This may explain the positive results of the described case studies and can pave the path for future advances in research and treatment in this field.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Amir Assouline
- Sagol Department of Neurobiology, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel.
- Institute for Information Processing and Decision Making, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel.
| | - Avi Mendelsohn
- Sagol Department of Neurobiology, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
- Institute for Information Processing and Decision Making, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
| | - Alon Reshef
- Ha'Emek Medical Center, Department of Psychiatry, Afula, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
117
|
Lin C, Oh MM, Disterhoft JF. Aging-Related Alterations to Persistent Firing in the Lateral Entorhinal Cortex Contribute to Deficits in Temporal Associative Memory. Front Aging Neurosci 2022; 14:838513. [PMID: 35360205 PMCID: PMC8963507 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.838513] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/17/2021] [Accepted: 02/07/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
With aging comes a myriad of different disorders, and cognitive decline is one of them. Studies have consistently shown a decline amongst aged subjects in their ability to acquire and maintain temporal associative memory. Defined as the memory of the association between two objects that are separated in time, temporal associative memory is dependent on neocortical structures such as the prefrontal cortex and temporal lobe structures. For this memory to be acquired, a mental trace of the first stimulus is necessary to bridge the temporal gap so the two stimuli can be properly associated. Persistent firing, the ability of the neuron to continue to fire action potentials even after the termination of a triggering stimulus, is one mechanism that is posited to support this mental trace. A recent study demonstrated a decline in persistent firing ability in pyramidal neurons of layer III of the lateral entorhinal cortex with aging, contributing to learning impairments in temporal associative memory acquisition. In this work, we explore the potential ways persistent firing in lateral entorhinal cortex (LEC) III supports temporal associative memory, and how aging may disrupt this mechanism within the temporal lobe system, resulting in impairment in this crucial behavior.
Collapse
|
118
|
Lim HY, Ahn JR, Lee I. The Interaction of Cue Type and Its Associated Behavioral Response Dissociates the Neural Activity between the Perirhinal and Postrhinal Cortices. eNeuro 2022; 9:ENEURO.0065-22.2022. [PMID: 35422417 PMCID: PMC9045475 DOI: 10.1523/eneuro.0065-22.2022] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/08/2022] [Revised: 04/04/2022] [Accepted: 04/08/2022] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
The perirhinal cortex (PER) and postrhinal cortex (POR) in the medial temporal lobe are commonly described as two distinct systems that process nonspatial and spatial information, respectively. Recent findings suggest that the two regions exhibit functional overlap when processing stimulus information, especially when associative responses are required in goal-directed behavior. However, we lack the neural correlates of this. In the current study, we recorded spiking activities for single units of the PER and POR as rats were required to choose a response associated with the identity of a visual object or scene stimulus. We found that similar proportions of cells fired selectively for either scene or object between the two regions. In the PER and POR, response-selective neurons showed higher contrast for different responses than stimulus-selective cells did for stimuli. More cells fired selectively for specific choice response in the POR than in the PER. The differential firing patterns of the PER and POR were best explained when the stimulus and response components were considered together: Stimulus-selective cells were modulated more by the response in the POR than in the PER, whereas response-selective cells in the PER were modulated more by object information than by scenes. Our results suggest that in a goal-directed memory task, the information processing in the PER and POR may be dynamically modulated not only by input stimulus information but also by the associated choice behavior and stimulus-response interaction.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Heung-Yeol Lim
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Seoul National University, Seoul 08826, Korea
| | - Jae-Rong Ahn
- Department of Biology, Tufts University, Medford, Massachusetts 02155
| | - Inah Lee
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Seoul National University, Seoul 08826, Korea
| |
Collapse
|
119
|
Liu Y, Levy S, Mau W, Geva N, Rubin A, Ziv Y, Hasselmo M, Howard M. Consistent population activity on the scale of minutes in the mouse hippocampus. Hippocampus 2022; 32:359-372. [PMID: 35225408 PMCID: PMC10085730 DOI: 10.1002/hipo.23409] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/19/2021] [Revised: 01/05/2022] [Accepted: 01/31/2022] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
Neurons in the hippocampus fire in consistent sequence over the timescale of seconds during the delay period of some memory experiments. For longer timescales, the firing of hippocampal neurons also changes slowly over minutes within experimental sessions. It was thought that these slow dynamics are caused by stochastic drift or a continuous change in the representation of the episode, rather than consistent sequences unfolding over minutes. This paper studies the consistency of contextual drift in three chronic calcium imaging recordings from the hippocampus CA1 region in mice. Computational measures of consistency show reliable sequences within experimental trials at the scale of seconds as one would expect from time cells or place cells during the trial, as well as across experimental trials on the scale of minutes within a recording session. Consistent sequences in the hippocampus are observed over a wide range of time scales, from seconds to minutes. The hippocampal activity could reflect a scale-invariant spatiotemporal context as suggested by theories of memory from cognitive psychology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yue Liu
- Department of Physics, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA.,Center for Systems Neuroscience, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA.,Center for Memory and Brain, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
| | - Samuel Levy
- Center for Systems Neuroscience, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA.,Center for Memory and Brain, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA.,Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
| | - William Mau
- Center for Systems Neuroscience, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA.,Center for Memory and Brain, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA.,Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
| | - Nitzan Geva
- Department of Neurobiology, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot, Israel
| | - Alon Rubin
- Department of Neurobiology, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot, Israel
| | - Yaniv Ziv
- Department of Neurobiology, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot, Israel
| | - Michael Hasselmo
- Center for Systems Neuroscience, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA.,Center for Memory and Brain, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA.,Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
| | - Marc Howard
- Department of Physics, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA.,Center for Systems Neuroscience, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA.,Center for Memory and Brain, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA.,Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
| |
Collapse
|
120
|
Lopez-Rojas J, de Solis CA, Leroy F, Kandel ER, Siegelbaum SA. A direct lateral entorhinal cortex to hippocampal CA2 circuit conveys social information required for social memory. Neuron 2022; 110:1559-1572.e4. [PMID: 35180391 PMCID: PMC9081137 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2022.01.028] [Citation(s) in RCA: 48] [Impact Index Per Article: 24.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/02/2021] [Revised: 12/21/2021] [Accepted: 01/24/2022] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
The hippocampus is essential for different forms of declarative memory, including social memory, the ability to recognize and remember a conspecific. Although recent studies identify the importance of the dorsal CA2 region of the hippocampus in social memory storage, little is known about its sources of social information. Because CA2, like other hippocampal regions, receives its major source of spatial and non-spatial information from the medial and lateral subdivisions of entorhinal cortex (MEC and LEC), respectively, we investigated the importance of these inputs for social memory. Whereas MEC inputs to CA2 are dispensable, the direct inputs to CA2 from LEC are both selectively activated during social exploration and required for social memory. This selective behavioral role of LEC is reflected in the stronger excitatory drive it provides to CA2 compared with MEC. Thus, a direct LEC → CA2 circuit is tuned to convey social information that is critical for social memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jeffrey Lopez-Rojas
- Department of Neuroscience, The Kavli Institute for Brain Science, Mortimer B. Zuckerman Mind Brain Behavior Institute, Vagelos College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027, USA.
| | - Christopher A de Solis
- Department of Neuroscience, The Kavli Institute for Brain Science, Mortimer B. Zuckerman Mind Brain Behavior Institute, Vagelos College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027, USA
| | - Felix Leroy
- Department of Neuroscience, The Kavli Institute for Brain Science, Mortimer B. Zuckerman Mind Brain Behavior Institute, Vagelos College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027, USA; Instituto de Neurociencias CSIC-UMH, San Juan de Alicante, Spain
| | - Eric R Kandel
- Department of Neuroscience, The Kavli Institute for Brain Science, Mortimer B. Zuckerman Mind Brain Behavior Institute, Vagelos College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027, USA; Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| | - Steven A Siegelbaum
- Department of Neuroscience, The Kavli Institute for Brain Science, Mortimer B. Zuckerman Mind Brain Behavior Institute, Vagelos College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
121
|
Hippocampal ensembles represent sequential relationships among an extended sequence of nonspatial events. Nat Commun 2022; 13:787. [PMID: 35136052 PMCID: PMC8825855 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-022-28057-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/31/2020] [Accepted: 12/28/2021] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
The hippocampus is critical to the temporal organization of our experiences. Although this fundamental capacity is conserved across modalities and species, its underlying neuronal mechanisms remain unclear. Here we recorded hippocampal activity as rats remembered an extended sequence of nonspatial events unfolding over several seconds, as in daily life episodes in humans. We then developed statistical machine learning methods to analyze the ensemble activity and discovered forms of sequential organization and coding important for order memory judgments. Specifically, we found that hippocampal ensembles provide significant temporal coding throughout nonspatial event sequences, differentiate distinct types of task-critical information sequentially within events, and exhibit theta-associated reactivation of the sequential relationships among events. We also demonstrate that nonspatial event representations are sequentially organized within individual theta cycles and precess across successive cycles. These findings suggest a fundamental function of the hippocampal network is to encode, preserve, and predict the sequential order of experiences. It remains unclear how hippocampal activity supports the temporal organization of our experiences. In this paper, the authors recorded from rats performing an odor sequence task and show that hippocampal ensembles represent the sequential relations among nonspatial events at different timescales.
