51
|
Abstract
Imagining personal future events is associated with well-being, but the nature and direction of this relationship are unclear. This study investigated whether imagining episodic future scenarios, experimentally manipulated for valence, have an immediate impact on affect. University students (N = 197) completed a 2 × 3 between-subject online study in which they imagined four personal events likely to occur in the future. Participants were directed to imagine positive or negative events or were undirected as to valence to additionally assess the valence and effect of self-directed imagined scenarios. Participants in all three conditions reported a change in positive affect immediately after the task, with both positive and nondirected thinking improving positive affect and negative future thoughts reducing it. However, negative affect only shifted in response to negative future thinking but not the other conditions. These findings demonstrate that there is an immediate causal effect of episodic future thinking on affect but only in specific directions and that this differs from the patterns shown in longer term measurements. The findings also suggest when self-directed that imagined future thoughts tend to mirror the valence and causal effect of positively induced thoughts. This study has implications for the ongoing debate around future thinking and well-being.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Janie Busby Grant
- Centre for Applied Psychology, 2234University of Canberra, Canberra, Australia
| | - Neil Wilson
- Counselling and Psychological Services, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
52
|
Linz R, Pauly R, Smallwood J, Engert V. Mind-wandering content differentially translates from lab to daily life and relates to subjective stress experience. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2019; 85:649-659. [PMID: 31832761 PMCID: PMC7900029 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-019-01275-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/09/2019] [Accepted: 11/29/2019] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Experience and thoughts that are unrelated to the external surroundings are pervasive features of human cognition. Research under the rubric of mind-wandering suggests that such internal experience is context-dependent, and that the content of ongoing thought differentially influences a range of associated outcomes. However, evidence on how the extent of mind-wandering and its content translate from the laboratory to daily life settings is scarce. Furthermore, the relationship between such patterns of thought with markers of stress in daily life remains underexplored. In the current study, we examined multiple aspects of mind-wandering of ninety-three healthy participants (47 women, 25.4 ± 3.9 years) in both the laboratory and daily life and explored two questions: (a) how are mind-wandering extent and content correlated across both settings, and (b) what are their relationships with subjective stress and salivary cortisol levels in daily life? Our results suggest that the extent of off-task thinking is not correlated across contexts, while features of content—i.e., social, future-directed and negative thought content—robustly translate. We also found that daily life subjective stress was linked to more on-task, negative, and future-directed thinking, suggesting stress was linked with the need to act on personally relevant goals. Based on these results we speculate that differences in the links between stress and ongoing thought in daily life may be one reason why patterns of thinking vary from lab to everyday life. More generally, these findings underline the need to consider both context and content in investigating mind-wandering and associated features of subjective experience, and call for caution in generalizing laboratory findings to participants’ daily lives.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Roman Linz
- Research Group "Social Stress and Family Health", Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Stephanstr. 1a, 04103, Leipzig, Germany.
| | - Reena Pauly
- Research Group "Social Stress and Family Health", Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Stephanstr. 1a, 04103, Leipzig, Germany
| | | | - Veronika Engert
- Research Group "Social Stress and Family Health", Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Stephanstr. 1a, 04103, Leipzig, Germany.,Department of Social Neuroscience, Institute of Psychosocial Medicine and Psychotherapy, University Hospital, Friedrich-Schiller University, Jena, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
53
|
Mildner JN, Tamir DI. Spontaneous Thought as an Unconstrained Memory Process. Trends Neurosci 2019; 42:763-777. [PMID: 31627848 DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2019.09.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/27/2019] [Revised: 08/23/2019] [Accepted: 09/05/2019] [Indexed: 01/12/2023]
Abstract
The stream of thought can flow freely, without much guidance from attention or cognitive control. What determines what we think about from one moment to the next? Spontaneous thought shares many commonalities with memory processes. We use insights from computational models of memory to explain how the stream of thought flows through the landscape of memory. In this framework of spontaneous thought, semantic memory scaffolds episodic memory to form the content of thought, and drifting context modulated by one's current state - both internal and external - constrains the area of memory to explore. This conceptualization of spontaneous thought can help to answer outstanding questions such as: what is the function of spontaneous thought, and how does the mind select what to think about?
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Judith N Mildner
- Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08540, USA.
| | - Diana I Tamir
- Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08540, USA
| |
Collapse
|
54
|
"All is not lost"-Rethinking the nature of memory and the self in dementia. Ageing Res Rev 2019; 54:100932. [PMID: 31238174 DOI: 10.1016/j.arr.2019.100932] [Citation(s) in RCA: 50] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/17/2019] [Revised: 06/07/2019] [Accepted: 06/20/2019] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
Memory and the self have long been considered intertwined, leading to the assumption that without memory, there can be no self. This line of reasoning has led to the misconception that a loss of memory in dementia necessarily results in a diminished sense of self. Here, we challenge this assumption by considering discrete facets of self-referential memory, and their relative profiles of loss and sparing, across three neurodegenerative disorders: Alzheimer's disease, semantic dementia, and frontotemporal dementia. By exploring canonical expressions of the self across past, present, and future contexts in dementia, relative to healthy ageing, we reconcile previous accounts of loss of self in dementia, and propose a new framework for understanding and managing everyday functioning and behaviour. Notably, our approach highlights the multifaceted and dynamic nature in which the temporally-extended self is likely to change in healthy and pathological ageing, with important ramifications for development of person-centred care. Collectively, we aim to promote a cohesive sense of self in dementia across past, present, and future contexts, by demonstrating how, ultimately, 'All is not lost'.
Collapse
|
55
|
Hasenkamp W. Fruits of the Buddhism-science dialogue in contemplative research. Curr Opin Psychol 2019; 28:126-132. [DOI: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.12.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/14/2018] [Revised: 12/04/2018] [Accepted: 12/05/2018] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
56
|
Cole S, Kvavilashvili L. Spontaneous future cognition: the past, present and future of an emerging topic. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2019; 83:631-650. [PMID: 31079226 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-019-01193-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/16/2019] [Accepted: 04/20/2019] [Indexed: 02/06/2023]
Abstract
In this Editorial, we discuss the past, present and future of an emerging and fast-developing field-spontaneous future cognition. In tracking the past of this research, the trajectories of research on mind-wandering, episodic future thinking and prospective memory are briefly examined, and their relation with spontaneous future cognition demarcated. Three broad methodological approaches (questionnaire, naturalistic and laboratory) used to study spontaneous future thoughts are described, providing an overview of the field. The present state of this research is represented by a themed analysis of the articles included in this Special Issue. The breadth of studies (covering cognitive mechanisms, developmental stages and psychopathology) have already led to important insights, especially concerning the conditions in which spontaneous future thoughts most commonly arise and who may be predisposed to experiencing them. In the future, greater effort should focus on developing a theoretical account of spontaneous future cognition-this may increase our understanding of how and why spontaneous future thoughts occur. If future research in this area reflects the diversity and depth within this Special Issue, a flourishing of research on spontaneous future cognition will be on the horizon in years or perhaps decades to come.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Scott Cole
- Psychology, School of Psychological and Social Sciences, York St John University, York, YO31 7EX, UK.
