1
|
Yue SC, Cakir GB, Shaikh A, Ghasia FF. Assessing inter-ocular fixational eye movements throughout the lifespan. Exp Brain Res 2024:10.1007/s00221-024-06936-2. [PMID: 39395061 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-024-06936-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/15/2024] [Accepted: 09/24/2024] [Indexed: 10/14/2024]
Abstract
This study aims to assess fixational eye movements (FEMs) obtained under binocular and monocular viewing in normal individuals across different age groups. We recruited 68 healthy participants divided into Group 1 (children, 3-9 years, n = 20), Group 2 (adolescents, 10-19 years, n = 26), and Group 3 (adults, 20-73 years, n = 22). FEMs were collected using a high-resolution video-based tracker under 3 viewing conditions: binocular viewing (BV), monocular viewing right eye (MV_RE), and monocular viewing left eye (MV_LE). We quantified fixation stability, the frequency, amplitude, and disconjugacy of fixational saccades, and inter-saccadic drift velocity in BV, MV_RE, and MV_LE. We also computed inter-ocular fixation stability under binocular viewing and monocular viewing in the 3 groups. Fixation instability (FI) and fixational saccade amplitudes were higher in Group 1 than in Group 3 whereas inter-saccadic drifts were increased in Group 3. Vergence stability was greater in binocular viewing than in monocular viewing likely due to binocular summation in all groups. However, the fixational saccade amplitude and drift velocity of the right and left eye did not significantly differ across different viewing conditions within each group. Interestingly, the inter-ocular fixation stability ratio and vergence stability showed no significant differences between the groups. In conclusion, FEMs differ across age groups but inter-ocular FEMs are immune to the effects of age and can be a valuable parameter while evaluating FEM abnormalities in diseases like amblyopia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sophie C Yue
- Case Western University School of Medicine, Cleveland, OH, USA
| | - Gokce B Cakir
- Cole Eye Institute, Cleveland Clinic, Cleveland, OH, USA
| | - Aasef Shaikh
- Department of Neurology, University Hospitals and Cleveland VA Medical Center, Cleveland, USA
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, USA
- Daroff-Dell'Osso Ocular Motility Laboratory, Louis Stokes Cleveland VA Medical Center, Cleveland, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
2
|
Hunfalvay M, Bolte T, Singh A, Greenstein E, Murray NP, Carrick FR. Age-Based Developmental Biomarkers in Eye Movements: A Retrospective Analysis Using Machine Learning. Brain Sci 2024; 14:686. [PMID: 39061426 PMCID: PMC11274786 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci14070686] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/08/2024] [Revised: 06/13/2024] [Accepted: 06/24/2024] [Indexed: 07/28/2024] Open
Abstract
This study aimed to identify when and how eye movements change across the human lifespan to benchmark developmental biomarkers. The sample size comprised 45,696 participants, ranging in age from 6 to 80 years old (M = 30.39; SD = 17.46). Participants completed six eye movement tests: Circular Smooth Pursuit, Horizontal Smooth Pursuit, Vertical Smooth Pursuit, Horizontal Saccades, Vertical Saccades, and Fixation Stability. These tests examined all four major eye movements (fixations, saccades, pursuits, and vergence) using 89 eye-tracking algorithms. A semi-supervised, self-training, machine learning classifier was used to group the data into age ranges. This classifier resulted in 12 age groups: 6-7, 8-11, 12-14, 15-25, 26-31, 32-38, 39-45, 46-53, 54-60, 61-68, 69-76, and 77-80 years. To provide a descriptive indication of the strength of the self-training classifier, a series of multiple analyses of variance (MANOVA) were conducted on the multivariate effect of the age groups by test set. Each MANOVA revealed a significant multivariate effect on age groups (p < 0.001). Developmental changes in eye movements across age categories were identified. Specifically, similarities were observed between very young and elderly individuals. Middle-aged individuals (30s) generally showed the best eye movement metrics. Clinicians and researchers may use the findings from this study to inform decision-making on patients' health and wellness and guide effective research methodologies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Melissa Hunfalvay
- RightEye LLC., 6107A, Suite 400, Rockledge Drive, Bethesda, MD 20814, USA; (T.B.); (A.S.)
- Biological Engineering, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA
| | - Takumi Bolte
- RightEye LLC., 6107A, Suite 400, Rockledge Drive, Bethesda, MD 20814, USA; (T.B.); (A.S.)
| | - Abhishek Singh
- RightEye LLC., 6107A, Suite 400, Rockledge Drive, Bethesda, MD 20814, USA; (T.B.); (A.S.)
| | - Ethan Greenstein
- Washington University in St. Louis, 1 Brookings Dr., St. Louis, MO 63130, USA;
| | - Nicholas P. Murray
- Visual Motor Laboratory, Department of Kinesiology, East Carolina University, Greenville, NC 27858, USA;
| | - Frederick Robert Carrick
- Neurology, University of Central Florida College of Medicine, Orlando, FL 23816, USA;
- Centre for Mental Health Research in Association with University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB3 9AJ, UK
- MGH Institute of Health Professions, Boston, MA 02129, USA
- Carrick Institute Neurology, Cape Canaveral, FL 32920, USA
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Justino J, Kolinsky R. Eye movements during reading in beginning and skilled readers: Impact of reading level or physiological maturation? Acta Psychol (Amst) 2023; 236:103927. [PMID: 37126894 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2023.103927] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/22/2022] [Revised: 04/19/2023] [Accepted: 04/24/2023] [Indexed: 05/03/2023] Open
Abstract
We begin by presenting and examining relevant data in the literature on eye movements in reading, from childhood to adulthood. In particular, we discuss the differences found in eye movements during reading between children in different age groups and with different reading levels and skilled adult readers in terms of word recognition and sentence processing. We then critically discuss two hypotheses that account for the differences between children and adults' eye movement during reading: one being reading age itself - the changes in eye movement patterns in reading are regulated by the level of reading proficiency and its automatization - and the other being the role of maturation of oculomotor control and, consequently, its possible changes in eye movement patterns during reading. Finally, we list gaps in the research field and suggest that future research will benefit from investigating eye movements during reading in ex-illiterate adults who are in the process of learning to read in order to isolate both reading and maturational factors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julia Justino
- Fonds de la Recherche Scientifique - FNRS (FRS-FNRS), Belgium; Unité de Recherche en Neurosciences Cognitives (Unescog/CRCN), Université Libre de Bruxelles, Belgium.
| | - Régine Kolinsky
- Fonds de la Recherche Scientifique - FNRS (FRS-FNRS), Belgium; Unité de Recherche en Neurosciences Cognitives (Unescog/CRCN), Université Libre de Bruxelles, Belgium.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Tobener E, Searer A, Doettl S, Plyler P. Oculomotor Findings in Videonystagmography across the Lifespan. J Am Acad Audiol 2023; 34:11-18. [PMID: 39288905 DOI: 10.1055/s-0042-1760437] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 09/19/2024]
Abstract
BACKGROUND When performing oculomotor testing during standard videonystagmography (VNG), the vestibular system is not actively stimulated. Therefore, responses are generated from the cerebellum, brainstem, and oculomotor tract. Many patients seen for vestibular testing fall outside of the standard age norms, making it difficult to determine whether an abnormal finding is due to age or oculomotor dysfunction. PURPOSE The purpose of this study was to further evaluate the effect of age on a standard clinical VNG oculomotor test battery consisting of saccades, smooth pursuit, and optokinetic (OPK) testing. RESEARCH DESIGN This is a cross-sectional, between-group prospective study comparing oculomotor tests between age groups. STUDY SAMPLE Twenty-one older adults between the ages of 60 and 90 years with no history of central or peripheral vestibular dysfunction were included in the study. Previously collected data from 29 children aged 4 to 6 years and 33 adults aged 20 to 60 years were also included. DATA COLLECTION AND ANALYSIS Participants completed oculomotor testing using infrared goggles consisting of saccades, smooth pursuit, and OPK. Statistical analyses were completed using multivariate analysis of variance and analysis of variance and follow-up analysis when indicated. RESULTS Significant group differences were noted for saccade latency and speed, smooth pursuit gain, and OPK gain and speed. Children and older adults demonstrated longer saccade latencies compared with the controls, and older adults exhibited slower saccade speed than the controls and children. These results also indicated that smooth pursuit gain was reduced for children and older adults compared with controls, and gain decreased across all groups as frequency increased. Analyses of OPK results indicated older adults had reduced gain and speed compared with the children and control group. CONCLUSIONS The findings of prolonged saccade latencies and reduced smooth pursuit gain in both children and older adults suggest possible cerebellar rather than attentional effects. However, other findings such as reduced saccade speed and reduced OPK gain were noted only in the older adults, which suggests oculomotor degeneration and/or insufficient coverage of the visual field during testing, respectively. These results also indicate the importance of age-specific normative data for use in clinical oculomotor testing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elizabeth Tobener
- Department of Audiology and Speech Pathology, University of Tennessee Health Science Center, Knoxville, Tennessee
| | - Ashlee Searer
- Department of Audiology and Speech Pathology, University of Tennessee Health Science Center, Knoxville, Tennessee
| | - Steven Doettl
- Department of Audiology and Speech Pathology, University of Tennessee Health Science Center, Knoxville, Tennessee
| | - Patrick Plyler
- Department of Audiology and Speech Pathology, University of Tennessee Health Science Center, Knoxville, Tennessee
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Borovsky A. Drivers of Lexical Processing and Implications for Early Learning. ANNUAL REVIEW OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2022; 4:21-40. [PMID: 38846449 PMCID: PMC11156262 DOI: 10.1146/annurev-devpsych-120920-042902] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/09/2024]
Abstract
Understanding words in unfolding speech requires the coordination of many skills to support successful and rapid comprehension of word meanings. This multifaceted ability emerges before our first birthday, matures over a protracted period of development, varies widely between individuals, forecasts future learning outcomes, and is influenced by immediate context, prior knowledge, and lifetime experience. This article highlights drivers of early lexical processing abilities while exploring questions regarding how learners begin to acquire, represent, and activate meaning in language. The review additionally explores how lexical processing and representation are connected while reflecting on how network science approaches can support richly detailed insights into this connection in young learners. Future research avenues are considered that focus on addressing how language processing and other cognitive skills are connected.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Arielle Borovsky
- Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana, USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Zhang Y, Ryali S, Cai W, Supekar K, Pasumarthy R, Padmanabhan A, Luna B, Menon V. Developmental maturation of causal signaling hubs in voluntary control of saccades and their functional controllability. Cereb Cortex 2022; 32:4746-4762. [PMID: 35094063 PMCID: PMC9627122 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhab514] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/17/2021] [Revised: 12/14/2021] [Accepted: 12/15/2021] [Indexed: 02/01/2023] Open
Abstract
The ability to adaptively respond to behaviorally relevant cues in the environment, including voluntary control of automatic but inappropriate responses and deployment of a goal-relevant alternative response, undergoes significant maturation from childhood to adulthood. Importantly, the maturation of voluntary control processes influences the developmental trajectories of several key cognitive domains, including executive function and emotion regulation. Understanding the maturation of voluntary control is therefore of fundamental importance, but little is known about the underlying causal functional circuit mechanisms. Here, we use state-space and control-theoretic modeling to investigate the maturation of causal signaling mechanisms underlying voluntary control over saccades. We demonstrate that directed causal interactions in a canonical saccade network undergo significant maturation between childhood and adulthood. Crucially, we show that the frontal eye field (FEF) is an immature causal signaling hub in children during control over saccades. Using control-theoretic analysis, we then demonstrate that the saccade network is less controllable in children and that greater energy is required to drive FEF dynamics in children compared to adults. Our findings provide novel evidence that strengthening of causal signaling hubs and controllability of FEF are key mechanisms underlying age-related improvements in the ability to plan and execute voluntary control over saccades.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yuan Zhang
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA 94305, USA
| | - Srikanth Ryali
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA 94305, USA
| | - Weidong Cai
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA 94305, USA
| | - Kaustubh Supekar
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA 94305, USA
| | - Ramkrishna Pasumarthy
- Department of Electrical Engineering, Robert Bosch Center of Data Sciences and Artificial Intelligence, Network Systems Learning, Control and Evolution Group, Indian Institute of Technology Madras, Chennai 600036, India
| | - Aarthi Padmanabhan
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA 94305, USA
| | - Bea Luna
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA
- Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA
| | - Vinod Menon
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA 94305, USA
- Department of Neurology & Neurological Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA 94305, USA
- Wu Tsai Neuroscience Institute, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA 94305, USA
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Canu D, Ioannou C, Müller K, Martin B, Fleischhaker C, Biscaldi M, Beauducel A, Smyrnis N, van Elst LT, Klein C. Visual search in neurodevelopmental disorders: evidence towards a continuum of impairment. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2022; 31:1-18. [PMID: 33751240 PMCID: PMC9343296 DOI: 10.1007/s00787-021-01756-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/02/2020] [Accepted: 03/08/2021] [Indexed: 02/07/2023]
Abstract
Disorders with neurodevelopmental aetiology such as Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) and Schizophrenia share commonalities at many levels of investigation despite phenotypic differences. Evidence of genetic overlap has led to the concept of a continuum of neurodevelopmental impairment along which these disorders can be positioned in aetiological, pathophysiological and developmental features. This concept requires their simultaneous comparison at different levels, which has not been accomplished so far. Given that cognitive impairments are core to the pathophysiology of these disorders, we provide for the first time differentiated head-to-head comparisons in a complex cognitive function, visual search, decomposing the task with eye movement-based process analyses. N = 103 late-adolescents with schizophrenia, ADHD, ASD and healthy controls took a serial visual search task, while their eye movements were recorded. Patients with schizophrenia presented the greatest level of impairment across different phases of search, followed by patients with ADHD, who shared with patients with schizophrenia elevated intra-subject variability in the pre-search stage. ASD was the least impaired group, but similar to schizophrenia in post-search processes and to schizophrenia and ADHD in pre-search processes and fixation duration while scanning the items. Importantly, the profiles of deviancy from controls were highly correlated between all three clinical groups, in line with the continuum idea. Findings suggest the existence of one common neurodevelopmental continuum of performance for the three disorders, while quantitative differences appear in the level of impairment. Given the relevance of cognitive impairments in these three disorders, we argue in favour of overlapping pathophysiological mechanisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniela Canu
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, Medical Center - University of Freiburg, Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany.