Collapse
|
122
|
The Entorhinal Cortex as a Gateway for Amygdala Influences on Memory Consolidation. Neuroscience 2022; 497:86-96. [PMID: 35122874 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2022.01.023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/14/2021] [Revised: 01/26/2022] [Accepted: 01/27/2022] [Indexed: 12/16/2022]
Abstract
The amygdala, specifically its basolateral nucleus (BLA), is a critical site integrating neuromodulatory influences on memory consolidation in other brain areas. Almost 20 years ago, we reported the first direct evidence that BLA activity is required for modulatory interventions in the entorhinal cortex (EC) to affect memory consolidation (Roesler, Roozendaal, and McGaugh, 2002). Since then, significant advances have been made in our understanding of how the EC participates in memory. For example, the characterization of grid cells specialized in processing spatial information in the medial EC (mEC) that act as major relayers of information to the hippocampus (HIP) has changed our view of memory processing by the EC; and the development of optogenetic technologies for manipulation of neuronal activity has recently enabled important new discoveries on the role of the BLA projections to the EC in memory. Here, we review the current evidence on interactions between the BLA and EC in synaptic plasticity and memory formation. The findings suggest that the EC may function as a gateway and mediator of modulatory influences from the BLA, which are then processed and relayed to the HIP. Through extensive reciprocal connections among the EC, HIP, and several cortical areas, information related to new memories is then consolidated by these multiple brain systems, through various molecular and cellular mechanisms acting in a distributed and highly concerted manner, during several hours after learning. A special note is made on the contribution by Ivan Izquierdo to our understanding of memory consolidation at the brain system level.
Collapse
|
123
|
Echeveste R, Ferrante E, Milone DH, Samengo I. Bridging physiological and perceptual views of autism by means of sampling-based Bayesian inference. Netw Neurosci 2022; 6:196-212. [PMID: 36605888 PMCID: PMC9810278 DOI: 10.1162/netn_a_00219] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/27/2021] [Accepted: 12/01/2021] [Indexed: 01/09/2023] Open
Abstract
Theories for autism spectrum disorder (ASD) have been formulated at different levels, ranging from physiological observations to perceptual and behavioral descriptions. Understanding the physiological underpinnings of perceptual traits in ASD remains a significant challenge in the field. Here we show how a recurrent neural circuit model that was optimized to perform sampling-based inference and displays characteristic features of cortical dynamics can help bridge this gap. The model was able to establish a mechanistic link between two descriptive levels for ASD: a physiological level, in terms of inhibitory dysfunction, neural variability, and oscillations, and a perceptual level, in terms of hypopriors in Bayesian computations. We took two parallel paths-inducing hypopriors in the probabilistic model, and an inhibitory dysfunction in the network model-which lead to consistent results in terms of the represented posteriors, providing support for the view that both descriptions might constitute two sides of the same coin.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rodrigo Echeveste
- Research Institute for Signals, Systems, and Computational Intelligence sinc(i) (FICH-UNL/CONICET), Santa Fe, Argentina,* Corresponding Author:
| | - Enzo Ferrante
- Research Institute for Signals, Systems, and Computational Intelligence sinc(i) (FICH-UNL/CONICET), Santa Fe, Argentina
| | - Diego H. Milone
- Research Institute for Signals, Systems, and Computational Intelligence sinc(i) (FICH-UNL/CONICET), Santa Fe, Argentina
| | - Inés Samengo
- Medical Physics Department and Balseiro Institute (CNEA-UNCUYO/CONICET), Bariloche, Argentina
| |
Collapse
|
124
|
Blankvoort S, Olsen LC, Kentros CG. Single Cell Transcriptomic and Chromatin Profiles Suggest Layer Vb Is the Only Layer With Shared Excitatory Cell Types in the Medial and Lateral Entorhinal Cortex. Front Neural Circuits 2022; 15:806154. [PMID: 35153682 PMCID: PMC8826650 DOI: 10.3389/fncir.2021.806154] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/31/2021] [Accepted: 12/06/2021] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
All brain functionality arises from the activity in neural circuits in different anatomical regions. These regions contain different circuits comprising unique cell types. An integral part to understanding neural circuits is a full census of the constituent parts, i.e., the neural cell types. This census can be based on different characteristics. Previously combinations of morphology and physiology, gene expression, and chromatin accessibility have been used in various cortical and subcortical regions. This has given an extensive yet incomplete overview of neural cell types. However, these techniques have not been applied to all brain regions. Here we apply single cell analysis of accessible chromatin on two similar but different cortical regions, the medial and the lateral entorhinal cortices. Even though these two regions are anatomically similar, their intrinsic and extrinsic connectivity are different. In 4,136 cells we identify 20 different clusters representing different cell types. As expected, excitatory cells show regionally specific clusters, whereas inhibitory neurons are shared between regions. We find that several deep layer excitatory neuronal cell types as defined by chromatin profile are also shared between the two different regions. Integration with a larger scRNA-seq dataset maintains this shared characteristic for cells in Layer Vb. Interestingly, this layer contains three clusters, two specific to either subregion and one shared between the two. These clusters can be putatively associated with particular functional and anatomical cell types found in this layer. This information is a step forwards into elucidating the cell types within the entorhinal circuit and by extension its functional underpinnings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stefan Blankvoort
- Kavli Institute for Systems Neuroscience and Centre for Neural Computation, NTNU, Trondheim, Norway
- *Correspondence: Stefan Blankvoort
| | - Lene Christin Olsen
- Department of Clinical and Molecular Medicine, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway
- BioCore Bioinformatics Core Facility, NTNU, Trondheim, Norway
- Department of Neurology, St. Olavs Hospital, Trondheim, Norway
| | - Clifford G. Kentros
- Kavli Institute for Systems Neuroscience and Centre for Neural Computation, NTNU, Trondheim, Norway
| |
Collapse
|
125
|
Rolls ET, Deco G, Huang CC, Feng J. The Effective Connectivity of the Human Hippocampal Memory System. Cereb Cortex 2022; 32:3706-3725. [PMID: 35034120 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhab442] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/13/2021] [Revised: 11/05/2021] [Accepted: 11/08/2021] [Indexed: 02/04/2023] Open
Abstract
Effective connectivity measurements in the human hippocampal memory system based on the resting-state blood oxygenation-level dependent signal were made in 172 participants in the Human Connectome Project to reveal the directionality and strength of the connectivity. A ventral "what" hippocampal stream involves the temporal lobe cortex, perirhinal and parahippocampal TF cortex, and entorhinal cortex. A dorsal "where" hippocampal stream connects parietal cortex with posterior and retrosplenial cingulate cortex, and with parahippocampal TH cortex, which, in turn, project to the presubiculum, which connects to the hippocampus. A third stream involves the orbitofrontal and ventromedial-prefrontal cortex with effective connectivity with the hippocampal, entorhinal, and perirhinal cortex. There is generally stronger forward connectivity to the hippocampus than backward. Thus separate "what," "where," and "reward" streams can converge in the hippocampus, from which back projections return to the sources. However, unlike the simple dual stream hippocampal model, there is a third stream related to reward value; there is some cross-connectivity between these systems before the hippocampus is reached; and the hippocampus has some effective connectivity with earlier stages of processing than the entorhinal cortex and presubiculum. These findings complement diffusion tractography and provide a foundation for new concepts on the operation of the human hippocampal memory system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Edmund T Rolls
- Oxford Centre for Computational Neuroscience, Oxford, UK
- Department of Computer Science, University of Warwick, Coventry CV4 7AL, UK
- Institute of Science and Technology for Brain Inspired Intelligence, Fudan University, Shanghai 200433, China
| | - Gustavo Deco
- Department of Information and Communication Technologies, Center for Brain and Cognition, Computational Neuroscience Group, Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Barcelona 08018, Spain
- Brain and Cognition, Pompeu Fabra University, Barcelona 08018, Spain
