1
|
Osvath M, Johansson M. A short natural history of mental time travels: a journey still travelled? Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2024; 379:20230402. [PMID: 39278257 PMCID: PMC11496716 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2023.0402] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/22/2024] [Revised: 06/28/2024] [Accepted: 07/09/2024] [Indexed: 09/18/2024] Open
Abstract
Tulving's introduction of episodic memory and the metaphor of mental time travel has immensely enriched our understanding of human cognition. However, his focus on human psychology, with limited consideration of evolutionary perspectives, led to the entrenched notion that mental time travel is uniquely human. We contend that adopting a phylogenetic perspective offers a deeper insight into cognition, revealing it as a continuous evolutionary process. Adherence to the uniqueness of pre-defined psychological concepts obstructs a more complete understanding. We offer a concise natural history to elucidate how events that occurred hundreds of millions of years ago have been pivotal for our ability to mentally time travel. We discuss how the human brain, utilizing parts with ancient origins in a networked manner, enables mental time travel. This underscores that episodic memories and mental time travel are not isolated mental constructs but integral to our perception and representation of the world. We conclude by examining recent evidence of neuroanatomical correlates found only in great apes, which show great variability, indicating the ongoing evolution of mental time travel in humans.This article is part of the theme issue 'Elements of episodic memory: lessons from 40 years of research'.
Collapse
|
2
|
Schreiber F, Schneider S, Newen A, Voigt B. Embodying anticipated affect enhances proactive behavior in 5-year-old children. J Exp Child Psychol 2024; 249:106099. [PMID: 39368238 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106099] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/29/2024] [Revised: 09/10/2024] [Accepted: 09/10/2024] [Indexed: 10/07/2024]
Abstract
Imagining anticipated affects can foster future-oriented behavior in adults. However, children often still have difficulties in vividly imagining how they will feel in a specific episode (affective episodic future thinking [EFT]). We investigated whether enacting anticipated affects helps children to imagine how they will feel and whether this enhances proactive behavior in turn. A total of 90 5-year-old children were randomly assigned to one of three groups. In the embodiment group, children were instructed to imagine and physically enact how positive and negative they would feel in an upcoming performance test. Children in the EFT-only group underwent a similar procedure but did not enact their future affect. In the control group, children were reminded of the upcoming test only without receiving a prompt to imagine the upcoming test. After the manipulation, children had the opportunity to play one of three games. One game was relevant for the test. Children's choice to play the relevant game in advance of the test served as an indicator for proactive behavior. Mechanisms (e.g., detailedness of the envisioned event) and moderators (theory of mind and neuroticism) of the link between embodied EFT and proactive behavior were explored. Children in the embodiment group chose the relevant game above chance level, but they did not choose the relevant game more often than children in the EFT-only group and the control group. Those results were independent of the assumed mediators and moderators.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Felix Schreiber
- Mental Health Research and Treatment Center, Department of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, Ruhr Universität Bochum, 44789 Bochum, Germany.
| | - Silvia Schneider
- Mental Health Research and Treatment Center, Department of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, Ruhr Universität Bochum, 44789 Bochum, Germany; German Center for Mental Health (DZPG), Bochum-Marburg partner site, 44787 Bochum, Germany
| | - Albert Newen
- Institute of Philosophy II, Ruhr Universität Bochum, 44789 Bochum, Germany
| | - Babett Voigt
- Mental Health Research and Treatment Center, Department of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, Ruhr Universität Bochum, 44789 Bochum, Germany; German Center for Mental Health (DZPG), Bochum-Marburg partner site, 44787 Bochum, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Atance CM, Ayson G, Martin-Ordas G. Moving beyond "Spoon" tasks: When do children autocue their episodic future thought? WILEY INTERDISCIPLINARY REVIEWS. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 2023; 14:e1646. [PMID: 37440219 DOI: 10.1002/wcs.1646] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/30/2022] [Revised: 02/10/2023] [Accepted: 03/07/2023] [Indexed: 07/14/2023]
Abstract
Much developmental (and comparative) research has used Tulving's Spoon test (i.e., whether an individual will select an item needed to solve a future problem) as the basis for designing tasks to measure episodic future thinking, defined as the capacity to mentally pre-experience the future. There is, however, intense debate about whether these tasks successfully do so. Most notably, it has been argued that children may pass (i.e., select an item with future utility) by drawing on non-episodic, associative processes, rather than on the capacity to represent the future, per se. Although subsequent developmental tasks have sought to address this limitation, we highlight what we argue is a more fundamental shortcoming of Spoon tasks: they prompt future-directed action making it impossible to determine whether children have used their episodic future thinking to guide their behavior. Accordingly, we know little about children's thought about the future that is independently generated (i.e., without prompting), or autocued, and is subsequently reflected (and measurable) by children's actions. We argue that this capacity is a critical, and heretofore overlooked, transition in future-oriented cognition that may not occur until middle childhood. We further hypothesize that it is reliant on children developing richer and more detailed future event representations, along with the necessary cognitive control to transform these representations into actions that serve to benefit their future selves. The time is ripe for researchers to explore this aspect of cognitive development and we suggest several novel approaches to do so. This article is categorized under: Cognitive Biology > Cognitive Development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Gladys Ayson
- School of Psychology, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Canada
| | - Gema Martin-Ordas
- Division of Psychology, University of Stirling, Stirling, UK