| | - Chara Ioannou
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, Medical Center - University of Freiburg, Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
| | - Katarina Müller
- Psychotherapeutisches Wohnheim für junge Menschen Leppermühle, Buseck, Germany
| | - Berthold Martin
- Psychotherapeutisches Wohnheim für junge Menschen Leppermühle, Buseck, Germany
| | - Christian Fleischhaker
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, Medical Center - University of Freiburg, Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
| | - Monica Biscaldi
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, Medical Center - University of Freiburg, Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
| | | | - Nikolaos Smyrnis
- 2nd Psychiatry Department, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Medical School, University General Hospital "ATTIKON", Athens, Greece
| | - Ludger Tebartz van Elst
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical Center - University of Freiburg, Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
| | - Christoph Klein
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, Medical Center - University of Freiburg, Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany.
- 2nd Psychiatry Department, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Medical School, University General Hospital "ATTIKON", Athens, Greece.
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Medical Faculty, University of Cologne, Cologne, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Schuck NW, Li AX, Wenke D, Ay-Bryson DS, Loewe AT, Gaschler R, Shing YL. Spontaneous discovery of novel task solutions in children. PLoS One 2022; 17:e0266253. [PMID: 35639714 PMCID: PMC9154107 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0266253] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/22/2021] [Accepted: 03/16/2022] [Indexed: 12/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Children often perform worse than adults on tasks that require focused attention. While this is commonly regarded as a sign of incomplete cognitive development, a broader attentional focus could also endow children with the ability to find novel solutions to a given task. To test this idea, we investigated children's ability to discover and use novel aspects of the environment that allowed them to improve their decision-making strategy. Participants were given a simple choice task in which the possibility of strategy improvement was neither mentioned by instructions nor encouraged by explicit error feedback. Among 47 children (8-10 years of age) who were instructed to perform the choice task across two experiments, 27.5% showed a full strategy change. This closely matched the proportion of adults who had the same insight (28.2% of n = 39). The amount of erroneous choices, working memory capacity and inhibitory control, in contrast, indicated substantial disadvantages of children in task execution and cognitive control. A task difficulty manipulation did not affect the results. The stark contrast between age-differences in different aspects of cognitive performance might offer a unique opportunity for educators in fostering learning in children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicolas W. Schuck
- Max Planck Research Group NeuroCode, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
- Max Planck UCL Centre for Computational Psychiatry and Ageing Research, Berlin, Germany
| | - Amy X. Li
- School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
| | - Dorit Wenke
- PFH Private University of Applied Sciences, Göttingen, Germany
| | - Destina S. Ay-Bryson
- Department of Rehabilitation Sciences, Humboldt University of Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Anika T. Loewe
- Max Planck Research Group NeuroCode, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
- Max Planck UCL Centre for Computational Psychiatry and Ageing Research, Berlin, Germany
| | | | - Yee Lee Shing
- Institute of Psychology, Goethe University Frankfurt, Frankfurt, Germany
- Center for Lifespan Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Yep R, Smorenburg ML, Riek HC, Calancie OG, Kirkpatrick RH, Perkins JE, Huang J, Coe BC, Brien DC, Munoz DP. Interleaved Pro/Anti-saccade Behavior Across the Lifespan. Front Aging Neurosci 2022; 14:842549. [PMID: 35663573 PMCID: PMC9159803 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.842549] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/23/2021] [Accepted: 05/02/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The capacity for inhibitory control is an important cognitive process that undergoes dynamic changes over the course of the lifespan. Robust characterization of this trajectory, considering age continuously and using flexible modeling techniques, is critical to advance our understanding of the neural mechanisms that differ in healthy aging and neurological disease. The interleaved pro/anti-saccade task (IPAST), in which pro- and anti-saccade trials are randomly interleaved within a block, provides a simple and sensitive means of assessing the neural circuitry underlying inhibitory control. We utilized IPAST data collected from a large cross-sectional cohort of normative participants (n = 604, 5-93 years of age), standardized pre-processing protocols, generalized additive modeling, and change point analysis to investigate the effect of age on saccade behavior and identify significant periods of change throughout the lifespan. Maturation of IPAST measures occurred throughout adolescence, while subsequent decline began as early as the mid-20s and continued into old age. Considering pro-saccade correct responses and anti-saccade direction errors made at express (short) and regular (long) latencies was crucial in differentiating developmental and aging processes. We additionally characterized the effect of age on voluntary override time, a novel measure describing the time at which voluntary processes begin to overcome automated processes on anti-saccade trials. Drawing on converging animal neurophysiology, human neuroimaging, and computational modeling literature, we propose potential frontal-parietal and frontal-striatal mechanisms that may mediate the behavioral changes revealed in our analysis. We liken the models presented here to "cognitive growth curves" which have important implications for improved detection of neurological disease states that emerge during vulnerable windows of developing and aging.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rachel Yep
- Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen’s University, Kingston, ON, Canada
| | | | - Heidi C. Riek
- Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen’s University, Kingston, ON, Canada
| | - Olivia G. Calancie
- Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen’s University, Kingston, ON, Canada
| | - Ryan H. Kirkpatrick
- Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen’s University, Kingston, ON, Canada
- Department of Medicine, Queen’s University, Kingston, ON, Canada
| | - Julia E. Perkins
- Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen’s University, Kingston, ON, Canada
| | - Jeff Huang
- Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen’s University, Kingston, ON, Canada
| | - Brian C. Coe
- Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen’s University, Kingston, ON, Canada
| | - Donald C. Brien
- Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen’s University, Kingston, ON, Canada
| | - Douglas P. Munoz
- Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen’s University, Kingston, ON, Canada
- Department of Medicine, Queen’s University, Kingston, ON, Canada
- Department of Biomedical and Molecular Sciences, Queen’s University, Kingston, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Galli J, Loi E, Molinaro A, Calza S, Franzoni A, Micheletti S, Rossi A, Semeraro F, Fazzi E. Age-Related Effects on the Spectrum of Cerebral Visual Impairment in Children With Cerebral Palsy. Front Hum Neurosci 2022; 16:750464. [PMID: 35308614 PMCID: PMC8924515 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.750464] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/30/2021] [Accepted: 01/17/2022] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Cerebral Visual Impairment (CVI) is a very common finding in children affected by Cerebral Palsy (CP). In this paper we studied the characteristics of CVI of a large group of children with CP and CVI, describing their neurovisual profiles according to three different age subgroups (subgroup 1: infants 6 months–2 years; subgroup 2: pre-school age 3–5 years; subgroup 3: school age ≥ 6 years). Methods We enrolled 180 subjects (104 males, mean age 66 ± 42.6 months; range 6–192 months) with CP and CVI for the study. We carried out a demographic and clinical data collection, neurological examination, developmental or cognitive assessment, and a video-recorded visual function assessment including an evaluation of ophthalmological characteristics, oculomotor functions, and basic visual functions. In school-aged children, we also performed an evaluation of their cognitive-visual profiles. Results There were signs of CVI in all the three subgroups. Subgroup 1 (62 children) and subgroup 2 (50 children) were different for fixation (p = 0.02), visual acuity (p = 0.03) and contrast sensitivity (p < 0.01), being more frequently impaired in younger children. Comparing subgroup 2 with subgroup 3 (68 children), the older children presented more frequently myopia (p = 0.02) while the younger ones esotropia (p = 0.02) and alteration in smooth pursuit (p = 0.03) and saccades (p < 0.01). Furthermore, fixation, smooth pursuit, visual acuity, contrast sensitivity and visual filed (p < 0.01) were more frequently impaired in younger children (subgroup 1) compared to the older ones. Multiple correspondence analysis (MCA) confirmed the different neurovisual profiles according to age: younger children with CP showed more signs of CVI compared to the older ones. 34 out of 68 children belonging to subgroup 3 underwent the cognitive visual evaluation; an impairment of cognitive visual skills was detected in 21 subjects. Conclusion Younger children with CP showed more signs of CVI compared to the older ones, likely for the physiological maturation of visual system and mechanisms of neuroplasticity. In this direction, we suggest an early neurovisual evaluation to detect any weak visual functions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jessica Galli
- Department of Clinical and Experimental Sciences, University of Brescia, Brescia, Italy
- Unit of Child Neurology and Psychiatry, ASST Spedali Civili of Brescia, Brescia, Italy
- *Correspondence: Jessica Galli,
| | - Erika Loi
- Unit of Child Neurology and Psychiatry, ASST Spedali Civili of Brescia, Brescia, Italy
- Department of Molecular and Translational Medicine, University of Brescia, Brescia, Italy
| | - Anna Molinaro
- Unit of Child Neurology and Psychiatry, ASST Spedali Civili of Brescia, Brescia, Italy
| | - Stefano Calza
- BDbiomed, BODaI Lab, University of Brescia, Brescia, Italy
| | - Alessandra Franzoni
- Department of Neurological and Vision Sciences, ASST Spedali Civili of Brescia, Brescia, Italy
| | - Serena Micheletti
- Unit of Child Neurology and Psychiatry, ASST Spedali Civili of Brescia, Brescia, Italy
| | - Andrea Rossi
- Unit of Child Neurology and Psychiatry, ASST Spedali Civili of Brescia, Brescia, Italy
| | - Francesco Semeraro
- Department of Neurological and Vision Sciences, ASST Spedali Civili of Brescia, Brescia, Italy
- Eye Clinic, University of Brescia, Brescia, Italy
| | - Elisa Fazzi
- Department of Clinical and Experimental Sciences, University of Brescia, Brescia, Italy
- Unit of Child Neurology and Psychiatry, ASST Spedali Civili of Brescia, Brescia, Italy
| | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Avni I, Meiri G, Michaelovski A, Menashe I, Shmuelof L, Dinstein I. Basic oculomotor function is similar in young children with ASD and typically developing controls. Autism Res 2021; 14:2580-2591. [PMID: 34405961 DOI: 10.1002/aur.2592] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/21/2020] [Revised: 06/29/2021] [Accepted: 07/28/2021] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
A variety of eye tracking studies have demonstrated that young children with ASD gaze at images and movies of social interactions differently than typically developing children. These findings have supported the hypothesis that gaze behavior differences are generated by a weaker preference for social stimuli in ASD children. The hypothesis assumes that gaze differences are not caused by abnormalities in oculomotor function including saccade frequency and kinematics. Previous studies of oculomotor function have mostly been performed with school-age children, adolescents, and adults using visual search, anti-saccade, and gap saccade tasks that are less suitable for young pre-school children. Here, we examined oculomotor function in 144 children (90 with ASD and 54 controls), 1-10-years-old, as they watched two animated movies interleaved with the presentation of multiple salient stimuli that elicited saccades-to-targets. The results revealed that the number of fixations, fixation duration, number of saccades, saccade duration, saccade accuracy, and saccade latency did not differ significantly across groups. Minor initial differences in saccade peak velocity were not supported by analysis with a linear mixed model. These findings suggest that most children with ASD exhibit similar oculomotor function to that of controls, when performing saccades-to-targets or freely viewing child-friendly movies. This suggests that previously reported gaze abnormalities in children with ASD are not due to underlying oculomotor deficiencies. LAY SUMMARY: This study demonstrates that children with ASD perform similar eye movements to those of controls when freely observing movies or making eye movements to targets. Similar results were apparent across groups in the number of eye movements, their accuracy, duration, and other measures that assess eye movement control. These findings are important for interpreting previously reported differences in gaze behavior of children with ASD, which are likely due to atypical social preferences rather than impaired control of eye movements.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Inbar Avni