| | | |
Collapse
|
57
|
Shi L, Sun J, Wu X, Wei D, Chen Q, Yang W, Chen H, Qiu J. Brain networks of happiness: dynamic functional connectivity among the default, cognitive and salience networks relates to subjective well-being. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2019; 13:851-862. [PMID: 30016499 PMCID: PMC6123521 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsy059] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/23/2018] [Accepted: 07/11/2018] [Indexed: 12/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Subjective well-being (SWB) reflects the cognitive and emotional evaluations of an individual's life and plays an important role in individual's success in health, work and social relationships. Although previous studies have revealed the spontaneous brain activity underlying SWB, little is known about the relationship between brain network interactions and SWB. The present study investigated the static and dynamic functional connectivity among large-scale brain networks during resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in relation to SWB in two large independent datasets. The results showed that SWB is negatively correlated with static functional connectivity between the salience network (SN) and the anterior default mode network (DMN). Dynamic functional network connectivity (dFNC) analysis found that SWB is negatively correlated with the fraction of time that participants spent in a brain state characterized by weak cross-network connectivity (between the DMN, SN and frontal-parietal network [FPN]) and strong within-network connectivity (within the DMN and within the FPN). This connectivity profile may account for the good mental adaptability and flexible information communication of people with high levels of SWB. The dFNC results were well replicated with different analysis parameters and further validated in an independent sample. Taken together, these findings reveal that the dynamic interaction between networks involved in self-reflection, emotional regulation and cognitive control underlies SWB.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Liang Shi
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality (SWU), Ministry of Education, Chongqing 400715, China.,School of Psychology, Southwest University (SWU),Chongqing 400715, China
| | - Jiangzhou Sun
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality (SWU), Ministry of Education, Chongqing 400715, China.,School of Psychology, Southwest University (SWU),Chongqing 400715, China
| | - Xinran Wu
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality (SWU), Ministry of Education, Chongqing 400715, China.,School of Psychology, Southwest University (SWU),Chongqing 400715, China
| | - Dongtao Wei
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality (SWU), Ministry of Education, Chongqing 400715, China.,School of Psychology, Southwest University (SWU),Chongqing 400715, China
| | - Qunlin Chen
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality (SWU), Ministry of Education, Chongqing 400715, China.,School of Psychology, Southwest University (SWU),Chongqing 400715, China
| | - Wenjing Yang
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality (SWU), Ministry of Education, Chongqing 400715, China.,School of Psychology, Southwest University (SWU),Chongqing 400715, China
| | - Hong Chen
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality (SWU), Ministry of Education, Chongqing 400715, China.,School of Psychology, Southwest University (SWU),Chongqing 400715, China
| | - Jiang Qiu
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality (SWU), Ministry of Education, Chongqing 400715, China.,School of Psychology, Southwest University (SWU),Chongqing 400715, China.,Southwest University Branch, Collaborative Innovation Center of Assessment toward Basic Education Quality, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, China
| |
Collapse
|
58
|
Keulers EHH, Jonkman LM. Mind wandering in children: Examining task-unrelated thoughts in computerized tasks and a classroom lesson, and the association with different executive functions. J Exp Child Psychol 2018; 179:276-290. [PMID: 30562634 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.11.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/08/2018] [Revised: 11/16/2018] [Accepted: 11/17/2018] [Indexed: 02/03/2023]
Abstract
Mind wandering is associated with worse performance on cognitively demanding tasks, but this concept is largely unexplored in typically developing children and little is known about the relation between mind wandering and specific executive functions (EFs). This study aimed, first, to measure and compare children's mind wandering in controlled computerized tasks as well as in an educational setting and, second, to examine the association between mind wandering and the three core EFs, namely inhibition, working memory, and set shifting/switching. A total of 52 children aged 9-11 years performed a classroom listening task and a computerized EF battery consisting of flanker, running span, and attention switching tasks. Mind wandering was measured using online probed and/or retrospective self-reports of task-unrelated thoughts (TUTs) during task performance. Children reported TUTs on 20-25% of the thought probes, which did not differ between classroom and EF tasks. Regression models, hierarchically adding the three core EFs, accounted for a small but significant portion of variance in TUT frequency when measured in class and retrospectively after EF tasks, but not when measured online in EF tasks. Children with worse inhibition were more prone to mind wander during classroom and EF tasks. Lower attention switching accuracy also explained variation in retrospectively reported TUTs during EF tasks. Working memory was not a significant predictor. These results suggest that mind wandering is common and reliably measurable in children in controlled computerized and educational tasks. Lower executive control abilities predict more frequent mind wandering, although different EFs are related to mind wandering in diverse tasks/measures.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Esther H H Keulers
- Department of Neuropsychology & Psychopharmacology, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, 6200 MD Maastricht, The Netherlands.
| | - Lisa M Jonkman
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, 6200 MD Maastricht, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
59
|
Irish M, Goldberg ZL, Alaeddin S, O'Callaghan C, Andrews-Hanna JR. Age-related changes in the temporal focus and self-referential content of spontaneous cognition during periods of low cognitive demand. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2018; 83:747-760. [PMID: 30291418 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-018-1102-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/28/2018] [Accepted: 09/24/2018] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
An intriguing aspect of human cognition is the unique capacity to mentally retreat from our immediate surroundings to consider perspectives distinct from the here and now. Despite increasing interest in this phenomenon, relatively little is known regarding age-related changes in off-task, self-generated thought (often referred to as "mind-wandering"), particularly under conditions of low cognitive demand. While a number of studies have investigated the temporal orientation of mind-wandering with increasing age, findings have been largely inconsistent. Here, we explored the frequency, temporal focus, and self-referential/social content of spontaneous task-unrelated, perceptually decoupled thought in 30 young and 33 healthy older adults using the Shape Expectations task, a validated experimental paradigm in which discrete facets of inner mentation are quantified along a conceptual continuum using open-ended report. Participants also completed the daydreaming subscale of the Imaginal Process Inventory (IPI) as a trait measure of mind-wandering propensity. Significant group differences emerged on the Shape Expectations task, with reduced instances of mind-wandering in the context of elevated task-related thoughts relative to younger adults. In terms of temporal focus, a preponderance of present/atemporal off-task thoughts was evident irrespective of group; however, significantly higher levels of future-oriented thoughts were provided by younger adults, contrasting with significantly higher instances of retrospection in the older group. In addition, older adults displayed significantly fewer incidences of self-referential cognition relative to their younger counterparts. Our findings indicate a distinct attenuation of off-task, self-generated thought processes with increasing age, with evidence for a shift in temporal focus and self-referential quality, during periods of low cognitive demand.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Muireann Irish
- Brain and Mind Centre and School of Psychology, The University of Sydney, Camperdown, Sydney, NSW, 2050, Australia. .,Australian Research Council Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Sydney, Australia.
| | - Zoë-Lee Goldberg
- Brain and Mind Centre and School of Psychology, The University of Sydney, Camperdown, Sydney, NSW, 2050, Australia
| | - Sara Alaeddin
- Brain and Mind Centre and School of Psychology, The University of Sydney, Camperdown, Sydney, NSW, 2050, Australia
| | - Claire O'Callaghan
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK.,Brain and Mind Centre and School of Medicine, The University of Sydney, Camperdown, Sydney, Australia
| | | |
Collapse
|
60
|
Zhou X, Lei X. Wandering Minds with Wandering Brain Networks. Neurosci Bull 2018; 34:1017-1028. [PMID: 30136075 DOI: 10.1007/s12264-018-0278-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/27/2018] [Accepted: 06/04/2018] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
The default mode network (DMN) is associated with the occurrence of mind-wandering or task-unrelated thought. In contrast, the frontal-parietal network (FPN) and visual network (VS) are involved in tasks with external stimuli. However, it is not clear how these functional network interactions support these two different processes - mind-wandering and on-task - especially with regard to individual variation in the mind-wandering experience. In this study, we investigated the functional connectivity and modular structure among the DMN, FPN, and VS. Our results showed that, compared to the on-task period, mind-wandering was associated with increased DMN activity and increased DMN-VS connectivity. Moreover, mind-wandering was accompanied by a large number of transitional nodes, which expressed a diversity of brain regions. Intriguingly, the functional connectivity of the FPN and VS was strongly correlated with individual behavioral performance. Our findings highlight the individual variation of mind-wandering, which implies the importance of other complementary large-scale brain networks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xinqi Zhou
- Sleep and NeuroImaging Center, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, 400715, China.,Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality of Ministry of Education, Chongqing, 400715, China
| | - Xu Lei
- Sleep and NeuroImaging Center, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, 400715, China. .,Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality of Ministry of Education, Chongqing, 400715, China.