- Department of Psychology, University of Oviedo, Asturias, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Horner K, Coundouris SP, Terrett G, Rendell PG, Henry JD. Self-initiating and applying episodic foresight in middle childhood. J Exp Child Psychol 2023; 233:105696. [PMID: 37167847 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2023.105696] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/05/2022] [Revised: 04/13/2023] [Accepted: 04/14/2023] [Indexed: 05/13/2023]
Abstract
This study provides an important extension to the growing literature on prospection in children by providing the first test of whether one's ability to engage in the functional (as opposed to the purely phenomenological) aspect of episodic foresight improves across middle childhood. Of the various forms of prospection, episodic foresight has been proposed to be one of the most flexible and functionally powerful, defined as the ability to not only imagine future events (simulative aspect) but also use those imaginings to guide behavior in the present (functional aspect). The current study tested 80 typically developing children aged 8 to 12 years using an extensive cognitive battery comprising Virtual Week Foresight, the Autobiographical Interview, and a series of crystallized and fluid intelligence measures. Whereas data indicated age-related improvements in detecting future-oriented problems and taking steps in the present in service of solving these, all children in this age bracket demonstrated a similar capacity for problem resolution (i.e., the ability to subsequently solve successfully identified problems). Results also revealed the importance of broader crystallized and fluid intelligence, but not episodic memory or episodic future thinking, in engaging in this capacity. Research is now required to understand the real-life consequences of episodic foresight during this developmental period as well as the ways in which parents and teachers can help to foster this capacity and consequently help to support children's growing desire for independence during this time.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katherine Horner
- School of Behavioural and Health Sciences, Faculty of Health Sciences, Australian Catholic University, Melbourne, Australia
| | - Sarah P Coundouris
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, St Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia.
| | - Gill Terrett
- School of Behavioural and Health Sciences, Faculty of Health Sciences, Australian Catholic University, Melbourne, Australia
| | - Peter G Rendell
- School of Behavioural and Health Sciences, Faculty of Health Sciences, Australian Catholic University, Melbourne, Australia; School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, St Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia
| | - Julie D Henry
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, St Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Tsui ASM, Atance CM. Young Children’s Saving and Their Episodic Future Thinking. JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 2022. [DOI: 10.1080/15248372.2022.2156516] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/23/2022]
|
6
|
Svane RP, Jensen TS, Hjuler TF, Sonne T, Kingo OS, Krøjgaard P. Episodic future thinking in 35-, 47-, and 55-month-old children. NORDIC PSYCHOLOGY 2021. [DOI: 10.1080/19012276.2021.1873829] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Riikka P. Svane
- Center on Autobiographical Memory Research, Department of Psychology and the Behavioral Sciences, Aarhus University, Aarhus C, Denmark
| | - Toril S. Jensen
- Center on Autobiographical Memory Research, Department of Psychology and the Behavioral Sciences, Aarhus University, Aarhus C, Denmark
| | - Tirill F. Hjuler
- Center on Autobiographical Memory Research, Department of Psychology and the Behavioral Sciences, Aarhus University, Aarhus C, Denmark
| | - Trine Sonne
- Center on Autobiographical Memory Research, Department of Psychology and the Behavioral Sciences, Aarhus University, Aarhus C, Denmark
| | - Osman S. Kingo
- Center on Autobiographical Memory Research, Department of Psychology and the Behavioral Sciences, Aarhus University, Aarhus C, Denmark
| | - Peter Krøjgaard
- Center on Autobiographical Memory Research, Department of Psychology and the Behavioral Sciences, Aarhus University, Aarhus C, Denmark
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Suddendorf T, Kirkland K, Bulley A, Redshaw J, Langley MC. It's in the bag: mobile containers in human evolution and child development. EVOLUTIONARY HUMAN SCIENCES 2020; 2:e48. [PMID: 37588341 PMCID: PMC10427442 DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2020.47] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022] Open
Abstract
Mobile containers are a keystone human innovation. Ethnographic data indicate that all human groups use containers such as bags, quivers and baskets, ensuring that individuals have important resources at the ready and are prepared for opportunities and threats before they materialize. Although there is speculation surrounding the invention of carrying devices, the current hard archaeological evidence only reaches back some 100,000 years. The dearth of ancient evidence may reflect not only taphonomic processes, but also a lack of attention to these devices. To begin investigating the origins of carrying devices we focus on exploring the basic cognitive processes involved in mobile container use and report an initial study on young children's understanding and deployment of such devices. We gave 3- to 7-year-old children (N = 106) the opportunity to spontaneously identify and use a basket to increase their own carrying capacity and thereby obtain more resources in the future. Performance improved linearly with age, as did the likelihood of recognizing that adults use mobile carrying devices to increase carrying capacity. We argue that the evolutionary and developmental origins of mobile containers reflect foundational cognitive processes that enable humans to think about their own limits and compensate for them.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas Suddendorf
- Centre for Psychology and Evolution, School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia4072, Australia
| | - Kelly Kirkland
- Centre for Psychology and Evolution, School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia4072, Australia
| | - Adam Bulley
- Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA02138, USA
- The University of Sydney, School of Psychology and Brain and Mind Centre, NSW2050, Australia
| | - Jonathan Redshaw
- Centre for Psychology and Evolution, School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia4072, Australia
| | - Michelle C. Langley
