1
|
Han S, Lee JY, Cho SI, Oh DJ, Yoon DH. Risk Factors for Various Cognitive Function Decline Trajectories in Adults Over 40 Years of Age: A Retrospective Cohort Study. Psychiatry Investig 2023; 20:293-300. [PMID: 37005386 PMCID: PMC10151656 DOI: 10.30773/pi.2022.0188] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/03/2022] [Revised: 09/15/2022] [Accepted: 09/18/2022] [Indexed: 04/04/2023] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE The aims of our study were to identify distinct trajectories of cognitive function using the group-based trajectory model. We also investigate which demographic factors act as risk factors for cognitive decline in each group. METHODS The data from the Seoul National University Hospital Healthcare System Gangnam Center, from 2005 to 2019. The number of study subjects was 637. We used a group-based model to identify cognitive function trajectories. Multinomial logistic regression was employed to define risk factors for cognitive function decline. RESULTS The cognitive function trajectories among adults over 40 years of age were heterogeneous. We identified four trajectories: high (27.3%), medium (41.0%), low (22.7%), and rapid decline (9.1%). Older age, male, low educational level, bad dietary habits, diabetes mellitus, technical worker, and lower income increased the likelihood of a cognitive function decline. CONCLUSION A younger age, a higher educational level, professional worker, good dietary habits, no diabetes mellitus, and no obesity improved cognitive function. A combination of these factors can improve "cognitive reserve" and delay cognitive decline. Interventions to prevent cognitive decline are needed after identification of high-risk groups for cognitive decline.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sujeong Han
- Department of Public Health Science, Graduate School of Public Health, Seoul National University, Seoul, Republic of Korea
| | - Jun-Young Lee
- Department of Psychiatry, SMG-SNU Boramae Medical Center, Seoul, Republic of Korea
- Department of Psychiatry, Seoul National University College of Medicine, Seoul, Republic of Korea
- Department of Medical Device Development, Seoul National University, Seoul, Republic of Korea
| | - Sung-il Cho
- Department of Public Health Science, Graduate School of Public Health, Seoul National University, Seoul, Republic of Korea
- Institute of Health and Environment, Graduate School of Public Health, Seoul National University, Seoul, Republic of Korea
| | - Dae Jong Oh
- Workplace Mental Health Institute, Kangbuk Samsung Hospital, Sungkyunkwan University School of Medicine, Seoul, Republic of Korea
| | - Dae Hyun Yoon
- Department of Psychiatry, Healthcare System Gangnam Center, Seoul National University Hospital, Seoul, Republic of Korea
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Peters M, Schulz H. Theory-of-mind abilities in older patients with common mental disorders - a cross-sectional study. Aging Ment Health 2022; 26:1661-1668. [PMID: 34180279 DOI: 10.1080/13607863.2021.1935461] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/19/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Theory-of-mind (ToM) abilities are a basic competence for social interactions and relationships. Numerous findings demonstrate ToM deficits in old age, but such findings are missing in clinical samples of older adults. METHOD In the present study, patients treated in two clinics for common mental disorders (N = 150, distributed among the age groups 40-54, 55-69 and ≥70) were compared with a sample of people of the same age without mental disorders. The Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET) was used, in which the task is to detect mental states in the eyes, which are presented in 36 pictures. RESULTS The two groups differed significantly from each other: the clinical samples achieved worse results than the nonclinical samples, and the older samples achieved worse results than the younger samples. In the multiple regression analysis significant beta-weights were found for executive functions, physical diseases (especially vascular diseases, in the clinical sample) and higher education. CONCLUSION Older patients show clinically significant deficits in ToM abilities, which should be taken into account in interventions promoting ToM abilities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Meinolf Peters
- Institute for Psychotherapy for the Elderly, Department of Education, Philipps-University, Marburg, Germany
| | - Holger Schulz
- Department of Medical Psychology, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Electrophysiological signatures of brain aging in autism spectrum disorder. Cortex 2022; 148:139-151. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2021.09.022] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/10/2021] [Revised: 07/19/2021] [Accepted: 09/06/2021] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
|
4
|
Statsenko Y, Habuza T, Gorkom KNV, Zaki N, Almansoori TM, Al Zahmi F, Ljubisavljevic MR, Belghali M. Proportional Changes in Cognitive Subdomains During Normal Brain Aging. Front Aging Neurosci 2021; 13:673469. [PMID: 34867263 PMCID: PMC8634589 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2021.673469] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/27/2021] [Accepted: 09/20/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Background: Neuroscience lacks a reliable method of screening the early stages of dementia. Objective: To improve the diagnostics of age-related cognitive functions by developing insight into the proportionality of age-related changes in cognitive subdomains. Materials and Methods: We composed a battery of psychophysiological tests and collected an open-access psychophysiological outcomes of brain atrophy (POBA) dataset by testing individuals without dementia. To extend the utility of machine learning (ML) classification in cognitive studies, we proposed estimates of the disproportional changes in cognitive functions: an index of simple reaction time to decision-making time (ISD), ISD with the accuracy performance (ISDA), and an index of performance in simple and complex visual-motor reaction with account for accuracy (ISCA). Studying the distribution of the values of the indices over age allowed us to verify whether diverse cognitive functions decline equally throughout life or there is a divergence in age-related cognitive changes. Results: Unsupervised ML clustering shows that the optimal number of homogeneous age groups is four. The sample is segregated into the following age-groups: Adolescents ∈ [0, 20), Young adults ∈ [20, 40), Midlife adults ∈ [40, 60) and Older adults ≥60 year of age. For ISD, ISDA, and ISCA values, only the median of the Adolescents group is different from that of the other three age-groups sharing a similar distribution pattern (p > 0.01). After neurodevelopment and maturation, the indices preserve almost constant values with a slight trend toward functional decline. The reaction to a moving object (RMO) test results (RMO_mean) follow another tendency. The Midlife adults group's median significantly differs from the remaining three age subsamples (p < 0.01). No general trend in age-related changes of this dependent variable is observed. For all the data (ISD, ISDA, ISCA, and RMO_mean), Levene's test reveals no significant changes of the variances in age-groups (p > 0.05). Homoscedasticity also supports our assumption about a linear dependency between the observed features and age. Conclusion: In healthy brain aging, there are proportional age-related changes in the time estimates of information processing speed and inhibitory control in task switching. Future studies should test patients with dementia to determine whether the changes of the aforementioned indicators follow different patterns.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yauhen Statsenko
- Department of Radiology, College of Medicine and Health Sciences, United Arab Emirates University, Al Ain, United Arab Emirates.,Big Data Analytics Center, United Arab Emirates University, Al Ain, United Arab Emirates
| | - Tetiana Habuza
- Big Data Analytics Center, United Arab Emirates University, Al Ain, United Arab Emirates.,Department of Computer Science and Software Engineering, College of Information Technology, United Arab Emirates University, Al Ain, United Arab Emirates
| | - Klaus Neidl-Van Gorkom
- Department of Radiology, College of Medicine and Health Sciences, United Arab Emirates University, Al Ain, United Arab Emirates
| | - Nazar Zaki
- Big Data Analytics Center, United Arab Emirates University, Al Ain, United Arab Emirates.,Department of Computer Science and Software Engineering, College of Information Technology, United Arab Emirates University, Al Ain, United Arab Emirates
| | - Taleb M Almansoori
- Department of Radiology, College of Medicine and Health Sciences, United Arab Emirates University, Al Ain, United Arab Emirates
| | - Fatmah Al Zahmi
- Department of Neurology, Mediclinic Middle East Parkview Hospital, Dubai, United Arab Emirates.,Department of Clinical Science, Mohammed Bin Rashid University of Medicine and Health Sciences, Dubai, United Arab Emirates
