51
|
Waris O, Soveri A, Ahti M, Hoffing RC, Ventus D, Jaeggi SM, Seitz AR, Laine M. A Latent Factor Analysis of Working Memory Measures Using Large-Scale Data. Front Psychol 2017; 8:1062. [PMID: 28701976 PMCID: PMC5487690 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01062] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/30/2017] [Accepted: 06/08/2017] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Working memory (WM) is a key cognitive system that is strongly related to other cognitive domains and relevant for everyday life. However, the structure of WM is yet to be determined. A number of WM models have been put forth especially by factor analytical studies. In broad terms, these models vary by their emphasis on WM contents (e.g., visuospatial, verbal) vs. WM processes (e.g., maintenance, updating) as critical, dissociable elements. Here we conducted confirmatory and exploratory factor analyses on a broad set of WM tasks, half of them numerical-verbal and half of them visuospatial, representing four commonly used task paradigms: simple span, complex span, running memory, and n-back. The tasks were selected to allow the detection of both content-based (visuospatial, numerical-verbal) and process-based (maintenance, updating) divisions. The data were collected online which allowed the recruitment of a large and demographically diverse sample of adults (n = 711). Both factor analytical methods pointed to a clear division according to task content for all paradigms except n-back, while there was no indication for a process-based division. Besides the content-based division, confirmatory factor analyses supported a model that also included a general WM factor. The n-back tasks had the highest loadings on the general factor, suggesting that this factor reflected high-level cognitive resources such as executive functioning and fluid intelligence that are engaged with all WM tasks, and possibly even more so with the n-back. Together with earlier findings that indicate high variability of process-based WM divisions, we conclude that the most robust division of WM is along its contents (visuospatial vs. numerical-verbal), rather than along its hypothetical subprocesses.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Otto Waris
- Department of Psychology, Åbo Akademi UniversityTurku, Finland
| | - Anna Soveri
- Department of Psychology, Åbo Akademi UniversityTurku, Finland
- Turku Brain and Mind Center, University of TurkuTurku, Finland
| | - Miikka Ahti
- Department of Psychology, University of TurkuTurku, Finland
| | - Russell C. Hoffing
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside, RiversideCA, United States
| | - Daniel Ventus
- Department of Psychology, Åbo Akademi UniversityTurku, Finland
| | - Susanne M. Jaeggi
- School of Education, University of California, Irvine, IrvineCA, United States
| | - Aaron R. Seitz
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside, RiversideCA, United States
| | - Matti Laine
- Department of Psychology, Åbo Akademi UniversityTurku, Finland
- Turku Brain and Mind Center, University of TurkuTurku, Finland
| |
Collapse
|
52
|
Self versus informant reports on the specific levels of functioning scale: Relationships to depression and cognition in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. SCHIZOPHRENIA RESEARCH-COGNITION 2017; 9:1-7. [PMID: 28740827 PMCID: PMC5514389 DOI: 10.1016/j.scog.2017.04.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/16/2017] [Accepted: 04/20/2017] [Indexed: 12/02/2022]
Abstract
The goal of the current study was to examine the relationships between insight and both cognitive function and depression in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder, and to determine if there were similar relationships across diagnostic categories. We examined discrepancies between self and informant reports of function on the Specific levels of function scale as a metric of insight for interpersonal, social acceptance, work and activities. We examined two samples of individuals with schizophrenia and/or schizoaffective disorder (Ns of 188 and 67 respectively). In Sample 1, cognition was measured using the Dot Probe Expectancy Task. In Sample 2, cognition was measured by averaging several subtests from the MATRICS consensus cognitive battery, as well as additional measures of working memory. In both samples, depression was measured using the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale. In both samples, we found significant relationships between worse cognition and overestimations of work function, as well as between higher depression levels and underestimation of interpersonal function. These relationships were specific to interpersonal and work function, with significantly stronger correlations with interpersonal and work function compared to the other areas of function. Similar results were found across diagnostic categories. These results have important implications for treatment planning, as they suggest the need to take into account depression and cognitive function when evaluating the patient's self-report of function, and highlight the utility of informant reports in evaluating function and treatment planning. Further, they add to the literature on the similarity across schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder in a variety of pathological mechanisms.