- Australian Research Centre for Human Evolution, Environmental Futures Research Institute, Griffith University, Nathan4111, Australia
- Forensics and Archaeology, School of Environment and Science, Griffith University, Nathan4111, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Langley MC, Suddendorf T. Mobile containers in human cognitive evolution studies: Understudied and underrepresented. Evol Anthropol 2020; 29:299-309. [DOI: 10.1002/evan.21857] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/14/2019] [Revised: 01/05/2020] [Accepted: 06/29/2020] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Michelle C. Langley
- Australian Research Centre for Human Evolution Environmental Futures Research Institute, Griffith University Brisbane Australia
| | - Thomas Suddendorf
- Centre for Psychology & Evolution, Early Cognitive Development Centre School of Psychology, University of Queensland Brisbane Australia
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Martin-Ordas G. It is about time: Conceptual and experimental evaluation of the temporal cognitive mechanisms in mental time travel. WILEY INTERDISCIPLINARY REVIEWS. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 2020; 11:e1530. [PMID: 32338829 DOI: 10.1002/wcs.1530] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/03/2020] [Revised: 03/20/2020] [Accepted: 04/04/2020] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
Mental time travel (MTT) is the ability that allows humans to mentally project themselves backwards in time to remember past events (i.e., episodic memory) or forwards in time to imagine future events (i.e., future thinking). Despite empirical evidence showing that animals might possess MTT abilities, some still claim that this ability is uniquely human. Recent debates have suggested that it is the temporal cognitive mechanism (i.e., ability to represent the sense of past and future) that makes MTT uniquely human. Advances in the field have been constrained by a lack of comparative data, methodological shortcomings that prevent meaningful comparisons, and a lack of clear conceptualizations of the temporal cognitive mechanism. Here I will present a comprehensive review into MTT in humans and animals-with a particular focus on great apes. I will examine three of the most prominent and influential theoretical models of human MTT. Drawing on these accounts, I suggest that a basic way of understanding time might be shared across species, however culture and language will play a critical role at shaping the way we elaborate mental representations about past and future events. This article is categorized under: Cognitive Biology > Evolutionary Roots of Cognition Psychology > Comparative Psychology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gema Martin-Ordas
- Division of Psychology, University of Stirling, Stirling, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Martin-Ordas G. What Human Planning Can Tell Us About Animal Planning: An Empirical Case. Front Psychol 2020; 11:635. [PMID: 32308638 PMCID: PMC7145970 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00635] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/10/2020] [Accepted: 03/17/2020] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
The ability to think about and plan for the future is a critical cognitive skill for our daily life. There is ongoing debate about whether other animals possess future thinking. Part of the difficulty in resolving this debate is that there is not a definite methodology that allow us to conclude that animals (and human children) are truly thinking about a future event. Research with humans-both children and adults- will benefit the field of comparative psychology by providing information about the range of humans' responses when they are faced with problems similar to those presented to other animals. Inspired by a problem that chimpanzees experienced in the wild, children of 4 and 5 years of age and young adults were presented with a situation in which they were expected to select two tools in order to obtain a reward. More older children than 4 years old successfully obtained the reward. Adults also succeeded at solving the problem. However, both children and adults struggled to select the two correct tools before any tool-use action was executed. While children's performance is discussed in the context of temporal components required to envisage future events, adults' performance is interpreted in the context of cognitive effort. These findings link developmental and adult cognition with comparative psychology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gema Martin-Ordas
- Department of Psychology, University of Stirling, Stirling, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Miller R, Frohnwieser A, Ding N, Troisi CA, Schiestl M, Gruber R, Taylor AH, Jelbert SA, Boeckle M, Clayton NS. A novel test of flexible planning in relation to executive function and language in young children. ROYAL SOCIETY OPEN SCIENCE 2020; 7:192015. [PMID: 32431882 PMCID: PMC7211888 DOI: 10.1098/rsos.192015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/18/2019] [Accepted: 03/10/2020] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
In adult humans, decisions involving the choice and use of tools for future events typically require episodic foresight. Previous studies suggest some non-human species are capable of future planning; however, these experiments often cannot fully exclude alternative learning explanations. Here, we used a novel tool-use paradigm aiming to address these critiques to test flexible planning in 3- to 5-year-old children, in relation to executive function and language abilities. In the flexible planning task, children were not verbally cued during testing, single trials avoided consistent exposure to stimulus-reward relationships, and training trials provided experience of a predictable return of reward. Furthermore, unlike most standard developmental studies, we incorporated short delays before and after tool choice. The critical test choice included two tools with equal prior reward experience-each only functional in one apparatus. We tested executive function and language abilities using several standardized tasks. Our results echoed standard developmental research: 4- and 5-year-olds outperformed 3-year-olds on the flexible planning task, and 5-year-old children outperformed younger children in most executive function and language tasks. Flexible planning performance did not correlate with executive function and language performance. This paradigm could be used to investigate flexible planning in a tool-use context in non-human species.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rachael Miller
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Anna Frohnwieser
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Ning Ding
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Camille A. Troisi
