1
|
Morse H, Jolly AA, Browning H, Clark A, Pomeroy V, Rossit S. Aging effects on extrapersonal (far-space) attention: cancellation and line bisection performance from 179 healthy adults. NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENT, AND COGNITION. SECTION B, AGING, NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND COGNITION 2024; 31:605-626. [PMID: 37314105 DOI: 10.1080/13825585.2023.2223903] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/22/2022] [Accepted: 06/07/2023] [Indexed: 06/15/2023]
Abstract
Assessment of cognitive impairments is a vital part of clinical practice. Cancellation (visual search) and line bisection are commonly used tasks to assess visuospatial attention. Despite the fact visuospatial attention is engaged in both near (within reach) and far-space (out of reach), most studies have been conducted in near-space alone. Moreover, despite their use in clinical practice, it is unclear whether cancellation and bisection tasks are related. Here, we investigated the impact of aging on cancellation and line bisection performance in far-space in a large healthy sample. We provide preliminary age-graded norms for assessing visuospatial attention in far-space calculated from a sample of 179 healthy adults, between the ages of 18-94 (mean age = 49.29). Cancellation and line bisection were presented on a large screen in far-space and completed using a wireless remote. Aging was accompanied by longer task duration for both tasks, slower search speed and poorer quality of search. However, there was no significant effect of aging on line bisection error. There was a significant correlation between the two tasks in that longer task duration in line bisection was associated with slower search speed and poorer quality of search. Overall, participants presented a leftward bias during cancellation and line bisection akin to pseudoneglect. Moreover, we found that irrespective of age, search speed was faster in males than females. We offer novel evidence that performance on cancellation and line bisection tasks are related to one another in far-space, but are also sensitive to age-related decline, and even sex differences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Helen Morse
- School of Psychology, University of East Anglia, Norwich, UK
| | - Amy A Jolly
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Hannah Browning
- School of Psychology, University of East Anglia, Norwich, UK
| | - Allan Clark
- Norwich Medical School, University of East Anglia, Norwich, UK
| | - Valerie Pomeroy
- Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, University of East Anglia, Norwich, UK
- NIHR Brain Injury MedTech Co-Operative, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
2
|
Laurin AS, Ouerfelli-Ethier J, Pisella L, Khan AZ. Reduced spatial attentional distribution in older adults. J Vis 2024; 24:8. [PMID: 38591941 PMCID: PMC11008755 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.4.8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/09/2023] [Accepted: 02/06/2024] [Indexed: 04/10/2024] Open
Abstract
Older adults show decline in visual search performance, but the underlying cause remains unclear. It has been suggested that older adults' altered performance may be related to reduced spatial attention to peripheral visual information compared with younger adults. In this study, 18 younger (M = 21.6 years) and 16 older (M = 69.1 years) participants performed pop-out and serial visual search tasks with variously sized gaze-contingent artificial central scotomas (3°, 5°, or 7° diameter). By occluding central vision, we measured how attention to the periphery was contributing to the search performance. We also tested the effect of target eccentricity on search times and eye movements. We hypothesized that, if attention is reduced primarily in the periphery in older adults, we would observe longer search times for more eccentric targets and with central occlusion. During the pop-out search, older adults showed a steeper decline in search performance with increasing eccentricity and central scotoma size compared with younger adults. In contrast, during the serial search, older adults had longer search times than younger adults overall, independent of target eccentricity and scotoma size. Longer search times were attributed to higher cost-per-item slopes, indicating increased difficulty in simultaneously processing complex symbols made up of separable features in aging, possibly stemming from challenges in spatially binding individual features. Altogether, our findings point to fewer attentional resources of simultaneous visual processing to distribute over space or separable features of objects, consistent with decreased dorsal visual stream functioning in aging.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Julie Ouerfelli-Ethier
- School of Optometry, University of Montreal, Montreal, QC, Canada
- Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon (CRNL), INSERM U1028, Bron, France
| | - Laure Pisella
- Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon (CRNL), INSERM U1028, Bron, France
| | | |
Collapse
|
3
|
Dietze N, Poth CH. Phasic alerting in visual search tasks. Atten Percept Psychophys 2024; 86:707-716. [PMID: 38240893 PMCID: PMC11062964 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-024-02844-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 01/02/2024] [Indexed: 02/01/2024]
Abstract
Many tasks require one to search for and find important objects in the visual environment. Visual search is strongly supported by cues indicating target objects to mechanisms of selective attention, which enable one to prioritise targets and ignore distractor objects. Besides selective attention, a major influence on performance across cognitive tasks is phasic alertness, a temporary increase of arousal induced by warning stimuli (alerting cues). Alerting cues provide no specific information on whose basis selective attention could be deployed, but have nevertheless been found to speed up perception and simple actions. It is still unclear, however, how alerting affects visual search. Therefore, in the present study, participants performed a visual search task with and without preceding visual alerting cues. Participants had to report the orientation of a target among several distractors. The target saliency was low in Experiment 1 and high in Experiment 2. In both experiments, we found that visual search was faster when a visual alerting cue was presented before the target display. Performance benefits occurred irrespective of how many distractors had been presented along with the target. Taken together, the findings reveal that visual alerting supports visual search independently of the complexity of the search process and the demands for selective attention.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Niklas Dietze
- Department of Psychology, Neuro‑Cognitive Psychology and Center for Cognitive Interaction Technology, Bielefeld University, P.O. box 10 01 31, 33501, Bielefeld, Germany.