- Cognitive and Brain Sciences Department, Ben Gurion University, Beer Sheva, Israel.,National Autism Research Center of Israel, Ben Gurion University, Beer Sheva, Israel
| | - Gal Meiri
- National Autism Research Center of Israel, Ben Gurion University, Beer Sheva, Israel.,Pre-school Psychiatry Unit, Soroka University Medical Center, Beer Sheba, Israel
| | - Analya Michaelovski
- National Autism Research Center of Israel, Ben Gurion University, Beer Sheva, Israel.,Zusman Child Development Center, Soroka University Medical Center, Beer Sheva, Israel
| | - Idan Menashe
- National Autism Research Center of Israel, Ben Gurion University, Beer Sheva, Israel.,Public Health Department, Ben-Gurion University, Beer Sheva, Israel
| | - Lior Shmuelof
- Cognitive and Brain Sciences Department, Ben Gurion University, Beer Sheva, Israel
| | - Ilan Dinstein
- Cognitive and Brain Sciences Department, Ben Gurion University, Beer Sheva, Israel.,National Autism Research Center of Israel, Ben Gurion University, Beer Sheva, Israel.,Psychology Department, Ben Gurion University, Beer Sheva, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Properties of fast vergence eye movements and horizontal saccades in athletes. Physiol Behav 2021; 235:113397. [PMID: 33775660 DOI: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2021.113397] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/01/2021] [Revised: 03/19/2021] [Accepted: 03/21/2021] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Visual information based on eye movements influences sports performance in 3-D space. The purpose of this study was to clarify the properties of fast vergence eye movements and horizontal saccades in athletes. Thirty-four college students were classified into three groups: twelve baseball players (BAS), ten track and field athletes (TRA) and twelve non-athletes (NON). The oculomotor tasks consisted of vergence eye movements (vergence task) including convergence and divergence, and horizontal saccades (saccade task). The participants made a rapid gaze shift toward a visual target. For the vergence task, the target was irradiated at 5 different vergence angles (3, 5, 10, 15, 20°) randomly. For the saccade task, the target was irradiated at 5 different saccade amplitudes (0, ±5, ±10°) randomly. The results showed that the peak eye velocity was higher in order of saccade, convergence, and divergence eye movements. The gain of convergence and saccades was higher than that of divergence. Furthermore, the latency of saccades was shorter than that of convergence and divergence. These results suggest that saccades and convergence are superior compared with divergence, and the dynamic property of convergence is partially analogous to horizontal saccades. The results of the comparison between the three groups showed that the latency of convergence was shorter for BAS than for NON. Therefore, different visual experience and baseball training could influence the gaze-shift dynamics during convergence eye movements.
Collapse
|
13
|
Franzen L, Stark Z, Johnson AP. Individuals with dyslexia use a different visual sampling strategy to read text. Sci Rep 2021; 11:6449. [PMID: 33742007 PMCID: PMC7979812 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-021-84945-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/16/2020] [Accepted: 02/18/2021] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Individuals with dyslexia present with reading-related deficits including inaccurate and/or less fluent word recognition and poor decoding abilities. Slow reading speed and worse text comprehension can occur as secondary consequences of these deficits. Reports of visual symptoms such as atypical eye movements during reading gave rise to a search for these deficits' underlying mechanisms. This study sought to replicate established behavioral deficits in reading and cognitive processing speed while investigating their underlying mechanisms in more detail by developing a comprehensive profile of eye movements specific to reading in adult dyslexia. Using a validated standardized reading assessment, our findings confirm a reading speed deficit among adults with dyslexia. We observed different eye movements in readers with dyslexia across numerous eye movement metrics including the duration of a stop (i.e., fixation), the length of jumps (i.e., saccades), and the number of times a reader's eyes expressed a jump atypical for reading. We conclude that individuals with dyslexia visually sample written information in a laborious and more effortful manner that is fundamentally different from those without dyslexia. Our findings suggest a mix of aberrant cognitive linguistic and oculomotor processes being present in adults with dyslexia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Léon Franzen
- Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Montréal, Canada.
- Institute of Neuroscience and Psychology, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, UK.
| | - Zoey Stark
- Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Montréal, Canada
| | - Aaron P Johnson
- Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Montréal, Canada
- CRIR/Lethbridge-Layton-Mackay Centre de Réadaptation du CIUSSS du Centre-Ouest-de-l'Île-de-Montréal, Montréal, Canada
- Réseau de Recherche en Santé de La Vision, Montréal, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Overbye K, Walhovd KB, Fjell AM, Tamnes CK, Huster RJ. Electrophysiological and behavioral indices of cognitive conflict processing across adolescence. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2021; 48:100929. [PMID: 33549993 PMCID: PMC7868601 DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2021.100929] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/12/2020] [Revised: 01/15/2021] [Accepted: 01/22/2021] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Cognitive control enables goal-oriented adaptation to a fast-changing environment and has a protracted development spanning into young adulthood. The neurocognitive processes underlying this development are poorly understood. In a cross-sectional sample of participants 8-19 years old (n = 108), we used blind source separation of EEG data recorded in a Flanker task to derive electrophysiological measures of attention and conflict processing, including a N2-like frontal negative component and a P3-like parietal positive component. Outside the recording session, we examined multiple behavioral measures of interference control derived from the Flanker, Stroop, and Anti-saccade tasks. We found a positive association between age and P3 amplitude, but no relationship between age and N2 amplitude. A stronger N2 was age-independently related to better performance on Stroop and Anti-saccade measures of interference control. A Gratton effect was found on the Flanker task, with slower reaction times on current congruent and better accuracy on current incongruent trials when preceded by incongruent as opposed to congruent trials. The Gratton effect on accuracy was positively associated with age. Together, the findings suggest a multifaceted developmental pattern of the neurocognitive processes involved in conflict processing across adolescence, with a more protracted development of the P3 compared to the N2.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Knut Overbye
- Center for Lifespan Changes in Brain and Cognition, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway.
| | - Kristine B Walhovd
- Center for Lifespan Changes in Brain and Cognition, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway; Department of Radiology and Nuclear Medicine, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway
| | - Anders M Fjell
- Center for Lifespan Changes in Brain and Cognition, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway; Department of Radiology and Nuclear Medicine, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway
| | - Christian K Tamnes
- PROMENTA Research Center, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway; Department of Psychiatric Research, Diakonhjemmet Hospital, Oslo, Norway; NORMENT, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
| | - Rene J Huster
- Multimodal Imaging and Cognitive Control Lab, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway; Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience Cluster, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Stewart EEM, Hübner C, Schütz AC. Stronger saccadic suppression of displacement and blanking effect in children. J Vis 2020; 20:13. [PMID: 33052408 PMCID: PMC7571331 DOI: 10.1167/jov.20.10.13] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/01/2020] [Accepted: 09/07/2020] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Humans do not notice small displacements to objects that occur during saccades, termed saccadic suppression of displacement (SSD), and this effect is reduced when a blank is introduced between the pre- and postsaccadic stimulus (Bridgeman, Hendry, & Stark, 1975; Deubel, Schneider, & Bridgeman, 1996). While these effects have been studied extensively in adults, it is unclear how these phenomena are characterized in children. A potentially related mechanism, saccadic suppression of contrast sensitivity-a prerequisite to achieve a stable percept-is stronger for children (Bruno, Brambati, Perani, & Morrone, 2006). However, the evidence for how transsaccadic stimulus displacements may be suppressed or integrated is mixed. While they can integrate basic visual feature information from an early age, they cannot integrate multisensory information (Gori, Viva, Sandini, & Burr, 2008; Nardini, Jones, Bedford, & Braddick, 2008), suggesting a failure in the ability to integrate more complex sensory information. We tested children 7 to 12 years old and adults 19 to 23 years old on their ability to perceive intrasaccadic stimulus displacements, with and without a postsaccadic blank. Results showed that children had stronger SSD than adults and a larger blanking effect. Children also had larger undershoots and more variability in their initial saccade endpoints, indicating greater intrinsic uncertainty, and they were faster in executing corrective saccades to account for these errors. Together, these results suggest that children may have a greater internal expectation or prediction of saccade error than adults; thus, the stronger SSD in children may be due to higher intrinsic uncertainty in target localization or saccade execution.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emma E M Stewart
- Allgemeine und Biologische Psychologie, Philipps-Universität Marburg, Marburg, Germany
| | - Carolin Hübner
- Allgemeine und Biologische Psychologie, Philipps-Universität Marburg, Marburg, Germany
| | - Alexander C Schütz
- Allgemeine und Biologische Psychologie, Philipps-Universität Marburg, Marburg, Germany
- Center for Mind, Brain and Behaviour, Philipps-Universität Marburg, Marburg, Germany
- https://www.uni-marburg.de/en/fb04/team-schuetz/team/alexander-schutz
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Hemptinne C, Deravet N, Orban de Xivry JJ, Lefèvre P, Yüksel D. Influence of prior and visual information on eye movements in amblyopic children. J Comput Neurosci 2020; 49:333-343. [PMID: 32901334 DOI: 10.1007/s10827-020-00764-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/05/2020] [Revised: 08/10/2020] [Accepted: 08/31/2020] [Indexed: 01/17/2023]
Abstract
This study analyzed the characteristics of pursuit and assessed the influence of prior and visual information on eye velocity and saccades in amblyopic and control children, in comparison to adults. Eye movements of 41 children (21 amblyopes and 20 controls) were compared to eye movements of 55 adults (18 amblyopes and 37 controls). Participants were asked to pursue a target moving at a constant velocity. The target was either a 'standard' target, with a uniform color intensity, or a 'noisy' target, with blurry edges, to mimic the blurriness of an amblyopic eye. Analysis of pursuit patterns showed that the onset was delayed, and the gain was decreased in control children with a noisy target in comparison to amblyopic or control children with a standard target. Furthermore, a significant effect of prior and visual information on pursuit velocity and saccades was found across all participants. Moreover, the modulation of the effect of visual information on the pursuit velocity by group, that is amblyopes or controls with a standard target, and controls with a noisy target, was more limited in children. In other words, the effect of visual information was higher in control adults with a standard target compared to control children with the same target. However, in the case of a blurry target, either in control participants with a noisy target or in amblyopic participants with a standard target, the effect of visual information was larger in children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Coralie Hemptinne
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain, La-Neuve, Belgium. .,Ophthalmology Department, Cliniques Universitaires Saint-Luc, Brussels, Belgium.