| | - Milos R Ljubisavljevic
- Department of Radiology, College of Medicine and Health Sciences, United Arab Emirates University, Al Ain, United Arab Emirates
| | - Maroua Belghali
- College of Education, United Arab Emirates University, Al Ain, United Arab Emirates
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Poptsi E, Moraitou D, Tsardoulias E, Symeonidisd AL, Tsolaki M. Is the Discrimination of Subjective Cognitive Decline from Cognitively Healthy Adulthood and Mild Cognitive Impairment Possible? A Pilot Study Utilizing the R4Alz Battery. J Alzheimers Dis 2021; 77:715-732. [PMID: 32741834 DOI: 10.3233/jad-200562] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/16/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND The early diagnosis of neurocognitive disorders before the symptoms' onset is the ultimate goal of the scientific community. REMEDES for Alzheimer (R4Alz) is a battery, designed for assessing cognitive control abilities in people with minor and major neurocognitive disorders. OBJECTIVE To investigate whether the R4Alz battery's tasks differentiate subjective cognitive decline (SCD) from cognitively healthy adults (CHA) and mild cognitive impairment (MCI). METHODS The R4Alz battery was administered to 175 Greek adults, categorized in five groups a) healthy young adults (HYA; n = 42), b) healthy middle-aged adults (HMaA; n = 33), c) healthy older adults (HOA; n = 14), d) community-dwelling older adults with SCD (n = 34), and e) people with MCI (n = 52). RESULTS Between the seven R4Alz subtasks, four showcased the best results for differentiating HOA from SCD: the working memory updating (WMCUT-S3), the inhibition and switching subtask (ICT/RST-S1&S2), the failure sets (FS) of the ICT/RST-S1&S2, and the cognitive flexibility subtask (ICT/RST-S3). The total score of the four R4Alz subtasks (R4AlzTot4) leads to an excellent discrimination among SCD and healthy adulthood, and to fare discrimination among SCD and MCI. CONCLUSION The R4Alz battery is a novel approach regarding the neuropsychological assessment of people with SCD, since it can very well assist toward discriminating SCD from HOA. The R4Alz is able to measure decline of specific cognitive control abilities - namely of working memory updating, and complex executive functions - which seem to be the neuropsychological substrate of cognitive complaints in community dwelling adults of advancing age.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eleni Poptsi
- Laboratory of Psychology, Section of Cognitive and Experimental Psychology, Faculty of Philosophy, School of Psychology, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki (AUTh), Macedonia, Hellas.,Lab of Neurodegenerative Diseases, Center for Interdisciplinary Research and Innovation, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki (LND, CIRI - AUTh) Macedonia, Hellas.,Greek Association of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders (GAADRD), Thessaloniki, Macedonia, Hellas
| | - Despina Moraitou
- Laboratory of Psychology, Section of Cognitive and Experimental Psychology, Faculty of Philosophy, School of Psychology, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki (AUTh), Macedonia, Hellas.,Lab of Neurodegenerative Diseases, Center for Interdisciplinary Research and Innovation, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki (LND, CIRI - AUTh) Macedonia, Hellas.,Greek Association of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders (GAADRD), Thessaloniki, Macedonia, Hellas
| | - Emmanouil Tsardoulias
- School of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Faculty of Engineering, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki (AUTh), Macedonia, Hellas
| | - Andreas L Symeonidisd
- School of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Faculty of Engineering, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki (AUTh), Macedonia, Hellas
| | - Magda Tsolaki
- Lab of Neurodegenerative Diseases, Center for Interdisciplinary Research and Innovation, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki (LND, CIRI - AUTh) Macedonia, Hellas.,Greek Association of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders (GAADRD), Thessaloniki, Macedonia, Hellas.,1st Department of Neurology, School of Medicine, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki (AUTh), Macedonia, Hellas
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Bhattacharyya KK, Hueluer G, Meng H, Hyer K. Movement-based mind-body practices and cognitive function in middle-aged and older adults: Findings from the Midlife in the United States (MIDUS) study. Complement Ther Med 2021; 60:102751. [PMID: 34118391 DOI: 10.1016/j.ctim.2021.102751] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/03/2020] [Revised: 01/28/2021] [Accepted: 06/08/2021] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Cognitive function is a key component of healthy aging. While conventional physical activities (walking, jogging, etc.) have been shown to support physical and cognitive health in late-life, it remains unclear whether traditional Eastern movement-based mind-body practices (MBP) have long-term cognitive benefits above and beyond conventional leisure physical activities. This study examines the relationship between movement-based MBP and cognitive function in middle-aged and older adults during a 10-year follow-up period. METHODS We used data from Waves 2 (2004-05) and 3 (2013-14) of the Midlife in the United States (MIDUS) study. MIDUS initially surveyed a national probability sample of community-living adults aged 24-75 years in 1995 (Wave 1). Tests of cognitive functioning measuring executive function and episodic memory were added in Wave 2 and repeated in Wave 3. We estimated multivariable linear regression models to examine the effect of MBP (Wave 2) on the episodic memory and executive function (Wave 3) while controlling for covariates (sociodemographic factors, health, and cognitive function at Wave 2). RESULTS A total of 2097 individuals aged 42-92 years (M = 64 ± 11, 56 % women) were included. After controlling for sociodemographic factors, health and functional status, and prior levels of cognitive function, engaging in MBP was independently associated with a smaller decline in episodic memory (b = 0.11, p = .03), but not executive function (b=0.03, p = .34). DISCUSSION The findings provide the first large population-based evidence supporting the cognitive benefits of MBP over a 10-year period among middle-aged and older adults. Future research should examine whether MBP are effective non-pharmacological intervention to attenuate age-related cognitive decline.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kallol Kumar Bhattacharyya
- Florida Policy Exchange Center on Aging, School of Aging Studies, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA.
| | - Gizem Hueluer
- Florida Policy Exchange Center on Aging, School of Aging Studies, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA
| | - Hongdao Meng
- Florida Policy Exchange Center on Aging, School of Aging Studies, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA
| | - Kathryn Hyer
- Florida Policy Exchange Center on Aging, School of Aging Studies, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Similar Theory of Mind Deficits in Community Dwelling Older Adults with Vascular Risk Profile and Patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment: The Case of Paradoxical Sarcasm Comprehension. Brain Sci 2021; 11:brainsci11050627. [PMID: 34068226 PMCID: PMC8153105 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci11050627] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/28/2021] [Revised: 05/11/2021] [Accepted: 05/12/2021] [Indexed: 12/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Recent studies deal with disorders and deficits caused by vascular syndrome in efforts for prediction and prevention. Cardiovascular health declines with age due to vascular risk factors, and this leads to an increasing risk of cognitive decline. Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is defined as the negative cognitive changes beyond what is expected in normal aging. The purpose of the study was to compare older adults with vascular risk factors (VRF), MCI patients, and healthy controls (HC) in social cognition and especially in theory of mind ability (ToM). The sample comprised a total of 109 adults, aged 50 to 85 years (M = 66.09, SD = 9.02). They were divided into three groups: (a) older adults with VRF, (b) MCI patients, and (c) healthy controls (HC). VRF and MCI did not differ significantly in age, educational level or gender as was the case with HC. Specifically, for assessing ToM, a social inference test was used, which was designed to measure sarcasm comprehension. Results showed that the performance of the VRF group and MCI patients is not differentiated, while HC performed higher compared to the other two groups. The findings may imply that the development of a vascular disorder affecting vessels of the brain is associated from its “first steps” to ToM decline, at least regarding specific aspects of it, such as paradoxical sarcasm understanding.