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
- School of Biological, Earth and Environmental Sciences, University College Cork, Cork, Ireland
| | - Martina Schiestl
- School of Psychology, Auckland University, Auckland, New Zealand
| | - Romana Gruber
- School of Psychology, Auckland University, Auckland, New Zealand
| | - Alex H. Taylor
- School of Psychology, Auckland University, Auckland, New Zealand
| | - Sarah A. Jelbert
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
- School of Psychological Science, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
| | - Markus Boeckle
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
- Department of Psychology and Psychodynamics, Karl Landsteiner University of Health Sciences, St Pölten, Austria
| | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
Abstract
Children's future-oriented cognition has become a well-established area of research over the last decade. Future-oriented cognition encompasses a range of processes, including those involved in conceiving the future, imagining and preparing for future events, and making decisions that will affect how the future unfolds. We consider recent empirical advances in the study of such processes by outlining key findings that have yielded a clearer picture of how future thinking emerges and changes over childhood. Our interest in future thinking stems from a broader interest in temporal cognition, and we argue that a consideration of developmental changes in how children understand and represent time itself provides a valuable framework in which to study future-oriented cognition.
Collapse
|
13
|
Gautam S, Suddendorf T, Henry JD, Redshaw J. A taxonomy of mental time travel and counterfactual thought: Insights from cognitive development. Behav Brain Res 2019; 374:112108. [DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112108] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/01/2019] [Revised: 07/15/2019] [Accepted: 07/20/2019] [Indexed: 01/25/2023]
|
14
|
Atance CM, Celebi SN, Mitchinson S, Mahy CEV. Thinking about the future: Comparing children's forced-choice versus "generative" responses in the "spoon test". J Exp Child Psychol 2019; 181:1-16. [PMID: 30690296 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.12.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/08/2018] [Revised: 12/13/2018] [Accepted: 12/15/2018] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
One of the most popular methods to assess children's foresight is to present children with a problem (e.g., locked box with no key) in one room and then later, in another room, give them the opportunity to select the item (e.g., key) that will solve it. Whether or not children choose the correct item to bring back to the first room is the dependent measure of interest in this "spoon test." Although children as young as 3 or 4 years typically succeed on this test, whether they would pass a more stringent version in which they must verbally generate (vs. select) the correct item in the absence of any cues is unknown. This is an important point given that humans must often make decisions about the future without being explicitly "prompted" by the future-oriented option. In Experiment 1, using an adapted version of the spoon test, we show that as the "generative" requirements of the task increase, 3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds' (N = 99) performance significantly decreases. We replicate this effect in Experiment 2 (N = 48 3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds) and also provide preliminary evidence that the capacity to verbally generate the correct item in a spoon test may draw more heavily on children's category fluency skills than does their capacity to select this item among a set of distracters. Our findings underscore the importance of examining more generative forms of future thought in young children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cristina M Atance
- School of Psychology, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Ontario K1N 6N5, Canada.
| | - Seyda Nur Celebi
- School of Psychology, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Ontario K1N 6N5, Canada
| | - Sarah Mitchinson
- Department of Psychology, Wilfrid Laurier University, Waterloo, Ontario N2L 3C5, Canada
| | - Caitlin E V Mahy
- Department of Psychology, Brock University, St. Catharines, Ontario L2S 3A1, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Terrett G, Horner K, White R, Henry JD, Kliegel M, Labuschagne I, Rendell PG. The relationship between episodic future thinking and prospective memory in middle childhood: Mechanisms depend on task type. J Exp Child Psychol 2018; 178:198-213. [PMID: 30388484 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.10.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/04/2018] [Revised: 10/09/2018] [Accepted: 10/09/2018] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Episodic future thinking (EFT), the ability to imagine experiencing a future event, and prospective memory (PM), the ability to remember and carry out a planned action, are core aspects of future-oriented cognition that have individually been the focus of research attention in the developmental literature. However, the relationship between EFT and PM, including the extent to which it varies with PM task type, remains poorly delineated, particularly in middle childhood. The current study tested this relationship in 62 typically developing children aged 8-12 years. Results indicated that EFT ability was significantly related to performance on three types of PM tasks (regular and irregular event based and regular time based). Age was not found to moderate the relationship. Children's performance on the retrospective memory component of the PM tasks mediated the relationship between EFT ability and their performance on three types of PM tasks. For irregular event-based tasks, however, EFT made an additional significant contribution. This study adds to the limited empirical literature supporting a relationship between EFT and PM in this age band and supports theoretical models arguing that EFT ability may support PM performance by strengthening the encoding of PM task details in retrospective memory. However, additional mechanisms were also indicated for irregular event-based PM tasks, possibly involving strengthening of cue-context associations. These data show for the first time that the contribution of EFT to children's PM performance varies across task types. This study provides an important and novel contribution to current understanding of the processes that underlie PM development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gill Terrett
- Cognition and Emotion Research Centre, School of Psychology, Australian Catholic University, Melbourne Campus, Fitzroy, VIC 3065, Australia.