| | - Nicolas Deravet
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain, La-Neuve, Belgium.,Institute of Information and Communication Technologies, Electronics, and Applied Mathematics, Louvain, La-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Jean-Jacques Orban de Xivry
- Department of Movement Sciences, Movement Control and Neuroplasticity Research Group, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Leuven, Belgium.,Leuven Brain Institute, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Philippe Lefèvre
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain, La-Neuve, Belgium.,Institute of Information and Communication Technologies, Electronics, and Applied Mathematics, Louvain, La-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Demet Yüksel
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain, La-Neuve, Belgium.,Ophthalmology Department, Cliniques Universitaires Saint-Luc, Brussels, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Shahin YM, Meier K, Giaschi D. Effect of Visual Field Location on Global Motion Perception: A Developmental Study. Perception 2020; 49:733-748. [PMID: 32673188 DOI: 10.1177/0301006620930901] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Previous work has shown that motion perception in school-age children is similar to that of adults for fast speeds but is immature at slow speeds for stimuli presented in the central visual field. This study examined whether visual field location affects this developmental pattern. We measured left/right and up/down global motion direction discrimination for fast and slow speeds in 7- to 10-year-old children and in adults with stimuli presented to upper, central, or lower visual fields. For left/right direction discrimination, children showed significantly higher (worse) coherence thresholds than adults for slow, but not fast, speeds in the central visual field. In the upper and lower visual fields, children showed significantly higher coherence thresholds than adults for both speeds. For up/down direction discrimination, children showed similar performance to adults for the central visual field. In the upper and lower visual fields, children performed significantly worse than adults; this finding was speed-tuned only for the lower visual field. Thus, children show immature global motion perception in the periphery even when performance in central vision is adult-like. These results enrich our understanding of motion perception development in children with typical vision.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yousef M Shahin
- Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, University of British Columbia, British Columbia, Canada
| | - Kimberly Meier
- Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Washington, United States; Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, University of British Columbia, British Columbia, Canada
| | - Deborah Giaschi
- Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, University of British Columbia, British Columbia, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Inhibiting saccades to a social stimulus: a developmental study. Sci Rep 2020; 10:4615. [PMID: 32165671 PMCID: PMC7067843 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-61188-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/28/2019] [Accepted: 02/07/2020] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
Faces are an important source of social signal throughout the lifespan. In adults, they have a prioritized access to the orienting system. Here we investigate when this effect emerges during development. We tested 139 children, early adolescents, adolescents and adults in a mixed pro- and anti-saccades task with faces, cars or noise patterns as visual targets. We observed an improvement in performance until about 15 years of age, replicating studies that used only meaningless stimuli as targets. Also, as previously reported, we observed that adults made more direction errors to faces than abstract patterns and cars. The children showed this effect too with regards to noise patterns but it was not specific since performance for cars and faces did not differ. The adolescents, in contrast, made more errors for faces than for cars but as many errors for noise patterns and faces. In all groups latencies for pro-saccades were faster towards faces. We discuss these findings with regards to the development of executive control in childhood and adolescence and the influence of social stimuli at different ages.
Collapse
|
19
|
Pradhan GN, Bogle JM, Cevette MJ, Stepanek J. Discovering Oculometric Patterns to Detect Cognitive Performance Changes in Healthy Youth Football Athletes. JOURNAL OF HEALTHCARE INFORMATICS RESEARCH 2019; 3:371-392. [PMID: 35415432 PMCID: PMC8982780 DOI: 10.1007/s41666-019-00045-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/21/2018] [Revised: 11/20/2018] [Accepted: 01/07/2019] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
In this paper, we focus on the application of oculometric patterns extracted from raw eye movements during a mental workload task to assess changes in cognitive performance in healthy youth athletes over the course of a typical sport season. Oculometric features pertaining to fixations and saccades were measured on 116 athletes in pre- and post-season testing. Participants were between 7 and 14 years of age at pre-season testing. Due to varied developmental rates, there were large interindividual performance differences during a mental workload task consisting of reading numbers. Based on different reading speeds, we classified three profiles (slow, moderate, and fast) and established their corresponding baselines for oculometric data. Within each profile, we describe changes in oculomotor function based on changes in cognitive performance during the season. To visualize these changes in multidimensional oculometric data, we also present a multidimensional visualization tool named DiViTo (diagnostic visualization tool). These experimental, computational informatics and visualization methodologies may serve to utilize oculometric information to detect changes in cognitive performance due to mild or severe cognitive impairment such as concussion/mild traumatic brain injury, as well as possibly other disorders such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorders, learning/reading disabilities, impairment of alertness, and neurocognitive function.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gaurav N. Pradhan
- Aerospace Medicine and Vestibular Research Laboratory, Mayo Clinic Arizona, 13400 E Shea Blvd, Scottsdale, AZ 85259 USA
| | - Jamie M. Bogle
- Aerospace Medicine and Vestibular Research Laboratory, Mayo Clinic Arizona, 13400 E Shea Blvd, Scottsdale, AZ 85259 USA
| | - Michael J. Cevette
- Aerospace Medicine and Vestibular Research Laboratory, Mayo Clinic Arizona, 13400 E Shea Blvd, Scottsdale, AZ 85259 USA
| | - Jan Stepanek
- Aerospace Medicine and Vestibular Research Laboratory, Mayo Clinic Arizona, 13400 E Shea Blvd, Scottsdale, AZ 85259 USA
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Vinuela-Navarro V, Erichsen JT, Williams C, Woodhouse JM. Quantitative Characterization of Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements in School-Age Children Using a Child-Friendly Setup. Transl Vis Sci Technol 2019; 8:8. [PMID: 31588373 PMCID: PMC6753964 DOI: 10.1167/tvst.8.5.8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/11/2019] [Accepted: 07/05/2019] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
PURPOSE It could be argued that current studies investigating smooth pursuit development in children do not provide an optimal measure of smooth pursuit characteristics, given that a significant number have failed to adjust their setup and procedures to the child population. This study aimed to characterize smooth pursuit in children using child-friendly stimuli and procedures. METHODS Eye movements were recorded in 169 children (4-11 years) and 10 adults, while a customized, animated stimulus was presented moving horizontally and vertically at 6°/s and 12°/s. Eye movement recordings from 43 children with delayed reading, two with nystagmus, two with strabismus, and two with unsuccessful calibration were excluded from the analysis. Velocity gain, proportion of smooth pursuit, and the number and amplitude of saccades during smooth pursuit were calculated for the remaining participants. Median and quartiles were calculated for each age group and pursuit condition. ANOVA was used to investigate the effect of age on smooth pursuit parameters. RESULTS Differences across ages were found in velocity gain (6°/s P < 0.01; 12°/s P < 0.05), as well as the number (12°/s P < 0.05) and amplitude of saccades (12°/s P < 0.05), for horizontal smooth pursuit. Post hoc tests showed that these parameters were different between children aged 7 or younger and adults. No significant differences were found across ages in any smooth pursuit parameter for the vertical direction (P > 0.05). CONCLUSIONS Using child-friendly methods, children over the age of 7 to 8 years demonstrated adultlike smooth pursuit. TRANSLATIONAL RELEVANCE Child-friendly procedures are critical for appropriately characterizing smooth pursuit eye movements in children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Cathy Williams
- Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, Bristol University, Bristol, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
21
|
Barsingerhorn AD, Boonstra FN, Goossens J. Saccade latencies during a preferential looking task and objective scoring of grating acuity in children with and without visual impairments. Acta Ophthalmol 2019; 97:616-625. [PMID: 30734501 PMCID: PMC6767583 DOI: 10.1111/aos.14011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/09/2018] [Accepted: 10/26/2018] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
PURPOSE We determined the latencies of orienting responses during a preferential looking task in children with normal vision and in children with visual impairments between 6 and 12 years old, and assessed the feasibility of scoring grating detection in these populations with video-based eye tracking. METHODS Children performed a computerized preferential looking test, while a remote eye tracker measured the children's eye movements. The stimuli consisted of a 2 × 2 grid, with three uniform grey fields and one target field consisting of a black-and-white square wave grating. The grating was presented randomly at one of the four locations. The spatial frequencies (1.05, 2.11 and 7.02 cyc/deg) were randomly interleaved, with 10 trials per spatial frequency. Three different methods were used to score the accuracy of the responses: (1) primary saccade ends on target, (1) gaze 50% of the presentation time on target, and (3) a combination of method 1 and 2 (i.e. primary saccade ends on target, and/or gaze 50% of the presentation time on target). RESULTS The combined scoring method was most reliable to determine whether children could resolve the gratings. Children with visual impairments had significantly lower accuracies than children with normal vision with all three scoring methods. In addition, saccade latencies decreased with age and were significantly longer (62 ± 15 ms) in children with visual impairments. CONCLUSION The use of eye tracking to assess grating detection with a preferential looking task in clinical populations provides valuable additional information, including objective detection measures and developmental delays in saccade latencies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Annemiek D. Barsingerhorn
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Biophysics SectionDonders Institute for Brain, Cognition and BehaviourRadboud University Medical Centre NijmegenNijmegenThe Netherlands
- Department of BiophysicsDonders Institute for Brain, Cognition and BehaviourRadboud University NijmegenNijmegenThe Netherlands
| | - F. Nienke Boonstra
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Biophysics SectionDonders Institute for Brain, Cognition and BehaviourRadboud University Medical Centre NijmegenNijmegenThe Netherlands
- Royal Dutch VisioNational Foundation for the Visually Impaired and BlindHuizenThe Netherlands
- Bartiméus, Institute for the Visually ImpairedZeistThe Netherlands
| | - Jeroen Goossens
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Biophysics SectionDonders Institute for Brain, Cognition and BehaviourRadboud University Medical Centre NijmegenNijmegenThe Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Mazumdar D, Meethal NSK, Panday M, Asokan R, Thepass G, George RJ, van der Steen J, Pel JJM. Effect of Age, Sex, Stimulus Intensity, and Eccentricity on Saccadic Reaction Time in Eye Movement Perimetry. Transl Vis Sci Technol 2019; 8:13. [PMID: 31388465 PMCID: PMC6670042 DOI: 10.1167/tvst.8.4.13] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/28/2019] [Accepted: 05/13/2019] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Purpose In eye movement perimetry (EMP), the extent of the visual field is tested by assessing the saccades using an eye tracker. The aim of the present study was to determine the effects of age and sex of the subjects, the eccentricity and intensity of the peripheral stimuli on saccadic reaction time (SRT), and the interaction between these parameters in healthy participants. Methods Healthy participants aged between 20 to 70 years underwent a complete ophthalmic examination and an EMP test. SRT was determined from detected peripheral stimuli of four intensity levels. A multilevel mixed-model analysis was used to verify the influence of subject and stimulus characteristics on SRT within the tested visual field. Results Ninety-five subjects (mean age 43.0 [15.0] years) were included. Age, stimulus intensity, and eccentricity had a statistically significant effect on SRT, not sex. SRTs were significantly faster with increasing stimulus intensity and decreasing eccentricity (P < 0.001). At the lowest stimulus intensity of 192 cd/m2, a significant interaction was found between age and eccentricity. Conclusions The current study demonstrated significant SRT dependence across the visual field measured up to 27°, irrespective of sex. The presented SRT values may serve as a first normative guide for EMP. Translational Relevance This report of SRT interaction can aid in refining its use as a measure of visual field responsiveness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Deepmala Mazumdar
- Vestibular and Ocular Motor Research Group, Department of Neuroscience, Erasmus MC, the Netherlands.,Medical and Vision Research Foundation, Chennai, India
| | - Najiya S Kadavath Meethal
- Vestibular and Ocular Motor Research Group, Department of Neuroscience, Erasmus MC, the Netherlands.,Medical and Vision Research Foundation, Chennai, India
| | - Manish Panday
- Medical and Vision Research Foundation, Chennai, India
| | - Rashima Asokan
- Medical and Vision Research Foundation, Chennai, India.,Elite School of Optometry, Chennai, India
| | - Gijs Thepass
- Vestibular and Ocular Motor Research Group, Department of Neuroscience, Erasmus MC, the Netherlands
| | | | - Johannes van der Steen
- Vestibular and Ocular Motor Research Group, Department of Neuroscience, Erasmus MC, the Netherlands.,Royal Dutch Visio, Huizen, the Netherlands
| | - Johan J M Pel
- Medical and Vision Research Foundation, Chennai, India