Collapse
|
53
|
Anderson-Montoya BL, Scerbo MW, Ramirez DE, Hubbard TW. Running Memory for Clinical Handoffs: A Look at Active and Passive Processing. HUMAN FACTORS 2017; 59:393-406. [PMID: 27793979 DOI: 10.1177/0018720816672514] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/06/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE The goal of the present study was to examine the effects of domain-relevant expertise on running memory and the ability to process handoffs of information. In addition, the role of active or passive processing was examined. BACKGROUND Currently, there is little research that addresses how individuals with different levels of expertise process information in running memory when the information is needed to perform a real-world task. METHOD Three groups of participants differing in their level of clinical expertise (novice, intermediate, and expert) performed an abstract running memory span task and two tasks resembling real-world activities, a clinical handoff task and an air traffic control (ATC) handoff task. For all tasks, list length and the amount of information to be recalled were manipulated. RESULTS Regarding processing strategy, all participants used passive processing for the running memory span and ATC tasks. The novices also used passive processing for the clinical task. The experts, however, appeared to use more active processing, and the intermediates fell in between. CONCLUSION Overall, the results indicated that individuals with clinical expertise and a developed mental model rely more on active processing of incoming information for the clinical task while individuals with little or no knowledge rely on passive processing. APPLICATION The results have implications about how training should be developed to aid less experienced personnel identify what information should be included in a handoff and what should not.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Dana E Ramirez
- Children's Hospital of the King's Daughters, Norfolk, Virginia
| | | |
Collapse
|
54
|
Kranz MB, Baniqued PL, Voss MW, Lee H, Kramer AF. Examining the Roles of Reasoning and Working Memory in Predicting Casual Game Performance across Extended Gameplay. Front Psychol 2017; 8:203. [PMID: 28326042 PMCID: PMC5339312 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00203] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/14/2016] [Accepted: 01/31/2017] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
The variety and availability of casual video games presents an exciting opportunity for applications such as cognitive training. Casual games have been associated with fluid abilities such as working memory (WM) and reasoning, but the importance of these cognitive constructs in predicting performance may change across extended gameplay and vary with game structure. The current investigation examined the relationship between cognitive abilities and casual game performance over time by analyzing first and final session performance over 4-5 weeks of game play. We focused on two groups of subjects who played different types of casual games previously shown to relate to WM and reasoning when played for a single session: (1) puzzle-based games played adaptively across sessions and (2) speeded switching games played non-adaptively across sessions. Reasoning uniquely predicted first session casual game scores for both groups and accounted for much of the relationship with WM. Furthermore, over time, WM became uniquely important for predicting casual game performance for the puzzle-based adaptive games but not for the speeded switching non-adaptive games. These results extend the burgeoning literature on cognitive abilities involved in video games by showing differential relationships of fluid abilities across different game types and extended play. More broadly, the current study illustrates the usefulness of using multiple cognitive measures in predicting performance, and provides potential directions for game-based cognitive training research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael B. Kranz
- Department of Psychology, Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana ChampaignUrbana, IL, USA
| | - Pauline L. Baniqued
- Department of Psychology, Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana ChampaignUrbana, IL, USA
- Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California BerkeleyBerkeley, CA, USA
| | - Michelle W. Voss
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of IowaIowa City, IA, USA
| | - Hyunkyu Lee
- Posit Science, Brain Plasticity InstituteSan Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Arthur F. Kramer
- Department of Psychology, Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana ChampaignUrbana, IL, USA
| |
Collapse
|
55
|
Gray S, Green S, Alt M, Hogan TP, Kuo T, Brinkley S, Cowan N. The Structure of Working Memory in Young Children and Its Relation to Intelligence. JOURNAL OF MEMORY AND LANGUAGE 2017; 92:183-201. [PMID: 27990060 PMCID: PMC5157932 DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2016.06.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 90] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/14/2023]
Abstract