| | - Katherine Horner
- Cognition and Emotion Research Centre, School of Psychology, Australian Catholic University, Melbourne Campus, Fitzroy, VIC 3065, Australia
| | - Roxanne White
- Cognition and Emotion Research Centre, School of Psychology, Australian Catholic University, Melbourne Campus, Fitzroy, VIC 3065, Australia
| | - Julie D Henry
- School of Psychology, University of Queensland, St. Lucia, QLD 4072, Australia
| | - Matthias Kliegel
- Department of Psychology, University of Geneva, CH-1211 Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Izelle Labuschagne
- Cognition and Emotion Research Centre, School of Psychology, Australian Catholic University, Melbourne Campus, Fitzroy, VIC 3065, Australia
| | - Peter G Rendell
- Cognition and Emotion Research Centre, School of Psychology, Australian Catholic University, Melbourne Campus, Fitzroy, VIC 3065, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Caza JS, Atance CM. Children's behavior and spontaneous talk in a future thinking task. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2018; 83:761-773. [PMID: 30187115 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-018-1089-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/26/2018] [Accepted: 08/31/2018] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
Abstract
We explored 3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds' (N = 120) "explicit" and "spontaneous" future-oriented cognition. Specifically, children had to think ahead to meet a future physiological need (desire for food) or psychological need (avoiding boredom). One group of children alternated between a room with candy and a room without candy, spending 3 min per visit. Children were explicitly asked which room they wanted to put extra candy in for a future visit to the lab (correct answer: room without candy). A second group of children underwent the same procedure but with toys as the resource instead of food (a replication of Atance et al. in J Exp Child Psychol 129:98-109, 2015). In the food condition, 3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds all placed candy in the correct room above chance, but only 4- and 5-year-olds were above chance in the toy condition. Overall, 4- and 5-year-olds outperformed 3-year-olds, and children performed better in the food condition than the toy condition. Children's spontaneous (or "involuntary") future thinking was assessed by coding their utterances while in the two rooms. Children who solved the explicit task uttered more task-relevant future and past statements than children who failed. Examining spontaneous talk also allowed us to explore children's spontaneous "solving" of the task before being asked an explicit test question. This research highlights the importance of varying stimuli in future thinking tasks and developing methods to capture spontaneous/involuntary future thinking in young children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julian S Caza
- School of Psychology, University of Ottawa, Vanier Hall, Room 6006, Ottawa, ON, K1N 6N5, Canada.
| | - Cristina M Atance
- School of Psychology, University of Ottawa, Vanier Hall, Room 6006, Ottawa, ON, K1N 6N5, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Redshaw J, Leamy T, Pincus P, Suddendorf T. Young children's capacity to imagine and prepare for certain and uncertain future outcomes. PLoS One 2018; 13:e0202606. [PMID: 30180188 PMCID: PMC6122780 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0202606] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/31/2018] [Accepted: 08/05/2018] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
The current study used a minimalist paradigm to examine young children's capacity to imagine and prepare for certain and uncertain immediate future outcomes. In a counterbalanced order, 2.5-year-old children (N = 32) completed twelve trials each of two tasks: (1) the forked tube task, which assessed their ability to cover two possible tube exits to ensure they would catch a single target with an uncertain future trajectory, and (2) the double tube task, which assessed their ability to cover two separate tube exits to ensure they would catch two targets with certain future trajectories. Even though the optimal preparatory action was the same across both tasks, children were much more likely to spontaneously and consistently demonstrate this action in the double tube task than the forked tube task. Children's responses were unaffected by the number of targets seen in the demonstration phase, and instead appeared to be based on the particular contingencies of each apparatus. These results are consistent with the possibility that young children specifically struggle to imagine and prepare for mutually exclusive versions of uncertain future events.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jonathan Redshaw
- School of Psychology, University of Queensland, St Lucia, Queensland, Australia
- * E-mail:
| | - Talia Leamy
- School of Psychology, University of Queensland, St Lucia, Queensland, Australia
| | - Phoebe Pincus
- School of Psychology, University of Queensland, St Lucia, Queensland, Australia
| | - Thomas Suddendorf
- School of Psychology, University of Queensland, St Lucia, Queensland, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Dickerson KL, Ainge JA, Seed AM. The Role of Association in Pre-schoolers’ Solutions to “Spoon Tests” of Future Planning. Curr Biol 2018; 28:2309-2313.e2. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.05.052] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/09/2018] [Revised: 04/03/2018] [Accepted: 05/17/2018] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
19
|
Martin-Ordas G. “First, I will get the marbles.” Children’s foresight abilities in a modified spoon task. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2018. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.07.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
|
20
|
Affiliation(s)
- Markus Boeckle
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK.