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Perdziak M, Gryncewicz W, Witkowska D, Sawosz P, Ober J. Gap effect and express saccades generation in amblyopia. J Vis 2019; 19:17. [PMID: 30977772 DOI: 10.1167/19.4.17] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Amblyopia is a neurodevelopmental vision disorder that is associated with abnormal visual stimulation during early childhood. Although our knowledge regarding spatial vision deficits in amblyopic subjects is well established, the neural control of eye movements in amblyopia is yet to be explored. In the present study we have evaluated the gap effect, and for the first time (to our best knowledge), express saccades generation in amblyopic (strabismic as well as anisometropic) and age-matched control subjects. We have compared the saccadic latency under different gap conditions ("no gap," 50 ms gap, and 200 ms gap), between the amblyopic and control groups. Our results have shown that saccadic latency was reduced during the gap paradigms both for amblyopic and control groups for all viewing conditions. Furthermore, the size of the gap effect was comparable for all groups and viewing conditions (both for short and long gap durations). In addition, consistent with previous results, the amblyopic eye has manifested an increased saccadic latency as compared to the nondominant eye in the control group. Regarding the occurrence of express saccades, the 200 ms gap condition was associated with an increased number of express saccades as compared to 50 ms gap and "no gap" conditions, both for amblyopic and control subjects. We did not observe any significant difference in terms of express saccades production between the control and amblyopic subjects. Our findings may suggest that amblyopia does not alter physiological mechanisms related to the efficiency of visual attention/fixation disengagement as supported by the observation that the gap effect and express saccades production was comparable between the normal and amblyopic subjects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maciej Perdziak
- Nalecz Institute of Biocybernetics and Biomedical Engineering, Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland.,Laboratory of Vision Science and Optometry, Faculty of Physics, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznan, Poland
| | - Wojciech Gryncewicz
- Nalecz Institute of Biocybernetics and Biomedical Engineering, Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland
| | - Dagmara Witkowska
- Nalecz Institute of Biocybernetics and Biomedical Engineering, Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland
| | - Piotr Sawosz
- Nalecz Institute of Biocybernetics and Biomedical Engineering, Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland
| | - Jan Ober
- Nalecz Institute of Biocybernetics and Biomedical Engineering, Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Developing attentional control in naturalistic dynamic road crossing situations. Sci Rep 2019; 9:4176. [PMID: 30862845 PMCID: PMC6414534 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-39737-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/17/2018] [Accepted: 01/24/2019] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
In the last 20 years, there has been increasing interest in studying visual attentional processes under more natural conditions. In the present study, we propose to determine the critical age at which children show similar to adult performance and attentional control in a visually guided task; in a naturalistic dynamic and socially relevant context: road crossing. We monitored visual exploration and crossing decisions in adults and children aged between 5 and 15 while they watched road traffic videos containing a range of traffic densities with or without pedestrians. 5–10 year old (y/o) children showed less systematic gaze patterns. More specifically, adults and 11–15 y/o children look mainly at the vehicles’ appearing point, which is an optimal location to sample diagnostic information for the task. In contrast, 5–10 y/os look more at socially relevant stimuli and attend to moving vehicles further down the trajectory when the traffic density is high. Critically, 5-10 y/o children also make an increased number of crossing decisions compared to 11–15 y/os and adults. Our findings reveal a critical shift around 10 y/o in attentional control and crossing decisions in a road crossing task.
Collapse
|
25
|
van Es DM, Knapen T. Implicit and explicit learning in reactive and voluntary saccade adaptation. PLoS One 2019; 14:e0203248. [PMID: 30650083 PMCID: PMC6334942 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0203248] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/16/2018] [Accepted: 12/18/2018] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Saccades can either be elicited automatically by salient peripheral stimuli or can additionally depend on explicit cognitive goals. Similarly, it is thought that motor adaptation is driven by the combination of a more automatic, implicit process and a more explicit, cognitive process. However, the degree to which such implicit and explicit learning contribute to the adaptation of more reactive and voluntary saccades remains elusive. To study this question, we employed a global saccadic adaptation paradigm with both increasing and decreasing saccade amplitudes. We assessed the resulting adaptation using a dual state model of motor adaptation. This model decomposes learning into a fast and slow process, which are thought to constitute explicit and implicit learning, respectively. Our results show that adaptation of reactive saccades is equally driven by fast and slow learning, while fast learning is nearly absent when adapting voluntary (i.e. scanning) saccades. This pattern of results was present both when saccade gain was increased or decreased. Our results suggest that the increased cognitive demands associated with voluntary compared to reactive saccade planning interfere specifically with explicit learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel Marten van Es
- Behavioural and Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Noord-Holland, the Netherlands
| | - Tomas Knapen
- Behavioural and Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Noord-Holland, the Netherlands
- Spinoza Centre for Neuroimaging, Royal Academy of Sciences, Amsterdam, Noord-Holland, the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Abstract
Oculomotor evaluation as part of videonystagmography is an integral tool in the assessment of vestibular function providing a global assessment of the neurological pathways associated with oculomotor function. The value of an oculomotor evaluation for pediatric evaluation is well established; however, many questions can also arise with the application to the pediatric population. Oculomotor function is age dependent which can have a significant effect on the test results obtain in children. The underlying neural substrates and age effects are discussed across the literature with specific results from recent research using clinical oculomotor equipment and protocols. The evidence suggests there are several key differences in the pediatric population compared with adults. These include longer saccade latencies, reduced smooth pursuit gain, increased optokinetic asymmetry, increased variability in all responses, and increased artifact in saccade and smooth pursuit testing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Steven M Doettl
- Department of Audiology and Speech Pathology, University of Tennessee Health Science Center, College of Health Professions, Knoxville, Tennessee
| | - Devin L McCaslin
- Division of Audiology, Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Mayo Clinic College of Medicine, Rochester, Minnesota
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Simkin SK, Misra SL, Kasture A, McGhee CN, Dai S. Clinical applicability of the Saccadic Vector Optokinetic Perimeter in children with and without visual impairment. Clin Exp Optom 2018; 102:70-78. [PMID: 29938834 DOI: 10.1111/cxo.12803] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/05/2017] [Revised: 04/22/2018] [Accepted: 05/24/2018] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Common paediatric perimetry techniques require central fixation and subjective responses, which may be challenging for young or cognitively impaired children. The Saccadic Vector Optokinetic Perimeter (SVOP) was designed to overcome these challenges by using infra-red eye tracking to assess the visual field. This study assessed the clinical feasibility of SVOP in children without visual impairment, comparing it to current paediatric techniques, and in children with visual impairment. METHODS Participants were recruited into two cohorts: children without visual impairment (visual acuity ≥ 6/7.5) and children with visual impairment (visual acuity ≤ 6/18). Children without visual impairment attempted the Goldmann perimeter, confrontation method, and SVOP. Children with visual impairment attempted SVOP, when clinically indicated, as part of a full ophthalmic assessment. Visual field results and test length were recorded. RESULTS Twenty-three children without visual impairment (4-14 years old) attempted all three visual field assessments. Full results were recorded for 91 per cent of children with SVOP, 87 per cent with Goldmann, and 100 per cent with confrontation SVOP was significantly faster than Goldmann (p < 0.001) and confrontation (p = 0.003). Thirty-five children with visual impairment (3-19 years old) with visual acuity ranging from 6/9.5 to worse than 6/360 (mean of 0.8 ± 0.4 logMAR) attempted SVOP, with 26 children able to complete SVOP. DISCUSSION This is the largest study to date of the clinical applicability of SVOP in children without visual impairment, as well as assessing the utility of SVOP in children with visual impairment. Further research is needed to assess the accuracy of SVOP in a range of paediatric ocular disorders. SVOP may potentially offer a visual field assessment method for children previously unable to complete current paediatric perimetry techniques.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Samantha K Simkin
- Department of Ophthalmology, New Zealand National Eye Centre, Faculty of Medical and Health Sciences, The University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
| | - Stuti L Misra
- Department of Ophthalmology, New Zealand National Eye Centre, Faculty of Medical and Health Sciences, The University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
| | - Apurva Kasture
- Department of Ophthalmology, New Zealand National Eye Centre, Faculty of Medical and Health Sciences, The University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
| | - Charles Nj McGhee
- Department of Ophthalmology, New Zealand National Eye Centre, Faculty of Medical and Health Sciences, The University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand.,Department of Ophthalmology, Auckland District Health Board, Auckland, New Zealand
| | - Shuan Dai
- Department of Ophthalmology, New Zealand National Eye Centre, Faculty of Medical and Health Sciences, The University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand.,Department of Ophthalmology, Auckland District Health Board, Auckland, New Zealand
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Luke SG, Asplund A. Prereaders’ eye movements during shared storybook reading are language-mediated but not predictive. VISUAL COGNITION 2018. [DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2018.1452323] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/17/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Steven G. Luke
- Department of Psychology, Brigham Young University, Provo, UT, USA
| | - Anna Asplund
- Department of Psychology, Brigham Young University, Provo, UT, USA
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Krishna O, Helo A, Rämä P, Aizawa K. Gaze distribution analysis and saliency prediction across age groups. PLoS One 2018; 13:e0193149. [PMID: 29474378 PMCID: PMC5825055 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0193149] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/21/2017] [Accepted: 02/04/2018] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Knowledge of the human visual system helps to develop better computational models of visual attention. State-of-the-art models have been developed to mimic the visual attention system of young adults that, however, largely ignore the variations that occur with age. In this paper, we investigated how visual scene processing changes with age and we propose an age-adapted framework that helps to develop a computational model that can predict saliency across different age groups. Our analysis uncovers how the explorativeness of an observer varies with age, how well saliency maps of an age group agree with fixation points of observers from the same or different age groups, and how age influences the center bias tendency. We analyzed the eye movement behavior of 82 observers belonging to four age groups while they explored visual scenes. Explorative- ness was quantified in terms of the entropy of a saliency map, and area under the curve (AUC) metrics was used to quantify the agreement analysis and the center bias tendency. Analysis results were used to develop age adapted saliency models. Our results suggest that the proposed age-adapted saliency model outperforms existing saliency models in predicting the regions of interest across age groups.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Onkar Krishna
- Dept. of Information and Communication Engineering, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
- * E-mail:
| | - Andrea Helo
- Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, Université Paris Descartes, Paris, France
- Departamento de Fonoaudiología, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile
| | - Pia Rämä
- Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, Université Paris Descartes, Paris, France
- CNRS (UMR 8242), Paris, France
| | - Kiyoharu Aizawa
- Dept. of Information and Communication Engineering, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Rider AT, Coutrot A, Pellicano E, Dakin SC, Mareschal I. Semantic content outweighs low-level saliency in determining children's and adults' fixation of movies. J Exp Child Psychol 2018; 166:293-309. [PMID: 28972928 PMCID: PMC5710995 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.09.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/11/2016] [Revised: 08/21/2017] [Accepted: 09/05/2017] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
To make sense of the visual world, we need to move our eyes to focus regions of interest on the high-resolution fovea. Eye movements, therefore, give us a way to infer mechanisms of visual processing and attention allocation. Here, we examined age-related differences in visual processing by recording eye movements from 37 children (aged 6-14years) and 10 adults while viewing three 5-min dynamic video clips taken from child-friendly movies. The data were analyzed in two complementary ways: (a) gaze based and (b) content based. First, similarity of scanpaths within and across age groups was examined using three different measures of variance (dispersion, clusters, and distance from center). Second, content-based models of fixation were compared to determine which of these provided the best account of our dynamic data. We found that the variance in eye movements decreased as a function of age, suggesting common attentional orienting. Comparison of the different models revealed that a model that relies on faces generally performed better than the other models tested, even for the youngest age group (<10years). However, the best predictor of a given participant's eye movements was the average of all other participants' eye movements both within the same age group and in different age groups. These findings have implications for understanding how children attend to visual information and highlight similarities in viewing strategies across development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrew T Rider
- UCL Institute of Ophthalmology, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, UK.