Collapse
|
8
|
Tsentidou G, Moraitou D, Tsolaki M. Cognition in Vascular Aging and Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Alzheimers Dis 2020; 72:55-70. [PMID: 31561369 DOI: 10.3233/jad-190638] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/11/2022]
Abstract
Cardiovascular health declines with age, due to vascular risk factors, and this leads to an increasing risk of cognitive decline. Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is defined as the negative cognitive changes beyond what is expected in normal aging. The purpose of the study was to compare older adults with vascular risk factors (VRF), MCI patients, and healthy controls (HC) in main dimensions of cognitive control. The sample comprised a total of 109 adults, aged 50 to 85 (M = 66.09, S.D. = 9.02). They were divided into three groups: 1) older adults with VRF, 2) MCI patients, and 3) healthy controls (HC). VRF and MCI did not differ significantly in age, educational level, or gender as was the case with HC. The tests used mainly examine inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and working memory processing. Results showed that the VRF group had more Set Loss Errors in drawing designs indicating deficits in establishing cognitive set and in cognitive shifting. MCI patients displayed lower performance in processing. Hence, different types of specific impairments emerge in vascular aging and MCI, and this may imply that discrete underlying pathologies may play a role in the development of somewhat different profiles of cognitive decline.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Glykeria Tsentidou
- Laboratoty of Psychology, Department of Experimental and Cognitive Psychology, School of Psychology, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Thessaloniki, Greece.,Laboratory of Neurodegenerative Diseases, Center for Interdisciplinary Research and Innovation (CIRI), AUTh, Greece
| | - Despina Moraitou
- Laboratoty of Psychology, Department of Experimental and Cognitive Psychology, School of Psychology, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Thessaloniki, Greece.,Greek Association of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders, Thessaloniki (GAADRD), Greece.,Laboratory of Neurodegenerative Diseases, Center for Interdisciplinary Research and Innovation (CIRI), AUTh, Greece
| | - Magda Tsolaki
- 1st Department of Neurology, Medical School, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki (AUTh), Greece.,Greek Association of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders, Thessaloniki (GAADRD), Greece.,Laboratory of Neurodegenerative Diseases, Center for Interdisciplinary Research and Innovation (CIRI), AUTh, Greece
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Larson-Dupuis C, Léveillé E, Desjardins M, Jodoin M, Bourassa MÈ, Bergeron H, Beaulieu C, Carrier J, Pepin V, De Beaumont L. Subtle long-term cognitive effects of a single mild traumatic brain injury and the impact of a three-month aerobic exercise intervention. J Sports Med Phys Fitness 2020; 61:87-95. [PMID: 32878422 DOI: 10.23736/s0022-4707.20.10918-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Although there is a growing body of literature on the impact of multiple concussions on cognitive function with aging, less is known about the long-term impact of sustaining a single mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI). Additionally, very few interventions exist to treat mTBI patients and prevent a possible accelerated cognitive decline. This study aimed to: 1) examine the long-term effects of a single mTBI on cognition in patients aged between 55 and 70 years old; and 2) evaluate the cognitive effects of an aerobic exercise program for these patients. METHODS Thirty-five participants (average age: 58.89, SD=4.14) were assessed using neuropsychological tests. Among them, 18 hadsustained a mTBI two to seven years earlier. Significant differences in information processing speed, executive function and visual memory were found between controls and mTBI patients. Sixteen of the mTBI patients then engaged in a 12-week physical exercise program. They were divided into equivalent groups: 1) aerobic training (cycle ergometers); or 2) stretching exercises. The participants' cardiopulmonary fitness (VO<inf>2max</inf>) was evaluated pre- and postintervention and neuropsychological tests were re-administered postintervention. RESULTS Participants from the aerobic group significantly improved their fitness compared to the stretching group. However, no between-group difference was found on neuropsychological measures postintervention. CONCLUSIONS In summary, this study shows long-term cognitive effects of mTBI in late adulthood patients. Moreover, the controlled, 12-week aerobic exercise program did not lead to cognitive improvements in our small mTBI sample. Lastly, future directions in optimizing mTBI intervention are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Camille Larson-Dupuis
- Montreal Sacred Heart Hospital Research Center, Montreal, QC, Canada.,Department of Psychology, University of Montreal, Montreal, QC, Canada
| | - Edith Léveillé
- Montreal Sacred Heart Hospital Research Center, Montreal, QC, Canada
| | | | - Marianne Jodoin
- Montreal Sacred Heart Hospital Research Center, Montreal, QC, Canada.,Department of Psychology, University of Montreal, Montreal, QC, Canada
| | | | - Hélène Bergeron
- Montreal Sacred Heart Hospital Research Center, Montreal, QC, Canada
| | | | - Julie Carrier
- Montreal Sacred Heart Hospital Research Center, Montreal, QC, Canada.,Department of Psychology, University of Montreal, Montreal, QC, Canada
| | - Véronique Pepin
- Montreal Sacred Heart Hospital Research Center, Montreal, QC, Canada.,Department of Health, Kinesiology, and Applied Physiology, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada
| | - Louis De Beaumont
- Montreal Sacred Heart Hospital Research Center, Montreal, QC, Canada - .,Department of Surgery, University of Montreal, Montreal, QC, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Aichele S, Ghisletta P. Memory Deficits Precede Increases in Depressive Symptoms in Later Adulthood. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2020; 74:943-953. [PMID: 29385518 DOI: 10.1093/geronb/gbx183] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/19/2017] [Accepted: 12/29/2017] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVES We examined bidirectional, time-ordered associations between age-related changes in depressive symptoms and memory. METHOD Data came from 107,599 community-dwelling adults, aged 49-90 years, who participated in the Survey of Health, Ageing, and Retirement in Europe (SHARE). Depressive symptoms were measured with the EURO-D inventory, and memory was evaluated as delayed recall of a 10-word list. Participants were assessed up to five times at 2-year intervals. Dynamic structural equation models were used to estimate longitudinal and time-ordered (lead-lag) relations between depressive symptoms and memory performance. RESULTS Depressive symptoms increased and memory scores decreased across the observed age range, with worsening mostly evident after age 62 years. These long-term changes were moderately negatively correlated (r = -.53, p < .001). A time-ordered effect emerged such that age-specific memory deficits preceded shorter-term increases in depression symptoms. This effect can be translated such that each 1-point decrement on a 10-point memory scale at a given age predicted a 14.5% increased risk for depression two years later. Statistical adjustment for covariates (sex, education, re-test, smoking, and body mass index) had little influence on these associations. CONCLUSION In later adulthood, lower memory performance at a given age predicts subsequent 2-year increases in depressive symptoms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stephen Aichele
- Swiss National Center of Competence in Research LIVES-Overcoming vulnerability: Life course perspectives, Universities of Lausanne and of Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Paolo Ghisletta
- Swiss National Center of Competence in Research LIVES-Overcoming vulnerability: Life course perspectives, Universities of Lausanne and of Geneva, Switzerland.,Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Geneva, Switzerland.,Swiss Distance Learning University, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Poptsi E, Moraitou D, Eleftheriou M, Kounti-Zafeiropoulou F, Papasozomenou C, Agogiatou C, Bakoglidou E, Batsila G, Liapi D, Markou N, Nikolaidou E, Ouzouni F, Soumpourou A, Vasiloglou M, Tsolaki M. Normative Data for the Montreal Cognitive Assessment in Greek Older Adults With Subjective Cognitive Decline, Mild Cognitive Impairment and Dementia. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 2019; 32:265-274. [PMID: 31159629 DOI: 10.1177/0891988719853046] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE The aim of the study was to provide normative data for the MoCA in a Greek cohort of people older than 60 years who meet criteria for subjective cognitive decline (SCD), mild cognitive impairment (MCI), or dementia in order to optimize cutoff scores for each diagnostic group. METHOD Seven hundred forty-six community-dwelling older adults, visitors of the Day Center of Alzheimer Hellas were randomly chosen. Three hundred seventy-nine of them met the criteria for dementia, 245 for MCI and 122 for SCD. RESULTS Initial statistical analyses showed that the total MoCA score is not affected by gender (P = .164), or age (P = .144) but is affected by educational level (P < .001). A cutoff score of 23 for low educational level (≤6 years) can distinguish people with SCD from MCI (sensitivity 71.4%, specificity 84.2%), while 26 is the cutoff score for middle educational level (7-12 years; sensitivity 73.2%, specificity 67.0%) and high educational level (≥13 years; sensitivity 77.6%, specificity 74.7%). Montreal Cognitive Assessment can discriminate older adults with SCD from dementia, with a cutoff score of 20 for low educational level (sensitivity 100%, specificity 92.3%) and a cutoff score 23 for middle educational level (sensitivity 97.6%, specificity 92.7%) and high educational level (sensitivity 98.5%, specificity 100%). CONCLUSION Montreal Cognitive Assessment is not affected by age or gender but is affected by the educational level. The discriminant potential of MoCA between SCD and MCI is good, while the discrimination of SCD from dementia is excellent.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eleni Poptsi