- Institució Catalana de la Recerca i Estudis Avançats (ICREA), Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Barcelona 08010, Spain
| | - Chu-Chung Huang
- Shanghai Key Laboratory of Brain Functional Genomics (Ministry of Education), School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, Shanghai 200062, China
| | - Jianfeng Feng
- Department of Computer Science, University of Warwick, Coventry CV4 7AL, UK
- Institute of Science and Technology for Brain Inspired Intelligence, Fudan University, Shanghai 200433, China
| |
Collapse
|
126
|
Ohana O, Alberini CM, Donato F. Introduction to the special issue on the ontogeny of hippocampal functions. Hippocampus 2022; 32:69-72. [PMID: 35005808 PMCID: PMC9303776 DOI: 10.1002/hipo.23406] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Ora Ohana
- Institute for Molecular and Cellular Cognition, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany
| | | | - Flavio Donato
- Biozentrum of the University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
127
|
Park SB, Lim HY, Lee EY, Yoo SW, Jung HS, Lee E, Sun W, Lee I. The fasciola cinereum subregion of the hippocampus is important for the acquisition of visual contextual memory. Prog Neurobiol 2022; 210:102217. [PMID: 34999186 DOI: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2022.102217] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/01/2021] [Revised: 11/29/2021] [Accepted: 01/04/2022] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
The fasciola cinereum (FC) is a subregion of the hippocampus that has received relatively little attention compared with other hippocampal subregions with respect to anatomical characteristics and functional significance. Here, we show that the FC exhibits clear anatomical borders with the distalmost region of the CA1. Principal neurons in the FC resemble the granule cells in the dentate gyrus (DG). However, adult neurogenesis was not found unlike in the DG. The FC receives inputs mostly from the lateral entorhinal cortex and perirhinal cortex while projecting exclusively to the crest of the DG within the hippocampus. Neurotoxic lesions in the FC using colchicine impaired the acquisition, but not retrieval, of visual contextual memory in rats. FC lesions also impaired place recognition and object-in-place memory. As the rat performed the contextual memory task on the T-maze, place cells in the FC exhibited robust place fields and were indiscriminable from those in CA1 with respect to the basic firing properties. However, place cells in the FC fired only transiently in their place fields on the maze compared with those in CA1. Our findings suggest that the episodic firing pattern of the place cells in the FC may play critical roles in learning a novel contextual environment by facilitating temoporally structured contextual pattern separation in the DG of the hippocampus.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Seong-Beom Park
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Seoul National University, Gwanak-ro 1, Shillim-dong, Gwanak-gu, Seoul, 08826, Republic of Korea
| | - Heung-Yeol Lim
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Seoul National University, Gwanak-ro 1, Shillim-dong, Gwanak-gu, Seoul, 08826, Republic of Korea
| | - Eun-Young Lee
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Seoul National University, Gwanak-ro 1, Shillim-dong, Gwanak-gu, Seoul, 08826, Republic of Korea
| | - Seung-Woo Yoo
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Seoul National University, Gwanak-ro 1, Shillim-dong, Gwanak-gu, Seoul, 08826, Republic of Korea
| | - Hyun-Suk Jung
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Seoul National University, Gwanak-ro 1, Shillim-dong, Gwanak-gu, Seoul, 08826, Republic of Korea
| | - Eunsoo Lee
- Department of Anatomy, College of Medicine, Korea University, Anam-dong 5, Seongbuk-gu, Seoul, 02841, Republic of Korea
| | - Woong Sun
- Department of Anatomy, College of Medicine, Korea University, Anam-dong 5, Seongbuk-gu, Seoul, 02841, Republic of Korea
| | - Inah Lee
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Seoul National University, Gwanak-ro 1, Shillim-dong, Gwanak-gu, Seoul, 08826, Republic of Korea.
| |
Collapse
|
128
|
Houser TM. Spatialization of Time in the Entorhinal-Hippocampal System. Front Behav Neurosci 2022; 15:807197. [PMID: 35069143 PMCID: PMC8770534 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2021.807197] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/01/2021] [Accepted: 12/06/2021] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
The functional role of the entorhinal-hippocampal system has been a long withstanding mystery. One key theory that has become most popular is that the entorhinal-hippocampal system represents space to facilitate navigation in one's surroundings. In this Perspective article, I introduce a novel idea that undermines the inherent uniqueness of spatial information in favor of time driving entorhinal-hippocampal activity. Specifically, by spatializing events that occur in succession (i.e., across time), the entorhinal-hippocampal system is critical for all types of cognitive representations. I back up this argument with empirical evidence that hints at a role for the entorhinal-hippocampal system in non-spatial representation, and computational models of the logarithmic compression of time in the brain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Troy M. Houser
- Department of Psychology, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR, United States
| |
Collapse
|
129
|
Hus Y. Detecting Time Concept Competence in Children with Autism Spectrum and Attention Disorders. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 2022; 18:2323-2348. [PMID: 36276427 PMCID: PMC9579054 DOI: 10.2147/ndt.s331985] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/22/2022] [Accepted: 07/12/2022] [Indexed: 11/07/2022] Open
Abstract
The importance of time concept in human existence is "ancient history" celebrated in the biblical book Ecclesiastes. Indeed, our time-sensitive mechanisms are literally carved into our biology and neurology on a molecular level, gifting us with neural clocks. However, time in human consciousness is not the time indicated by physical clocks: time is a subjective reality in our psychological makeup due to the nature of the temporal neural mechanisms and unique properties of physical time. Nonetheless, subjective time requires anchoring to physical time which permeates our language, endeavors, and entire existence, a process hinging on time-related skills such as estimates and measures of passage and duration of time. Moreover, accurate time reading, a critical adaptive life-skill, is imperative for effective function in all societal activities. Because it embodies the complexity of the time construct, it is central to instruction of time concept in primary education. It is often measured in children by clock drawings, a cognitive integrative skill with errors pointing to neuroanatomical differences impacting the integrity of executive function. Time competence in children with atypical neurobiological development and high prevalence, as in autism spectrum disorders (ASD), and attention disorders (ADHD), is often compromised, calling for investigation of its function. This thematic review article aims to: 1) discuss the complexity of time concept and its underlying bio-neurological mechanisms, 2) elucidate difficulties children with ASD and those with ADHD exhibit in temporal development, and 3) demonstrate the use of a set of clinical tools in uncovering temporal competence and ecological executive function in two children with ASD, and a child with ADHD, using a clock drawing task and error analyses; children's time knowledge questionnaire; a behavior rating parent questionnaire examining ecological executive function, and parent open-ended questions related to their children's time difficulties. A discussion, directions, and a take-home message round out the article.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yvette Hus
- Cyprus University of Technology, Department of Rehabilitation Sciences, Theralab Research Collaborator Under Direction of Prof. Kakia Petinou, Limassol, Cyprus
| |
Collapse
|
130
|