| | - Antoine Coutrot
- Centre for Mathematics and Physics in Life Sciences and Experimental Biology (CoMPLEX), University College London, London WC1E 6BT, UK
| | - Elizabeth Pellicano
- Centre for Research in Autism and Education (CRAE), Department of Psychology and Human Development, UCL Institute of Education, University College London, London WC1H 0AL, UK; School of Psychology, University of Western Australia, Crawley, Perth, Western Australia 6009, Australia
| | - Steven C Dakin
- UCL Institute of Ophthalmology, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, UK; School of Optometry and Vision Science, University of Auckland, Auckland 1010, New Zealand
| | | |
Collapse
|
31
|
Kunita K, Fujiwara K, Kiyota N, Yaguchi C, Kiyota T. Developmental changes in shortening of pro-saccade reaction time while maintaining neck flexion position. J Physiol Anthropol 2018; 37:2. [PMID: 29321065 PMCID: PMC5763661 DOI: 10.1186/s40101-017-0161-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/18/2017] [Accepted: 12/26/2017] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
Background We investigated developmental changes in shortening of pro-saccade reaction time while maintaining neck flexion. Methods Subjects comprised 135 children (3–14 years) and 29 young adults (19–23 years). Children were divided into six groups in 2-year age strata. Pro-saccade reaction tasks for 30 s were performed in neck rest and flexion positions. Reaction times under each position were averaged in every 10-s period. Results Under neck rest position, reaction time in the 0–10 s period was significantly longer in the 3- to 4-year-old group than in the 5- to 6-year-old group and above. No significant age effect was found for reaction time in the 0–10 s period in the 5- to 6-year-old group and above. Although a significant effect of neck flexion was not observed until the 9- to 10-year-old group, significant shortening of reaction time with neck flexion was found in the 11- to 12-year-old group and above. Furthermore, this shortening was maintained until the first 20–s period in the 11- to 12-year-old group and during the entire 30 s in the 13- to 14-year-old and above. Conclusions These results suggest that brain activation with the maintenance of neck flexion, related to shortening of the pro-saccade reaction time, was found from a later age of approximately 11 years and above, compared with the age at which information-processing function in the pro-saccade was enhanced. In addition, brain activation with the maintenance of neck flexion was sustained longer with age.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kenji Kunita
- Department of Sports Instruction, Faculty of Sports and Human, Sapporo International University, 4-1-4-1 Kiyota, Kiyota-ku, Sapporo, 004-8602, Japan.
| | - Katsuo Fujiwara
- Department of Sports and Health, Kanazawa Gakuin University, 10 Sue-machi, Kanazawa, 920-1392, Japan
| | - Naoe Kiyota
- Department of Rehabilitation, Japan Health Care College, 6-17-3 Megumino-nishi, Eniwa, 061-1373, Japan
| | - Chie Yaguchi
- Department of Rehabilitation, Japan Health Care College, 6-17-3 Megumino-nishi, Eniwa, 061-1373, Japan
| | - Takeo Kiyota
- Department of Sports Instruction, Faculty of Sports and Human, Sapporo International University, 4-1-4-1 Kiyota, Kiyota-ku, Sapporo, 004-8602, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Perdziak M, Witkowska D, Gryncewicz W, Ober J. Strabismic amblyopia affects decision processes preceding saccadic response. Biocybern Biomed Eng 2018. [DOI: 10.1016/j.bbe.2017.12.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
33
|
Coe BC, Munoz DP. Mechanisms of saccade suppression revealed in the anti-saccade task. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2017; 372:rstb.2016.0192. [PMID: 28242726 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0192] [Citation(s) in RCA: 92] [Impact Index Per Article: 13.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 12/14/2016] [Indexed: 01/03/2023] Open
Abstract
The anti-saccade task has emerged as an important tool for investigating the complex nature of voluntary behaviour. In this task, participants are instructed to suppress the natural response to look at a peripheral visual stimulus and look in the opposite direction instead. Analysis of saccadic reaction times (SRT: the time from stimulus appearance to the first saccade) and the frequency of direction errors (i.e. looking toward the stimulus) provide insight into saccade suppression mechanisms in the brain. Some direction errors are reflexive responses with very short SRTs (express latency saccades), while other direction errors are driven by automated responses and have longer SRTs. These different types of errors reveal that the anti-saccade task requires different forms of suppression, and neurophysiological experiments in macaques have revealed several potential mechanisms. At the start of an anti-saccade trial, pre-emptive top-down inhibition of saccade generating neurons in the frontal eye fields and superior colliculus must be present before the stimulus appears to prevent express latency direction errors. After the stimulus appears, voluntary anti-saccade commands must compete with, and override, automated visually initiated saccade commands to prevent longer latency direction errors. The frequencies of these types of direction errors, as well as SRTs, change throughout the lifespan and reveal time courses for development, maturation, and ageing. Additionally, patients diagnosed with a variety of neurological and/or psychiatric disorders affecting the frontal lobes and/or basal ganglia produce markedly different SRT distributions and types of direction errors, which highlight specific deficits in saccade suppression and inhibitory control. The anti-saccade task therefore provides valuable insight into the neural mechanisms of saccade suppression and is a valuable tool in a clinical setting.This article is part of the themed issue 'Movement suppression: brain mechanisms for stopping and stillness'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brian C Coe
- Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario, Canada K7l 3N6
| | - Douglas P Munoz
- Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario, Canada K7l 3N6
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Correlating Multi-dimensional Oculometrics with Cognitive Performance in Healthy Youth Athletes. JOURNAL OF HEALTHCARE INFORMATICS RESEARCH 2017; 2:132-151. [DOI: 10.1007/s41666-017-0011-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/15/2017] [Revised: 10/04/2017] [Accepted: 11/01/2017] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
35
|
Influence of semantic consistency and perceptual features on visual attention during scene viewing in toddlers. Infant Behav Dev 2017; 49:248-266. [DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.09.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/12/2017] [Revised: 09/14/2017] [Accepted: 09/16/2017] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
|
36
|
Ordaz SJ, Fritz BL, Forbes EE, Luna B. The influence of pubertal maturation on antisaccade performance. Dev Sci 2017; 21:e12568. [PMID: 28557196 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12568] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/15/2016] [Accepted: 02/23/2017] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
Adolescence is a period characterized by continued improvements in inhibitory control, and this persisting immaturity is believed to interact with affective/motivational behavior to generate the impulsive and risk-taking behavior evidenced at this time. Puberty is a central event of adolescence that has been shown to influence affective/motivational behavior. However, despite plausible mechanisms by which puberty might influence inhibitory control, researchers have yet to test this possibility rigorously. Thus, we designed a study to examine the unique role of pubertal maturation, independent of age, in the development of inhibitory control. In order to minimize age-related variability while maximizing pubertal status variability, we recruited 78 participants (34 F) whose ages narrowly spanned the mean age of gonadarche for each sex (F: ages 11-13, M: ages 12-14). Two complementary measures were used to assess pubertal status: (1) circulating blood serum testosterone and estradiol levels reflecting internal manifestations of pubertal maturation, and (2) Tanner staging by a trained nurse reflecting pubertal maturation's external manifestations. Inhibitory control was assessed using the antisaccade task, and findings were adjusted for the potential effect of age. Results revealed no association between testosterone levels and error rates or response latencies in either sex. In girls, estradiol levels were not associated with error rates, but were associated with faster response latencies. There was similarly no association between Tanner status and error rates, although girls in more advanced pubertal stages showed faster response latencies. Power analyses indicate that findings of a lack of association did not reflect limited statistical power. Thus, in a study designed to isolate the effects of pubertal maturation independent of age, both external and internal indices of pubertal maturation converged to indicate that age-related improvements in cold antisaccade performance are independent of pubertal maturation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah J Ordaz
- Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, USA
| | - Barbara L Fritz
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, USA
| | - Erika E Forbes
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, USA
| | - Beatriz Luna
- Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, USA.,Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, USA
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
Kuhn G, Teszka R. Don't get misdirected! Differences in overt and covert attentional inhibition between children and adults. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2017; 71:688-694. [PMID: 28084918 DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1277770] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
Previous research has revealed marked improvements in cognitive control between the age of 10 years and adulthood. The aim of the current study was to explore differences in attentional control between adults and children within a natural context, namely whilst they were watching a magic trick. We measured participants' eye movements whilst they watched a misdirection trick in which attentional misdirection was used to prevent observers from noticing a salient visual event. Half of our participants failed to detect this event even though it took place in full view. Children below the age of 10 were significantly less likely to notice the event than the adults and were also more reliably overtly misdirected (i.e., where they looked). Our results illustrate that within a more naturalistic context children are significantly more distracted than adults, and this distraction can have major implications on their visual awareness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gustav Kuhn
- Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London, London, UK
| | - Robert Teszka
- Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
38
|
Marini F, Squeri V, Morasso P, Campus C, Konczak J, Masia L. Robot-aided developmental assessment of wrist proprioception in children. J Neuroeng Rehabil 2017; 14:3. [PMID: 28069028 PMCID: PMC5223571 DOI: 10.1186/s12984-016-0215-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/05/2016] [Accepted: 12/23/2016] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Several neurodevelopmental disorders and brain injuries in children have been associated with proprioceptive dysfunction that will negatively affect their movement. Unfortunately, there is lack of reliable and objective clinical examination protocols and our current knowledge of how proprioception evolves in typically developing children is still sparse. METHODS Using a robotic exoskeleton, we investigated proprioceptive acuity of the wrist in a group of 49 typically developing healthy children (8-15 years), and a group of 40 young adults. Without vision participants performed an ipsilateral wrist joint position matching task that required them to reproduce (match) a previously experienced target position. All three joint degrees-of-freedom of the wrist/hand complex were assessed. Accuracy and precision were evaluated as a measure of proprioceptive acuity. The cross-sectional data indicating the time course of development of acuity were then fitted by four models in order to determine which function best describes developmental changes in proprioception across age. RESULTS First, the robot-aided assessment proved to be an easy to administer method for objectively measuring proprioceptive acuity in both children and adult populations. Second, proprioceptive acuity continued to develop throughout middle childhood and early adolescence, improving by more than 50% with respect to the youngest group. Adult levels of performance were reached approximately by the age of 12 years. An inverse-root function best described the development of proprioceptive acuity across the age groups. Third, wrist/forearm proprioception is anisotropic across the three DoFs with the Abduction/Adduction exhibiting a higher level of acuity than those of Flexion/extension and Pronation/Supination. This anisotropy did not change across development. CONCLUSIONS Proprioceptive development for the wrist continues well into early adolescence. Our normative data obtained trough this novel robot-aided assessment method provide a basis against which proprioceptive function of pediatric population can be compared. This may aid the design of more effective sensorimotor intervention programs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Francesca Marini
- Motor Learning and Robotic Rehabilitation Laboratory, Department of Robotics, Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Via Morego 30, Genova, 16163, Italy
| | - Valentina Squeri
- Motor Learning and Robotic Rehabilitation Laboratory, Department of Robotics, Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Via Morego 30, Genova, 16163, Italy
| | - Pietro Morasso
- Motor Learning and Robotic Rehabilitation Laboratory, Department of Robotics, Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Via Morego 30, Genova, 16163, Italy
| | - Claudio Campus
- Motor Learning and Robotic Rehabilitation Laboratory, Department of Robotics, Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Via Morego 30, Genova, 16163, Italy
| | - Jürgen Konczak
- Human Sensorimotor Control Laboratory, School of Kinesiology and Center for Clinical Movement Science, University of Minnesota, USA, 1900 University Ave S E, Minneapolis, 24105, USA
| | - Lorenzo Masia
- School of Mechanical & Aerospace Engineering, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Singapore, 639798.