- 1 Greek Association of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders, Thessaloniki, Greece.,2 Laboratory of Psychology, Section of Cognitive and Experimental Psychology, School of Psychology, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki (AUTh), Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Despina Moraitou
- 1 Greek Association of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders, Thessaloniki, Greece.,2 Laboratory of Psychology, Section of Cognitive and Experimental Psychology, School of Psychology, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki (AUTh), Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Marina Eleftheriou
- 1 Greek Association of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders, Thessaloniki, Greece
| | | | - Chrysa Papasozomenou
- 1 Greek Association of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders, Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Christina Agogiatou
- 1 Greek Association of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders, Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Evaggelia Bakoglidou
- 1 Greek Association of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders, Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Georgia Batsila
- 1 Greek Association of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders, Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Despina Liapi
- 1 Greek Association of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders, Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Nefeli Markou
- 1 Greek Association of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders, Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Evdokia Nikolaidou
- 1 Greek Association of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders, Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Fani Ouzouni
- 1 Greek Association of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders, Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Aikaterini Soumpourou
- 1 Greek Association of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders, Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Maria Vasiloglou
- 1 Greek Association of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders, Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Magda Tsolaki
- 1 Greek Association of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders, Thessaloniki, Greece.,4 1st Department of Neurology, Medical School, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki (AUTh), Thessaloniki, Greece
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Pantsiou K, Sfakianaki O, Papaliagkas V, Savvoulidou D, Costa V, Papantoniou G, Moraitou D. Inhibitory Control, Task/Rule Switching, and Cognitive Planning in Vascular Dementia: Are There Any Differences From Vascular Aging? Front Aging Neurosci 2018; 10:330. [PMID: 30410439 PMCID: PMC6211074 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00330] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/04/2018] [Accepted: 09/28/2018] [Indexed: 01/06/2023] Open
Abstract
Recent studies have shown that patients diagnosed with Vascular Dementia (VaD) exhibit deficits in executive functions. According to “vascular hypothesis of cognitive aging,” community-dwelling older adults having risk factors for vascular disease development (RVD) may suffer from cognitive decline of the same type. The aim of the study was to assess the level of specific executive functions (EF) that have been revealed as most affected by vascular abnormalities, in older adults with incipient VaD and RVD. Subsequently specific ways of EF measuring could be suggested for more accurate diagnosis of early stage VaD. The study compared three adult groups (N = 60): (a) patients diagnosed with incipient VaD, according to DSM-5 criteria (n = 20); (b) community-dwelling older adults presenting cardiovascular risk factors (RVD; n = 20); (c) healthy young adult controls (n = 20). Three types of executive functions were examined: inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility as rule/task switching, and planning. The following D-KEFS subtests were administered for their evaluation: The ‘Color-Word Interference Test,’ the ‘Verbal Fluency Test,’ and the ‘Tower Test.’ Mixed-measures ANOVA, MANOVA, and one-way ANOVA as well as Scheffe post hoc test were applied to the data of the scores in each condition of each test. The results showed that VaD patients had significantly lower performance in test conditions requiring switching and planning, compared to RVD group and young controls. The specific deficits of VaD patients, compared to older adults presenting RVD according to multiple-group path analyses were: more uncorrected errors in inhibition, the use of semantic knowledge primarily instead of switching ability to switch between semantic categories, as well as a lower level of movement precision in planning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Krystallia Pantsiou
- Lab of Psychology, Department of Experimental and Cognitive Psychology, School of Psychology, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Ourania Sfakianaki
- Lab of Psychology, Department of Experimental and Cognitive Psychology, School of Psychology, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Vasileios Papaliagkas
- Laboratory of Clinical Neurophysiology, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Dimitra Savvoulidou
- Lab of Psychology, Department of Experimental and Cognitive Psychology, School of Psychology, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Vassiliki Costa
- 1st Neurology Department, AHEPA Hospital, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Thessaloniki, Greece
| | - Georgia Papantoniou
- Department of Early Childhood Education, School of Education, University of Ioannina, Ioannina, Greece
| | - Despina Moraitou
- Lab of Psychology, Department of Experimental and Cognitive Psychology, School of Psychology, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Thessaloniki, Greece
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Robinson SA, Lachman ME. Perceived control and cognition in adulthood: The mediating role of physical activity. Psychol Aging 2018; 33:769-781. [PMID: 29985012 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000273] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The cognitive benefits of a greater sense of control are well-established; however, only recently have the mechanisms involved in this relationship been explored. Because of its well-established cognitive benefits and positive relationship to perceived control, physical activity has been suggested as a potential mediator. However, with age, not only does cognition tend to decline, but so does one's perception of control and their level of physical activity. Therefore, it is important to understand the relationship between these variables from a life span perspective. The goal of the current study was to expand on past work that investigated physical activity as a mediator between perceived control and change in cognition across 4 years to a multi-decade perspective that examines these variables as they change from midlife to older adulthood. To do so, we used longitudinal data across 20 years from the Midlife in the United States Study. Our results show that perceiving more control over one's life predicted less decline in cognition 20 years later, and this relationship was mediated by an increase in physical activity. We consider limitations and future directions to further our understanding of the role of physical activity in the relationship between perceived control and cognitive aging. (PsycINFO Database Record
Collapse
|
14
|
Estimates of age-related memory decline are inflated by unrecognized Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging 2018; 70:170-179. [PMID: 30015036 DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.06.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/10/2018] [Revised: 05/17/2018] [Accepted: 06/04/2018] [Indexed: 01/23/2023]
Abstract
Cognitive decline is considered an inevitable consequence of aging; however, estimates of cognitive aging may be influenced negatively by undetected preclinical Alzheimer's disease (AD). This study aimed to determine the extent to which estimates of cognitive aging were biased by preclinical AD. Cognitively normal older adults (n = 494) with amyloid-β status determined from positron emission tomography neuroimaging underwent serial neuropsychological assessment at 18-month intervals over 72 months. Estimates of the effects of age on verbal memory, working memory, executive function, and processing speed were derived using linear mixed models. The presence of preclinical AD and clinical progression to mild cognitive impairment or dementia during the study were then added to these models as covariates. Initially, age was associated with decline across all 4 cognitive domains. With the effects of elevated amyloid-β and clinical progression controlled, age was no longer associated with decline in verbal or working memory. However, the magnitude of decline was reduced only slightly for executive function and was unchanged for processing speed. Thus, considered together, the results of the study indicate that undetected preclinical AD negatively biases estimates of age-related cognitive decline for verbal and working memory.