This study investigated the structure of working memory in young school-age children by testing the fit of three competing theoretical models using a wide variety of tasks. The best fitting models were then used to assess the relationship between working memory and nonverbal measures of fluid reasoning (Gf) and visual processing (Gv) intelligence. One hundred sixty-eight English-speaking 7-9 year olds with typical development, from three states, participated. Results showed that Cowan's three-factor embedded processes model fit the data slightly better than Baddeley and Hitch's (1974) three-factor model (specified according to Baddeley, 1986) and decisively better than Baddeley's (2000) four-factor model that included an episodic buffer. The focus of attention factor in Cowan's model was a significant predictor of Gf and Gv. The results suggest that the focus of attention, rather than storage, drives the relationship between working memory, Gf, and Gv in young school-age children. Our results do not rule out the Baddeley and Hitch model, but they place constraints on both it and Cowan's model. A common attentional component is needed for feature binding, running digit span, and visual short-term memory tasks; phonological storage is separate, as is a component of central executive processing involved in task manipulation. The results contribute to a zeitgeist in which working memory models are coming together on common ground (cf. Cowan, Saults, & Blume, 2014; Hu, Allen, Baddeley, & Hitch, 2016).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shelley Gray
- Arizona State University, PO Box 870102, Tempe, AZ, US 85287-0102
| | - Samuel Green
- Arizona State University, PO Box 870102, Tempe, AZ, US 85287-0102
| | - Mary Alt
- University of Arizona, PO Box 210071, Tucson, AZ, US 85721
| | - Tiffany P. Hogan
- MGH Institute of Health Professions, Charlestown Navy Yard, 36 1 Avenue, Boston, MA, US 02129
| | - Trudy Kuo
- Arizona State University, PO Box 870102, Tempe, AZ, US 85287-0102
| | - Shara Brinkley
- Arizona State University, PO Box 870102, Tempe, AZ, US 85287-0102
| | - Nelson Cowan
- University of Missouri–Columbia, 210 McAlester Hall, Columbia, MO, US 65211
| |
Collapse
|
56
|
Abstract
One of the reasons why working memory capacity is so widely researched is its substantial relationship with fluid intelligence. Although this relationship has been found in numerous studies, researchers have been unable to provide a conclusive answer as to why the two constructs are related. In a recent study, researchers examined which attributes of Raven's Progressive Matrices were most strongly linked with working memory capacity (Wiley, Jarosz, Cushen, & Colflesh, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 256-263, 2011). In that study, Raven's problems that required a novel combination of rules to solve were more strongly correlated with working memory capacity than were problems that did not. In the present study, we wanted to conceptually replicate the Wiley et al. results while controlling for a few potential confounds. Thus, we experimentally manipulated whether a problem required a novel combination of rules and found that repeated-rule-combination problems were more strongly related to working memory capacity than were novel-rule-combination problems. The relationship to other measures of fluid intelligence did not change based on whether the problem required a novel rule combination.
Collapse
|
57
|
On the Relation of Working Memory and Multitasking: Memory Span and Synthetic Work Performance. JOURNAL OF APPLIED RESEARCH IN MEMORY AND COGNITION 2016. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2016.05.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
58
|
Measuring working memory capacity on the web with the online working memory lab (the OWL). JOURNAL OF APPLIED RESEARCH IN MEMORY AND COGNITION 2016. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2016.07.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
|
59
|
|
60
|
Barlev M, Mermelstein S, German TC. Core Intuitions About Persons Coexist and Interfere With Acquired Christian Beliefs About God. Cogn Sci 2016; 41 Suppl 3:425-454. [PMID: 27882596 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12435] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/10/2015] [Revised: 06/04/2016] [Accepted: 06/20/2016] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
This study tested the hypothesis that in the minds of adult religious adherents, acquired beliefs about the extraordinary characteristics of God coexist with, rather than replace, an initial representation of God formed by co-option of the evolved person concept. In three experiments, Christian religious adherents were asked to evaluate a series of statements for which core intuitions about persons and acquired Christian beliefs about God were consistent (i.e., true according to both [e.g., "God has beliefs that are true"] or false according to both [e.g., "All beliefs God has are false"]) or inconsistent (i.e., true on intuition but false theologically [e.g., "God has beliefs that are false"] or false on intuition but true theologically [e.g., "All beliefs God has are true"]). Participants were less accurate and slower to respond to inconsistent versus consistent statements, suggesting that the core intuitions both coexisted alongside and interfered with the acquired beliefs (Experiments 1 and 2). In Experiment 2 when responding under time pressure participants were disproportionately more likely to make errors on inconsistent versus consistent statements than when responding with no time pressure, suggesting that the resolution of interference requires cognitive resources the functioning of which decreases under cognitive load. In Experiment 3 a plausible alternative interpretation of these findings was ruled out by demonstrating that the response accuracy and time differences on consistent versus inconsistent statements occur for God-a supernatural religious entity-but not for a natural religious entity (a priest).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael Barlev
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara
| | - Spencer Mermelstein
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara