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
Patten E, Labban JD, Casenhiser DM, Cotton CL. Synchrony Detection of Linguistic Stimuli in the Presence of Faces: Neuropsychological Implications for Language Development in ASD. Dev Neuropsychol 2017; 41:362-374. [PMID: 28059555 DOI: 10.1080/87565641.2016.1243113] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
Abstract
Children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) may be impaired in their ability to detect audiovisual synchrony and their ability may be influenced by the nature of the stimuli. We investigated the possibility that synchrony detection is disrupted by the presence of human faces by testing children with ASD using a preferential looking language-based paradigm. Children with low language abilities were significantly worse at detecting synchrony when the stimuli include an unobscured face than when the face was obscured. Findings suggest that the presence of faces may make multisensory processing more difficult. Implications for interventions are discussed, particularly those targeting attention to faces.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elena Patten
- a Department of Audiology and Speech Pathology , The University of Tennessee Health Science Center , Knoxville , Tennessee
| | - Jeffrey D Labban
- b Department of Kinesiology , University of North Carolina at Greensboro , Greensboro , North Carolina
| | - Devin M Casenhiser
- a Department of Audiology and Speech Pathology , The University of Tennessee Health Science Center , Knoxville , Tennessee
| | - Catherine L Cotton
- c Department of Communication Sciences & Disorders , University of North Carolina at Greensboro , Greensboro , North Carolina
| |
Collapse
|
40
|
Tiadi A, Seassau M, Gerard CL, Bucci MP. Differences between Dyslexic and Non-Dyslexic Children in the Performance of Phonological Visual-Auditory Recognition Tasks: An Eye-Tracking Study. PLoS One 2016; 11:e0159190. [PMID: 27438352 PMCID: PMC4954654 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0159190] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/08/2015] [Accepted: 06/28/2016] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
The object of this study was to explore further phonological visual-auditory recognition tasks in a group of fifty-six healthy children (mean age: 9.9 ± 0.3) and to compare these data to those recorded in twenty-six age-matched dyslexic children (mean age: 9.8 ± 0.2). Eye movements from both eyes were recorded using an infrared video-oculography system (MobileEBT® e(y)e BRAIN). The recognition task was performed under four conditions in which the target object was displayed either with phonologically unrelated objects (baseline condition), or with cohort or rhyme objects (cohort and rhyme conditions, respectively), or both together (rhyme + cohort condition). The percentage of the total time spent on the targets and the latency of the first saccade on the target were measured. Results in healthy children showed that the percentage of the total time spent in the baseline condition was significantly longer than in the other conditions, and that the latency of the first saccade in the cohort condition was significantly longer than in the other conditions; interestingly, the latency decreased significantly with the increasing age of the children. The developmental trend of phonological awareness was also observed in healthy children only. In contrast, we observed that for dyslexic children the total time spent on the target was similar in all four conditions tested, and also that they had similar latency values in both cohort and rhyme conditions. These findings suggest a different sensitivity to the phonological competitors between dyslexic and non-dyslexic children. Also, the eye-tracking technique provides online information about phonological awareness capabilities in children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aimé Tiadi
- UMR 1141 Institut National de la Santé Et de Recherche Médicale- Paris Diderot, Robert Debré Hospital, Paris, France
| | | | | | - Maria Pia Bucci
- UMR 1141 Institut National de la Santé Et de Recherche Médicale- Paris Diderot, Robert Debré Hospital, Paris, France
| |
Collapse
|
41
|
Hwang K, Ghuman AS, Manoach DS, Jones SR, Luna B. Frontal preparatory neural oscillations associated with cognitive control: A developmental study comparing young adults and adolescents. Neuroimage 2016; 136:139-48. [PMID: 27173759 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/14/2015] [Revised: 03/24/2016] [Accepted: 05/05/2016] [Indexed: 01/22/2023] Open
Abstract
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies suggest that age-related changes in the frontal cortex may underlie developmental improvements in cognitive control. In the present study we used magnetoencephalography (MEG) to identify frontal oscillatory neurodynamics that support age-related improvements in cognitive control during adolescence. We characterized the differences in neural oscillations in adolescents and adults during the preparation to suppress a prepotent saccade (antisaccade trials-AS) compared to preparing to generate a more automatic saccade (prosaccade trials-PS). We found that for adults, AS were associated with increased beta-band (16-38Hz) power in the dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), enhanced alpha- to low beta-band (10-18Hz) power in the frontal eye field (FEF) that predicted performance, and increased cross-frequency alpha-beta (10-26Hz) amplitude coupling between the DLPFC and the FEF. Developmental comparisons between adults and adolescents revealed similar engagement of DLPFC beta-band power but weaker FEF alpha-band power, and lower cross-frequency coupling between the DLPFC and the FEF in adolescents. These results suggest that lateral prefrontal neural activity associated with cognitive control is adult-like by adolescence; the development of cognitive control from adolescence to adulthood is instead associated with increases in frontal connectivity and strengthening of inhibition signaling for suppressing task-incompatible processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kai Hwang
- Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California Berkeley, Berkeley, CA, United States; Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States; Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States; Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Carnegie Mellon University and University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States.
| | - Avniel S Ghuman
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States; Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Carnegie Mellon University and University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States; Department of Neurological Surgery, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
| | - Dara S Manoach
- Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States; Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Charlestown, MA, United States
| | - Stephanie R Jones
- Department of Neuroscience, Brown University, Providence, RI, United States
| | - Beatriz Luna
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States; Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States; Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Carnegie Mellon University and University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
| |
Collapse
|
42
|
Interference between oculomotor and postural tasks in 7–8-year-old children and adults. Exp Brain Res 2016; 234:1667-77. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-016-4565-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/20/2015] [Accepted: 01/18/2016] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
|
43
|
Marek S, Hwang K, Foran W, Hallquist MN, Luna B. The Contribution of Network Organization and Integration to the Development of Cognitive Control. PLoS Biol 2015; 13:e1002328. [PMID: 26713863 PMCID: PMC4694653 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002328] [Citation(s) in RCA: 214] [Impact Index Per Article: 23.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/24/2015] [Accepted: 11/12/2015] [Indexed: 01/19/2023] Open
Abstract
Cognitive control, which continues to mature throughout adolescence, is supported by the ability for well-defined organized brain networks to flexibly integrate information. However, the development of intrinsic brain network organization and its relationship to observed improvements in cognitive control are not well understood. In the present study, we used resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging (RS-fMRI), graph theory, the antisaccade task, and rigorous head motion control to characterize and relate developmental changes in network organization, connectivity strength, and integration to inhibitory control development. Subjects were 192 10–26-y-olds who were imaged during 5 min of rest. In contrast to initial studies, our results indicate that network organization is stable throughout adolescence. However, cross-network integration, predominantly of the cingulo-opercular/salience network, increased with age. Importantly, this increased integration of the cingulo-opercular/salience network significantly moderated the robust effect of age on the latency to initiate a correct inhibitory control response. These results provide compelling evidence that the transition to adult-level inhibitory control is dependent upon the refinement and strengthening of integration between specialized networks. Our findings support a novel, two-stage model of neural development, in which networks stabilize prior to adolescence and subsequently increase their integration to support the cross-domain incorporation of information processing critical for mature cognitive control. This study reveals that although the organization of functional brain networks remains stable during adolescence, between-network integration continues to increase, underlying maturation in cognitive control. Adolescence is a unique period of brain development, with major changes occurring across the brain at many different levels of brain functioning. At the macroscopic level, the brain is composed of individual regions that collaborate in networks to perform diverse cognitive functions. Some networks of brain regions perform lower-level sensorimotor processing, while other networks orchestrate more complex functions, such as cognitive control. The affiliation of each region to a network is referred to as network organization. Brain regions not only can communicate with other regions belonging to their own network but also with regions in other networks. Brain regions that communicate with regions belonging to other networks display a high level of integration since they link their network with another network. We found that during adolescence, network organization does not change. However, integration continues to increase, underscoring the notion that brain function becomes more distributed and collaborative during this unique period of development. Furthermore, this increased network integration underlies improvements in cognitive control. Thus, we provide a network-based account for improvements in cognitive functioning during adolescence.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Scott Marek
- Center for Neuroscience, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America
- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America
- * E-mail:
| | - Kai Hwang
- Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California Berkeley, Berkeley, California, United States of America
| | - William Foran
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America
| | - Michael N. Hallquist
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America
- Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, State College, Pennsylvania, United States of America
| | - Beatriz Luna
- Center for Neuroscience, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America
- Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America
- Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
44
|
Matsuo Y, Watanabe M, Taniike M, Mohri I, Kobashi S, Tachibana M, Kobayashi Y, Kitamura Y. Gap Effect Abnormalities during a Visually Guided Pro-Saccade Task in Children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0125573. [PMID: 26018057 PMCID: PMC4446099 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0125573] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/19/2014] [Accepted: 03/25/2015] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder that starts in early childhood and has a comprehensive impact on psychosocial activity and education as well as general health across the lifespan. Despite its prevalence, the current diagnostic criteria for ADHD are debated. Saccadic eye movements are easy to quantify and may be a quantitative biomarker for a wide variety of neurological and psychiatric disorders, including ADHD. The goal of this study was to examine whether children with ADHD exhibit abnormalities during a visually guided pro-saccadic eye-movement and to clarify the neurophysiological mechanisms associated with their behavioral impairments. Thirty-seven children with ADHD (aged 5–11 years) and 88 typically developing (TD) children (aged 5–11 years) were asked to perform a simple saccadic eye-movement task in which step and gap conditions were randomly interleaved. We evaluated the gap effect, which is the difference in the reaction time between the two conditions. Children with ADHD had a significantly longer reaction time than TD children (p < 0.01) and the gap effect was markedly attenuated (p < 0.01). These results suggest that the measurement of saccadic eye movements may provide a novel method for evaluating the behavioral symptoms and clinical features of ADHD, and that the gap effect is a potential biomarker for the diagnosis of ADHD in early childhood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yuka Matsuo