| | - Nicola S Clayton
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Comparing the non-linguistic hallmarks of episodic memory systems in corvids and children. Curr Opin Behav Sci 2017. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.07.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
|
22
|
Scarf D, Boden H, Labuschagne LG, Gross J, Hayne H. "What" and "where" was when? Memory for the temporal order of episodic events in children. Dev Psychobiol 2017; 59:1039-1045. [PMID: 28833042 DOI: 10.1002/dev.21553] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/04/2017] [Accepted: 07/17/2017] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
In the past, researchers have shown that the individual components of episodic memory (i.e "what," "where," and "when") may emerge at different points in development. Specifically, while children as young as three can accurately report the "what" and "where" of an event, they struggle to accurately report when the event occurred. One explanation for children's difficulty in reporting when an event took place is a rudimentary understanding, and ability to use, temporal terms. In the current experiment, we employed a physical timeline to aid children's reporting of the order in which a series of episodic events occurred. Overall, while 4-, 5-, and 6-year olds performed above chance, 3-year olds did not. Our findings suggest that 3-year olds' limited ability to produce temporal terms may not be the rate-limiting step preventing them from identifying when events occurred in their recent past.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Damian Scarf
- Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
| | - Hannah Boden
- Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
| | | | - Julien Gross
- Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
| | - Harlene Hayne
- Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Boden H, Labuschagne LG, Hinten AE, Scarf D. Episodic foresight beyond the very next event in 3- and 4-year-old children. Dev Psychobiol 2017; 59:927-931. [PMID: 28731582 DOI: 10.1002/dev.21544] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/09/2016] [Accepted: 06/12/2017] [Indexed: 11/11/2022]
Abstract
Testing episodic foresight in children generally involves presenting them with a problem in one location (e.g., Room A) and, after a spending a delay in a different location, telling them they will be returning to Room A. Before they go, children are presented with a number of items, one of which will allow them to solve the problem in Room A. At around 3 to 4 years of age children display episodic foresight, selecting the item that will allow them to solve the problem. To date, however, no study has assessed whether 3- and 4-year-old children can plan beyond the very next event, selecting the correct item when there is a delay before returning to Room A. Here, we show that 3- and 4-year-old children can pass when a delay is imposed but that their performance is significantly worse than when they are planning for an immediate event.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hannah Boden
- Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
| | | | - Ashley E Hinten
- Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
| | - Damian Scarf
- Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Martin-Ordas G. 'Will I want these stickers tomorrow?' Preschoolers' ability to predict current and future needs. BRITISH JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2017; 35:568-581. [PMID: 28727162 DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12195] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/29/2016] [Revised: 06/20/2017] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Between 3 and 5 years of age, children develop the ability to plan for their own and others' future needs; however, they have great difficulty predicting future needs that conflict with current ones. Importantly, this ability has only been tested in the domain of physiological states (e.g., thirst). Therefore, it is still an open question whether in a different context preschoolers can disengage from their current needs to secure a different future one. In a Resource Allocation task, 4- and 5-year-olds had to distribute three types of rewards between themselves and another child for either 'right now' or 'tomorrow'. Children's current needs were manipulated by providing them (or not) with their preferred reward at beginning of the task. Only 5-year-olds could predict future needs that conflict with their current ones and act accordingly. Younger children's performance is discussed in the context of temporal and social distance. Statement of contribution What is already known on this subject? By the age of 4, children can plan for their own and others' future needs. Seven-year-old children still have difficulty predicting future physiological needs that conflict with their current ones. What does this study add? In a Resource Allocation task, preschoolers had to share rewards with another child for 'right now' or 'tomorrow'. Children's current needs were manipulated by providing them (or not) with their preferred reward. This study shows that 5-year-olds can predict future (non-physiological) needs that conflict with their current ones.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gema Martin-Ordas
- Centre for Behaviour and Evolution, Newcastle University, UK.,Department of Psychology, University of Stirling, UK
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Kabadayi C, Osvath M. Ravens parallel great apes in flexible planning for tool-use and bartering. Science 2017; 357:202-204. [DOI: 10.1126/science.aam8138] [Citation(s) in RCA: 123] [Impact Index Per Article: 17.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/27/2017] [Accepted: 06/08/2017] [Indexed: 11/02/2022]
|
26
|
Gautam S, Bulley A, von Hippel W, Suddendorf T. Affective forecasting bias in preschool children. J Exp Child Psychol 2017; 159:175-184. [PMID: 28288413 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.02.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/18/2016] [Revised: 02/06/2017] [Accepted: 02/06/2017] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Shalini Gautam
- School of Psychology, University of Queensland, St. Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia.