| |
Collapse
|
61
|
The Critical Role of the Hippocampus in Mind Wandering. J Neurosci 2018; 38:6439-6441. [PMID: 30021763 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.0995-18.2018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/18/2018] [Revised: 06/07/2018] [Accepted: 06/07/2018] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
|
62
|
Pepin G, Malin S, Jallais C, Moreau F, Fort A, Navarro J, Ndiaye D, Gabaude C. Do distinct mind wandering differently disrupt drivers? Interpretation of physiological and behavioral pattern with a data triangulation method. Conscious Cogn 2018; 62:69-81. [DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2018.04.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/23/2017] [Revised: 03/27/2018] [Accepted: 04/12/2018] [Indexed: 10/17/2022]
|
63
|
Fox KCR, Andrews-Hanna JR, Mills C, Dixon ML, Markovic J, Thompson E, Christoff K. Affective neuroscience of self-generated thought. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2018; 1426:25-51. [PMID: 29754412 DOI: 10.1111/nyas.13740] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/21/2017] [Revised: 03/16/2018] [Accepted: 03/28/2018] [Indexed: 01/05/2023]
Abstract
Despite increasing scientific interest in self-generated thought-mental content largely independent of the immediate environment-there has yet to be any comprehensive synthesis of the subjective experience and neural correlates of affect in these forms of thinking. Here, we aim to develop an integrated affective neuroscience encompassing many forms of self-generated thought-normal and pathological, moderate and excessive, in waking and in sleep. In synthesizing existing literature on this topic, we reveal consistent findings pertaining to the prevalence, valence, and variability of emotion in self-generated thought, and highlight how these factors might interact with self-generated thought to influence general well-being. We integrate these psychological findings with recent neuroimaging research, bringing attention to the neural correlates of affect in self-generated thought. We show that affect in self-generated thought is prevalent, positively biased, highly variable (both within and across individuals), and consistently recruits many brain areas implicated in emotional processing, including the orbitofrontal cortex, amygdala, insula, and medial prefrontal cortex. Many factors modulate these typical psychological and neural patterns, however; the emerging affective neuroscience of self-generated thought must endeavor to link brain function and subjective experience in both everyday self-generated thought as well as its dysfunctions in mental illness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kieran C R Fox
- Department of Neurology and Neurological Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, California
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
| | - Jessica R Andrews-Hanna
- Department of Psychology and Interdisciplinary Program in Cognitive Science, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona
| | - Caitlin Mills
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
| | - Matthew L Dixon
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| | - Jelena Markovic
- Department of Philosophy, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
| | - Evan Thompson
- Department of Philosophy, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
| | - Kalina Christoff
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
- Centre for Brain Health, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
64
|
Tracking arousal state and mind wandering with pupillometry. COGNITIVE AFFECTIVE & BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE 2018; 18:638-664. [DOI: 10.3758/s13415-018-0594-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 58] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/23/2023]
|
65
|
McCormick C, Ciaramelli E, De Luca F, Maguire EA. Comparing and Contrasting the Cognitive Effects of Hippocampal and Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Damage: A Review of Human Lesion Studies. Neuroscience 2018; 374:295-318. [PMID: 28827088 PMCID: PMC6053620 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.07.066] [Citation(s) in RCA: 96] [Impact Index Per Article: 13.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/16/2017] [Revised: 07/24/2017] [Accepted: 07/28/2017] [Indexed: 01/10/2023]
Abstract
The hippocampus and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) are closely connected brain regions whose functions are still debated. In order to offer a fresh perspective on understanding the contributions of these two brain regions to cognition, in this review we considered cognitive tasks that usually elicit deficits in hippocampal-damaged patients (e.g., autobiographical memory retrieval), and examined the performance of vmPFC-lesioned patients on these tasks. We then took cognitive tasks where performance is typically compromised following vmPFC damage (e.g., decision making), and looked at how these are affected by hippocampal lesions. Three salient motifs emerged. First, there are surprising gaps in our knowledge about how hippocampal and vmPFC patients perform on tasks typically associated with the other group. Second, while hippocampal or vmPFC damage seems to adversely affect performance on so-called hippocampal tasks, the performance of hippocampal and vmPFC patients clearly diverges on classic vmPFC tasks. Third, although performance appears analogous on hippocampal tasks, on closer inspection, there are significant disparities between hippocampal and vmPFC patients. Based on these findings, we suggest a tentative hierarchical model to explain the functions of the hippocampus and vmPFC. We propose that the vmPFC initiates the construction of mental scenes by coordinating the curation of relevant elements from neocortical areas, which are then funneled into the hippocampus to build a scene. The vmPFC then engages in iterative re-initiation via feedback loops with neocortex and hippocampus to facilitate the flow and integration of the multiple scenes that comprise the coherent unfolding of an extended mental event.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cornelia McCormick
- Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, Institute of Neurology, University College London, 12 Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK
| | - Elisa Ciaramelli
- Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università di Bologna, Bologna, Italy; Centro studi e ricerche di Neuroscienze Cognitive, Cesena, Italy
| | - Flavia De Luca
- Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università di Bologna, Bologna, Italy; Centro studi e ricerche di Neuroscienze Cognitive, Cesena, Italy
| | - Eleanor A Maguire
- Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, Institute of Neurology, University College London, 12 Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
66
|
McCormick C, Rosenthal CR, Miller TD, Maguire EA. Mind-Wandering in People with Hippocampal Damage. J Neurosci 2018; 38:2745-2754. [PMID: 29440532 PMCID: PMC5851780 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.1812-17.2018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 76] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/29/2017] [Revised: 01/21/2018] [Accepted: 01/24/2018] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Subjective inner experiences, such as mind-wandering, represent the fundaments of human cognition. Although the precise function of mind-wandering is still debated, it is increasingly acknowledged to have influence across cognition on processes such as future planning, creative thinking, and problem-solving and even on depressive rumination and other mental health disorders. Recently, there has been important progress in characterizing mind-wandering and identifying the associated neural networks. Two prominent features of mind-wandering are mental time travel and visuospatial imagery, which are often linked with the hippocampus. People with selective bilateral hippocampal damage cannot vividly recall events from their past, envision their future, or imagine fictitious scenes. This raises the question of whether the hippocampus plays a causal role in mind-wandering and, if so, in what way. Leveraging a unique opportunity to shadow people (all males) with bilateral hippocampal damage for several days, we examined, for the first time, what they thought about spontaneously, without direct task demands. We found that they engaged in as much mind-wandering as control participants. However, whereas controls thought about the past, present, and future, imagining vivid visual scenes, hippocampal damage resulted in thoughts primarily about the present comprising verbally mediated semantic knowledge. These findings expose the hippocampus as a key pillar in the neural architecture of mind-wandering and also reveal its impact beyond episodic memory, placing it at the heart of our mental life.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Humans tend to mind-wander ∼30-50% of their waking time. Two prominent features of this pervasive form of thought are mental time travel and visuospatial imagery, which are often associated with the hippocampus. To examine whether the hippocampus plays a causal role in mind-wandering, we examined the frequency and phenomenology of mind-wandering in patients with selective bilateral hippocampal damage. We found that they engaged in as much mind-wandering as controls. However, hippocampal damage changed the form and content of mind-wandering from flexible, episodic, and scene based to abstract, semanticized, and verbal. These findings expose the hippocampus as a key pillar in the neural architecture of mind-wandering and reveal its impact beyond episodic memory, placing it at the heart of our mental life.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cornelia McCormick
- Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, Institute of Neurology, University College London, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom, and
| | - Clive R Rosenthal
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Oxford OX3 9DU, United Kingdom
| | - Thomas D Miller
- Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Oxford OX3 9DU, United Kingdom
| | - Eleanor A Maguire
- Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, Institute of Neurology, University College London, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom, and
| |
Collapse
|
67
|
Perceived Cognition after Percutaneous Coronary Intervention: Association with Quality of Life, Mood and Fatigue in the THORESCI Study. Int J Behav Med 2018; 24:552-562. [PMID: 28032322 PMCID: PMC5509816 DOI: 10.1007/s12529-016-9624-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/11/2022]
Abstract
Purpose Percutaneous coronary intervention (PCI) is a common invasive procedure for the treatment of coronary artery diseases. Long-term cognitive functioning after PCI and its association with health-related quality of life (HRQL) and psychological factors is relatively unknown. The aim of this study is to examine whether perceived cognitive functioning during the year after PCI is associated with HRQL over this time period, and whether mood, fatigue, and age are associated with changes in perceived cognition and HRQL. Methods Patients undergoing PCI (n = 384, 79% male, mean age = 63, SD = 10) were recruited in the observational Tilburg Health Outcome Registry of Emotional Stress after Coronary Intervention (THORESCI) cohort study. Perceived concentration and attention problems, HRQL, mood, and fatigue were assessed at baseline, at 1-month and 12-month follow-up. Results General linear mixed modeling analysis showed that across time, between- and within-subject differences in perceived concentration problems were associated with a reduced HRQL in all domains independent of clinical and demographic covariates. Only a part of this association could be explained by negative mood, fatigue, and older age. Similar findings were found for between-subject differences in perceived attention problems. Conclusions Between-subject differences and within-subject changes in perceived cognition in PCI patients were strongly associated with HRQL across time, such that poorer perceived cognition was associated with poorer HRQL, independent of demographic and clinical variables. Most of the associations were also independent of mood and fatigue. The results should increase the awareness of clinicians for the role of cognition in the cardiac rehabilitation and recovery post-PCI.
Collapse
|
68
|
Shrimpton D, McGann D, Riby LM. Daydream Believer: Rumination, Self-Reflection and the Temporal Focus of Mind Wandering Content. EUROPES JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY 2018; 13:794-809. [PMID: 29358989 PMCID: PMC5763464 DOI: 10.5964/ejop.v13i4.1425] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/28/2017] [Accepted: 05/18/2017] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Current research into mind-wandering is beginning to acknowledge that this process is one of heterogeneity. Following on from previous findings highlighting the role of self-focus during mind wandering, the present study aimed to examine individual differences in rumination and self-reflection and the impact such styles of self-focus may have on mind-wandering experiences. Thirty-three participants were required to complete the Sustained Attention Response Task (SART), aimed at inducing mind-wandering episodes, whilst also probing the content of thought in terms of temporal focus. Self-report questionnaires were also administered after the SART to measure dispositional differences in style and beliefs regarding mind-wandering and assessments of individual differences in rumination and self-reflection. Those individuals with reflective self-focus showed a strong positive association with positive and constructive thoughts. Critically, ruminative self-focus was positively associated with a tendency for the mind to wander towards anguished fantasies, failures and aggression, but it was also positively associated with positive and constructive thoughts. Furthermore, while dispositional differences in self-focus showed no relationship with the temporal perspective of thoughts when probed during a cognitive task, performance on the task itself was related to whether participants were thinking about the past, present or future during that activity. Such findings are discussed in line with previous research, and provide a further step towards accounting for the heterogeneous nature of mind-wandering.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daisy Shrimpton
- Department of Psychology, Northumbria University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom
| | - Deborah McGann
- Department of Psychology, Northumbria University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom
| | - Leigh M Riby
- Department of Psychology, Northumbria University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
69
|
Taruffi L, Pehrs C, Skouras S, Koelsch S. Effects of Sad and Happy Music on Mind-Wandering and the Default Mode Network. Sci Rep 2017; 7:14396. [PMID: 29089542 PMCID: PMC5663956 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-017-14849-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 54] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/20/2017] [Accepted: 10/17/2017] [Indexed: 12/31/2022] Open
Abstract
Music is a ubiquitous phenomenon in human cultures, mostly due to its power to evoke and regulate emotions. However, effects of music evoking different emotional experiences such as sadness and happiness on cognition, and in particular on self-generated thought, are unknown. Here we use probe-caught thought sampling and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate the influence of sad and happy music on mind-wandering and its underlying neuronal mechanisms. In three experiments we found that sad music, compared with happy music, is associated with stronger mind-wandering (Experiments 1A and 1B) and greater centrality of the nodes of the Default Mode Network (DMN) (Experiment 2). Thus, our results demonstrate that, when listening to sad vs. happy music, people withdraw their attention inwards and engage in spontaneous, self-referential cognitive processes. Importantly, our results also underscore that DMN activity can be modulated as a function of sad and happy music. These findings call for a systematic investigation of the relation between music and thought, having broad implications for the use of music in education and clinical settings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Liila Taruffi
- Department of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany.
| | - Corinna Pehrs
- Department of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Stavros Skouras
- Department of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Stefan Koelsch
- Department of Biological and Medical Psychology, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
| |
Collapse
|
70
|
Mills C, Raffaelli Q, Irving ZC, Stan D, Christoff K. Is an off-task mind a freely-moving mind? Examining the relationship between different dimensions of thought. Conscious Cogn 2017; 58:20-33. [PMID: 29107470 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2017.10.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 64] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/23/2017] [Revised: 09/20/2017] [Accepted: 10/02/2017] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Mind wandering is frequently defined as task-unrelated or perceptually decoupled thought. However, these definitions may not capture the dynamic features of a wandering mind, such as its tendency to 'move freely'. Here we test the relationship between three theoretically dissociable dimensions of thought: freedom of movement in thought, task-relatedness, and perceptual decoupling (i.e., lack of awareness of surroundings). Using everyday life experience sampling, thought probes were randomly delivered to participants' phones for ten days. Results revealed weak intra-individual correlations between freedom of movement in thought and task-unrelatedness, as well as perceptual decoupling. Within our dataset, over 40% of thoughts would have been misclassified under the assumption that off-task thought is inherently freely moving. Overall, freedom of movement appears to be an independent dimension of thought that is not captured by the two most common measures of mind wandering. Future work focusing on the dynamics of thought may be crucial for improving our understanding of the wandering mind.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Caitlin Mills
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Canada.
| | | | - Zachary C Irving
- Department of Philosophy, University of California, Berkeley, USA.
| | - Dylan Stan
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Canada.