Vandrey B, Armstrong J, Brown CM, Garden DLF, Nolan MF. Fan cells in lateral entorhinal cortex directly influence medial entorhinal cortex through synaptic connections in layer 1. eLife 2022; 11:83008. [PMID: 36562467 PMCID: PMC9822265 DOI: 10.7554/elife.83008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/26/2022] [Accepted: 12/19/2022] [Indexed: 12/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Standard models for spatial and episodic memory suggest that the lateral entorhinal cortex (LEC) and medial entorhinal cortex (MEC) send parallel independent inputs to the hippocampus, each carrying different types of information. Here, we evaluate the possibility that information is integrated between divisions of the entorhinal cortex prior to reaching the hippocampus. We demonstrate that, in mice, fan cells in layer 2 (L2) of LEC that receive neocortical inputs, and that project to the hippocampal dentate gyrus, also send axon collaterals to layer 1 (L1) of the MEC. Activation of inputs from fan cells evokes monosynaptic glutamatergic excitation of stellate and pyramidal cells in L2 of the MEC, typically followed by inhibition that contains fast and slow components mediated by GABAA and GABAB receptors, respectively. Inputs from fan cells also directly activate interneurons in L1 and L2 of MEC, with synaptic connections from L1 interneurons accounting for slow feedforward inhibition of L2 principal cell populations. The relative strength of excitation and inhibition following fan cell activation differs substantially between neurons and is largely independent of anatomical location. Our results demonstrate that the LEC, in addition to directly influencing the hippocampus, can activate or inhibit major hippocampal inputs arising from the MEC. Thus, local circuits in the superficial MEC may combine spatial information with sensory and higher order signals from the LEC, providing a substrate for integration of 'what' and 'where' components of episodic memories.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brianna Vandrey
- Centre for Discovery Brain Sciences, University of EdinburghEdinburghUnited Kingdom
| | - Jack Armstrong
- Centre for Discovery Brain Sciences, University of EdinburghEdinburghUnited Kingdom
| | - Christina M Brown
- Centre for Discovery Brain Sciences, University of EdinburghEdinburghUnited Kingdom
| | - Derek LF Garden
- Centre for Discovery Brain Sciences, University of EdinburghEdinburghUnited Kingdom
| | - Matthew F Nolan
- Centre for Discovery Brain Sciences, University of EdinburghEdinburghUnited Kingdom,Simons Initiative for the Developing Brain, University of EdinburghEdinburghUnited Kingdom,Centre for Statistics, University of EdinburghEdinburghUnited Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
131
|
Ohara S, Yoshino R, Kimura K, Kawamura T, Tanabe S, Zheng A, Nakamura S, Inoue KI, Takada M, Tsutsui KI, Witter MP. Laminar Organization of the Entorhinal Cortex in Macaque Monkeys Based on Cell-Type-Specific Markers and Connectivity. Front Neural Circuits 2021; 15:790116. [PMID: 34949991 PMCID: PMC8688913 DOI: 10.3389/fncir.2021.790116] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/06/2021] [Accepted: 11/16/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The entorhinal cortex (EC) is a major gateway between the hippocampus and telencephalic structures, and plays a critical role in memory and navigation. Through the use of various molecular markers and genetic tools, neuron types constituting EC are well studied in rodents, and their layer-dependent distributions, connections, and functions have also been characterized. In primates, however, such cell-type-specific understandings are lagging. To bridge the gap between rodents and primates, here we provide the first cell-type-based global map of EC in macaque monkeys. The laminar organization of the monkey EC was systematically examined and compared with that of the rodent EC by using immunohistochemistry for molecular markers which have been well characterized in the rodent EC: reelin, calbindin, and Purkinje cell protein 4 (PCP4). We further employed retrograde neuron labeling from the nucleus accumbens and amygdala to identify the EC output layer. This cell-type-based approach enabled us to apply the latest laminar definition of rodent EC to monkeys. Based on the similarity of the laminar organization, the monkey EC can be divided into two subdivisions: rostral and caudal EC. These subdivisions likely correspond to the lateral and medial EC in rodents, respectively. In addition, we found an overall absence of a clear laminar arrangement of layer V neurons in the rostral EC, unlike rodents. The cell-type-based architectural map provided in this study will accelerate the application of genetic tools in monkeys for better understanding of the role of EC in memory and navigation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shinya Ohara
- Laboratory of Systems Neuroscience, Graduate School of Life Sciences, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan.,PRESTO, Japan Science and Technology Agency (JST), Tokyo, Japan
| | - Rintaro Yoshino
- Laboratory of Systems Neuroscience, Graduate School of Life Sciences, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan
| | - Kei Kimura
- Systems Neuroscience Section, Department of Neuroscience, Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, Inuyama, Japan
| | - Taichi Kawamura
- Laboratory of Systems Neuroscience, Graduate School of Life Sciences, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan
| | - Soshi Tanabe
- Systems Neuroscience Section, Department of Neuroscience, Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, Inuyama, Japan
| | - Andi Zheng
- Systems Neuroscience Section, Department of Neuroscience, Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, Inuyama, Japan
| | - Shinya Nakamura
- Laboratory of Systems Neuroscience, Graduate School of Life Sciences, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan
| | - Ken-Ichi Inoue
- Systems Neuroscience Section, Department of Neuroscience, Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, Inuyama, Japan
| | - Masahiko Takada
- Systems Neuroscience Section, Department of Neuroscience, Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, Inuyama, Japan
| | - Ken-Ichiro Tsutsui
- Laboratory of Systems Neuroscience, Graduate School of Life Sciences, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan.,Laboratory of Systems Neuroscience, Graduate School of Medicine, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan
| | - Menno P Witter
- Laboratory of Systems Neuroscience, Graduate School of Life Sciences, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan.,Laboratory of Systems Neuroscience, Graduate School of Medicine, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan.,Department of Developmental Neuroscience, Graduate School of Medicine, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
132
|
Ma Q, Rolls ET, Huang CC, Cheng W, Feng J. Extensive cortical functional connectivity of the human hippocampal memory system. Cortex 2021; 147:83-101. [PMID: 35026557 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2021.11.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/05/2021] [Revised: 10/12/2021] [Accepted: 11/22/2021] [Indexed: 01/09/2023]
Abstract
The cortical connections of the human hippocampal memory system are fundamental to understanding its operation in health and disease, especially in the context of the great development of the human cortex. The functional connectivity of the human hippocampal system was analyzed in 172 participants imaged at 7T in the Human Connectome Project. The human hippocampus has high functional connectivity not only with the entorhinal cortex, but also with areas that are more distant in the ventral 'what' stream including the perirhinal cortex and temporal cortical visual areas. Parahippocampal gyrus TF in humans has connectivity with this ventral 'what' subsystem. Correspondingly for the dorsal stream, the hippocampus has high functional connectivity not only with the presubiculum, but also with areas more distant, the medial parahippocampal cortex TH which includes the parahippocampal place or scene area, the posterior cingulate including retrosplenial cortex, and the parietal cortex. Further, there is considerable cross connectivity between the ventral and dorsal streams with the hippocampus. The findings are supported by anatomical connections, which together provide an unprecedented and quantitative overview of the extensive cortical connectivity of the human hippocampal system that goes beyond hierarchically organised and segregated pathways connecting the hippocampus and neocortex, and leads to new concepts on the operation of the hippocampal memory system in humans.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Qing Ma
- Institute of Science and Technology for Brain Inspired Intelligence, Fudan University, Shanghai, China
| | - Edmund T Rolls
- Institute of Science and Technology for Brain Inspired Intelligence, Fudan University, Shanghai, China; Department of Computer Science, University of Warwick, Coventry, UK; Oxford Centre for Computational Neuroscience, Oxford, UK.
| | - Chu-Chung Huang
- Shanghai Key Laboratory of Brain Functional Genomics (Ministry of Education), School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China
| | - Wei Cheng
- Institute of Science and Technology for Brain Inspired Intelligence, Fudan University, Shanghai, China; Key Laboratory of Computational Neuroscience and Brain-Inspired Intelligence, Fudan University, Ministry of Education, Shanghai 200433, China; Fudan ISTBI-ZJNU Algorithm Centre for Brain-inspired Intelligence, Zhejiang Normal University, Jinhua, China.
| | - Jianfeng Feng
- Institute of Science and Technology for Brain Inspired Intelligence, Fudan University, Shanghai, China; Department of Computer Science, University of Warwick, Coventry, UK; Key Laboratory of Computational Neuroscience and Brain-Inspired Intelligence, Fudan University, Ministry of Education, Shanghai 200433, China; Fudan ISTBI-ZJNU Algorithm Centre for Brain-inspired Intelligence, Zhejiang Normal University, Jinhua, China.