| | - Christian H Poth
- Department of Psychology, Neuro‑Cognitive Psychology and Center for Cognitive Interaction Technology, Bielefeld University, P.O. box 10 01 31, 33501, Bielefeld, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Vizitiu C, Stara V, Antognoli L, Dinculescu A, Mosoi A, Kristaly DM, Nistorescu A, Rampioni M, Dominey K, Marin M, Rossi L, Moraru SA, Vasile CE, Dugan C. An IoT-based cognitive impairment detection device: A newly proposed method in older adults care-choice reaction time-device development and data-driven validation. Digit Health 2024; 10:20552076241293597. [PMID: 39502483 PMCID: PMC11536570 DOI: 10.1177/20552076241293597] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/17/2024] [Accepted: 09/27/2024] [Indexed: 11/08/2024] Open
Abstract
Background Research shows that older adults' performance on choice reaction time (CRT) tests can predict cognitive decline. A simple CRT tool could help detect mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and preclinical dementia, allowing for further stratification of cognitive disorders on-site or via telemedicine. Objective The primary objective was to develop a CRT testing device and protocol to differentiate between two cognitive impairment categories: (a) subjective cognitive decline (SCD) and non-amnestic mild cognitive impairment (na-MCI), and (b) amnestic mild cognitive impairment (a-MCI) and multiple-domain a-MCI (a-MCI-MD). Methods A pilot study in Italy and Romania with 35 older adults (ages 61-85) assessed cognitive function using the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) and a CRT color response task. Reaction time, accuracy, and demographics were recorded, and machine learning classifiers analyzed performance differences to predict preclinical dementia and screen for mild cognitive deficits. Results Moderate correlations were found between the MMSE score and both mean reaction time and mean accuracy rate. There was a significant difference between the two groups' reaction time for blue light, but not for any other colors or for mean accuracy rate. SVM and RUSBoosted trees were found to have the best preclinical dementia prediction capabilities among the tested classifier algorithms, both presenting an accuracy rate of 77.1%. Conclusions CRT testing with machine learning effectively differentiates cognitive capacities in older adults, facilitating early diagnosis and stratification of neurocognitive diseases and can also identify impairments from stressors like dehydration and sleep deprivation. This study highlights the potential of portable CRT devices for monitoring cognitive function, including SCD and MCI.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cristian Vizitiu
- The Space Applications and Technologies Laboratory, Institute of Space Science—Subsidiary of INFLPR (National Institute for Laser, Plasma and Radiation Physics), Magurele, Romania
- Department of Automatics and Information Technology, Faculty of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science, Transilvania University of Brasov, Brasov, Romania
| | - Vera Stara
- Centre for Innovative Models for Aging Care and Technology, IRCCS INRCA (Scientific Institute for Research, Hospitalization and Healthcare—National Institute of Health and Science on Aging), Ancona, Italy
| | - Luca Antognoli
- Centre for Innovative Models for Aging Care and Technology, IRCCS INRCA (Scientific Institute for Research, Hospitalization and Healthcare—National Institute of Health and Science on Aging), Ancona, Italy
| | - Adrian Dinculescu
- The Space Applications and Technologies Laboratory, Institute of Space Science—Subsidiary of INFLPR (National Institute for Laser, Plasma and Radiation Physics), Magurele, Romania
| | - Adrian Mosoi
- Department of Psychology, Education and Teacher Training, Faculty of Psychology and Education Sciences, Brasov, Romania
| | - Dominic M. Kristaly
- Department of Automatics and Information Technology, Faculty of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science, Transilvania University of Brasov, Brasov, Romania
| | - Alexandru Nistorescu
- The Space Applications and Technologies Laboratory, Institute of Space Science—Subsidiary of INFLPR (National Institute for Laser, Plasma and Radiation Physics), Magurele, Romania
| | - Margherita Rampioni
- Centre for Innovative Models for Aging Care and Technology, IRCCS INRCA (Scientific Institute for Research, Hospitalization and Healthcare—National Institute of Health and Science on Aging), Ancona, Italy
| | - Kevin Dominey
- The Space Applications and Technologies Laboratory, Institute of Space Science—Subsidiary of INFLPR (National Institute for Laser, Plasma and Radiation Physics), Magurele, Romania
| | - Mihaela Marin
- The Space Applications and Technologies Laboratory, Institute of Space Science—Subsidiary of INFLPR (National Institute for Laser, Plasma and Radiation Physics), Magurele, Romania
| | - Lorena Rossi
- Centre for Innovative Models for Aging Care and Technology, IRCCS INRCA (Scientific Institute for Research, Hospitalization and Healthcare—National Institute of Health and Science on Aging), Ancona, Italy
| | - Sorin-Aurel Moraru
- Department of Automatics and Information Technology, Faculty of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science, Transilvania University of Brasov, Brasov, Romania
| | - Costin-Emanuel Vasile
- Department of Devices, Circuits and Electronic Architectures, Faculty of Electronics, Telecommunications and Information Technology, National University of Science and Technology Politehnica Bucharest, Bucharest, Romania
| | - Cosmin Dugan
- Internal Medicine Department, Bucharest University Emergency Hospital, Bucharest, Romania
- Faculty of General Medicine, Carol Davila University of Medicine and Pharmacy, Bucharest, Romania
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Merenstein JL, Mullin HA, Madden DJ. Age-related differences in frontoparietal activation for target and distractor singletons during visual search. Atten Percept Psychophys 2023; 85:749-768. [PMID: 36627473 PMCID: PMC10066832 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-022-02640-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 12/12/2022] [Indexed: 01/11/2023]
Abstract
Age-related decline in visual search performance has been associated with different patterns of activation in frontoparietal regions using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), but whether these age-related effects represent specific influences of target and distractor processing is unclear. Therefore, we acquired event-related fMRI data from 68 healthy, community-dwelling adults ages 18-78 years, during both conjunction (T/F target among rotated Ts and Fs) and feature (T/F target among Os) search. Some displays contained a color singleton that could correspond to either the target or a distractor. A diffusion decision analysis indicated age-related increases in sensorimotor response time across all task conditions, but an age-related decrease in the rate of evidence accumulation (drift rate) was specific to conjunction search. Moreover, the color singleton facilitated search performance when occurring as a target and disrupted performance when occurring as a distractor, but only during conjunction search, and these effects were independent of age. The fMRI data indicated that decreased search efficiency for conjunction relative to feature search was evident as widespread frontoparietal activation. Activation within the left insula mediated the age-related decrease in drift rate for conjunction search, whereas this relation in the FEF and parietal cortex was significant only for individuals younger than 30 or 44 years, respectively. Finally, distractor singletons were associated with significant parietal activation, whereas target singletons were associated with significant frontoparietal deactivation, and this latter effect increased with adult age. Age-related differences in frontoparietal activation therefore reflect both the overall efficiency of search and the enhancement from salient targets.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jenna L. Merenstein
- Brain Imaging and Analysis Center, Duke University Medical Center, Durham, NC 27710, USA
| | - Hollie A. Mullin
- Brain Imaging and Analysis Center, Duke University Medical Center, Durham, NC 27710, USA
| | - David J. Madden
- Brain Imaging and Analysis Center, Duke University Medical Center, Durham, NC 27710, USA
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University Medical Center, Durham, NC 27710, USA
- Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708, USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
|
7
|
Gil-Gómez de Liaño B, Wolfe JM. The FORAGEKID Game: Hybrid-Foraging as a new way to study aspects of executive function in development. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2022. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2022.101233] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/03/2022]
|
8
|
Niu Y, Sun J, Wang B, Yang Y, Wen X, Xiang J. Trajectories of brain entropy across lifetime estimated by resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging. Hum Brain Mapp 2022; 43:4359-4369. [PMID: 35615859 PMCID: PMC9435012 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.25959] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/13/2021] [Revised: 04/07/2022] [Accepted: 04/10/2022] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