| | - Kalina Christoff
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Canada; Centre for Brain Health, University of British Columbia, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
71
|
Kuehner C, Welz A, Reinhard I, Alpers GW. Lab meets real life: A laboratory assessment of spontaneous thought and its ecological validity. PLoS One 2017; 12:e0184488. [PMID: 28910351 PMCID: PMC5598976 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0184488] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/23/2017] [Accepted: 08/24/2017] [Indexed: 01/28/2023] Open
Abstract
People's minds frequently wander towards self-generated thoughts, which are unrelated to external stimuli or demands. These phenomena, referred to as "spontaneous thought" (ST) and "mind wandering" (MW), have previously been linked with both costs and benefits. Current assessments of ST and MW have predominantly been conducted in the laboratory, whereas studies on the ecological validity of such lab-related constructs and their interrelations are rare. The current study examined the stability of ST dimensions assessed in the lab and their predictive value with respect to MW, repetitive negative thought (uncontrollable rumination, RUM), and affect in daily life. Forty-three university students were assessed with the Amsterdam Resting State Questionnaire (2nd version) to assess ten ST dimensions during the resting state in two laboratory sessions, which were separated by five days of electronic ambulatory assessment (AA). During AA, individuals indicated the intensity of MW and RUM, as well as of positive and negative affect in daily life ten times a day. ST dimensions measured in the lab were moderately stable across one week. Five out of ten ST lab dimensions were predicted by mental health-related symptoms or by dispositional cognitive traits. Hierarchical linear models revealed that a number of ST lab dimensions predicted cognitive and affective states in daily life. Mediation analyses showed that RUM, but not MW per se, accounted for the relationship between specific ST lab dimensions and mood in daily life. By using a simple resting state task, we could demonstrate that a number of lab dimensions of spontaneous thought are moderately stable, are predicted by mental health symptoms and cognitive traits, and show plausible associations with categories of self-generated thought and mood in daily life.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christine Kuehner
- Research Group Longitudinal and Intervention Research, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany
- * E-mail:
| | - Annett Welz
- Research Group Longitudinal and Intervention Research, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany
| | - Iris Reinhard
- Department of Biostatistics, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany
| | - Georg W. Alpers
- Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences, University of Mannheim, and Otto-Selz-Institute, University of Mannheim, Mannheim, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
72
|
Unsworth N, McMillan BD. Attentional disengagements in educational contexts: a diary investigation of everyday mind-wandering and distraction. COGNITIVE RESEARCH-PRINCIPLES AND IMPLICATIONS 2017; 2:32. [PMID: 28890917 PMCID: PMC5569648 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-017-0070-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/21/2016] [Accepted: 07/06/2017] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
Abstract
The present study examined everyday attentional disengagements in educational contexts. Undergraduate students completed various cognitive ability measures in the laboratory and recorded everyday mind-wandering and distraction in a diary over the course of a week. Participants reported mind-wandering and being distracted both in class and while studying and there were a number of different subtypes of attentional disengagements. Individual differences in cognitive abilities were related to some, but not all, everyday attentional disengagements and motivation and interest in classes were related to specific subtypes of disengagements. Finally, academic performance was related to fluid intelligence and motivation, but not to everyday disengagements. These results provide importance evidence on the different types of attentional disengagements that are prevalent in undergraduate students and for whom disengagements are most likely.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nash Unsworth
- Department of Psychology, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR 97403 USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
73
|
Marchetti I, Koster EH, Klinger E, Alloy LB. Spontaneous Thought and Vulnerability to Mood Disorders: The Dark Side of the Wandering Mind. Clin Psychol Sci 2016; 4:835-857. [PMID: 28785510 PMCID: PMC5544025 DOI: 10.1177/2167702615622383] [Citation(s) in RCA: 98] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/19/2023]
Abstract
There is increasing interest in spontaneous thought, namely task-unrelated or rest-related mental activity. Spontaneous thought is an umbrella term for processes like mindwandering, involuntary autobiographical memory, and daydreaming, with evidence elucidating adaptive and maladaptive consequences. In this theoretical framework, we propose that, apart from its positive functions, spontaneous thought is a precursor for cognitive vulnerability in individuals who are at-risk for mood disorders. Importantly, spontaneous thought mostly focuses on unattained goals and evaluates the discrepancy between current and desired status (Klinger, 1971, 2013a). In individuals who stably (i.e., trait negative affectivity) or transitorily (i.e., stress) experience negative emotions in reaction to goal-discrepancy, spontaneous thought fosters major cognitive vulnerabilities (e.g., rumination, hopelessness, low self-esteem, and cognitive reactivity) which, in turn, enhance depression. Furthermore, we also highlight preliminary links between spontaneous thought and bipolar disorder. The evidence for this framework is reviewed and we discuss theoretical and clinical implications of our proposal.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Igor Marchetti
- Ghent University, Department of Experimental-Clinical and Health Psychology, Henri Dunantlaan 2, Ghent, B-9000, Belgium
| | - Ernst H.W. Koster
- Ghent University, Department of Experimental-Clinical and Health Psychology, Henri Dunantlaan 2, Ghent, B-9000, Belgium
| | - Eric Klinger
- University of Minnesota, Morris, Psychology Discipline, Division of Social Sciences, Morris, MN 56267, USA
| | - Lauren B. Alloy
- Temple University, Department of Psychology, Weiss Hall, 1701 N. 13th Street, Philadelphia, PA 19122, USA
| |
Collapse
|
74
|
Dispositional mindfulness and the wandering mind: Implications for attentional control in older adults. Conscious Cogn 2016; 44:193-204. [PMID: 27541935 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.08.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/22/2015] [Revised: 05/03/2016] [Accepted: 08/10/2016] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
Age-related cognitive decline brings decreases in functional status. Dispositional mindfulness, the tendency towards present-moment attention, is hypothesized to correspond with enhanced attention, whereas mind-wandering may be detrimental to cognition. The relationships among mindfulness, task-related and task-unrelated thought, and attentional control performance on Go/No-Go and Continuous Performance tasks were examined in older adults. Dispositional mindfulness was negatively associated with task-unrelated thought and was positively associated with reactive control, but not proactive control or Go/No-Go performance. Although mind-wandering was not directly associated with performance, task-unrelated thought mediated the mindfulness-proactive control relation. Fewer task-unrelated thoughts were associated with lower proactive control. Interestingly, this effect was moderated by working memory such that it was present for those with low-average, but not high, working memory. This study highlights the importance of dispositional mindfulness and mind-wandering propensity in accounting for individual differences in attentional control in older adults, providing important targets for future cognitive remediation interventions.