| |
Collapse
|
133
|
Syversen IF, Witter MP, Kobro-Flatmoen A, Goa PE, Navarro Schröder T, Doeller CF. Structural connectivity-based segmentation of the human entorhinal cortex. Neuroimage 2021; 245:118723. [PMID: 34780919 PMCID: PMC8756143 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118723] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/30/2021] [Revised: 10/22/2021] [Accepted: 11/11/2021] [Indexed: 12/15/2022] Open
Abstract
The medial (MEC) and lateral entorhinal cortex (LEC), widely studied in rodents, are well defined and characterized. In humans, however, the exact locations of their homologues remain uncertain. Previous functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have subdivided the human EC into posteromedial (pmEC) and anterolateral (alEC) parts, but uncertainty remains about the choice of imaging modality and seed regions, in particular in light of a substantial revision of the classical model of EC connectivity based on novel insights from rodent anatomy. Here, we used structural, not functional imaging, namely diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and probabilistic tractography to segment the human EC based on differential connectivity to other brain regions known to project selectively to MEC or LEC. We defined MEC as more strongly connected with presubiculum and retrosplenial cortex (RSC), and LEC as more strongly connected with distal CA1 and proximal subiculum (dCA1pSub) and lateral orbitofrontal cortex (OFC). Although our DTI segmentation had a larger medial-lateral component than in the previous fMRI studies, our results show that the human MEC and LEC homologues have a border oriented both towards the posterior-anterior and medial-lateral axes, supporting the differentiation between pmEC and alEC.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ingrid Framås Syversen
- Kavli Institute for Systems Neuroscience, NTNU - Norwegian University of Science and Technology, MH, NTNU, Postbox 8905, Trondheim 7491, Norway.
| | - Menno P Witter
- Kavli Institute for Systems Neuroscience, NTNU - Norwegian University of Science and Technology, MH, NTNU, Postbox 8905, Trondheim 7491, Norway
| | - Asgeir Kobro-Flatmoen
- Kavli Institute for Systems Neuroscience, NTNU - Norwegian University of Science and Technology, MH, NTNU, Postbox 8905, Trondheim 7491, Norway
| | - Pål Erik Goa
- Department of Physics, NTNU - Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway
| | - Tobias Navarro Schröder
- Kavli Institute for Systems Neuroscience, NTNU - Norwegian University of Science and Technology, MH, NTNU, Postbox 8905, Trondheim 7491, Norway
| | - Christian F Doeller
- Kavli Institute for Systems Neuroscience, NTNU - Norwegian University of Science and Technology, MH, NTNU, Postbox 8905, Trondheim 7491, Norway; Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany; Institute of Psychology, Leipzig University, Leipzig, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
134
|
Do Q, Hasselmo ME. Neural circuits and symbolic processing. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2021; 186:107552. [PMID: 34763073 PMCID: PMC10121157 DOI: 10.1016/j.nlm.2021.107552] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/13/2021] [Revised: 10/14/2021] [Accepted: 11/02/2021] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
The ability to use symbols is a defining feature of human intelligence. However, neuroscience has yet to explain the fundamental neural circuit mechanisms for flexibly representing and manipulating abstract concepts. This article will review the research on neural models for symbolic processing. The review first focuses on the question of how symbols could possibly be represented in neural circuits. The review then addresses how neural symbolic representations could be flexibly combined to meet a wide range of reasoning demands. Finally, the review assesses the research on program synthesis and proposes that the most flexible neural representation of symbolic processing would involve the capacity to rapidly synthesize neural operations analogous to lambda calculus to solve complex cognitive tasks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Quan Do
- Center for Systems Neuroscience, Boston University, 610 Commonwealth Ave, Boston, MA 02215, United States.
| | - Michael E Hasselmo
- Center for Systems Neuroscience, Boston University, 610 Commonwealth Ave, Boston, MA 02215, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
135
|
Ross TW, Easton A. The Hippocampal Horizon: Constructing and Segmenting Experience for Episodic Memory. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2021; 132:181-196. [PMID: 34826509 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.11.038] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/04/2021] [Revised: 11/19/2021] [Accepted: 11/22/2021] [Indexed: 12/29/2022]
Abstract
How do we recollect specific events that have occurred during continuous ongoing experience? There is converging evidence from non-human animals that spatially modulated cellular activity of the hippocampal formation supports the construction of ongoing events. On the other hand, recent human oriented event cognition models have outlined that our experience is segmented into discrete units, and that such segmentation can operate on shorter or longer timescales. Here, we describe a unification of how these dynamic physiological mechanisms of the hippocampus relate to ongoing externally and internally driven event segmentation, facilitating the demarcation of specific moments during experience. Our cross-species interdisciplinary approach offers a novel perspective in the way we construct and remember specific events, leading to the generation of many new hypotheses for future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- T W Ross
- Department of Psychology, Durham University, South Road, Durham, DH1 3LE, United Kingdom; Centre for Learning and Memory Processes, Durham University, United Kingdom.
| | - A Easton
- Department of Psychology, Durham University, South Road, Durham, DH1 3LE, United Kingdom; Centre for Learning and Memory Processes, Durham University, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
136
|
Xu X, Song L, Kringel R, Hanganu-Opatz IL. Developmental decrease of entorhinal-hippocampal communication in immune-challenged DISC1 knockdown mice. Nat Commun 2021; 12:6810. [PMID: 34815409 PMCID: PMC8611076 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-021-27114-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/02/2021] [Accepted: 11/02/2021] [Indexed: 01/06/2023] Open
Abstract
The prefrontal-hippocampal dysfunction that underlies cognitive deficits in mental disorders emerges during early development. The lateral entorhinal cortex (LEC) is tightly interconnected with both prefrontal cortex (PFC) and hippocampus (HP), yet its contribution to the early dysfunction is fully unknown. Here we show that mice that mimic the dual genetic (G) -environmental (E) etiology (GE mice) of psychiatric risk have poor LEC-dependent recognition memory at pre-juvenile age and abnormal communication within LEC-HP-PFC networks throughout development. These functional and behavioral deficits relate to sparser projections from LEC to CA1 and decreased efficiency of axonal terminals to activate the hippocampal circuits in neonatal GE mice. In contrast, the direct entorhinal drive to PFC is not affected, yet the PFC is indirectly compromised, as target of the under-activated HP. Thus, the entorhinal-hippocampal circuit is already impaired from neonatal age on in GE mice. The authors show that mice that mimic the dual genetic-environmental etiology of psychiatric risk have poor lateral entorhinal cortex-dependent recognition memory already at pre-juvenile age and abnormal communication within LECHP-PFC networks throughout development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xiaxia Xu
- Institute of Developmental Neurophysiology, Center for Molecular Neurobiology, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, 20251, Hamburg, Germany.
| | - Lingzhen Song
- Institute of Developmental Neurophysiology, Center for Molecular Neurobiology, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, 20251, Hamburg, Germany
| | - Rebecca Kringel
- Institute of Developmental Neurophysiology, Center for Molecular Neurobiology, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, 20251, Hamburg, Germany
| | - Ileana L Hanganu-Opatz
- Institute of Developmental Neurophysiology, Center for Molecular Neurobiology, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, 20251, Hamburg, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
137
|
K Namboodiri VM, Stuber GD. The learning of prospective and retrospective cognitive maps within neural circuits. Neuron 2021; 109:3552-3575. [PMID: 34678148 PMCID: PMC8809184 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.09.034] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/02/2021] [Revised: 08/26/2021] [Accepted: 09/16/2021] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
Brain circuits are thought to form a "cognitive map" to process and store statistical relationships in the environment. A cognitive map is commonly defined as a mental representation that describes environmental states (i.e., variables or events) and the relationship between these states. This process is commonly conceptualized as a prospective process, as it is based on the relationships between states in chronological order (e.g., does reward follow a given state?). In this perspective, we expand this concept on the basis of recent findings to postulate that in addition to a prospective map, the brain forms and uses a retrospective cognitive map (e.g., does a given state precede reward?). In doing so, we demonstrate that many neural signals and behaviors (e.g., habits) that seem inflexible and non-cognitive can result from retrospective cognitive maps. Together, we present a significant conceptual reframing of the neurobiological study of associative learning, memory, and decision making.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Vijay Mohan K Namboodiri
- Department of Neurology, Center for Integrative Neuroscience, Kavli Institute for Fundamental Neuroscience, Neuroscience Graduate Program, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA 94158, USA.
| | - Garret D Stuber
- Center for the Neurobiology of Addiction, Pain, and Emotion, Department of Anesthesiology and Pain Medicine, Department of Pharmacology, Neuroscience Graduate Program, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
138
|
Time coding in rat dorsolateral striatum. Neuron 2021; 109:3663-3673.e6. [PMID: 34508666 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.08.020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/16/2021] [Revised: 05/28/2021] [Accepted: 08/16/2021] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
Abstract
To assess the role of dorsolateral striatum (DLS) in time coding, we recorded neuronal activity in rats tasked with comparing the durations of two sequential vibrations. Bayesian decoding of population activity revealed a representation of the unfolding of the trial across time. However, further analyses demonstrated a distinction between the encoding of trial time and perceived time. First, DLS did not show a privileged representation of the stimulus durations compared with other time spans. Second, higher intensity vibrations were perceived as longer; however, time decoded from DLS was unaffected by vibration intensity. Third, DLS did not encode stimulus duration differently on correct versus incorrect trials. Finally, in rats trained to compare the intensities of two sequential vibrations, stimulus duration was encoded even though it was a perceptually irrelevant feature. These findings lead us to posit that temporal information is inherent to DLS activity irrespective of the rat's ongoing percept.