The human brain is a complex system of interconnected brain regions that form functional networks with differing roles in cognition and behavior. However, the trajectories of these functional networks across development are unclear and designing a metric to track the complex trajectory of these characteristics throughout the lifespan is challenging. Here, permutation entropy (PE) was used to examine age‐related variations in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in healthy subjects aged 6–85 from global, network, and nodal perspectives. The global PE followed an inverted U‐shaped trajectory that peaked at approximately age 40. The trajectory of the motor and somatosensory functional network was more consistent with a linear model and increased with age; other functional networks showed inverted U‐shaped trajectories that peaked between 25 and 52 years of age. All nodes showed inverted U‐shaped trajectories. Using cluster analysis, the peak ages of nodes were grouped into three clusters (at 24, 38, and 51 years). Overall, we characterized four aging trajectories: networks with a linear increase, early peak age, intermediate peak age, and older peak age. These findings suggest possible complexity in trajectories at critical age points regarding changes in related functional brain networks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yan Niu
- College of Information and Computer, Taiyuan University of Technology, Taiyuan, China
| | - Jie Sun
- College of Information and Computer, Taiyuan University of Technology, Taiyuan, China
| | - Bin Wang
- College of Information and Computer, Taiyuan University of Technology, Taiyuan, China
| | - Yanli Yang
- College of Information and Computer, Taiyuan University of Technology, Taiyuan, China
| | - Xin Wen
- College of Software, Taiyuan University of Technology, Taiyuan, China
| | - Jie Xiang
- College of Information and Computer, Taiyuan University of Technology, Taiyuan, China
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Par RYX, Teo CWL, Tan JJK, Ung YW, Heng KS, Hang LT, Yeo M, Yap WN. An open-label, single-arm pilot study of tocotrienols supplementation on improving memory and attention in healthy young adults. J Funct Foods 2022. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jff.2022.105055] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022] Open
|
10
|
The central executive network and executive function in healthy and persons with schizophrenia groups: a meta-analysis of structural and functional MRI. Brain Imaging Behav 2021; 16:1451-1464. [PMID: 34775552 DOI: 10.1007/s11682-021-00589-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 10/17/2021] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
This meta-analysis evaluated the extent to which executive function can be understood with structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Studies included structural in schizophrenia (k = 8; n = 241) and healthy controls (k = 12; n = 1660), and functional in schizophrenia (k = 4; n = 104) and healthy controls (k = 12; n = 712). Results revealed a positive association in the brain behavior relationship when pooled across schizophrenia and control samples for structural (pr = 0.27) and functional (pr = 0.29) modalities. Subgroup analyses revealed no significant difference for functional neuroimaging (pr = .43, 95%CI = -.08-.77, p = .088) but with structural neuroimaging (pr = .37, 95%CI = -.08-.69, p = .015) the association to executive functions is lower in the control group. Subgroup analyses also revealed no significant differences in the strength of the brain-behavior relationship in the schizophrenia group (pr = .59, 95%CI = .58-.61, p = .881) or the control group (pr = 0.19, 95%CI = 0.18-0.19, p = 0.920), suggesting concordance.
Collapse
|
11
|
Age-related changes in visual search: manipulation of colour cues based on cone contrast and opponent modulation space. Sci Rep 2020; 10:21328. [PMID: 33288789 PMCID: PMC7721812 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-78303-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/03/2019] [Accepted: 11/16/2020] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Reduced retinal illuminance affects colour perception in older adults, and studies show that they exhibit deficiencies in yellow-blue (YB) discrimination. However, the influence of colour cues on the visual attention in older individuals remains unclarified. Visual attention refers to the cognitive model by which we prioritise regions within the visual space and selectively process information. The present study aimed to explore the effect of colour on visual search performance in older observers. In our experiment, younger observers wearing glasses with a filter that simulated the spectral transmittance of the aging human lens and older observers performed two types of search tasks, feature search (FS) and conjunction search (CS), under three colour conditions: red-green, YB, and luminance. Targets and distractors were designed on the basis of the Derrington–Krauskopf–Lennie colour representation. In FS tasks, reaction times changed according to colour in all groups, especially under the YB condition, regardless of the presence or absence of distractors. In CS tasks with distractors, older participants and younger participants wearing glasses showed slower responses under chromatic conditions than under the achromatic condition. These results provide preliminary evidence that, for older observers, visual search performance may be affected by impairments in chromatic colour discrimination.
Collapse
|
12
|
Lin CS. Functional Adaptation of Oromotor Functions and Aging: A Focused Review of the Evidence From Brain Neuroimaging Research. Front Aging Neurosci 2020; 11:354. [PMID: 31998112 PMCID: PMC6962247 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00354] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/12/2019] [Accepted: 12/05/2019] [Indexed: 12/11/2022] Open
Abstract
“Practice makes perfect” is a principle widely applied when one is acquiring a new sensorimotor skill to cope with challenges from a new environment. In terms of oral healthcare, the traditional view holds that restoring decayed structures is one of the primary aims of treatment. This assumes that the patient’s oromotor functions would be recovered back to normal levels after the restoration. However, in older patients, such a structural–functional coupling after dental treatment shows a great degree of individual variations. For example, after prosthodontic treatment, some patients would adapt themselves quickly to the new dentures, while others would not. In this Focused Review, I argue that the functional aspects of adaptation—which would be predominantly associated with the brain mechanisms of cognitive processing and motor learning—play a critical role in the individual differences in the adaptive behaviors of oromotor functions. This thesis is critical to geriatric oral healthcare since the variation in the capacity of cognitive processing and motor learning is critically associated with aging. In this review, (a) the association between aging and the brain-stomatognathic axis will be introduced; (b) the brain mechanisms underlying the association between aging, compensatory behavior, and motor learning will be briefly summarized; (c) the neuroimaging evidence that suggests the role of cognitive processing and motor learning in oromotor functions will be summarized, and critically, the brain mechanisms underlying mastication and swallowing in older people will be discussed; and (d) based on the current knowledge, an experimental framework for investigating the association between aging and the functional adaptation of oromotor functions will be proposed. Finally, I will comment on the practical implications of this framework and postulate questions open for future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chia-Shu Lin
- Department of Dentistry, School of Dentistry, National Yang-Ming University, Taipei, Taiwan.,Institute of Brain Science, School of Medicine, National Yang-Ming University, Taipei, Taiwan.,Brain Research Center, National Yang-Ming University, Taipei, Taiwan
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Semi-Autonomous Vehicles as a Cognitive Assistive Device for Older Adults. Geriatrics (Basel) 2019; 4:geriatrics4040063. [PMID: 31744041 PMCID: PMC6961042 DOI: 10.3390/geriatrics4040063] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/30/2019] [Revised: 11/11/2019] [Accepted: 11/14/2019] [Indexed: 01/08/2023] Open
Abstract
Losing the capacity to drive due to age-related cognitive decline can have a detrimental impact on the daily life functioning of older adults living alone and in remote areas. Semi-autonomous vehicles (SAVs) could have the potential to preserve driving independence of this population with high health needs. This paper explores if SAVs could be used as a cognitive assistive device for older aging drivers with cognitive challenges. We illustrate the impact of age-related changes of cognitive functions on driving capacity. Furthermore, following an overview on the current state of SAVs, we propose a model for connecting cognitive health needs of older drivers to SAVs. The model demonstrates the connections between cognitive changes experienced by aging drivers, their impact on actual driving, car sensors’ features, and vehicle automation. Finally, we present challenges that should be considered when using the constantly changing smart vehicle technology, adapting it to aging drivers and vice versa. This paper sheds light on age-related cognitive characteristics that should be considered when developing future SAVs manufacturing policies which may potentially help decrease the impact of cognitive change on older adult drivers.