| | - Tamsin C German
- Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara
| |
Collapse
|
61
|
Yoga practice improves executive function by attenuating stress levels. Biol Psychol 2016; 121:109-116. [PMID: 27794449 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.10.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 62] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/27/2016] [Revised: 10/17/2016] [Accepted: 10/21/2016] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Prolonged activation of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal system is thought to have deleterious effects on brain function. Neuroendocrine studies suggest that brain exposure to higher cortisol concentrations contribute to cognitive deficits as we age. Mind-body techniques such as yoga have shown to improve stress levels by restoring the body's sympathetic-parasympathetic balance. The objective of this study was to determine whether yoga practice moderated the stress response resulting in improved executive function. METHODS Sedentary community dwelling older adults (N=118, Mean age=62.02) were randomized to an 8-week yoga intervention or a stretching control group. At baseline and following 8 weeks, all participants completed measures of executive function, self-reported stress and anxiety and provided saliva samples before and after cognitive testing to assess cortisol. RESULTS Yoga participants showed improved accuracy on executive function measures and an attenuated cortisol response compared to their stretching counterparts who showed increased cortisol levels and poor cognitive performance at follow up. The change in cortisol levels as well as self-reported stress and anxiety levels predicted performance on the running span task, n-back working memory and task switching paradigm (β's=0.27-0.38, p's≤0.05 for yoga and β's=-0.37-0.47, p's≤0.01 for stretching control). CONCLUSION Eight weeks of regular yoga practice resulted in improved working memory performance that was mediated by an attenuated response to stress as measured by self-report stress and objective salivary cortisol measurements. This trial offers evidence for non-traditional physical activity interventions such as yoga that may be helpful in restoring HPA balance in older adults, thereby preventing cognitive decline.
Collapse
|
62
|
Engelhardt LE, Mann FD, Briley DA, Church JA, Harden KP, Tucker-Drob EM. Strong genetic overlap between executive functions and intelligence. J Exp Psychol Gen 2016; 145:1141-59. [PMID: 27359131 PMCID: PMC5001920 DOI: 10.1037/xge0000195] [Citation(s) in RCA: 55] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
Abstract
Executive functions (EFs) are cognitive processes that control, monitor, and coordinate more basic cognitive processes. EFs play instrumental roles in models of complex reasoning, learning, and decision making, and individual differences in EFs have been consistently linked with individual differences in intelligence. By middle childhood, genetic factors account for a moderate proportion of the variance in intelligence, and these effects increase in magnitude through adolescence. Genetic influences on EFs are very high, even in middle childhood, but the extent to which these genetic influences overlap with those on intelligence is unclear. We examined genetic and environmental overlap between EFs and intelligence in a racially and socioeconomically diverse sample of 811 twins ages 7 to 15 years (M = 10.91, SD = 1.74) from the Texas Twin Project. A general EF factor representing variance common to inhibition, switching, working memory, and updating domains accounted for substantial proportions of variance in intelligence, primarily via a genetic pathway. General EF continued to have a strong, genetically mediated association with intelligence even after controlling for processing speed. Residual variation in general intelligence was influenced only by shared and nonshared environmental factors, and there remained no genetic variance in general intelligence that was unique of EF. Genetic variance independent of EF did remain, however, in a more specific perceptual reasoning ability. These results provide evidence that genetic influences on general intelligence are highly overlapping with those on EF. (PsycINFO Database Record
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Frank D Mann
- Department of Psychology, University of Texas at Austin
| | - Daniel A Briley
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
63
|
Kane MJ, Meier ME, Smeekens BA, Gross GM, Chun CA, Silvia PJ, Kwapil TR. Individual differences in the executive control of attention, memory, and thought, and their associations with schizotypy. J Exp Psychol Gen 2016; 145:1017-1048. [PMID: 27454042 PMCID: PMC4965188 DOI: 10.1037/xge0000184] [Citation(s) in RCA: 82] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/11/2022]
Abstract