Collapse
|
15
|
Infurna FJ, Mayer A, Anstey KJ. The effect of perceived control on self-reported cardiovascular disease incidence across adulthood and old age. Psychol Health 2017. [PMID: 28648098 DOI: 10.1080/08870446.2017.1341513] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE To examine whether rates of change in perceived control are predictive of cardiovascular disease (CVD) incidence across adulthood and old age. DESIGN We used the PATH Through Life Project (n = 7103, M = 40, SD = 16; 52% women), a longitudinal panel survey that encompasses three cohorts at Time 1, ages 20-24, 40-44 and 60-64, who have been assessed three times at four-year intervals. METHOD We examined whether rates of change in perceived control were associated with CVD incidence over 8 years of time, over and above that of baseline levels of perceived control and known risk factors for CVD. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES Self-reported CVD incidence. RESULTS Increases in perceived control over time were associated with decreased likelihood of 8-year incidence of CVD and these effects were independent of socio-demographics, covariates and baseline levels of perceived control. The effects were consistent across young adulthood, midlife and old age and for men and women. CONCLUSIONS Findings demonstrate the importance of changes in perceived control as a predictor of CVD incidence across adulthood and old age. We suggest future research using mediation analysis to test reverse causality and mechanisms underlying the effects of perceived control on CVD incidence.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Frank J Infurna
- a Department of Psychology , Arizona State University , Tempe , AZ , USA
| | | | - Kaarin J Anstey
- c Centre for Research on Ageing, Health and Wellbeing , The Australian National University , Canberra , Australia
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Clouston SAP, Denier N. Mental retirement and health selection: Analyses from the U.S. Health and Retirement Study. Soc Sci Med 2017; 178:78-86. [PMID: 28213301 PMCID: PMC5400287 DOI: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.01.019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/04/2015] [Revised: 01/09/2017] [Accepted: 01/16/2017] [Indexed: 01/05/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Research has recently suggested that retirement may decrease cognitive engagement, resulting in cognitive aging. Few studies have systematically documented whether or how selectivity into retirement shapes the relationship between retirement and cognitive aging. METHODS We draw on data from the Health and Retirement Study (1998-2012) to examine the relationship between cognition and retirement for 18,575 labor force participants. Longitudinal regression discontinuity modeling was used to examine performance and decline in episodic memory. Models differentiated three forms of selection bias: indirect and direct selection as well as reverse causation. To further interrogate the disuse hypothesis, we adjust for confounding from health and socioeconomic sources. RESULTS Results revealed that individuals who retired over the course of the panel were substantially different in terms of health, wealth and cognition when compared to those who remained employed. However, accounting for observed selection biases, significant associations were found linking longer retirement with more rapid cognitive decline. DISCUSSION This study examined respondents who were in the labor force at baseline and transitioned into retirement. Analyses suggested that those who retired over the course of the panel had worse overall functioning, but also experienced more rapid declines after retirement that increased the rate of aging by two-fold, resulting in yearly losses of 3.7% (95% CI = [3.5, 4.0]) of one standard deviation in functioning attributable to retirement. Results are supportive of the view that retirement is associated with more rapid cognitive aging.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sean A P Clouston
- Program in Public Health & Department of Preventive Medicine, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA.
| | - Nicole Denier
- Department of Sociology, McGill University, Montréal, QC, Canada; Department of Sociology, Colby College, Waterville, ME, USA
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Lyu J, Kim HY. Gender-Specific Incidence and Predictors of Cognitive Impairment among Older Koreans: Findings from a 6-Year Prospective Cohort Study. Psychiatry Investig 2016; 13:473-479. [PMID: 27757124 PMCID: PMC5067340 DOI: 10.4306/pi.2016.13.5.473] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/15/2015] [Revised: 10/16/2015] [Accepted: 12/28/2015] [Indexed: 01/15/2023] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE This study investigated gender-specific incidence and predictors of cognitive impairment among community-dwelling older adults in South Korea. METHODS Using data from the 2006 and 2012 Korean Longitudinal Study of Ageing (KLoSA), 925 females and 834 males aged 65 and over without cognitive impairment at 2006 were analyzed separately. Cognitive impairment was measured based on the Korean version of the Mini-Mental State Exam (K-MMSE) normative score. Generalized Estimating Equations (GEE) was conducted to examine the predictors of cognitive impairment at 6-year follow up. RESULTS Incidence of cognitive impairment at 2012 was significantly higher for women (30.5%) than men (26.1%). GEE result showed that depression was significantly associated with cognitive impairment for both genders (female: OR=2.26, 95% CI=1.63-3.12; male: OR=3.26, 95% CI=2.19-4.83). Having IADL limitations (OR=1.15, 95% CI=1.03-1.28), high blood pressure (OR=1.72, 95% CI=1.27-2.34), poor hearing (OR=1.94, 95% CI=1.29-2.92), regular exercise (OR=0.67, 95% CI=0.45-0.99), and normal weight (OR=1.39, 95% CI=1.03-1.86) were significant predictors of cognitive impairment only among women. In contrast, age (OR=1.04, 95% CI=1.01-1.07) and ADL limitations (OR=1.48, 95% CI=1.21-1.82) were significant predictors of cognitive impairment at follow-up only among men. CONCLUSION Findings of this study show gender-specific predictors of cognitive impairment among older Koreans. This study can provide information for clinicians and policy makers to develop different intervention strategies considering gender differences in the progress of cognitive impairment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jiyoung Lyu
- National Institute of Dementia, Seongnam, Republic of Korea
| | - Hae-Young Kim
- Department of Health Policy and Management, College of Health Sciences & Department of Public Health Sciences, Graduate School, Korea University, Seoul, Republic of Korea
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Infurna FJ, Gerstorf D, Ram N, Schupp J, Wagner GG, Heckhausen J. Maintaining Perceived Control with Unemployment Facilitates Future Adjustment. JOURNAL OF VOCATIONAL BEHAVIOR 2016; 93:103-119. [PMID: 26924845 DOI: 10.1016/j.jvb.2016.01.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
Unemployment is a major challenge to individuals' development. An important personal resource to ameliorate the negative impact of unemployment may be perceived control, a general-purpose belief system. Little is known, however, about how perceived control itself changes with the experience of unemployment and what the antecedents, correlates, and consequences of such change in perceived control are in different ages. We use data from the German Socio-Economic Panel Study (N = 413 who experienced unemployment and N = 413 case-matched controls; time period of data collection: 1994 - 1996) to examine whether perceived control changes with unemployment, explore the role of socio-demographic, psychosocial and health factors in moderating such change, and investigate whether levels of perceived control prior to unemployment and unemployment-related change in perceived control predict unemployment-related outcomes up to five years following. Results indicated that, on average, perceived control remained relatively stable with unemployment, and that younger and older workers did not differ in this regard. However, there were sizeable individual differences in change in perceived control, with women and those with fewer years of education experiencing greater unemployment-related declines in perceived control. Lower levels of perceived control prior to unemployment and steeper unemployment-related decrements in perceived control were each associated with a higher risk of remaining unemployed in the 12 months immediately following unemployment. Steeper unemployment-related declines in perceived control also predicted lower life satisfaction up to five years following. We discuss possible pathways by which perceived control may facilitate adjustment to unemployment, consider the role of perceived control for better understanding the dynamics of unemployment, and suggest routes for further more process-oriented inquiry.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Denis Gerstorf
- Humboldt University Berlin, Germany; German Institute for Economic Research (DIW Berlin), Germany; Pennsylvania State University, University Park, USA
| | - Nilam Ram
- German Institute for Economic Research (DIW Berlin), Germany; Pennsylvania State University, University Park, USA; Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
| | - Jürgen Schupp
- German Institute for Economic Research (DIW Berlin), Germany; Free University, Berlin, Germany
| | - Gert G Wagner
- German Institute for Economic Research (DIW Berlin), Germany; Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany; Berlin University of Technology, Berlin, Germany
| | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Aichele S, Rabbitt P, Ghisletta P. Think Fast, Feel Fine, Live Long: A 29-Year Study of Cognition, Health, and Survival in Middle-Aged and Older Adults. Psychol Sci 2016; 27:518-29. [PMID: 26917212 DOI: 10.1177/0956797615626906] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/02/2015] [Accepted: 12/21/2015] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
In a 29-year study of 6,203 individuals ranging in age from 41 to 96 years at initial assessment, we evaluated the relative and combined influence of 65 mortality risk factors, which included sociodemographic variables, lifestyle attributes, medical indices, and multiple cognitive abilities. Reductions in mortality risk were most associated with higher self-rated health, female gender, fewer years as a smoker, and smaller decrements in processing speed with age. Thus, two psychological variables-subjective health status and processing speed-were among the top predictors of survival. We suggest that these psychological attributes, unlike risk factors that are more narrowly defined, reflect (and are influenced by) a broad range of health-related behaviors and characteristics. Information about these attributes can be obtained with relatively little effort or cost and-given the tractability of these measures in different cultural contexts-may prove expedient for prevention, diagnosis, and treatment of conditions related to increased mortality risk in diverse human populations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stephen Aichele
- Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Geneva
| | | | - Paolo Ghisletta
- Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Geneva Distance Learning University, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Kuller LH, Lopez OL, Becker JT, Chang Y, Newman AB. Risk of dementia and death in the long-term follow-up of the Pittsburgh Cardiovascular Health Study-Cognition Study. Alzheimers Dement 2016; 12:170-183. [PMID: 26519786 PMCID: PMC4744537 DOI: 10.1016/j.jalz.2015.08.165] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/30/2015] [Revised: 07/30/2015] [Accepted: 08/26/2015] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Increasing life expectancy has resulted in a larger population of older individuals at risk of dementia. METHODS The Cardiovascular Health Study-Cognition Study followed 532 participants from 1998-99 (mean age 79) to 2013 (mean age 93) for death and dementia. RESULTS Risk of death was determined by extent of coronary artery calcium, high-sensitivity cardiac troponin, brain natriuretic peptide, and white matter grade. Significant predictors of dementia were age, apolipoprotein-E4, vocabulary raw score, hippocampal volume, ventricular size, cognitive performance, and number of blocks walked. By 2013, 160 of 532 were alive, including 19 cognitively normal. Those with normal cognition had higher grade education, better cognition test scores, greater hippocampal volume, faster gait speed, and number of blocks walked as compared with survivors who were demented. DISCUSSION Few survived free of dementia and disability. Prevention and delay of cognitive decline for this older population is an imperative.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lewis H Kuller
- Department of Epidemiology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA.