| | - Adam Bulley
- School of Psychology, University of Queensland, St. Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia
| | - William von Hippel
- School of Psychology, University of Queensland, St. Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia
| | - Thomas Suddendorf
- School of Psychology, University of Queensland, St. Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Suddendorf T. The Emergence of Episodic Foresight and Its Consequences. CHILD DEVELOPMENT PERSPECTIVES 2017. [DOI: 10.1111/cdep.12233] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/04/2023]
|
28
|
Acting with the future in mind is impaired in long-term opiate users. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2017; 234:99-108. [PMID: 27714425 DOI: 10.1007/s00213-016-4442-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/20/2016] [Accepted: 09/16/2016] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
RATIONALE Episodic foresight is a fundamental human capacity. It refers to the ability to simulate future situations and organise current actions accordingly. While there is some evidence that opiate users have a reduced capacity to imagine themselves in future situations, no study to date has assessed whether opiate users show deficits in the ability to take steps in the present in anticipation of future needs. OBJECTIVE In this study, we assessed whether this functional aspect of episodic foresight is impaired in chronic opiate users and the extent to which any deficits are associated with executive dysfunction. METHODS AND RESULTS Participants were 33 long-term opiate users enrolled in an opiate substitution program and 34 controls. Relative to controls, the opiate users displayed significant impairment (medium effect size η 2p = 0.08) in the two behavioural measures of episodic foresight used (items acquired and items used in the VW Foresight task). Furthermore, executive functioning was associated with foresight ability, although this was restricted to items acquired, and the associations were generally stronger for the control group. CONCLUSIONS These data provide important evidence suggesting that the functional aspect of episodic foresight is disrupted in long-term opiate users. While these deficits appear to have some links to impaired executive control, additional work is needed to gain a more complete understanding of the underlying cognitive and neural mechanisms involved. This, in turn, will have important implications for tailoring interventions with opiate users to maximise the likelihood of successful independent functioning.
Collapse
|
29
|
Redshaw J, Henry JD, Suddendorf T. Disentangling the effect of event-based cues on children’s time-based prospective memory performance. J Exp Child Psychol 2016; 150:130-140. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.05.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/01/2016] [Revised: 05/17/2016] [Accepted: 05/17/2016] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
30
|
Davis JTM, Cullen E, Suddendorf T. Understanding deliberate practice in preschool-aged children. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2016; 69:361-80. [DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1082140] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
Deliberate practice is essential for skill acquisition and expertise and may be a direct consequence of episodic foresight. However, little is known about how deliberate practice develops in children. We present two experiments testing children's ability to selectively practise a behaviour that was going to be useful in future and to reason about the role of practice in skill formation. Five-year-olds demonstrated an explicit understanding of deliberate practice both in selectively choosing to practise a future-relevant skill and in predicting skill change in others based on their practice. Four-year-olds showed some capacities, but failed to demonstrate consistent understanding of the relationship between practice and skill improvement. Children's understanding of this relationship was significantly related to their understanding of how information leads to knowledge, suggesting that both may draw on similar cognitive developmental changes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jac T. M. Davis
- School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Elizabeth Cullen
- School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
| | - Thomas Suddendorf
- School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
|
32
|
Thinking ahead about where something is needed: New insights about episodic foresight in preschoolers. J Exp Child Psychol 2015; 129:98-109. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.09.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/13/2013] [Revised: 09/05/2014] [Accepted: 09/05/2014] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
|
33
|
Nielsen M, Gigante J, Collier-Baker E. Direct cost does not impact on young children's spontaneous helping behavior. Front Psychol 2014; 5:1509. [PMID: 25566167 PMCID: PMC4274792 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01509] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/22/2014] [Accepted: 12/08/2014] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
The propensity of humans to engage in prosocial behavior is unlike that of any other species. Individuals will help others even when it comes at a cost to themselves, and even when the others are complete strangers. However, to date, scant empirical evidence has been forthcoming on young children's altruistic tendencies. To investigate this 45 4-year-olds were presented with a task in which they had opportunity to help an adult confederate retrieve a reward from a novel box. In a control condition children were given no information about the effect of potential helping behavior. Alternatively they were informed that helping would either cost them (i.e., they would miss out on getting the reward) or benefit them (i.e., they would get the reward). It was hypothesized that children would be less likely, and slower, to help in the cost condition, compared to the other two conditions. This hypothesis was not supported: children across all conditions provided help at near ceiling levels.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mark Nielsen
- Early Cognitive Development Centre, School of Psychology, The University of QueenslandBrisbane, QLD, Australia
- School of Applied Human Sciences, University of KwaZulu-NatalDurban, South Africa
| | - Julia Gigante
- Early Cognitive Development Centre, School of Psychology, The University of QueenslandBrisbane, QLD, Australia
| | - Emma Collier-Baker