A large correlational study took a latent-variable approach to the generality of executive control by testing the individual-differences structure of executive-attention capabilities and assessing their prediction of schizotypy, a multidimensional construct (with negative, positive, disorganized, and paranoid factors) conveying risk for schizophrenia. Although schizophrenia is convincingly linked to executive deficits, the schizotypy literature is equivocal. Subjects completed tasks of working memory capacity (WMC), attention restraint (inhibiting prepotent responses), and attention constraint (focusing visual attention amid distractors), the latter 2 in an effort to fractionate the "inhibition" construct. We also assessed mind-wandering propensity (via in-task thought probes) and coefficient of variation in response times (RT CoV) from several tasks as more novel indices of executive attention. WMC, attention restraint, attention constraint, mind wandering, and RT CoV were correlated but separable constructs, indicating some distinctions among "attention control" abilities; WMC correlated more strongly with attentional restraint than constraint, and mind wandering correlated more strongly with attentional restraint, attentional constraint, and RT CoV than with WMC. Across structural models, no executive construct predicted negative schizotypy and only mind wandering and RT CoV consistently (but modestly) predicted positive, disorganized, and paranoid schizotypy; stalwart executive constructs in the schizophrenia literature-WMC and attention restraint-showed little to no predictive power, beyond restraint's prediction of paranoia. Either executive deficits are consequences rather than risk factors for schizophrenia, or executive failures barely precede or precipitate diagnosable schizophrenia symptoms. (PsycINFO Database Record
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Matt E Meier
- Department of Psychology, Western Carolina University
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
64
|
Wentink MM, Berger MAM, de Kloet AJ, Meesters J, Band GPH, Wolterbeek R, Goossens PH, Vliet Vlieland TPM. The effects of an 8-week computer-based brain training programme on cognitive functioning, QoL and self-efficacy after stroke. Neuropsychol Rehabil 2016; 26:847-65. [DOI: 10.1080/09602011.2016.1162175] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
65
|
Key-DeLyria SE, Altmann LJP. Executive Function and Ambiguous Sentence Comprehension. AMERICAN JOURNAL OF SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY 2016; 25:252-267. [PMID: 27214025 DOI: 10.1044/2015_ajslp-14-0111] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/07/2014] [Accepted: 09/13/2015] [Indexed: 06/05/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE Sentence comprehension is a critical skill in today's literate society. Recent evidence suggests that processing and comprehending language taps cognitive as well as linguistic abilities, a finding that has critical import for clinicians who have clients with language disorders. To promote awareness of the impact of cognition, especially executive function (EF) and working memory (WM), this opinion article presents current views of how sentences are processed and links the various steps of the process to specific EF and WM subcomponents. METHOD The article focuses on ambiguous sentences, pointing out the similar types of processing needed when resolving an ambiguity and performing EF tasks. RESULTS We discuss the potential overlap between the neurobiology of sentence processing and EF and the evidence supporting a link between EF and sentence processes. CONCLUSION Awareness of the potential role of EF and WM in sentence comprehension will help clinicians be more aware of potential cognitive-linguistic deficits in their clients. Future research will help to clarify the link between EF and sentence comprehension.
Collapse
|
66
|
Lynn SK, Ibagon C, Bui E, Palitz SA, Simon NM, Barrett LF. Working memory capacity is associated with optimal adaptation of response bias to perceptual sensitivity in emotion perception. Emotion 2016; 16:155-63. [PMID: 26461251 PMCID: PMC5579757 DOI: 10.1037/emo0000111] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Emotion perception, inferring the emotional state of another person, is a frequent judgment made under perceptual uncertainty (e.g., a scowling facial expression can indicate anger or concentration) and behavioral risk (e.g., incorrect judgment can be costly to the perceiver). Working memory capacity (WMC), the ability to maintain controlled processing in the face of competing demands, is an important component of many decisions. We investigated the association of WMC and anger perception in a task in which "angry" and "not angry" categories comprised overlapping ranges of scowl intensity, and correct and incorrect responses earned and lost points, respectively. Participants attempted to earn as many points as they could; adopting an optimal response bias would maximize decision utility. Participants with higher WMC more optimally tuned their anger perception response bias to accommodate their perceptual sensitivity (their ability to discriminate the categories) than did participants with lower WMC. Other factors that influence response bias (i.e., the relative base rate of angry vs. not angry faces and the decision costs and benefits) were ruled out as contributors to the WMC-bias relationship. Our results suggest that WMC optimizes emotion perception by contributing to perceivers' ability to adjust their response bias to account for their level of perceptual sensitivity, likely an important component of adapting emotion perception to dynamic social interactions and changing circumstances. (PsycINFO Database Record
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Eric Bui
- Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital
| | | | - Naomi M Simon
- Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital
| | | |
Collapse
|
67
|