Collapse
|
14
|
Response-level processing during visual feature search: Effects of frontoparietal activation and adult age. Atten Percept Psychophys 2019; 82:330-349. [PMID: 31376024 PMCID: PMC6995405 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-019-01823-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022]
Abstract
Previous research suggests that feature search performance is relatively resistant to age-related decline. However, little is known regarding the neural mechanisms underlying the age-related constancy of feature search. In this experiment, we used a diffusion decision model of reaction time (RT), and event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate age-related differences in response-level processing during visual feature search. Participants were 80 healthy, right-handed, community-dwelling individuals, 19–79 years of age. Analyses of search performance indicated that targets accompanied by response-incompatible distractors were associated with a significant increase in the nondecision-time (t0) model parameter, possibly reflecting the additional time required for response execution. Nondecision time increased significantly with increasing age, but no age-related effects were evident in drift rate, cautiousness (boundary separation, a), or in the specific effects of response compatibility. Nondecision time was also associated with a pattern of activation and deactivation in frontoparietal regions. The relation of age to nondecision time was indirect, mediated by this pattern of frontoparietal activation and deactivation. Response-compatible and -incompatible trials were associated with specific patterns of activation in the medial and superior parietal cortex, and frontal eye field, but these activation effects did not mediate the relation between age and search performance. These findings suggest that, in the context of a highly efficient feature search task, the age-related influence of frontoparietal activation is operative at a relatively general level, which is common to the task conditions, rather than at the response level specifically.
Collapse
|
15
|
Abstract
Healthy aging is associated with numerous deficits in cognitive function, which have been attributed to changes within the prefrontal cortex (PFC). This chapter summarizes some of the most prominent cognitive changes associated with age-related alterations in the anatomy and physiology of the PFC. Specifically, aging of the PFC results in deficient aspects of cognitive control, including sustained attention, selective attention, inhibitory control, working memory, and multitasking abilities. Yet, not all cognitive functions associated with the PFC exhibit age-related declines, such as arithmetic, comprehension, emotion perception, and emotional control. Moreover, not all older adults exhibit declines in cognition. Multiple life-course and lifestyle factors, as well as genetics, play a role in the trajectory of cognitive performance across the life span. Thus many adults retain cognitive function well into advanced age. Moreover, the brain remains plastic throughout life and there is increasing evidence that most age-related declines in cognition can be remediated by various methods such as physical exercise, cognitive training, or noninvasive brain stimulation. Overall, because cognitive aging is associated with numerous life-course and lifestyle factors, successful aging likely begins in early life, while maintaining cognition or remediating declines is a life-long process.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Theodore P Zanto
- Department of Neurology, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States; Neuroscape, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
| | - Adam Gazzaley
- Department of Neurology, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States; Departments of Physiology and Psychiatry, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States; Neuroscape, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Zhou Y, Li Y, Wang K. Bispectral Index Monitoring During Anesthesia Promotes Early Postoperative Recovery of Cognitive Function and Reduces Acute Delirium in Elderly Patients with Colon Carcinoma: A Prospective Controlled Study using the Attention Network Test. Med Sci Monit 2018; 24:7785-7793. [PMID: 30378591 PMCID: PMC6354643 DOI: 10.12659/msm.910124] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Bispectral index (BIS) monitoring can reduce the duration of anesthesia. This study aimed to evaluate the effects of BIS monitoring during surgery for resection of colon carcinoma in elderly patients using the Attention Network Test (ANT) to study alerting, orienting, and executive functions, and the Confusion Assessment Method (CAM). Material/Methods Eighty-one patients (65–75 years) underwent radical surgery for colon carcinoma with general intravenous anesthesia, propofol (6–8 mg/kg/h), vecuronium (0.03–0.05 mg/kg/min), and remifentanil (0.1–0.2 μg/kg/min). The BIS group (n=41) underwent adjustment of anesthesia to maintain a BIS value between 40–60; the non-BIS group (N=40) underwent standard intraoperative hemodynamic monitoring. Primary endpoints were alerting, orienting, and executive functions assessed pre-operatively and on postoperative days 1 and 5 using the ANT; the secondary endpoint was delirium during the first five postoperative days, assessed using the CAM. Results Propofol and remifentanil doses were significantly lower in the BIS group compared with the non-BIS group (P<0.001). Alerting, orienting, and executive functions showed no differences between the two groups pre-operatively but were impaired in both groups on postoperative day 1 compared with pre-operative levels (P<0.001). On postoperative day 5, alerting (P=0.607) and orienting (P=0.851) functions recovered in the BIS group but remained impaired in the non-BIS group (P<0.001). Delirium was significantly lower in the BIS group compared with the non-BIS group (17.5% vs. 27.5%) (P<0.001). Conclusions BIS-guided anesthesia was associated with reduced anesthetic exposure, early postoperative recovery of alerting and orienting functions, and reduced postoperative delirium.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yumei Zhou
- Department of Anesthesiology, First Affiliated Hospital, Anhui Medical University, Hefei, Anhui, China (mainland)
| | - Yuanhai Li
- Department of Anesthesiology, First Affiliated Hospital, Anhui Medical University, Hefei, Anhui, China (mainland)
| | - Kai Wang
- Department of Neurology, First Affiliated Hospital, Anhui Medical University, Hefei, Anhui, China (mainland)
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Sampedro-Piquero P, Álvarez-Suárez P, Moreno-Fernández RD, García-Castro G, Cuesta M, Begega A. Environmental Enrichment Results in Both Brain Connectivity Efficiency and Selective Improvement in Different Behavioral Tasks. Neuroscience 2018; 388:374-383. [PMID: 30086366 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.07.036] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/04/2018] [Revised: 07/19/2018] [Accepted: 07/20/2018] [Indexed: 12/26/2022]
Abstract
Exposure to environmental enrichment (EE) has been a useful model for studying the effects of experience on brain plasticity, but to date, few is known about the impact of this condition on the brain functional networks that probably underlies the multiple behavioral improvements. Hence, we assessed the effect of an EE protocol in adult Wistar rats on the performance in several behavioral tasks testing different domains (Open field (OP): locomotor activity; Elevated-zero maze (EZM): anxiety-related behaviors; 5-choice serial reaction time task (5-CSRTT): attentional processes; 4-arm radial water maze (4-RAWM): spatial memory) in order to check its effectiveness in a wide range of functions. After this, we analyzed the functional brain connectivity underlying each experimental condition through cytochrome C oxidase (COx) histochemistry. Our EE protocol reduced both locomotor activity in the OP and anxiety-related behaviors in the EZM. On the other hand, enriched rats showed more accuracy in the 4-RAWM, whereas 5-CSRTT performance was not significantly ameliorated by EE condition. In relation to COx functional connectivity, we found that EE reduced the number of strong positive correlations both in basal and training conditions, suggesting a modulating effect on specific brain connections. Our results suggest that EE seems to have a selective effect on specific brain regions, such as prefrontal cortex and hippocampus, leading to a more efficient brain connectivity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- P Sampedro-Piquero
- Departamento de Psicobiología y Metodología de las Ciencias del Comportamiento, Instituto de Investigación Biomédica de Málaga (IBIMA), Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Málaga, Spain.