- Department of Social and Environmental Medicine, Graduate School of Medicine, Osaka University, Suita, Osaka, Japan
| | - Masayuki Watanabe
- Department of Biomedical and Molecular Sciences, Queens University, Kingston, Ontario, Canada
| | - Masako Taniike
- Molecular Research Center for Children’s Mental Development, United Graduate School of Child Development, Osaka University, Suita, Osaka, Japan
| | - Ikuko Mohri
- Molecular Research Center for Children’s Mental Development, United Graduate School of Child Development, Osaka University, Suita, Osaka, Japan
| | - Syoji Kobashi
- Department of Electrical Engineering and Computer Sciences, Graduate School of Engineering, University of Hyogo, Shosha, Himeji, Hyogo, Japan
| | - Masaya Tachibana
- Department of Neuroscience, Mayo Clinic Florida, Jacksonville, Florida, United States of America
| | - Yasushi Kobayashi
- Visual Neuroscience Laboratory, Graduate School of Frontier Biosciences, Osaka University, Suita, Osaka, Japan
- Center for Information and Neural Networks, National Institute of Information and Communications Technology and Osaka University, Suita, Osaka, Japan
| | - Yuri Kitamura
- Department of Social and Environmental Medicine, Graduate School of Medicine, Osaka University, Suita, Osaka, Japan
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
45
|
Gonzalez CC, Mon-Williams M, Burke MR. Children and older adults exhibit distinct sub-optimal cost-benefit functions when preparing to move their eyes and hands. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0117783. [PMID: 25659134 PMCID: PMC4320084 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0117783] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/16/2014] [Accepted: 01/02/2015] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Numerous activities require an individual to respond quickly to the correct stimulus. The provision of advance information allows response priming but heightened responses can cause errors (responding too early or reacting to the wrong stimulus). Thus, a balance is required between the online cognitive mechanisms (inhibitory and anticipatory) used to prepare and execute a motor response at the appropriate time. We investigated the use of advance information in 71 participants across four different age groups: (i) children, (ii) young adults, (iii) middle-aged adults, and (iv) older adults. We implemented ‘cued’ and ‘non-cued’ conditions to assess age-related changes in saccadic and touch responses to targets in three movement conditions: (a) Eyes only; (b) Hands only; (c) Eyes and Hand. Children made less saccade errors compared to young adults, but they also exhibited longer response times in cued versus non-cued conditions. In contrast, older adults showed faster responses in cued conditions but exhibited more errors. The results indicate that young adults (18–25 years) achieve an optimal balance between anticipation and execution. In contrast, children show benefits (few errors) and costs (slow responses) of good inhibition when preparing a motor response based on advance information; whilst older adults show the benefits and costs associated with a prospective response strategy (i.e., good anticipation).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Claudia C. Gonzalez
- Sports, Health and Exercise Sciences, Brunel University, Uxbridge, United Kingdom
- * E-mail:
| | - Mark Mon-Williams
- School of Psychology, Faculty of Medicine and Health, University of Leeds, Leeds, United Kingdom
| | - Melanie R. Burke
- School of Psychology, Faculty of Medicine and Health, University of Leeds, Leeds, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
46
|
Ego C, Orban de Xivry JJ, Nassogne MC, Yüksel D, Lefèvre P. Spontaneous improvement in oculomotor function of children with cerebral palsy. RESEARCH IN DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 2015; 36C:630-644. [PMID: 25462523 DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.10.025] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/09/2014] [Accepted: 10/16/2014] [Indexed: 06/04/2023]
Abstract
Eye movements are essential to get a clear vision of moving objects. In the present study, we assessed quantitatively the oculomotor deficits of children with cerebral palsy (CP). We recorded eye movements of 51 children with cerebral palsy (aged 5-16 years) with relatively mild motor impairment and compared their performance with age-matched control and premature children. Overall eye movements of children with CP are unexpectedly close to those of controls even though some oculomotor parameters are biased by the side of hemiplegia. Importantly, the difference in performance between children with CP and controls decreases with age, demonstrating that the oculomotor function of children with CP develops as fast as or even faster than controls for some visual tracking parameters. That is, oculomotor function spontaneously improves over the course of childhood. This evolution highlights the ability of lesioned brain of children with CP to compensate for impaired motor function beyond what would be achieved by normal development on its own.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Caroline Ego
- ICTEAM, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium; Institute of Neuroscience (IoNS), Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium.
| | - Jean-Jacques Orban de Xivry
- ICTEAM, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium; Institute of Neuroscience (IoNS), Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium.
| | - Marie-Cécile Nassogne
- Neuropediatric Department, Cliniques universitaires Saint-Luc, Brussels, Belgium; Institute of Neuroscience (IoNS), Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium.
| | - Demet Yüksel
- Ophthalmology Department, Cliniques universitaires Saint-Luc, Brussels, Belgium; Institute of Neuroscience (IoNS), Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium.
| | - Philippe Lefèvre
- ICTEAM, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium; Institute of Neuroscience (IoNS), Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium.
| |
Collapse
|
47
|
Patten E, Watson LR, Baranek GT. Temporal Synchrony Detection and Associations with Language in Young Children with ASD. AUTISM RESEARCH AND TREATMENT 2014; 2014:678346. [PMID: 25614835 PMCID: PMC4295130 DOI: 10.1155/2014/678346] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/27/2014] [Revised: 12/03/2014] [Accepted: 12/09/2014] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Temporally synchronous audio-visual stimuli serve to recruit attention and enhance learning, including language learning in infants. Although few studies have examined this effect on children with autism, it appears that the ability to detect temporal synchrony between auditory and visual stimuli may be impaired, particularly given social-linguistic stimuli delivered via oral movement and spoken language pairings. However, children with autism can detect audio-visual synchrony given nonsocial stimuli (objects dropping and their corresponding sounds). We tested whether preschool children with autism could detect audio-visual synchrony given video recordings of linguistic stimuli paired with movement of related toys in the absence of faces. As a group, children with autism demonstrated the ability to detect audio-visual synchrony. Further, the amount of time they attended to the synchronous condition was positively correlated with receptive language. Findings suggest that object manipulations may enhance multisensory processing in linguistic contexts. Moreover, associations between synchrony detection and language development suggest that better processing of multisensory stimuli may guide and direct attention to communicative events thus enhancing linguistic development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elena Patten
- Department of Audiology and Speech Pathology, University of Tennessee Health Science Center, 434 South Stadium Hall, Knoxville, TN 37996, USA
| | - Linda R. Watson
- Division of Speech & Hearing Sciences, CB No. 7190, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC 27599, USA
| | - Grace T. Baranek
- Division of Occupational Sciences, CB No. 7122, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC 27599, USA
| |
Collapse
|
48
|
Padmanabhan A, Garver K, O'Hearn K, Nawarawong N, Liu R, Minshew N, Sweeney J, Luna B. Developmental changes in brain function underlying inhibitory control in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res 2014; 8:123-35. [PMID: 25382787 DOI: 10.1002/aur.1398] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/18/2013] [Accepted: 05/28/2014] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
The development of inhibitory control-the ability to suppress inappropriate actions in order to make goal-directed responses-is often impaired in autism spectrum disorders (ASD). In the present study, we examined whether the impairments in inhibitory control evident in ASD reflect-in part-differences in the development of the neural substrates of inhibitory control from adolescence into adulthood. We conducted a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study on the anti-saccade task, a probe of inhibitory control, in high-functioning adolescents and adults with ASD compared to a matched group of typically developing (TD) individuals. The ASD group did not show the age-related improvements in behavioral performance from adolescence to adulthood evident in the typical group, consistent with previous behavioral work. The fMRI results indicated that much of the circuitry recruited by the ASD group was similar to the TD group. However, the ASD group demonstrated some unique patterns, including: (a) a failure to recruit the frontal eye field during response preparation in adolescence but comparable recruitment in adulthood; (b) greater recruitment of putamen in adolescence and precuneus in adolescence and adulthood than the TD group; and (c) decreased recruitment in the inferior parietal lobule relative to TD groups. Taken together, these results suggest that brain circuitry underlying inhibitory control develops differently from adolescence to adulthood in ASD. Specifically, there may be relative underdevelopment of brain processes underlying inhibitory control in ASD, which may lead to engagement of subcortical compensatory processes.
Collapse
|
49
|
Tiadi A, Seassau M, Bui-Quoc E, Gerard CL, Bucci MP. Vertical saccades in dyslexic children. RESEARCH IN DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 2014; 35:3175-3181. [PMID: 25151607 DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.07.057] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/13/2014] [Revised: 07/08/2014] [Accepted: 07/29/2014] [Indexed: 06/03/2023]
Abstract
Vertical saccades have never been studied in dyslexic children. We examined vertical visually guided saccades in fifty-six dyslexic children (mean age: 10.5±2.56 years old) and fifty-six age matched non dyslexic children (mean age: 10.3±1.74 years old). Binocular eye movements were recorded using an infrared video-oculography system (mobileEBT®, e(ye)BRAIN). Dyslexic children showed significantly longer latency than the non dyslexic group, also the occurrence of anticipatory and express saccades was more important in dyslexic than in non dyslexic children. The gain and the mean velocity values were significantly smaller in dyslexic than in non dyslexic children. Finally, the up-down asymmetry reported in normal population for the gain and the velocity of vertical saccades was observed in dyslexic children and interestingly, dyslexic children also reported an up-down asymmetry for the mean latency. Taken together all these findings suggested impairment in cortical areas responsible of vertical saccades performance and also at peripheral level of the extra-ocular oblique muscles; moreover, a visuo-attentionnal bias could explain the up-down asymmetry reported for the vertical saccade triggering.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aimé Tiadi
- UMR 1141 INSERM-Université Paris 7, Hôpital Robert Debré, 48 Bl Sérurier, 75019 Paris, France.
| | - Magali Seassau
- e(ye) Brain, 1 bis rue Jean le Galleu, 94200, Ivry sur Seine, France
| | - Emmanuel Bui-Quoc
- Service d'Ophtalmologie, Hôpital Robert Debré, 48 Boulevard Sérurier, 75019 Paris, France
| | - Christophe-Loïc Gerard
- Service de Psychopathologie de l'enfant et de l'adolescent, Hôpital Robert Debré, 48 Bl Sérurier, 75019 Paris, France
| | - Maria Pia Bucci
- UMR 1141 INSERM-Université Paris 7, Hôpital Robert Debré, 48 Bl Sérurier, 75019 Paris, France
| |
Collapse
|
50
|
Helo A, Pannasch S, Sirri L, Rämä P. The maturation of eye movement behavior: Scene viewing characteristics in children and adults. Vision Res 2014; 103:83-91. [DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2014.08.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/10/2014] [Revised: 08/03/2014] [Accepted: 08/04/2014] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
|