Collapse
|
139
|
Fifty years of the brain's sense of space. Nature 2021; 599:376-377. [PMID: 34737409 DOI: 10.1038/d41586-021-03010-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
140
|
Kim IB, Park SC. The Entorhinal Cortex and Adult Neurogenesis in Major Depression. Int J Mol Sci 2021; 22:11725. [PMID: 34769155 PMCID: PMC8583901 DOI: 10.3390/ijms222111725] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/23/2021] [Revised: 10/26/2021] [Accepted: 10/26/2021] [Indexed: 12/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Depression is characterized by impairments in adult neurogenesis. Reduced hippocampal function, which is suggestive of neurogenesis impairments, is associated with depression-related phenotypes. As adult neurogenesis operates in an activity-dependent manner, disruption of hippocampal neurogenesis in depression may be a consequence of neural circuitry impairments. In particular, the entorhinal cortex is known to have a regulatory effect on the neural circuitry related to hippocampal function and adult neurogenesis. However, a comprehensive understanding of how disruption of the neural circuitry can lead to neurogenesis impairments in depression remains unclear with respect to the regulatory role of the entorhinal cortex. This review highlights recent findings suggesting neural circuitry-regulated neurogenesis, with a focus on the potential role of the entorhinal cortex in hippocampal neurogenesis in depression-related cognitive and emotional phenotypes. Taken together, these findings may provide a better understanding of the entorhinal cortex-regulated hippocampal neurogenesis model of depression.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Il Bin Kim
- Department of Psychiatry, Hanyang University Guri Hospital, Guri 11923, Korea;
- Graduate School of Medical Science and Engineering, Korea Advanced Institute of Science and Technology (KAIST), Daejeon 34141, Korea
| | - Seon-Cheol Park
- Department of Psychiatry, Hanyang University Guri Hospital, Guri 11923, Korea;
- Department of Psychiatry, Hanyang University College of Medicine, Seoul 04763, Korea
| |
Collapse
|
141
|
Yu XT, Yu J, Choi A, Takehara-Nishiuchi K. Lateral entorhinal cortex supports the development of prefrontal network activity that bridges temporally discontiguous stimuli. Hippocampus 2021; 31:1285-1299. [PMID: 34606152 DOI: 10.1002/hipo.23389] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/08/2020] [Revised: 09/14/2021] [Accepted: 09/16/2021] [Indexed: 01/16/2023]
Abstract
The lateral entorhinal cortex (LEC) is an essential component of the brain circuitry supporting long-term memory by serving as an interface between the hippocampus and neocortex. Dysfunction of the LEC affects sensory coding in the hippocampus, leading to a view that the LEC provides the hippocampus with highly processed sensory information. It remains unclear, however, how the LEC modulates neural processing in the neocortical regions. To address this point, we pharmacologically inactivated the LEC of male rats during a temporal associative learning task and examined its impact on local network activity in one of the LEC's efferent targets, the prelimbic region of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC). Rats were exposed to two neutral stimuli, one of which was paired with an aversive eyelid shock over a short temporal delay. The LEC inhibition reduced the expression of anticipatory blinking responses to the reinforced stimuli without increasing responses to nonreinforced stimuli. In control rats, both the reinforced and nonreinforced stimuli evoked a short-lived, wide-band increase in the prelimbic network activity. With learning, the initial increase of gamma-band activity started to extend into the interval between the reinforced neutral stimulus and the eyelid shock. LEC inhibition attenuated the learning-induced sustained activity, without affecting the initial transient activity. These results suggest that the integrity of LEC is necessary for the formation of temporal stimulus associations and its neural correlates in the mPFC. Given the minimal effects on the innate network responses to sensory stimuli, the LEC appears not to be the main source of sensory inputs to the mPFC; rather it may provide a framework that shapes the mPFC network response to behaviorally relevant cues.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xiaotian Tag Yu
- Department of Cell and Systems Biology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
| | - Jessica Yu
- Human Biology Program, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
| | - Allison Choi
- Human Biology Program, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
| | - Kaori Takehara-Nishiuchi
- Department of Cell and Systems Biology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada.,Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada.,Collaborative Program in Neuroscience, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
142
|
White PA. The extended present: an informational context for perception. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2021; 220:103403. [PMID: 34454251 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2021.103403] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/07/2021] [Revised: 08/04/2021] [Accepted: 08/19/2021] [Indexed: 01/29/2023] Open
Abstract
Several previous authors have proposed a kind of specious or subjective present moment that covers a few seconds of recent information. This article proposes a new hypothesis about the subjective present, renamed the extended present, defined not in terms of time covered but as a thematically connected information structure held in working memory and in transiently accessible form in long-term memory. The three key features of the extended present are that information in it is thematically connected, both internally and to current attended perceptual input, it is organised in a hierarchical structure, and all information in it is marked with temporal information, specifically ordinal and duration information. Temporal boundaries to the information structure are determined by hierarchical structure processing and by limits on processing and storage capacity. Supporting evidence for the importance of hierarchical structure analysis is found in the domains of music perception, speech and language processing, perception and production of goal-directed action, and exact arithmetical calculation. Temporal information marking is also discussed and a possible mechanism for representing ordinal and duration information on the time scale of the extended present is proposed. It is hypothesised that the extended present functions primarily as an informational context for making sense of current perceptual input, and as an enabler for perception and generation of complex structures and operations in language, action, music, exact calculation, and other domains.
Collapse
|
143
|
González-Marrero I, Hernandez-Garcia JA, Gonzalez-Davila E, Carmona-Calero EM, Gonzalez-Toledo JM, Castañeyra-Ruiz L, Hernandez-Abad LG, Castañeyra-Perdomo A. Variations of the grid and place cells in the entorhinal cortex and dentate gyrus of 6 individuals aged 56 to 87 years. Neurologia 2021:S0213-4853(21)00118-3. [PMID: 34531045 DOI: 10.1016/j.nrl.2021.04.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/25/2021] [Revised: 04/26/2021] [Accepted: 04/27/2021] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
INTRODUCTION The relationship between the entorhinal cortex and the hippocampus has been studied by different authors, who have highlighted the importance of grid cells, place cells, and the trisynaptic circuit in the processes that they regulate: the persistence of spatial, explicit, and recent memory and their possible impairment with ageing. OBJECTIVE We aimed to determine whether older age causes changes in the size and number of grid cells contained in layer III of the entorhinal cortex and in the granular layer of the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus. METHODS We conducted post-mortem studies of the brains of 6 individuals aged 56-87 years. The brain sections containing the dentate gyrus and the adjacent entorhinal cortex were stained according to the Klüver-Barrera method, then the Image J software was used to measure the individual neuronal area, the total neuronal area, and the number of neurons contained in rectangular areas in layer III of the entorhinal cortex and layer II of the dentate gyrus. Statistical analysis was subsequently performed. RESULTS We observed an age-related reduction in the cell population of the external pyramidal layer of the entorhinal cortex, and in the number of neurons in the granular layer of the dentate gyrus. CONCLUSION Our results indicate that ageing causes a decrease in the size and density of grid cells of the entorhinal cortex and place cells of the dentate gyrus.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- I González-Marrero
- Unidad de Anatomía y Embriología Humana, Departamento de Ciencias Médicas Básicas, Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud, Universidad de La Laguna, Tenerife, Islas Canaria, España
| | - J A Hernandez-Garcia
- Unidad de Anatomía y Embriología Humana, Departamento de Ciencias Médicas Básicas, Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud, Universidad de La Laguna, Tenerife, Islas Canaria, España
| | - E Gonzalez-Davila
- Departamento de Matemáticas, Estadística e Investigación Operativa, Universidad de La Laguna, Tenerife, Islas Canaria, España
| | - E M Carmona-Calero
- Unidad de Anatomía y Embriología Humana, Departamento de Ciencias Médicas Básicas, Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud, Universidad de La Laguna, Tenerife, Islas Canaria, España; Instituto de Investigación y Ciencias, Puerto del Rosario, Fuerteventura, Islas Canarias, España
| | - J M Gonzalez-Toledo
- Unidad de Anatomía y Embriología Humana, Departamento de Ciencias Médicas Básicas, Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud, Universidad de La Laguna, Tenerife, Islas Canaria, España
| | - L Castañeyra-Ruiz
- Department of Neurological Surgery, Washington University School of Medicine and the St. Louis Children's Hospital, St. Louis, Missouri, Estados Unidos
| | - L G Hernandez-Abad
- Unidad de Anatomía y Embriología Humana, Departamento de Ciencias Médicas Básicas, Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud, Universidad de La Laguna, Tenerife, Islas Canaria, España; Instituto de Investigación y Ciencias, Puerto del Rosario, Fuerteventura, Islas Canarias, España
| | - A Castañeyra-Perdomo
- Unidad de Anatomía y Embriología Humana, Departamento de Ciencias Médicas Básicas, Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud, Universidad de La Laguna, Tenerife, Islas Canaria, España; Instituto de Investigación y Ciencias, Puerto del Rosario, Fuerteventura, Islas Canarias, España.