- Early Cognitive Development Centre, School of Psychology, The University of QueenslandBrisbane, QLD, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Scarf D, Smith C, Stuart M. A spoon full of studies helps the comparison go down: a comparative analysis of Tulving's spoon test. Front Psychol 2014; 5:893. [PMID: 25161644 PMCID: PMC4130454 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00893] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/01/2014] [Accepted: 07/28/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Mental time travel refers to the ability to cast one’s mind back in time to re-experience a past event and forward in time to pre-experience events that may occur in the future. Tulving (2005), an authority on mental time travel, holds that this ability is unique to humans. Anticipating that comparative psychologists would challenge this claim, Tulving (2005) proposed his spoon test, a test specifically designed to assess whether non-human animals are capable of mental time travel. A number of studies have now employed the spoon test to assess mental time travel in non-human animals. Here, we review the evidence for mental time travel in primates. To provide a benchmark, we also review studies that have employed the spoon test with preschool children. The review demonstrates that if we compare the performance of great apes to that of preschool children, and hold them to the same criteria, the data suggest mental travel is present but not ubiquitous in great apes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Damian Scarf
- Department of Psychology, University of Otago Dunedin, New Zealand
| | | | - Michael Stuart
- Department of Psychology, University of Otago Dunedin, New Zealand
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
Martin-Ordas G, Atance CM, Caza JS. How do episodic and semantic memory contribute to episodic foresight in young children? Front Psychol 2014; 5:732. [PMID: 25071690 PMCID: PMC4086199 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00732] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/26/2014] [Accepted: 06/24/2014] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Humans are able to transcend the present and mentally travel to another time, place, or perspective. Mentally projecting ourselves backwards (i.e., episodic memory) or forwards (i.e., episodic foresight) in time are crucial characteristics of the human memory system. Indeed, over the past few years, episodic memory has been argued to be involved both in our capacity to retrieve our personal past experiences and in our ability to imagine and foresee future scenarios. However, recent theory and findings suggest that semantic memory also plays a significant role in imagining future scenarios. We draw on Tulving’s definition of episodic and semantic memory to provide a critical analysis of their role in episodic foresight tasks described in the developmental literature. We conclude by suggesting future directions of research that could further our understanding of how both episodic memory and semantic memory are intimately connected to episodic foresight.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gema Martin-Ordas
- Cognitive Zoology, Department of Cognitive Science, Lund University Lund, Sweden
| | | | - Julian S Caza
- School of Psychology, University of Ottawa Ottawa, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Payne G, Taylor R, Hayne H, Scarf D. Mental time travel for self and other in three- and four-year-old children. Memory 2014; 23:675-82. [DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2014.921310] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
37
|
Metzinger T. The myth of cognitive agency: subpersonal thinking as a cyclically recurring loss of mental autonomy. Front Psychol 2013; 4:931. [PMID: 24427144 PMCID: PMC3868016 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00931] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/09/2013] [Accepted: 11/25/2013] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
This metatheoretical paper investigates mind wandering from the perspective of philosophy of mind. It has two central claims. The first is that, on a conceptual level, mind wandering can be fruitfully described as a specific form of mental autonomy loss. The second is that, given empirical constraints, most of what we call “conscious thought” is better analyzed as a subpersonal process that more often than not lacks crucial properties traditionally taken to be the hallmark of personal-level cognition - such as mental agency, explicit, consciously experienced goal-directedness, or availability for veto control. I claim that for roughly two thirds of our conscious life-time we do not possess mental autonomy (M-autonomy) in this sense. Empirical data from research on mind wandering and nocturnal dreaming clearly show that phenomenally represented cognitive processing is mostly an automatic, non-agentive process and that personal-level cognition is an exception rather than the rule. This raises an interesting new version of the mind-body problem: How is subpersonal cognition causally related to personal-level thought? More fine-grained phenomenological descriptions for what we called “conscious thought” in the past are needed, as well as a functional decomposition of umbrella terms like “mind wandering” into different target phenomena and a better understanding of the frequent dynamic transitions between spontaneous, task-unrelated thought and meta-awareness. In an attempt to lay some very first conceptual foundations for the now burgeoning field of research on mind wandering, the third section proposes two new criteria for individuating single episodes of mind-wandering, namely, the “self-representational blink” (SRB) and a sudden shift in the phenomenological “unit of identification” (UI). I close by specifying a list of potentially innovative research goals that could serve to establish a stronger connection between mind wandering research and philosophy of mind.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas Metzinger
- Philosophisches Seminar, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, Germany ; Frankfurt Institute for Advanced Studies Frankfurt am Main, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
38
|
Suddendorf T, Redshaw J. The development of mental scenario building and episodic foresight. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2013; 1296:135-53. [PMID: 23855564 DOI: 10.1111/nyas.12189] [Citation(s) in RCA: 55] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Episodic foresight is the future-directed counterpart of episodic memory. It is a sophisticated, potentially uniquely human capacity, with tremendous adaptive consequences. Here we review what is currently known about its development through early childhood. We tackle this from two distinct perspectives. First, we present the first systematic evaluation of the development of purported components of mental scenario building as highlighted by a theater metaphor: the stage, the playwright, the set, the actors, the director, the executive producer, and the broadcaster. We find that, although there are diverse developmental trajectories, by 4 years of age children have acquired the basic cognitive components required to mentally construct specific future events. Second, we examine recent attempts to test children's episodic foresight more directly and find that results are in line with those examining the development of required components. This is not to say that children younger than four have no inkling of upcoming events or that older children have nothing left to learn about constructing the future. Episodic foresight, and its neurocognitive foundations, continues to develop throughout childhood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas Suddendorf
- Early Cognitive Development Centre; School of Psychology; University of Queensland; Australia
| | - Jonathan Redshaw
- Early Cognitive Development Centre; School of Psychology; University of Queensland; Australia
| |
Collapse
|