Collapse
|
75
|
Grady C, Sarraf S, Saverino C, Campbell K. Age differences in the functional interactions among the default, frontoparietal control, and dorsal attention networks. Neurobiol Aging 2016; 41:159-172. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.02.020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 239] [Impact Index Per Article: 26.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/16/2015] [Revised: 01/25/2016] [Accepted: 02/20/2016] [Indexed: 02/02/2023]
|
76
|
Mooneyham BW, Mrazek MD, Mrazek AJ, Schooler JW. Signal or noise: brain network interactions underlying the experience and training of mindfulness. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2016; 1369:240-56. [PMID: 27038003 DOI: 10.1111/nyas.13044] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/13/2015] [Revised: 02/12/2016] [Accepted: 02/22/2016] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
A broad set of brain regions has been associated with the experience and training of mindfulness. Many of these regions lie within key intrinsic brain networks, including the executive control, salience, and default networks. In this paper, we review the existing literature on the cognitive neuroscience of mindfulness through the lens of network science. We describe the characteristics of the intrinsic brain networks implicated in mindfulness and summarize the relevant findings pertaining to changes in functional connectivity (FC) within and between these networks. Convergence across these findings suggests that mindfulness may be associated with increased FC between two regions within the default network: the posterior cingulate cortex and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Additionally, extensive meditation experience may be associated with increased FC between the insula and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. However, little consensus has emerged within the existing literature owing to the diversity of operational definitions of mindfulness, neuroimaging methods, and network characterizations. We describe several challenges to develop a coherent cognitive neuroscience of mindfulness and to provide detailed recommendations for future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Benjamin W Mooneyham
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, University of California Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, California
| | - Michael D Mrazek
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, University of California Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, California
| | - Alissa J Mrazek
- Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois
| | - Jonathan W Schooler
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, University of California Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, California
| |
Collapse
|
77
|
Spronken M, Holland RW, Figner B, Dijksterhuis A. Temporal focus, temporal distance, and mind-wandering valence: Results from an experience sampling and an experimental study. Conscious Cogn 2016; 41:104-18. [PMID: 26897298 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.02.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/21/2015] [Revised: 02/05/2016] [Accepted: 02/07/2016] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
When mind-wandering, people may think about events that happened in the past, or events that may happen in the future. Using experience sampling, we first aimed to replicate the finding that future-oriented thoughts show a greater positivity bias than past-oriented thoughts. Furthermore, we investigated whether there is a relation between the temporal distance of past- and future-oriented thoughts and the frequency of positive thoughts, a factor that has received little attention in previous work. Second, we experimentally investigated the relation between temporal focus, temporal distance, and thought valence. Both studies showed that future-oriented thoughts were more positive compared to past-oriented thoughts. Regarding temporal distance, thoughts about the distant past and future were more positive than thoughts about the near past and future in the experiment. However, the experience sampling study did not provide clear insight into this relation. Potential theoretical and methodological explanations for these findings are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maitta Spronken
- Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University, Postbus 9104, 6500 HE Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
| | - Rob W Holland
- Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University, Postbus 9104, 6500 HE Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
| | - Bernd Figner
- Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University, Postbus 9104, 6500 HE Nijmegen, The Netherlands; Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, The Netherlands.
| | - Ap Dijksterhuis
- Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University, Postbus 9104, 6500 HE Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
| |
Collapse
|
78
|
Villena-González M, López V, Rodríguez E. Orienting attention to visual or verbal/auditory imagery differentially impairs the processing of visual stimuli. Neuroimage 2016; 132:71-78. [PMID: 26876471 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/26/2015] [Revised: 01/29/2016] [Accepted: 02/08/2016] [Indexed: 10/22/2022] Open
Abstract
When attention is oriented toward inner thoughts, as spontaneously occurs during mind wandering, the processing of external information is attenuated. However, the potential effects of thought's content regarding sensory attenuation are still unknown. The present study aims to assess if the representational format of thoughts, such as visual imagery or inner speech, might differentially affect the sensory processing of external stimuli. We recorded the brain activity of 20 participants (12 women) while they were exposed to a probe visual stimulus in three different conditions: executing a task on the visual probe (externally oriented attention), and two conditions involving inward-turned attention i.e. generating inner speech and performing visual imagery. Event-related potentials results showed that the P1 amplitude, related with sensory response, was significantly attenuated during both task involving inward attention compared with external task. When both representational formats were compared, the visual imagery condition showed stronger attenuation in sensory processing than inner speech condition. Alpha power in visual areas was measured as an index of cortical inhibition. Larger alpha amplitude was found when participants engaged in an internal thought contrasted with the external task, with visual imagery showing even more alpha power than inner speech condition. Our results show, for the first time to our knowledge, that visual attentional processing to external stimuli during self-generated thoughts is differentially affected by the representational format of the ongoing train of thoughts.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mario Villena-González
- Escuela de Psicología, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago CP 7820436, Chile
| | - Vladimir López
- Escuela de Psicología, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago CP 7820436, Chile
| | - Eugenio Rodríguez
- Escuela de Psicología, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago CP 7820436, Chile.
| |
Collapse
|
79
|
Poerio GL, Totterdell P, Emerson LM, Miles E. Social Daydreaming and Adjustment: An Experience-Sampling Study of Socio-Emotional Adaptation During a Life Transition. Front Psychol 2016; 7:13. [PMID: 26834685 PMCID: PMC4720731 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/19/2015] [Accepted: 01/05/2016] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Estimates suggest that up to half of waking life is spent daydreaming; that is, engaged in thought that is independent of, and unrelated to, one’s current task. Emerging research indicates that daydreams are predominately social suggesting that daydreams may serve socio-emotional functions. Here we explore the functional role of social daydreaming for socio-emotional adjustment during an important and stressful life transition (the transition to university) using experience-sampling with 103 participants over 28 days. Over time, social daydreams increased in their positive characteristics and positive emotional outcomes; specifically, participants reported that their daydreams made them feel more socially connected and less lonely, and that the content of their daydreams became less fanciful and involved higher quality relationships. These characteristics then predicted less loneliness at the end of the study, which, in turn was associated with greater social adaptation to university. Feelings of connection resulting from social daydreams were also associated with less emotional inertia in participants who reported being less socially adapted to university. Findings indicate that social daydreaming is functional for promoting socio-emotional adjustment to an important life event. We highlight the need to consider the social content of stimulus-independent cognitions, their characteristics, and patterns of change, to specify how social thoughts enable socio-emotional adaptation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Peter Totterdell
- Department of Psychology, University of YorkYork, UK; Department of Psychology, University of SheffieldSheffield, UK
| | - Lisa-Marie Emerson
- Department of Psychology, University of YorkYork, UK; Department of Psychology, University of SheffieldSheffield, UK
| | - Eleanor Miles
- School of Psychology, University of Sussex Brighton, UK
| |
Collapse
|
80
|
Wu J, Dong D, Jackson T, Wang Y, Huang J, Chen H. The Neural Correlates of Optimistic and Depressive Tendencies of Self-Evaluations and Resting-State Default Mode Network. Front Hum Neurosci 2015; 9:618. [PMID: 26635573 PMCID: PMC4649049 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00618] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/15/2015] [Accepted: 10/26/2015] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Unrealistic optimism is common among people making self-evaluations while reduced optimism has been linked to increased depressive symptoms. Given the importance of optimism for adaptive functioning, surprisingly little is known about resting brain states underlying optimistic and depressive tendencies. In the current study, two resting-state indices were used to examine neural correlates of the default mode network (DMN) associated with optimistic and depressive self-evaluation tendencies in a non-clinical young adult sample (N = 49). The analysis was constrained due to the self-referential nature of the DMN. Across different indices, bilateral superior frontal gyri of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and bilateral superior medial frontal gyri of the dorsal medial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC) played a key role in maintaining spontaneous optimistic self-evaluative tendencies. Conversely, decreased activity in the DLPFC and bilateral medial orbitofrontal cortices (OFC) were related to accentuated depressive symptoms. Together, results highlight the pivotal roles of the DLPFC and DMPFC in mediating valences of self-referential content.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jinfeng Wu