| | - Oscar L Lopez
- Department of Neurology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA; Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
| | - James T Becker
- Department of Neurology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA; Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA; Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
| | - Yuefang Chang
- Department of Neurological Surgery, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
| | - Anne B Newman
- Department of Epidemiology, Center for Aging and Population Health, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Maintaining older brain functionality: A targeted review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2015; 55:453-77. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.06.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 132] [Impact Index Per Article: 14.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/19/2015] [Revised: 06/01/2015] [Accepted: 06/03/2015] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
|
22
|
Song J, Mailick MR, Greenberg JS, Ryff CD, Lachman ME. Cognitive Aging in Parents of Children with Disabilities. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2015; 71:821-30. [PMID: 25804212 DOI: 10.1093/geronb/gbv015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/14/2014] [Accepted: 02/04/2015] [Indexed: 12/20/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE This study examines the cognitive functioning of parents of children with disabilities, specifically, whether there is an evidence of accelerated cognitive aging among these parents. In addition, the study investigates the moderating influences of two variables: parents' gender and stress from negative parenting experience. METHOD The analyses utilize data from the National Survey of Midlife in the United States (2005). The analytic sample consisted of two groups of parents, who completed the cognitive battery, the interview, and the mail-back survey: 128 parents who had children with childhood-onset disabilities and 512 matched comparison parents who had only nondisabled children. RESULTS Age differences in episodic memory were more pronounced among mothers of children with disabilities than among mothers with nondisabled children, especially among mothers with higher levels of negative parenting experience. In contrast, there were no interaction effects of parenting status, age, and negative parenting experience among fathers. DISCUSSION The results show that parenting children with disabilities over a prolonged period of time jeopardizes cognitive function (especially memory) among older mothers, possibly via the mechanism of heightened parenting stress due to higher levels of negative parenting experience.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | - Carol D Ryff
- Institute on Aging and Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison
| | - Margie E Lachman
- Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, Waltham, Massachusetts
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Luszcz MA, Anstey KJ, Ghisletta P. Subjective Beliefs, Memory and Functional Health: Change and Associations over 12 Years in the Australian Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Gerontology 2015; 61:241-50. [DOI: 10.1159/000369800] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/25/2014] [Accepted: 11/11/2014] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
|
24
|
Abstract
This study explored effects of the metabolic syndrome (MetS) on language in aging. MetS is a constellation of five vascular and metabolic risk factors associated with the development of chronic diseases and increased risk of mortality, as well as brain and cognitive impairments. We tested 281 English-speaking older adults aged 55-84, free of stroke and dementia. Presence of MetS was based on the harmonized criteria (Alberti et al., 2009). Language performance was assessed by measures of accuracy and reaction time on two tasks of lexical retrieval and two tasks of sentence processing. Regression analyses, adjusted for age, education, gender, diabetes, hypertension, and heart disease, demonstrated that participants with MetS had significantly lower accuracy on measures of lexical retrieval (action naming) and sentence processing (embedded sentences, both subject and object relative clauses). Reaction time was slightly faster on the test of embedded sentences among those with MetS. MetS adversely affects the language performance of older adults, impairing accuracy of both lexical retrieval and sentence processing. This finding reinforces and extends results of earlier research documenting the negative influence of potentially treatable medical conditions (diabetes, hypertension) on language performance in aging. The unanticipated finding that persons with MetS were faster in processing embedded sentences may represent an impairment of timing functions among older individuals with MetS.
Collapse
|
25
|
Goral M, Campanelli L, Spiro A. Language dominance and inhibition abilities in bilingual older adults. BILINGUALISM (CAMBRIDGE, ENGLAND) 2015; 18:79-89. [PMID: 27531968 PMCID: PMC4983451 DOI: 10.1017/s1366728913000126] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/30/2023]
Abstract
This study aimed to examine the so-called bilingual advantage in older adults' performance in three cognitive domains and to identify whether language use and bilingual type (dominant vs. balanced) predicted performance. The participants were 106 Spanish-English bilinguals ranging in age from 50 years to 84 years. Three cognitive domains were examined (each by a single test): inhibition (the Simon task), alternating attention (the Trail Making test), and working memory (Month Ordering). The data revealed that age was negatively correlated to performance in each domain. Bilingual type - balanced vs. dominant - predicted performance and interacted with age only on the inhibition measure (the Simon task). Balanced bilinguals showed age-related inhibition decline (i.e., greater Simon effect with increasing age); in contrast, dominant bilinguals showed little or no age-related change. The findings suggest that bilingualism may offer cognitive advantage in older age only for a subset of bilinguals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mira Goral
- Lehman College & the Graduate Center of the City University of New York
| | | | - Avron Spiro
- VA Boston Healthcare System & Boston University Schools of Public Health and Medicine
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Horvat P, Richards M, Kubinova R, Pajak A, Malyutina S, Shishkin S, Pikhart H, Peasey A, Marmot MG, Singh-Manoux A, Bobak M. Alcohol consumption, drinking patterns, and cognitive function in older Eastern European adults. Neurology 2014; 84:287-95. [PMID: 25503981 PMCID: PMC4335999 DOI: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000001164] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
Objective: To investigate associations of frequency, quantity, binge, and problem drinking with cognitive function in older Eastern European adults. Methods: The investigation included 14,575 participants, aged 47 to 78 years at cognitive assessment in 2006–2008 from Novosibirsk (Russia), Krakow (Poland), and 6 Czech towns participating in the HAPIEE (Health, Alcohol, and Psychosocial Factors in Eastern Europe) prospective cohort study. Average response rates were 59% at baseline (2002–2005) and 63% in 2006–2008. Alcohol consumption was assessed at baseline and in 2006–2008. Cognitive tests included immediate and delayed word recall, semantic fluency (animal naming), and letter cancellation. Associations between alcohol indices and cognitive scores were analyzed cross-sectionally (all measures from 2006 to 2008) and prospectively (alcohol and covariates from 2002 to 2005 and cognition from 2006 to 2008). Results: In cross-sectional analyses, nondrinkers had lower cognitive scores and female moderate drinkers had better cognitive performance than light drinkers. Heavy, binge, and problem drinking were not consistently associated with cognitive function. Few associations were replicated in prospective analyses. Participants who stopped drinking during follow-up had worse cognition than stable drinkers; in men, regression coefficients (95% confidence interval) ranged from −0.26 (−0.36, −0.16) for immediate recall to −0.14 (−0.24, −0.04) for fluency. Conclusion: Regular and episodic heavy drinking were not consistently associated with cognitive function. Worse cognition in participants who stopped drinking during follow-up suggests that inclusion of less healthy ex-drinkers may partly explain poorer cognition in nondrinkers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pia Horvat
- From the Department of Epidemiology & Public Health (P.H., H.P., A. Peasey, M.G.M., A.S.-M., M.B.), and MRC Unit for Lifelong Health and Ageing at UCL (M.R.), University College London, UK; National Institute of Public Health (R.K.), Prague, Czech Republic; Department of Epidemiology and Population Studies (A. Pajak), Jagellonian University Collegium Medicum, Krakow, Poland; Institute of Internal and Preventive Medicine Siberian Branch under the Russian Academy of Medical Sciences (S.M., S.S.), Novosibirsk; Novosibirsk State Medical University (S.M.), Novosibirsk, Russia; and INSERM (A.S.-M.), U1018, Hopital Paul Brousse, Villejuif, France.