| | | | - R D Moreno-Fernández
- Departamento de Psicobiología y Metodología de las Ciencias del Comportamiento, Instituto de Investigación Biomédica de Málaga (IBIMA), Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Málaga, Spain
| | - G García-Castro
- Instituto de Neurociencias del Principado de Asturias INEUROPA, Departamento de Psicología, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Oviedo, Spain
| | - M Cuesta
- Instituto de Neurociencias del Principado de Asturias INEUROPA, Departamento de Psicología, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Oviedo, Spain
| | - A Begega
- Instituto de Neurociencias del Principado de Asturias INEUROPA, Departamento de Psicología, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Oviedo, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Effects of Aging on the Obstacle Negotiation Strategy while Stepping over Multiple Obstacles. Sci Rep 2018; 8:8576. [PMID: 29872074 PMCID: PMC5988752 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-018-26807-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/08/2017] [Accepted: 05/11/2018] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
Forty-seven percent of falling accidents in older adults are caused by tripping over obstacles. Understanding what strategies are involved in obstacle negotiation in older adults could reduce fall risks. There is a paucity of research investigating how healthy adults negotiate multiple obstacles, which may better reflect the complexity of the real environment. The presence of a second obstacle has induced mixed results in the obstacle negotiation of healthy older adults with the interval between obstacles two steps apart. This study extended the knowledge to understand what strategies healthy younger and older adults used to step over two obstacles placed at three-step-length apart. Twenty healthy subjects performed 2 tasks: level ground walking and stepping over two obstacles. The height of each obstacle was set at 10% of subjects' leg height. We found that aging significantly increased the toe clearance in leading and trailing legs when stepping over the obstacles at a three-step-length interval. Toe clearance was higher while stepping over the second obstacle than the first one in older adults. These results had two-fold meanings: the three-step-length interval was long enough to trigger the adjustment of the obstacle negotiation strategy, and aging led older adults to use conservative negotiation strategies.
Collapse
|
19
|
Rodriguez JA, Lamb DG, Salazar L, Correa LN, Mosquera DM, Schwartz ZJ, Cohen RA, Falchook AD, Heilman KM. Background distraction during vertical solid and character line bisections. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2018; 40:887-894. [PMID: 29614901 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2018.1444735] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/17/2022]
Abstract
Background-objectives: When vertical lines are positioned above or below the center of the page, line bisection deviates toward the center of the page, suggesting that the edges of the page distract the allocation of attention to the line. A letter-character line (LCL) bisection requires both global and focal attention, to identify the target letter closest to the line's center. If more focal and less global attention is allocated to a LCL, more global attentional resources may be available and inadvertently allocated to the page. Alternatively, if the allocation of focal attention to a LCL inhibits global attentional processing, there may be less distraction by the page. METHOD Twenty-four healthy adults (12 older) bisected vertical solid and character lines centered, or positioned closer to the top or bottom of the page. RESULTS There was no difference between bisection of solid and character lines centered on the page. Page-related deviations were greater with character lines than solid line bisections, and greater for lines positioned toward the top than the bottom of the page. With lines positioned toward the top, the older participants' attempted bisections were higher than those of the younger participants. CONCLUSIONS These results suggest that the allocation of focal attention increases global attentional distractibility and that global-background attentional distraction is greater when the vertical lines are placed in the upper part of the page. Older participants appeared to be less distracted when lines were placed toward the top of the page, but the reason for this age difference requires further research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julio A Rodriguez
- a Department of Neurology , University of Florida , Gainesville , FL , USA.,b Ponce School of Medicine and Health Sciences , Ponce , Puerto Rico.,c Center for Neuropsychological Studies, College of Medicine , University of Florida , Gainesville , FL , USA
| | - Damon G Lamb
- a Department of Neurology , University of Florida , Gainesville , FL , USA.,c Center for Neuropsychological Studies, College of Medicine , University of Florida , Gainesville , FL , USA.,d Malcom Randall Veterans Affairs Medical Center , Gainesville , FL , USA.,e Cognitive Aging and Memory Center, Institute on Aging , University of Florida , Gainesville , FL , USA.,f Department of Clinical and Health Psychology , University of Florida , Gainesville , FL , USA
| | - Liliana Salazar
- a Department of Neurology , University of Florida , Gainesville , FL , USA.,d Malcom Randall Veterans Affairs Medical Center , Gainesville , FL , USA
| | - Lauren N Correa
- a Department of Neurology , University of Florida , Gainesville , FL , USA.,c Center for Neuropsychological Studies, College of Medicine , University of Florida , Gainesville , FL , USA
| | - Diana M Mosquera
- a Department of Neurology , University of Florida , Gainesville , FL , USA.,c Center for Neuropsychological Studies, College of Medicine , University of Florida , Gainesville , FL , USA.,g Florida State University College of Medicine , Tallahassee , FL , USA
| | - Zared J Schwartz
- c Center for Neuropsychological Studies, College of Medicine , University of Florida , Gainesville , FL , USA
| | - Ronald A Cohen
- e Cognitive Aging and Memory Center, Institute on Aging , University of Florida , Gainesville , FL , USA.,f Department of Clinical and Health Psychology , University of Florida , Gainesville , FL , USA
| | - Adam D Falchook
- a Department of Neurology , University of Florida , Gainesville , FL , USA.,c Center for Neuropsychological Studies, College of Medicine , University of Florida , Gainesville , FL , USA.,d Malcom Randall Veterans Affairs Medical Center , Gainesville , FL , USA
| | - Kenneth M Heilman
- a Department of Neurology , University of Florida , Gainesville , FL , USA.,c Center for Neuropsychological Studies, College of Medicine , University of Florida , Gainesville , FL , USA.,d Malcom Randall Veterans Affairs Medical Center , Gainesville , FL , USA.,e Cognitive Aging and Memory Center, Institute on Aging , University of Florida , Gainesville , FL , USA
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Systemic klotho is associated with KLOTHO variation and predicts intrinsic cortical connectivity in healthy human aging. Brain Imaging Behav 2018; 11:391-400. [PMID: 27714549 PMCID: PMC5382127 DOI: 10.1007/s11682-016-9598-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/08/2023]
Abstract
Cognitive decline is a major biomedical challenge as the global population ages. Elevated levels of the longevity factor klotho suppress aging, enhance cognition, and promote synaptic plasticity and neural resilience against aging and Alzheimer’s disease (AD)-related pathogenic proteins. Here, we examined the relationship between human genetic variants of KLOTHO and systemic klotho levels – and assessed neuroanatomic correlates of serum klotho in a cohort of healthy older adults. Serum klotho levels were increased with KL-VS heterozygosity, as anticipated. We report, for the first time, that serum klotho levels were paradoxically decreased with KL-VS homozygosity. Further, we found that higher serum klotho levels were associated with measures of greater intrinsic connectivity in key functional networks of the brain vulnerable to aging and AD such as the fronto-parietal and default mode networks. Our findings suggest that elevated klotho promotes a resilient brain, possibly through increased network connectivity of critical brain regions.