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University Chongqing, China
| | - Debo Dong
- School of Life Science and Technology, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China Chengdu, China
| | - Todd Jackson
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University Chongqing, China
| | - Yu Wang
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University Chongqing, China
| | - Junfeng Huang
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University Chongqing, China
| | - Hong Chen
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University Chongqing, China
| |
Collapse
|
81
|
Luo Y, Kong F, Qi S, You X, Huang X. Resting-state functional connectivity of the default mode network associated with happiness. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2015; 11:516-24. [PMID: 26500289 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsv132] [Citation(s) in RCA: 61] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/16/2014] [Accepted: 10/20/2015] [Indexed: 01/05/2023] Open
Abstract
Happiness refers to people's cognitive and affective evaluation of their life. Why are some people happier than others? One reason might be that unhappy people are prone to ruminate more than happy people. The default mode network (DMN) is normally active during rest and is implicated in rumination. We hypothesized that unhappiness may be associated with increased default-mode functional connectivity during rest, including the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and inferior parietal lobule (IPL). The hyperconnectivity of these areas may be associated with higher levels of rumination. One hundred forty-eight healthy participants underwent a resting-state fMRI scan. A group-independent component analysis identified the DMNs. Results indicated increased functional connectivity in the DMN was associated with lower levels of happiness. Specifically, relative to happy people, unhappy people exhibited greater functional connectivity in the anterior medial cortex (bilateral MPFC), posterior medial cortex regions (bilateral PCC) and posterior parietal cortex (left IPL). Moreover, the increased functional connectivity of the MPFC, PCC and IPL, correlated positively with the inclination to ruminate. These results highlight the important role of the DMN in the neural correlates of happiness, and suggest that rumination may play an important role in people's perceived happiness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yangmei Luo
- Key Laboratory of Behavior and Cognitive Psychology in Shaanxi Province, School of Psychology, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi'an 710062, China Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality of Ministry of Education, School of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China
| | - Feng Kong
- State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, China
| | - Senqing Qi
- MOE Key Laboratory of Modern Teaching Technology, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi'an 710062, China
| | - Xuqun You
- Key Laboratory of Behavior and Cognitive Psychology in Shaanxi Province, School of Psychology, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi'an 710062, China
| | - Xiting Huang
- Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality of Ministry of Education, School of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China
| |
Collapse
|
82
|
Smallwood J, Schooler JW. The Science of Mind Wandering: Empirically Navigating the Stream of Consciousness. Annu Rev Psychol 2015; 66:487-518. [DOI: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015331] [Citation(s) in RCA: 785] [Impact Index Per Article: 78.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Jonathan Smallwood
- Department of Psychology, The University of York, Heslington, York YO10 5DD United Kingdom;
| | - Jonathan W. Schooler
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara, California 93106
| |
Collapse
|
83
|
Fox KCR, Thompson E, Andrews-Hanna JR, Christoff K. Is thinking really aversive? A commentary on Wilson et al.'s "Just think: the challenges of the disengaged mind". Front Psychol 2014; 5:1427. [PMID: 25538668 PMCID: PMC4260464 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01427] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/11/2014] [Accepted: 11/22/2014] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Kieran C R Fox
- Cognitive Neuroscience of Thought Laboratory, Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia Vancouver, BC, Canada
| | - Evan Thompson
- Department of Philosophy, University of British Columbia Vancouver, BC, Canada
| | | | - Kalina Christoff
- Cognitive Neuroscience of Thought Laboratory, Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia Vancouver, BC, Canada ; Brain Research Centre, University of British Columbia Vancouver, BC, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
84
|
Kaiser RH, Andrews-Hanna JR, Spielberg JM, Warren SL, Sutton BP, Miller GA, Heller W, Banich MT. Distracted and down: neural mechanisms of affective interference in subclinical depression. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2014; 10:654-63. [PMID: 25062838 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsu100] [Citation(s) in RCA: 79] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/25/2014] [Accepted: 07/17/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Previous studies have shown that depressed individuals have difficulty directing attention away from negative distractors, a phenomenon known as affective interference. However, findings are mixed regarding the neural mechanisms and network dynamics of affective interference. The present study addressed these issues by comparing neural activation during emotion-word and color-word Stroop tasks in participants with varying levels of (primarily subclinical) depression. Depressive symptoms predicted increased activation to negative distractors in areas of dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), regions implicated in cognitive control and internally directed attention, respectively. Increased dACC activity was also observed in the group-average response to incongruent distractors, suggesting that dACC activity during affective interference is related to overtaxed cognitive control. In contrast, regions of PCC were deactivated across the group in response to incongruent distractors, suggesting that PCC activity during affective interference represents task-independent processing. A psychophysiological interaction emerged in which higher depression predicted more positively correlated activity between dACC and PCC during affective interference, i.e. greater connectivity between cognitive control and internal-attention systems. These findings suggest that, when individuals high in depression are confronted by negative material, increased attention to internal thoughts and difficulty shifting resources to the external world interfere with goal-directed behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Roselinde H Kaiser
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School and McLean Hospital, Belmont, MA 02478, Department of Psychology and Beckman Institute Biomedical Imaging Center, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL 61820, Department of Psychology, University of California Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94720, Department of Psychology, Palo Alto University, Palo Alto, CA 94304, Department of Psychology, Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095 and Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, USA Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School and McLean Hospital, Belmont, MA 02478, Department of Psychology and Beckman Institute Biomedical Imaging Center, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL 61820, Department of Psychology, University of California Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94720, Department of Psychology, Palo Alto University, Palo Alto, CA 94304, Department of Psychology, Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095 and Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, USA
| | - Jessica R Andrews-Hanna
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School and McLean Hospital, Belmont, MA 02478, Department of Psychology and Beckman Institute Biomedical Imaging Center, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL 61820, Department of Psychology, University of California Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94720, Department of Psychology, Palo Alto University, Palo Alto, CA 94304, Department of Psychology, Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095 and Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, USA
| | - Jeffrey M Spielberg
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School and McLean Hospital, Belmont, MA 02478, Department of Psychology and Beckman Institute Biomedical Imaging Center, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL 61820, Department of Psychology, University of California Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94720, Department of Psychology, Palo Alto University, Palo Alto, CA 94304, Department of Psychology, Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095 and Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, USA Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School and McLean Hospital, Belmont, MA 02478, Department of Psychology and Beckman Institute Biomedical Imaging Center, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL 61820, Department of Psychology, University of California Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94720, Department of Psychology, Palo Alto University, Palo Alto, CA 94304, Department of Psychology, Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095 and Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, USA
| | - Stacie L Warren
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School and McLean Hospital, Belmont, MA 02478, Department of Psychology and Beckman Institute Biomedical Imaging Center, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL 61820, Department of Psychology, University of California Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94720, Department of Psychology, Palo Alto University, Palo Alto, CA 94304, Department of Psychology, Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095 and Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, USA Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School and McLean Hospital, Belmont, MA 02478, Department of Psychology and Beckman Institute Biomedical Imaging Center, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL 61820, Department of Psychology, University of California Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94720, Department of Psychology, Palo Alto University, Palo Alto, CA 94304, Department of Psychology, Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095 and Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, USA
| | - Bradley P Sutton
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School and McLean Hospital, Belmont, MA 02478, Department of Psychology and Beckman Institute Biomedical Imaging Center, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL 61820, Department of Psychology, University of California Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94720, Department of Psychology, Palo Alto University, Palo Alto, CA 94304, Department of Psychology, Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095 and Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, USA