| | - Marcus Richards
- From the Department of Epidemiology & Public Health (P.H., H.P., A. Peasey, M.G.M., A.S.-M., M.B.), and MRC Unit for Lifelong Health and Ageing at UCL (M.R.), University College London, UK; National Institute of Public Health (R.K.), Prague, Czech Republic; Department of Epidemiology and Population Studies (A. Pajak), Jagellonian University Collegium Medicum, Krakow, Poland; Institute of Internal and Preventive Medicine Siberian Branch under the Russian Academy of Medical Sciences (S.M., S.S.), Novosibirsk; Novosibirsk State Medical University (S.M.), Novosibirsk, Russia; and INSERM (A.S.-M.), U1018, Hopital Paul Brousse, Villejuif, France
| | - Ruzena Kubinova
- From the Department of Epidemiology & Public Health (P.H., H.P., A. Peasey, M.G.M., A.S.-M., M.B.), and MRC Unit for Lifelong Health and Ageing at UCL (M.R.), University College London, UK; National Institute of Public Health (R.K.), Prague, Czech Republic; Department of Epidemiology and Population Studies (A. Pajak), Jagellonian University Collegium Medicum, Krakow, Poland; Institute of Internal and Preventive Medicine Siberian Branch under the Russian Academy of Medical Sciences (S.M., S.S.), Novosibirsk; Novosibirsk State Medical University (S.M.), Novosibirsk, Russia; and INSERM (A.S.-M.), U1018, Hopital Paul Brousse, Villejuif, France
| | - Andrzej Pajak
- From the Department of Epidemiology & Public Health (P.H., H.P., A. Peasey, M.G.M., A.S.-M., M.B.), and MRC Unit for Lifelong Health and Ageing at UCL (M.R.), University College London, UK; National Institute of Public Health (R.K.), Prague, Czech Republic; Department of Epidemiology and Population Studies (A. Pajak), Jagellonian University Collegium Medicum, Krakow, Poland; Institute of Internal and Preventive Medicine Siberian Branch under the Russian Academy of Medical Sciences (S.M., S.S.), Novosibirsk; Novosibirsk State Medical University (S.M.), Novosibirsk, Russia; and INSERM (A.S.-M.), U1018, Hopital Paul Brousse, Villejuif, France
| | - Sofia Malyutina
- From the Department of Epidemiology & Public Health (P.H., H.P., A. Peasey, M.G.M., A.S.-M., M.B.), and MRC Unit for Lifelong Health and Ageing at UCL (M.R.), University College London, UK; National Institute of Public Health (R.K.), Prague, Czech Republic; Department of Epidemiology and Population Studies (A. Pajak), Jagellonian University Collegium Medicum, Krakow, Poland; Institute of Internal and Preventive Medicine Siberian Branch under the Russian Academy of Medical Sciences (S.M., S.S.), Novosibirsk; Novosibirsk State Medical University (S.M.), Novosibirsk, Russia; and INSERM (A.S.-M.), U1018, Hopital Paul Brousse, Villejuif, France
| | - Sergey Shishkin
- From the Department of Epidemiology & Public Health (P.H., H.P., A. Peasey, M.G.M., A.S.-M., M.B.), and MRC Unit for Lifelong Health and Ageing at UCL (M.R.), University College London, UK; National Institute of Public Health (R.K.), Prague, Czech Republic; Department of Epidemiology and Population Studies (A. Pajak), Jagellonian University Collegium Medicum, Krakow, Poland; Institute of Internal and Preventive Medicine Siberian Branch under the Russian Academy of Medical Sciences (S.M., S.S.), Novosibirsk; Novosibirsk State Medical University (S.M.), Novosibirsk, Russia; and INSERM (A.S.-M.), U1018, Hopital Paul Brousse, Villejuif, France
| | - Hynek Pikhart
- From the Department of Epidemiology & Public Health (P.H., H.P., A. Peasey, M.G.M., A.S.-M., M.B.), and MRC Unit for Lifelong Health and Ageing at UCL (M.R.), University College London, UK; National Institute of Public Health (R.K.), Prague, Czech Republic; Department of Epidemiology and Population Studies (A. Pajak), Jagellonian University Collegium Medicum, Krakow, Poland; Institute of Internal and Preventive Medicine Siberian Branch under the Russian Academy of Medical Sciences (S.M., S.S.), Novosibirsk; Novosibirsk State Medical University (S.M.), Novosibirsk, Russia; and INSERM (A.S.-M.), U1018, Hopital Paul Brousse, Villejuif, France
| | - Anne Peasey
- From the Department of Epidemiology & Public Health (P.H., H.P., A. Peasey, M.G.M., A.S.-M., M.B.), and MRC Unit for Lifelong Health and Ageing at UCL (M.R.), University College London, UK; National Institute of Public Health (R.K.), Prague, Czech Republic; Department of Epidemiology and Population Studies (A. Pajak), Jagellonian University Collegium Medicum, Krakow, Poland; Institute of Internal and Preventive Medicine Siberian Branch under the Russian Academy of Medical Sciences (S.M., S.S.), Novosibirsk; Novosibirsk State Medical University (S.M.), Novosibirsk, Russia; and INSERM (A.S.-M.), U1018, Hopital Paul Brousse, Villejuif, France
| | - M G Marmot
- From the Department of Epidemiology & Public Health (P.H., H.P., A. Peasey, M.G.M., A.S.-M., M.B.), and MRC Unit for Lifelong Health and Ageing at UCL (M.R.), University College London, UK; National Institute of Public Health (R.K.), Prague, Czech Republic; Department of Epidemiology and Population Studies (A. Pajak), Jagellonian University Collegium Medicum, Krakow, Poland; Institute of Internal and Preventive Medicine Siberian Branch under the Russian Academy of Medical Sciences (S.M., S.S.), Novosibirsk; Novosibirsk State Medical University (S.M.), Novosibirsk, Russia; and INSERM (A.S.-M.), U1018, Hopital Paul Brousse, Villejuif, France
| | - Archana Singh-Manoux
- From the Department of Epidemiology & Public Health (P.H., H.P., A. Peasey, M.G.M., A.S.-M., M.B.), and MRC Unit for Lifelong Health and Ageing at UCL (M.R.), University College London, UK; National Institute of Public Health (R.K.), Prague, Czech Republic; Department of Epidemiology and Population Studies (A. Pajak), Jagellonian University Collegium Medicum, Krakow, Poland; Institute of Internal and Preventive Medicine Siberian Branch under the Russian Academy of Medical Sciences (S.M., S.S.), Novosibirsk; Novosibirsk State Medical University (S.M.), Novosibirsk, Russia; and INSERM (A.S.-M.), U1018, Hopital Paul Brousse, Villejuif, France
| | - Martin Bobak
- From the Department of Epidemiology & Public Health (P.H., H.P., A. Peasey, M.G.M., A.S.-M., M.B.), and MRC Unit for Lifelong Health and Ageing at UCL (M.R.), University College London, UK; National Institute of Public Health (R.K.), Prague, Czech Republic; Department of Epidemiology and Population Studies (A. Pajak), Jagellonian University Collegium Medicum, Krakow, Poland; Institute of Internal and Preventive Medicine Siberian Branch under the Russian Academy of Medical Sciences (S.M., S.S.), Novosibirsk; Novosibirsk State Medical University (S.M.), Novosibirsk, Russia; and INSERM (A.S.-M.), U1018, Hopital Paul Brousse, Villejuif, France
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Schneider BC, Gross AL, Bangen KJ, Skinner JC, Benitez A, Glymour MM, Sachs BC, Shih RA, Sisco S, Manly JJ, Luchsinger JA. Association of vascular risk factors with cognition in a multiethnic sample. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2014; 70:532-44. [PMID: 24821298 DOI: 10.1093/geronb/gbu040] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/02/2013] [Accepted: 03/24/2014] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVES To examine the relationship between cardiovascular risk factors (CVRFs) and cognitive performance in a multiethnic sample of older adults. METHOD We used longitudinal data from the Washington Heights-Inwood Columbia Aging Project. A composite score including smoking, stroke, heart disease, diabetes, hypertension, and central obesity represented CVRFs. Multiple group parallel process multivariate random effects regression models were used to model cognitive functioning and examine the contribution of CVRFs to baseline performance and change in general cognitive processing, memory, and executive functioning. RESULTS Presence of each CVRF was associated with a 0.1 SD lower score in general cognitive processing, memory, and executive functioning in black and Hispanic participants relative to whites. Baseline CVRFs were associated with poorer baseline cognitive performances among black women and Hispanic men. CVRF increase was related to baseline cognitive performance only among Hispanics. CVRFs were not related to cognitive decline. After adjustment for medications, CVRFs were not associated with cognition in Hispanic participants. DISCUSSION CVRFs are associated with poorer cognitive functioning, but not cognitive decline, among minority older adults. These relationships vary by gender and medication use. Consideration of unique racial, ethnic, and cultural factors is needed when examining relationships between CVRFs and cognition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brooke C Schneider