Collapse
|
21
|
Deslauriers J, Ansado J, Marrelec G, Provost JS, Joanette Y. Increase of posterior connectivity in aging within the Ventral Attention Network: A functional connectivity analysis using independent component analysis. Brain Res 2016; 1657:288-296. [PMID: 28012826 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.12.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/29/2016] [Revised: 11/23/2016] [Accepted: 12/20/2016] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
Multiple studies have found neurofunctional changes in normal aging in a context of selective attention. Furthermore, many articles report intrahemispheric alteration in functional networks. However, little is known about age-related changes within the Ventral Attention Network (VAN), which underlies selective attention. The aim of this study is to examine age-related changes within the VAN, focusing on connectivity between its regions. Here we report our findings on the analysis of 27 participants' (13 younger and 14 older healthy adults) BOLD signals as well as their performance on a letter-matching task. We identified the VAN independently for both groups using spatial independent component analysis. Three main findings emerged: First, younger adults were faster and more accurate on the task. Second, older adults had greater connectivity among posterior regions (right temporoparietal junction, right superior parietal lobule, right middle temporal gyrus and left cerebellum crus I) than younger adults but lower connectivity among anterior regions (right anterior insula, right medial superior frontal gyrus and right middle frontal gyrus). Older adults also had more connectivity between anterior and posterior regions than younger adults. Finally, correlations between connectivity and response time on the task showed a trend toward connectivity in posterior regions for the older group and in anterior regions for the younger group. Thus, this study shows that intrahemispheric neurofunctional changes in aging also affect the VAN. The results suggest that, in contexts of selective attention, posterior regions increased in importance for older adults, while anterior regions had reduced centrality.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Johnathan Deslauriers
- Centre de Recherche, Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal, Canada; Université de Montréal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada; Université du Québec en Outaouais, Gatineau, Quebec, Canada
| | | | - Guillaume Marrelec
- Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, CNRS, INSERM, Laboratoire d'imagerie biomédicale (LIB), F-75013 Paris, France
| | | | - Yves Joanette
- Centre de Recherche, Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal, Canada; Université de Montréal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Computerized Cognitive Tests Are Associated with Biomarkers of Alzheimer's Disease in Cognitively Normal Individuals 10 Years Prior. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2016; 22:968-977. [PMID: 27903332 PMCID: PMC5154173 DOI: 10.1017/s1355617716000722] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/18/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Evidence suggests that Alzheimer's disease (AD) biomarkers become abnormal many years before the emergence of clinical symptoms of AD, raising the possibility that biomarker levels measured in cognitively normal individuals would be associated with cognitive performance many years later. This study examined whether performance on computerized cognitive tests is associated with levels of cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) biomarkers of amyloid, tau, and phosphorylated tau (p-tau) obtained approximately 10 years earlier, when individuals were cognitively normal and primarily middle-aged. METHODS Individuals from the BIOCARD cohort (mean age at testing=69 years) were tested on two computerized tasks hypothesized to rely on brain regions affected by the early accumulation of AD pathology: (1) a Paired Associates Learning (PAL) task (n=67) and (2) a visual search task (n=86). RESULTS In regression analyses, poorer performance on the PAL task was associated with higher levels of CSF p-tau obtained years earlier, whereas worse performance in the visual search task was associated with lower levels of CSF Aβ1-42. CONCLUSIONS These findings suggest that AD biomarker levels may be differentially predictive of specific cognitive functions many years later. In line with the pattern of early accumulation of AD pathology, the PAL task, hypothesized to rely on medial temporal lobe function, was associated with CSF p-tau, whereas the visual search task, hypothesized to rely on frontoparietal function, was associated with CSF amyloid. Studies using amyloid and tau PET imaging will be useful in examining these hypothesized relationships further. (JINS, 2016, 22, 968-977).
Collapse
|
23
|
Vanotti S, Cores EV, Eizaguirre B, Angeles M, Rey R, Villa A, Cáceres F. Normatization of the symbol digit modalities test-oral version in a Latin American country. APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY-ADULT 2015; 22:46-53. [PMID: 25529591 DOI: 10.1080/23279095.2013.831866] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
The aim of this study was to standardize the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT)-Oral version in a healthy population living in Argentina and to analyze the influence that age, gender, and education have on the SDMT. Secondarily, it is intended to analyze the performance of patients with multiple sclerosis (MS) on this test. Two hundred ninety-seven healthy participants were evaluated; they had an average age of 39.28 years and 13.87 years of schooling; 77.8% were women. The sample was segmented according to age in three groups: younger than 35 years old, 36 to 50 years old, and 51 to 70 years old. The sample was also segmented according to years of schooling in three groups: 11 years or less, 12 to 16 years, and more than 16 years. All participants were evaluated with the oral version of the SDMT. A clinical sample of 111 patients with MS was also assessed. The mean on the SDMT for the total sample was 51.34 (SD=12.76). The differences were significant between all groups, p<.05, according to age. The participants with a higher level of education performed better than did those with moderate education and those with less schooling, p<.05. There was a significant difference between patients with MS and healthy controls, p<.01. The SDMT is influenced by age as well as by schooling, although not by gender. The norms displayed here will be useful to accurately evaluate the yield of the patients in the neuropsychological clinic when comparing them with their group of reference. It was also demonstrated that the SDMT can discriminate between patients with MS and healthy people.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sandra Vanotti
- a Multiple Sclerosis Clinic, Neurosciences Institute of Buenos Aires , Buenos Aires , Argentina
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
24
|
Cui C, Noronha A, Warren KR, Koob GF, Sinha R, Thakkar M, Matochik J, Crews FT, Chandler LJ, Pfefferbaum A, Becker HC, Lovinger D, Everitt BJ, Egli M, Mandyam CD, Fein G, Potenza MN, Harris RA, Grant KA, Roberto M, Meyerhoff DJ, Sullivan EV. Brain pathways to recovery from alcohol dependence. Alcohol 2015; 49:435-52. [PMID: 26074423 PMCID: PMC4468789 DOI: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2015.04.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/19/2014] [Revised: 04/09/2015] [Accepted: 04/09/2015] [Indexed: 12/28/2022]
Abstract
This article highlights the research presentations at the satellite symposium on "Brain Pathways to Recovery from Alcohol Dependence" held at the 2013 Society for Neuroscience Annual Meeting. The purpose of this symposium was to provide an up to date overview of research efforts focusing on understanding brain mechanisms that contribute to recovery from alcohol dependence. A panel of scientists from the alcohol and addiction research field presented their insights and perspectives on brain mechanisms that may underlie both recovery and lack of recovery from alcohol dependence. The four sessions of the symposium encompassed multilevel studies exploring mechanisms underlying relapse and craving associated with sustained alcohol abstinence, cognitive function deficit and recovery, and translational studies on preventing relapse and promoting recovery. Gaps in our knowledge and research opportunities were also discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Changhai Cui
- National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, Bethesda, MD, USA.