| | - Gregory A Miller
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School and McLean Hospital, Belmont, MA 02478, Department of Psychology and Beckman Institute Biomedical Imaging Center, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL 61820, Department of Psychology, University of California Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94720, Department of Psychology, Palo Alto University, Palo Alto, CA 94304, Department of Psychology, Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095 and Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, USA Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School and McLean Hospital, Belmont, MA 02478, Department of Psychology and Beckman Institute Biomedical Imaging Center, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL 61820, Department of Psychology, University of California Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94720, Department of Psychology, Palo Alto University, Palo Alto, CA 94304, Department of Psychology, Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095 and Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, USA Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School and McLean Hospital, Belmont, MA 02478, Department of Psychology and Beckman Institute Biomedical Imaging Center, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL 61820, Department of Psychology, University of California Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94720, Department of Psychology, Palo Alto University, Palo Alto, CA 94304, Department of Psychology, Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California Los Angeles, Los Ang
| | - Wendy Heller
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School and McLean Hospital, Belmont, MA 02478, Department of Psychology and Beckman Institute Biomedical Imaging Center, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL 61820, Department of Psychology, University of California Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94720, Department of Psychology, Palo Alto University, Palo Alto, CA 94304, Department of Psychology, Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095 and Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, USA
| | - Marie T Banich
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School and McLean Hospital, Belmont, MA 02478, Department of Psychology and Beckman Institute Biomedical Imaging Center, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL 61820, Department of Psychology, University of California Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94720, Department of Psychology, Palo Alto University, Palo Alto, CA 94304, Department of Psychology, Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095 and Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, USA Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School and McLean Hospital, Belmont, MA 02478, Department of Psychology and Beckman Institute Biomedical Imaging Center, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL 61820, Department of Psychology, University of California Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94720, Department of Psychology, Palo Alto University, Palo Alto, CA 94304, Department of Psychology, Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095 and Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309, USA
| |
Collapse
|
85
|
Gorgolewski KJ, Lurie D, Urchs S, Kipping JA, Craddock RC, Milham MP, Margulies DS, Smallwood J. A correspondence between individual differences in the brain's intrinsic functional architecture and the content and form of self-generated thoughts. PLoS One 2014; 9:e97176. [PMID: 24824880 PMCID: PMC4019564 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0097176] [Citation(s) in RCA: 113] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/09/2013] [Accepted: 04/16/2014] [Indexed: 12/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Although neural activity often reflects the processing of external inputs, intrinsic fluctuations in activity have been observed throughout the brain. These may relate to patterns of self-generated thought that can occur while not performing goal-driven tasks. To understand the relationship between self-generated mental activity and intrinsic neural fluctuations, we developed the New York Cognition Questionnaire (NYC-Q) to assess the content and form of an individual's experiences during the acquisition of resting-state fMRI data. The data were collected as a part of the Nathan Kline Rockland Enhanced sample. We decomposed NYC-Q scores using exploratory factor analysis and found that self-reported thoughts clustered into distinct dimensions of content (future related, past related, positive, negative, and social) and form (words, images, and specificity). We used these components to perform an individual difference analysis exploring how differences in the types of self-generated thoughts relate to whole brain measures of intrinsic brain activity (fractional amplitude of low frequency fluctuations, regional homogeneity, and degree centrality). We found patterns of self-generated thoughts related to changes that were distributed across a wide range of cortical areas. For example, individuals who reported greater imagery exhibited greater low frequency fluctuations in a region of perigenual cingulate cortex, a region that is known to participate in the so-called default-mode network. We also found certain forms of thought were associated with other areas, such as primary visual cortex, the insula, and the cerebellum. For example, individuals who reported greater future thought exhibited less homogeneous neural fluctuations in a region of lateral occipital cortex, a result that is consistent with the claim that particular types of self-generated thought depend on processes that are decoupled from sensory processes. These data provide evidence that self-generated thought is a heterogeneous category of experience and that studying its content can be helpful in understanding brain dynamics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Krzysztof J. Gorgolewski
- Max Planck Research Group: Neuroanatomy and Connectivity, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Dan Lurie
- Child Mind Institute, New York, New York, United States of America
| | - Sebastian Urchs
- Max Planck Research Group: Neuroanatomy and Connectivity, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Judy A. Kipping
- Department of Neurology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| | - R. Cameron Craddock
- Child Mind Institute, New York, New York, United States of America
- Nathan Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, Orangeburg, New York, United States of America
| | - Michael P. Milham
- Child Mind Institute, New York, New York, United States of America
- Nathan Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, Orangeburg, New York, United States of America
| | - Daniel S. Margulies
- Max Planck Research Group: Neuroanatomy and Connectivity, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Jonathan Smallwood
- Department of Psychology, University of York, Hesslington, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
86
|
Andrews-Hanna JR, Smallwood J, Spreng RN. The default network and self-generated thought: component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2014; 1316:29-52. [PMID: 24502540 DOI: 10.1111/nyas.12360] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1257] [Impact Index Per Article: 114.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/14/2022]
Abstract
Though only a decade has elapsed since the default network (DN) was first defined as a large-scale brain system, recent years have brought great insight into the network's adaptive functions. A growing theme highlights the DN as playing a key role in internally directed or self-generated thought. Here, we synthesize recent findings from cognitive science, neuroscience, and clinical psychology to focus attention on two emerging topics as current and future directions surrounding the DN. First, we present evidence that self-generated thought is a multifaceted construct whose component processes are supported by different subsystems within the network. Second, we highlight the dynamic nature of the DN, emphasizing its interaction with executive control systems when regulating aspects of internal thought. We conclude by discussing clinical implications of disruptions to the integrity of the network, and consider disorders when thought content becomes polarized or network interactions become disrupted or imbalanced.
Collapse
|
87
|
Andrews-Hanna JR, Saxe R, Yarkoni T. Contributions of episodic retrieval and mentalizing to autobiographical thought: evidence from functional neuroimaging, resting-state connectivity, and fMRI meta-analyses. Neuroimage 2014; 91:324-35. [PMID: 24486981 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.032] [Citation(s) in RCA: 212] [Impact Index Per Article: 19.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/02/2013] [Revised: 01/04/2014] [Accepted: 01/18/2014] [Indexed: 10/25/2022] Open
Abstract
A growing number of studies suggest the brain's "default network" becomes engaged when individuals recall their personal past or simulate their future. Recent reports of heterogeneity within the network raise the possibility that these autobiographical processes comprised of multiple component processes, each supported by distinct functional-anatomic subsystems. We previously hypothesized that a medial temporal subsystem contributes to autobiographical memory and future thought by enabling individuals to retrieve prior information and bind this information into a mental scene. Conversely, a dorsal medial subsystem was proposed to support social-reflective aspects of autobiographical thought, allowing individuals to reflect on the mental states of one's self and others (i.e. "mentalizing"). To test these hypotheses, we first examined activity in the default network subsystems as participants performed two commonly employed tasks of episodic retrieval and mentalizing. In a subset of participants, relationships among task-evoked regions were examined at rest, in the absence of an overt task. Finally, large-scale fMRI meta-analyses were conducted to identify brain regions that most strongly predicted the presence of episodic retrieval and mentalizing, and these results were compared to meta-analyses of autobiographical tasks. Across studies, laboratory-based episodic retrieval tasks were preferentially linked to the medial temporal subsystem, while mentalizing tasks were preferentially linked to the dorsal medial subsystem. In turn, autobiographical tasks engaged aspects of both subsystems. These results suggest the default network is a heterogeneous brain system whose subsystems support distinct component processes of autobiographical thought.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jessica R Andrews-Hanna
- Institute for Cognitive Science, University of Colorado Boulder, 1777 Exposition Drive, Boulder, CO 80309, USA.
| | - Rebecca Saxe
- Dept. of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 77 Massachusetts Ave, Cambridge, MA 02139-4307, USA.
| | - Tal Yarkoni
- Institute for Cognitive Science, University of Colorado Boulder, 1777 Exposition Drive, Boulder, CO 80309, USA.
| |
Collapse
|