- Psychology Service, VA Greater Los Angeles Healthcare System, California
| | - Alden L Gross
- Department of Epidemiology, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, Maryland. Center on Aging and Health, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland
| | | | - Jeannine C Skinner
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Washington School of Medicine, Seattle
| | - Andreana Benitez
- Department of Radiology and Radiological Sciences, Center for Biomedical Imaging, Medical University of South Carolina, Charleston
| | - M Maria Glymour
- Department of Society, Human Development, and Health, Harvard School of Public Health, Boston, Massachusetts. Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, University of California, San Francisco
| | - Bonnie C Sachs
- Department of Neurology, Virginia Commonwealth University School of Medicine, Richmond
| | | | - Shannon Sisco
- North Florida/South Georgia Veterans Health System, Department of Veterans Affairs, Gainesville
| | - Jennifer J Manly
- Cognitive Neuroscience Division, Department of Neurology, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York, New York
| | - José A Luchsinger
- Department of Epidemiology, Joseph P. Mailman School of Public Health, and Department of Medicine, Columbia University Medical Center, New York.
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Retrospective lifetime dietary patterns predict cognitive performance in community-dwelling older Australians. Br J Nutr 2014; 112:228-37. [PMID: 24709049 DOI: 10.1017/s0007114514000646] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/18/2022]
Abstract
Dietary intake is a modifiable exposure that may have an impact on cognitive outcomes in older age. The long-term aetiology of cognitive decline and dementia, however, suggests that the relevance of dietary intake extends across the lifetime. In the present study, we tested whether retrospective dietary patterns from the life periods of childhood, early adulthood, adulthood and middle age predicted cognitive performance in a cognitively healthy sample of 352 older Australian adults >65 years. Participants completed the Lifetime Diet Questionnaire and a battery of cognitive tests designed to comprehensively assess multiple cognitive domains. In separate regression models, lifetime dietary patterns were the predictors of cognitive factor scores representing ten constructs derived by confirmatory factor analysis of the cognitive test battery. All regression models were progressively adjusted for the potential confounders of current diet, age, sex, years of education, English as native language, smoking history, income level, apoE ɛ4 status, physical activity, other past dietary patterns and health-related variables. In the adjusted models, lifetime dietary patterns predicted cognitive performance in this sample of older adults. In models additionally adjusted for intake from the other life periods and mechanistic health-related variables, dietary patterns from the childhood period alone reached significance. Higher consumption of the 'coffee and high-sugar, high-fat extras' pattern predicted poorer performance on simple/choice reaction time, working memory, retrieval fluency, short-term memory and reasoning. The 'vegetable and non-processed' pattern negatively predicted simple/choice reaction time, and the 'traditional Australian' pattern positively predicted perceptual speed and retrieval fluency. Identifying early-life dietary antecedents of older-age cognitive performance contributes to formulating strategies for delaying or preventing cognitive decline.
Collapse
|
29
|
Guidetti G. The role of cognitive processes in vestibular disorders. HEARING, BALANCE AND COMMUNICATION 2013. [DOI: 10.3109/21695717.2013.765085] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/15/2022]
|
30
|
Cahana-Amitay D, Albert ML, Ojo EA, Sayers J, Goral M, Obler LK, Spiro A. Effects of hypertension and diabetes on sentence comprehension in aging. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2012; 68:513-21. [PMID: 23052364 DOI: 10.1093/geronb/gbs085] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/20/2023] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVES To assess the impact of hypertension and diabetes mellitus on sentence comprehension in older adults. METHOD Two hundred and ninety-five adults aged 55 to 84 (52% men) participated in this study. Self-report mail survey combined with medical evaluations were used to determine eligibility. Multiple sources were used to determine whether hypertension and diabetes were present or absent and controlled or uncontrolled. Sentence comprehension was evaluated with two tasks: embedded sentences (ES) and sentences with multiple negatives (MN). Outcome measures were percent accuracy and mean reaction time of correct responses on each task. RESULTS Regression models adjusted for age, gender, and education showed that the presence of hypertension impaired comprehension on the multiple negatives task (p < .01), whereas the presence of diabetes impaired the comprehension of embedded sentences (p < .05). Uncontrolled diabetes significantly impaired accurate comprehension of sentences with multiple negatives (p < .05). No significant patterns were found for reaction time. DISCUSSION The presence of hypertension and diabetes adversely affected sentence comprehension, but the relative contribution of each was different. These findings support the researchers' earlier speculations on the neurobiological mechanisms underlying the effects of hypertension and diabetes on language and cognition in aging. Uncontrolled disease status demonstrated more complicated age-related effects on sentence processing, highlighting the clinical importance for cognitive aging of identifying and managing vascular risk factors.
Collapse
|
31
|
Kremen WS, Lachman ME, Pruessner JC, Sliwinski M, Wilson RS. Mechanisms of age-related cognitive change and targets for intervention: social interactions and stress. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2012; 67:760-5. [PMID: 22570134 DOI: 10.1093/gerona/gls125] [Citation(s) in RCA: 49] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/13/2023] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND The effects of biological and physical factors on cognitive aging are widely studied. Less is known about the role of psychosocial factors such as stress and social relationships for cognitive functioning. METHODS Speakers in Session IV of the Summit focused on possible mechanisms linking social interactions and stressful experiences to cognitive changes with aging. RESULTS Elevated cortisol, repetitive thinking, negative emotions, neuroticism, chronic stress, and early life adversity were negatively associated with memory and other cognitive dimensions in later life. In contrast, supportive social relationships were found to be positively related to cognitive functioning. Some evidence was provided for multidirectional, causal relationships involving stress and negative affect as both antecedents and consequences of cognitive decline. CONCLUSIONS The findings contribute to understanding the potential underlying causal processes linking psychosocial factors and cognitive aging with a developmental focus on the etiology of declines and onset of cognitive impairments. This work provides an important foundation for future research to identify modifiable factors and to design interventions to minimize cognitive declines and optimize cognitive health in adulthood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- William S Kremen
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|