| | - Antonio Noronha
- National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, Bethesda, MD, USA
| | - Kenneth R Warren
- National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, Bethesda, MD, USA
| | - George F Koob
- National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, Bethesda, MD, USA; Committee on the Neurobiology of Addictive Disorders, The Scripps Research Institute, La Jolla, CA, USA
| | - Rajita Sinha
- Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA
| | - Mahesh Thakkar
- Department of Neurology, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA
| | - John Matochik
- National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, Bethesda, MD, USA
| | - Fulton T Crews
- Bowles Center for Alcohol Studies, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
| | - L Judson Chandler
- Department of Neurosciences, Medical University of South Carolina, Charleston, SC, USA
| | - Adolf Pfefferbaum
- Neuroscience Program, Center for Health Science, SRI International, Menlo Park, CA, USA
| | - Howard C Becker
- Department of Neurosciences, Medical University of South Carolina, Charleston, SC, USA
| | - David Lovinger
- Laboratory of Integrative Neuroscience, National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, Bethesda, MD, USA
| | - Barry J Everitt
- Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Mark Egli
- National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, Bethesda, MD, USA
| | - Chitra D Mandyam
- Committee on the Neurobiology of Addictive Disorders, The Scripps Research Institute, La Jolla, CA, USA
| | - George Fein
- Neurobehavioral Research, Inc., Ala Moana Pacific Center, Honolulu, HI, USA
| | - Marc N Potenza
- Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA
| | - R Adron Harris
- Waggoner Center for Alcohol and Addiction Research, University of Texas, Austin, TX, USA
| | - Kathleen A Grant
- Oregon National Primate Research Center, Oregon Health & Science University, Beaverton, OR, USA
| | - Marisa Roberto
- Committee on the Neurobiology of Addictive Disorders, The Scripps Research Institute, La Jolla, CA, USA
| | - Dieter J Meyerhoff
- Department of Radiology and Biomedical Imaging, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Edith V Sullivan
- Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Port NL, Trimberger J, Hitzeman S, Redick B, Beckerman S. Micro and regular saccades across the lifespan during a visual search of "Where's Waldo" puzzles. Vision Res 2015; 118:144-57. [PMID: 26049037 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2015.05.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/23/2014] [Revised: 05/01/2015] [Accepted: 05/18/2015] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Despite the fact that different aspects of visual-motor control mature at different rates and aging is associated with declines in both sensory and motor function, little is known about the relationship between microsaccades and either development or aging. Using a sample of 343 individuals ranging in age from 4 to 66 and a task that has been shown to elicit a high frequency of microsaccades (solving Where's Waldo puzzles), we explored microsaccade frequency and kinematics (main sequence curves) as a function of age. Taking advantage of the large size of our dataset (183,893 saccades), we also address (a) the saccade amplitude limit at which video eye trackers are able to accurately measure microsaccades and (b) the degree and consistency of saccade kinematics at varying amplitudes and directions. Using a modification of the Engbert-Mergenthaler saccade detector, we found that even the smallest amplitude movements (0.25-0.5°) demonstrate basic saccade kinematics. With regard to development and aging, both microsaccade and regular saccade frequency exhibited a very small increase across the life span. Visual search ability, as per many other aspects of visual performance, exhibited a U-shaped function over the lifespan. Finally, both large horizontal and moderate vertical directional biases were detected for all saccade sizes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicholas L Port
- School of Optometry, Indiana University, 800 E Atwater Ave, Bloomington, IN 47405, United States; Program in Neuroscience, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Cognitive Science Program, Indiana University, United States.
| | - Jane Trimberger
- School of Optometry, Indiana University, 800 E Atwater Ave, Bloomington, IN 47405, United States; Illinois College of Optometry, 3241 South Michigan Avenue, Chicago, IL 60616, United States
| | - Steve Hitzeman
- School of Optometry, Indiana University, 800 E Atwater Ave, Bloomington, IN 47405, United States
| | - Bryan Redick
- School of Optometry, Indiana University, 800 E Atwater Ave, Bloomington, IN 47405, United States
| | - Stephen Beckerman
- Illinois College of Optometry, 3241 South Michigan Avenue, Chicago, IL 60616, United States
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Woods DL, Wyma JM, Yund EW, Herron TJ, Reed B. Age-related slowing of response selection and production in a visual choice reaction time task. Front Hum Neurosci 2015; 9:193. [PMID: 25954175 PMCID: PMC4407573 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00193] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/12/2015] [Accepted: 03/24/2015] [Indexed: 12/05/2022] Open
Abstract
Aging is associated with delayed processing in choice reaction time (CRT) tasks, but the processing stages most impacted by aging have not been clearly identified. Here, we analyzed CRT latencies in a computerized serial visual feature-conjunction task. Participants responded to a target letter (probability 40%) by pressing one mouse button, and responded to distractor letters differing either in color, shape, or both features from the target (probabilities 20% each) by pressing the other mouse button. Stimuli were presented randomly to the left and right visual fields and stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs) were adaptively reduced following correct responses using a staircase procedure. In Experiment 1, we tested 1466 participants who ranged in age from 18 to 65 years. CRT latencies increased significantly with age (r = 0.47, 2.80 ms/year). Central processing time (CPT), isolated by subtracting simple reaction times (SRT) (obtained in a companion experiment performed on the same day) from CRT latencies, accounted for more than 80% of age-related CRT slowing, with most of the remaining increase in latency due to slowed motor responses. Participants were faster and more accurate when the stimulus location was spatially compatible with the mouse button used for responding, and this effect increased slightly with age. Participants took longer to respond to distractors with target color or shape than to distractors with no target features. However, the additional time needed to discriminate the more target-like distractors did not increase with age. In Experiment 2, we replicated the findings of Experiment 1 in a second population of 178 participants (ages 18–82 years). CRT latencies did not differ significantly in the two experiments, and similar effects of age, distractor similarity, and stimulus-response spatial compatibility were found. The results suggest that the age-related slowing in visual CRT latencies is largely due to delays in response selection and production.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David L Woods
- Human Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory, Veterans Affairs Northern California Health Care System Martinez, CA, USA ; The Department of Neurology, University of California Davis Sacramento, CA, USA ; Center for Neurosciences, University of California Davis CA, USA ; Center for Mind and Brain, University of California Davis CA, USA
| | - John M Wyma
- Human Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory, Veterans Affairs Northern California Health Care System Martinez, CA, USA
| | - E William Yund
- Human Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory, Veterans Affairs Northern California Health Care System Martinez, CA, USA
| | - Timothy J Herron
- Human Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory, Veterans Affairs Northern California Health Care System Martinez, CA, USA