| |
Collapse
|
144
|
Kong X, Luo S, Wang YF, Yang GF, Lu GM, Zhang LJ. [ 18F]PBR146 and [ 18F]DPA-714 in vivo Imaging of Neuroinflammation in Chronic Hepatic Encephalopathy Rats. Front Neurosci 2021; 15:678144. [PMID: 34483820 PMCID: PMC8415356 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2021.678144] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/09/2021] [Accepted: 07/28/2021] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Neuroinflammation is an important pathogenesis of hepatic encephalopathy (HE). The upregulation of translocator protein (TSPO) during neuroinflammation provides an imaging molecular target to evaluate the severity of neuroinflammation in chronic HE rats. [18F]DPA-714 and [18F]PBR146 targeting TSPO are often used for neuroinflammation imaging. This study performed bile duct ligation (BDL) in rats to simulate chronic HE model, tested the behavioral experiments, and conducted [18F]PBR146 and [18F]DPA-714 micro-PET/CT scans followed analyzing the average %ID/g values of the whole brain, brain regions and main organs of subjects. After sacrifice the rats, the blood plasma samples were taken for blood biochemical indexes and plasma inflammatory factor levels examination, the liver and brain specimens were obtained for pathological analysis. The BDL rats showed chronic liver failure with defects in cognition, motor coordination ability and mental state. [18F]PBR146 and [18F]DPA-714 micro-PET/CT imaging results were similar in whole brain of BDL group and Sham group. Besides, some regional brain areas in BDL rats were found abnormal uptakes mainly located in basal ganglia area, auditory cortex, motor cortex, cingulate gyrus, somatosensory cortex, hippocampus, thalamus, midbrain, and medulla oblongata, and these regions also correlated with behavioral alterations. In conclusion, both [18F]PBR146 and [18F]DPA-714 had the similar imaging effects in hepatic encephalopathy models could quantitatively evaluate neuroinflammation load and distribution. The difference brain regions with higher uptake values of radiotracers in BDL rats were correlated with behavioral alterations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xiang Kong
- Department of Diagnostic Radiology, Jinling Hospital, Medical School of Nanjing University, Nanjing, China
| | - Song Luo
- Department of Diagnostic Radiology, Jinling Hospital, Medical School of Nanjing University, Nanjing, China
| | - Yun Fei Wang
- Department of Diagnostic Radiology, Jinling Hospital, Medical School of Nanjing University, Nanjing, China
| | - Gui Fen Yang
- Department of Nuclear Medicine, Jinling Hospital, Medical School of Nanjing University, Nanjing, China
| | - Guang Ming Lu
- Department of Diagnostic Radiology, Jinling Hospital, Medical School of Nanjing University, Nanjing, China
| | - Long Jiang Zhang
- Department of Diagnostic Radiology, Jinling Hospital, Medical School of Nanjing University, Nanjing, China
| |
Collapse
|
145
|
Vo A, Tabrizi NS, Hunt T, Cayanan K, Chitale S, Anderson LG, Tenney S, White AO, Sabariego M, Hales JB. Medial entorhinal cortex lesions produce delay-dependent disruptions in memory for elapsed time. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2021; 185:107507. [PMID: 34474155 DOI: 10.1016/j.nlm.2021.107507] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/28/2021] [Revised: 07/30/2021] [Accepted: 08/20/2021] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
Our memory for time is a fundamental ability that we use to judge the duration of events, put our experiences into a temporal context, and decide when to initiate actions. The medial entorhinal cortex (MEC), with its direct projections to the hippocampus, has been proposed to be the key source of temporal information for hippocampal time cells. However, the behavioral relevance of such temporal firing patterns remains unclear, as most of the paradigms used for the study of temporal processing and time cells are either spatial tasks or tasks for which MEC function is not required. In this study, we asked whether the MEC is necessary for rats to perform a time duration discrimination task (TDD), in which rats were trained to discriminate between 10-s and 20-s delay intervals. After reaching a 90% performance criterion, the rats were assigned to receive an excitotoxic MEC-lesion or sham-lesion surgery. We found that after recovering from surgery, rats with MEC lesions were impaired on the TDD task in comparison to rats with sham lesions, failing to return to criterion performance. Their impairment, however, was specific to the longer, 20-s delay trials. These results indicate that time processing is dependent on MEC neural computations only for delays that exceed 10 s, perhaps because long-term memory resources are needed to keep track of longer time intervals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Annette Vo
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of San Diego, San Diego, CA 92110, USA
| | - Nina S Tabrizi
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of San Diego, San Diego, CA 92110, USA
| | - Thomas Hunt
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of San Diego, San Diego, CA 92110, USA
| | - Kayla Cayanan
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of San Diego, San Diego, CA 92110, USA
| | - Saee Chitale
- Program in Neuroscience and Behavior, Mount Holyoke College, South Hadley, MA 01075, USA
| | - Lucy G Anderson
- Program in Neuroscience and Behavior, Mount Holyoke College, South Hadley, MA 01075, USA
| | - Sarah Tenney
- Program in Neuroscience and Behavior, Mount Holyoke College, South Hadley, MA 01075, USA
| | - André O White
- Program in Neuroscience and Behavior, Mount Holyoke College, South Hadley, MA 01075, USA; Department of Biological Sciences, Mount Holyoke College, South Hadley, MA 01075, USA
| | - Marta Sabariego
- Program in Neuroscience and Behavior, Mount Holyoke College, South Hadley, MA 01075, USA.