| | - Bruce Reed
- The Department of Neurology, University of California Davis Sacramento, CA, USA ; Alzheimer's Disease Center, Department of Neurology, University of California Davis CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Ten Brink AF, Van der Stigchel S, Visser-Meily JMA, Nijboer TCW. You never know where you are going until you know where you have been: Disorganized search after stroke. J Neuropsychol 2015; 10:256-75. [DOI: 10.1111/jnp.12068] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/29/2014] [Revised: 01/12/2015] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Antonia F. Ten Brink
- Brain Center Rudolf Magnus and Center of Excellence for Rehabilitation Medicine; University Medical Center Utrecht and De Hoogstraat Rehabilitation; The Netherlands
| | - Stefan Van der Stigchel
- Department of Experimental Psychology; Helmholtz Institute; Utrecht University; The Netherlands
| | - Johanna M. A. Visser-Meily
- Brain Center Rudolf Magnus and Center of Excellence for Rehabilitation Medicine; University Medical Center Utrecht and De Hoogstraat Rehabilitation; The Netherlands
| | - Tanja C. W. Nijboer
- Brain Center Rudolf Magnus and Center of Excellence for Rehabilitation Medicine; University Medical Center Utrecht and De Hoogstraat Rehabilitation; The Netherlands
- Department of Experimental Psychology; Helmholtz Institute; Utrecht University; The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Yokoyama JS, Sturm VE, Bonham LW, Klein E, Arfanakis K, Yu L, Coppola G, Kramer JH, Bennett DA, Miller BL, Dubal DB. Variation in longevity gene KLOTHO is associated with greater cortical volumes. Ann Clin Transl Neurol 2015; 2:215-30. [PMID: 25815349 PMCID: PMC4369272 DOI: 10.1002/acn3.161] [Citation(s) in RCA: 72] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/21/2014] [Accepted: 11/24/2014] [Indexed: 01/10/2023] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Identifying genetic variation associated with brain structures in aging may elucidate new biologic mechanisms underlying resilience to cognitive decline. We investigated whether carrying one copy of the protective haplotype "KL-VS" in longevity gene KLOTHO (KL) is associated with greater gray matter volume in healthy human aging compared to carrying no copies. METHODS We performed unbiased whole-brain analysis in cognitively normal older adults from two independent cohorts to assess the relationship between KL-VS and gray matter volume using voxel-based morphometry. RESULTS We found that KL-VS heterozygosity was associated with greater volume in right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (rDLPFC). Because rDLPFC is important for executive function, we analyzed working memory and processing speed in individuals. KL-VS heterozygosity was associated with enhanced executive function. Larger rDLPFC volume correlated with better executive function across the lifespan examined. Statistical analysis suggested that volume partially mediates the effect of genotype on cognition. INTERPRETATION These results suggest that variation in KL is associated with bigger brain volume and better function.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jennifer S Yokoyama
- Department of Neurology, University of California San FranciscoSan Francisco, California, 94158
| | - Virginia E Sturm
- Department of Neurology, University of California San FranciscoSan Francisco, California, 94158
| | - Luke W Bonham
- Department of Neurology, University of California San FranciscoSan Francisco, California, 94158
| | - Eric Klein
- Department of Neurology and Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior, The David Geffen School of Medicine at University of California Los AngelesLos Angeles, California, 90095
| | - Konstantinos Arfanakis
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Illinois Institute of TechnologyChicago, Illinois, 60616
- Rush Alzheimer's Disease Center, Rush University Medical CenterChicago, Illinois, 60612
| | - Lei Yu
- Rush Alzheimer's Disease Center, Rush University Medical CenterChicago, Illinois, 60612
| | - Giovanni Coppola
- Department of Neurology and Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior, The David Geffen School of Medicine at University of California Los AngelesLos Angeles, California, 90095
| | - Joel H Kramer
- Department of Neurology, University of California San FranciscoSan Francisco, California, 94158
| | - David A Bennett
- Rush Alzheimer's Disease Center, Rush University Medical CenterChicago, Illinois, 60612
| | - Bruce L Miller
- Department of Neurology, University of California San FranciscoSan Francisco, California, 94158
| | - Dena B Dubal
- Department of Neurology, University of California San FranciscoSan Francisco, California, 94158
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Schmitter-Edgecombe M, Robertson K. Recovery of visual search following moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2015; 37:162-77. [PMID: 25671675 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2014.998170] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Deficits in attentional abilities can significantly impact rehabilitation and recovery from traumatic brain injury (TBI). This study investigated the nature and recovery of preattentive (parallel) and attentive (serial) visual search abilities after TBI. METHOD Participants were 40 individuals with moderate to severe TBI who were tested following emergence from posttraumatic amnesia and approximately 8 months post injury, as well as 40 age- and education-matched controls. Preattentive (automatic) and attentive (controlled) visual search situations were created by manipulating the saliency of the target item amongst distractor items in visual displays. The relationship between preattentive and attentive visual search rates and follow-up community integration were also explored. RESULTS The results revealed intact parallel (automatic) processing skills in the TBI group both postacutely and at follow-up. In contrast, when attentional demands on visual search were increased by reducing the saliency of the target, the TBI group demonstrated poorer performances than the control group both postacutely and 8 months post injury. Neither preattentive nor attentive visual search slope values correlated with follow-up community integration. CONCLUSIONS These results suggest that utilizing intact preattentive visual search skills during rehabilitation may help to reduce high mental workload situations, thereby improving the rehabilitation process. For example, making commonly used objects more salient in the environment should increase reliance or more automatic visual search processes and reduce visual search time for individuals with TBI.
Collapse
|
30
|
Abstract
Deficits of attention, emotion, and cognition occur in individuals with alcohol abuse and addiction. This review elucidates the concepts of attention, emotion, and cognition and references research on the underlying neural networks and their compromise in alcohol use disorder. Neuroimaging research on adolescents with family history of alcoholism contributes to the understanding of pre-existing brain structural conditions and characterization of cognition and attention processes in high-risk individuals. Attention and cognition interact with other brain functions, including perceptual selection, salience, emotion, reward, and memory, through interconnected neural networks. Recent research reports compromised microstructural and functional network connectivity in alcoholism, which can have an effect on the dynamic tuning between brain systems, e.g., the frontally based executive control system, the limbic emotion system, and the midbrain-striatal reward system, thereby impeding cognitive flexibility and behavioral adaptation to changing environments. Finally, we introduce concepts of functional compensation, the capacity to generate attentional resources for performance enhancement, and brain structure recovery with abstinence. An understanding of the neural mechanisms of attention, emotion, and cognition will likely provide the basis for better treatment strategies for developing skills that enhance alcoholism therapy adherence and quality of life, and reduce the propensity for relapse.
Collapse
|