| | - Jena B Hales
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of San Diego, San Diego, CA 92110, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
146
|
|
147
|
de Sousa AF, Chowdhury A, Silva AJ. Dimensions and mechanisms of memory organization. Neuron 2021; 109:2649-2662. [PMID: 34242564 PMCID: PMC8416710 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.06.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/19/2021] [Revised: 04/17/2021] [Accepted: 06/11/2021] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
Abstract
Memory formation is dynamic in nature, and acquisition of new information is often influenced by previous experiences. Memories sharing certain attributes are known to interact so that retrieval of one increases the likelihood of retrieving the other, raising the possibility that related memories are organized into associative mnemonic structures of interconnected representations. Although the formation and retrieval of single memories have been studied extensively, very little is known about the brain mechanisms that organize and link related memories. Here we review studies that suggest the existence of mnemonic structures in humans and animal models. These studies suggest three main dimensions of experience that can serve to organize related memories: time, space, and perceptual/conceptual similarities. We propose potential molecular, cellular, and systems mechanisms that might support organization of memories according to these dimensions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- André F de Sousa
- Departments of Neurobiology, Psychiatry & Biobehavioral Sciences, and Psychology, Integrative Center for Learning and Memory, Brain Research Institute, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA
| | - Ananya Chowdhury
- Departments of Neurobiology, Psychiatry & Biobehavioral Sciences, and Psychology, Integrative Center for Learning and Memory, Brain Research Institute, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA
| | - Alcino J Silva
- Departments of Neurobiology, Psychiatry & Biobehavioral Sciences, and Psychology, Integrative Center for Learning and Memory, Brain Research Institute, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
148
|
Liu C, Ye Z, Chen C, Axmacher N, Xue G. Hippocampal Representations of Event Structure and Temporal Context during Episodic Temporal Order Memory. Cereb Cortex 2021; 32:1520-1534. [PMID: 34464439 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhab304] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/14/2021] [Revised: 07/31/2021] [Accepted: 08/01/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The hippocampus plays an important role in representing spatial locations and sequences and in transforming representations. How these representational structures and operations support memory for the temporal order of random items is still poorly understood. We addressed this question by leveraging the method of loci, a powerful mnemonic strategy for temporal order memory that particularly recruits hippocampus-dependent computations of spatial locations and associations. Applying representational similarity analysis to functional magnetic resonance imaging activation patterns revealed that hippocampal subfields contained representations of multiple features of sequence structure, including spatial locations, location distance, and sequence boundaries, as well as episodic-like temporal context. Critically, the hippocampal CA1 exhibited spatial transformation of representational patterns, showing lower pattern similarity for items in same locations than closely matched different locations during retrieval, whereas the CA23DG exhibited sequential transformation of representational patterns, showing lower pattern similarity for items in near locations than in far locations during encoding. These transformations enabled the encoding of multiple items in the same location and disambiguation of adjacent items. Our results suggest that the hippocampus can flexibly reconfigure multiplexed event structure representations to support accurate temporal order memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chuqi Liu
- State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning & IDG/McGovern Institute of Brain Research, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, PR China
| | - Zhifang Ye
- State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning & IDG/McGovern Institute of Brain Research, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, PR China.,Department of Psychology, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR 97403, USA
| | - Chuansheng Chen
- Department of Psychological Science, University of California, Irvine, CA 92697, USA
| | - Nikolai Axmacher
- State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning & IDG/McGovern Institute of Brain Research, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, PR China.,Department of Neuropsychology, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology, Ruhr University Bochum, Bochum 44801, Germany
| | - Gui Xue
- State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning & IDG/McGovern Institute of Brain Research, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, PR China
| |
Collapse
|
149
|
Huang CC, Rolls ET, Hsu CCH, Feng J, Lin CP. Extensive Cortical Connectivity of the Human Hippocampal Memory System: Beyond the "What" and "Where" Dual Stream Model. Cereb Cortex 2021; 31:4652-4669. [PMID: 34013342 PMCID: PMC8866812 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhab113] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/10/2021] [Revised: 04/09/2021] [Accepted: 04/10/2021] [Indexed: 10/06/2023] Open
Abstract
The human hippocampus is involved in forming new memories: damage impairs memory. The dual stream model suggests that object "what" representations from ventral stream temporal cortex project to the hippocampus via the perirhinal and then lateral entorhinal cortex, and spatial "where" representations from the dorsal parietal stream via the parahippocampal gyrus and then medial entorhinal cortex. The hippocampus can then associate these inputs to form episodic memories of what happened where. Diffusion tractography was used to reveal the direct connections of hippocampal system areas in humans. This provides evidence that the human hippocampus has extensive direct cortical connections, with connections that bypass the entorhinal cortex to connect with the perirhinal and parahippocampal cortex, with the temporal pole, with the posterior and retrosplenial cingulate cortex, and even with early sensory cortical areas. The connections are less hierarchical and segregated than in the dual stream model. This provides a foundation for a conceptualization for how the hippocampal memory system connects with the cerebral cortex and operates in humans. One implication is that prehippocampal cortical areas such as the parahippocampal TF and TH subregions and perirhinal cortices may implement specialized computations that can benefit from inputs from the dorsal and ventral streams.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chu-Chung Huang
- Key Laboratory of Brain Functional Genomics (MOE & STCSM), Affiliated Mental Health Center (ECNU), School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, Shanghai 200062, China
- Shanghai Changning Mental Health Center, Shanghai 200335, China
| | - Edmund T Rolls
- Institute of Science and Technology for Brain Inspired Intelligence, Fudan University, Shanghai 200433, China
- Department of Computer Science, University of Warwick, Coventry CV4 7AL, UK
- Oxford Centre for Computational Neuroscience, Oxford, UK
| | - Chih-Chin Heather Hsu
- Center for Geriatrics and Gerontology, Taipei Veterans General Hospital, Taipei 11217, Taiwan
- Institute of Neuroscience, National Yang-Ming Chiao Tung University, Taipei 11217, Taiwan
| | - Jianfeng Feng
- Institute of Science and Technology for Brain Inspired Intelligence, Fudan University, Shanghai 200433, China
- Department of Computer Science, University of Warwick, Coventry CV4 7AL, UK
| | - Ching-Po Lin
- Institute of Science and Technology for Brain Inspired Intelligence, Fudan University, Shanghai 200433, China
- Center for Geriatrics and Gerontology, Taipei Veterans General Hospital, Taipei 11217, Taiwan
- Brain Research Center, National Yang-Ming Chiao Tung University, Taipei 11217, Taiwan
| |
Collapse
|
150
|
Reddy L, Zoefel B, Possel JK, Peters J, Dijksterhuis DE, Poncet M, van Straaten ECW, Baayen JC, Idema S, Self MW. Human Hippocampal Neurons Track Moments in a Sequence of Events. J Neurosci 2021; 41:6714-6725. [PMID: 34183446 PMCID: PMC8336696 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.3157-20.2021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/14/2020] [Revised: 05/31/2021] [Accepted: 06/06/2021] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
An indispensable feature of episodic memory is our ability to temporally piece together different elements of an experience into a coherent memory. Hippocampal time cells-neurons that represent temporal information-may play a critical role in this process. Although these cells have been repeatedly found in rodents, it is still unclear to what extent similar temporal selectivity exists in the human hippocampus. Here, we show that temporal context modulates the firing activity of human hippocampal neurons during structured temporal experiences. We recorded neuronal activity in the human brain while patients of either sex learned predictable sequences of pictures. We report that human time cells fire at successive moments in this task. Furthermore, time cells also signaled inherently changing temporal contexts during empty 10 s gap periods between trials while participants waited for the task to resume. Finally, population activity allowed for decoding temporal epoch identity, both during sequence learning and during the gap periods. These findings suggest that human hippocampal neurons could play an essential role in temporally organizing distinct moments of an experience in episodic memory.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Episodic memory refers to our ability to remember the what, where, and when of a past experience. Representing time is an important component of this form of memory. Here, we show that neurons in the human hippocampus represent temporal information. This temporal signature was observed both when participants were actively engaged in a memory task, as well as during 10-s-long gaps when they were asked to wait before performing the task. Furthermore, the activity of the population of hippocampal cells allowed for decoding one temporal epoch from another. These results suggest a robust representation of time in the human hippocampus.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Leila Reddy
- Centre de Recherche Cerveau et Cognition, Université de Toulouse III, Paul Sabatier, 31059 Toulouse, France
- Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Unité Mixte de Recherche 5549, Faculté de Médecine de Purpan, Toulouse 31052, France
- Artificial and Natural Intelligence Toulouse Institute, Toulouse 31052, France
| | - Benedikt Zoefel
- Centre de Recherche Cerveau et Cognition, Université de Toulouse III, Paul Sabatier, 31059 Toulouse, France
- Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Unité Mixte de Recherche 5549, Faculté de Médecine de Purpan, Toulouse 31052, France
| | - Jessy K Possel
- Vision and Cognition Group, Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, 1105 BA Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Judith Peters
- Vision and Cognition Group, Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, 1105 BA Amsterdam, The Netherlands
- Cognitive Neuroscience Department, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, 6229 EV Maastricht, The Netherlands
| | - Doris E Dijksterhuis
- Vision and Cognition Group, Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, 1105 BA Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Marlene Poncet
- Centre de Recherche Cerveau et Cognition, Université de Toulouse III, Paul Sabatier, 31059 Toulouse, France
- Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Unité Mixte de Recherche 5549, Faculté de Médecine de Purpan, Toulouse 31052, France
- School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St. Andrews, KY16 9JP St. Andrews, Scotland
| | - Elisabeth C W van Straaten
- Department of Neurology and Clinical Neurophysiology, Amsterdam University Medical Center, 1081 HV Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Johannes C Baayen
- Department of Neurosurgery, Amsterdam University Medical Center, 1081 HV Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Sander Idema
- Department of Neurosurgery, Amsterdam University Medical Center, 1081 HV Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Matthew W Self
- Vision and Cognition Group, Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, 1105 BA Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|