Weerts AP, Pattyn N, Putcha L, Hoag SW, Van Ombergen A, Hallgren E, Van de Heyning PH, Wuyts FL. Restricted sedation and absence of cognitive impairments after administration of intranasal scopolamine. J Psychopharmacol 2015; 29:1231-5. [PMID: 26268532 DOI: 10.1177/0269881115598414] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Space motion sickness in astronauts during spaceflight causes significant discomfort, which might impede their functionality. Pharmacological treatment has been mainly restricted to promethazine. Transdermal and oral scopolamine have also been used in space; however, their use was reduced due to unpredictable effectiveness and side effects. Recently, intranasal scopolamine administration has gained much interest, since this route ensures fast and reliable absorption with a decreased incidence of undesirable side effects. The aim of this study was to evaluate the effect of intranasal scopolamine on cognitive performance and to determine its side effects. METHODS This double-blind, placebo controlled, repeated measures study evaluated vigilant attention, short-term memory, implicit memory and working memory. Side effects were reported on a 22-item questionnaire and sleepiness was assessed by the Karolinska, Stanford and Epworth Sleepiness Scales. RESULTS Scopolamine had no effect on cognitive function. Only the Karolinska score was significantly increased for scopolamine compared to placebo. Participants reported a dry mouth and dizziness after receiving scopolamine. DISCUSSION Results show that intranasal scopolamine did not impair cognitive performance. Intranasal scopolamine might be a good alternative to promethazine for the alleviation of space motion sickness, since the agent has minimal sedative effects and does not hamper cognitive performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aurélie P Weerts
- Antwerp University Research Centre for Equilibrium and Aerospace (AUREA), Antwerp University Hospital and University of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium
| | - Nathalie Pattyn
- Research Unit VIPER, Department LIFE, Royal Military Academy, Brussels, Belgium Research Unit Biological Psychology, Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium
| | - Lakshmi Putcha
- Biomedical Operations and Research Branch, NASA - Johnson Space Center, Houston, TX, USA
| | - Stephen W Hoag
- School of Pharmacy, Department of Pharmaceutical Sciences, University of Maryland, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Angelique Van Ombergen
- Antwerp University Research Centre for Equilibrium and Aerospace (AUREA), Antwerp University Hospital and University of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium
| | - Emma Hallgren
- Antwerp University Research Centre for Equilibrium and Aerospace (AUREA), Antwerp University Hospital and University of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium
| | - Paul H Van de Heyning
- Antwerp University Research Centre for Equilibrium and Aerospace (AUREA), Antwerp University Hospital and University of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium
| | - Floris L Wuyts
- Antwerp University Research Centre for Equilibrium and Aerospace (AUREA), Antwerp University Hospital and University of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
68
|
Baniqued PL, Allen CM, Kranz MB, Johnson K, Sipolins A, Dickens C, Ward N, Geyer A, Kramer AF. Working Memory, Reasoning, and Task Switching Training: Transfer Effects, Limitations, and Great Expectations? PLoS One 2015; 10:e0142169. [PMID: 26555341 PMCID: PMC4640538 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0142169] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/28/2015] [Accepted: 10/19/2015] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Although some studies have shown that cognitive training can produce improvements to untrained cognitive domains (far transfer), many others fail to show these effects, especially when it comes to improving fluid intelligence. The current study was designed to overcome several limitations of previous training studies by incorporating training expectancy assessments, an active control group, and "Mind Frontiers," a video game-based mobile program comprised of six adaptive, cognitively demanding training tasks that have been found to lead to increased scores in fluid intelligence (Gf) tests. We hypothesize that such integrated training may lead to broad improvements in cognitive abilities by targeting aspects of working memory, executive function, reasoning, and problem solving. Ninety participants completed 20 hour-and-a-half long training sessions over four to five weeks, 45 of whom played Mind Frontiers and 45 of whom completed visual search and change detection tasks (active control). After training, the Mind Frontiers group improved in working memory n-back tests, a composite measure of perceptual speed, and a composite measure of reaction time in reasoning tests. No training-related improvements were found in reasoning accuracy or other working memory tests, nor in composite measures of episodic memory, selective attention, divided attention, and multi-tasking. Perceived self-improvement in the tested abilities did not differ between groups. A general expectancy difference in problem-solving was observed between groups, but this perceived benefit did not correlate with training-related improvement. In summary, although these findings provide modest evidence regarding the efficacy of an integrated cognitive training program, more research is needed to determine the utility of Mind Frontiers as a cognitive training tool.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pauline L. Baniqued
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
| | - Courtney M. Allen
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
| | - Michael B. Kranz
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
| | - Kathryn Johnson
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
| | - Aldis Sipolins
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
| | - Charles Dickens
- Aptima, Inc., Woburn, Massachusetts, United States of America
| | - Nathan Ward
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
| | - Alexandra Geyer
- Aptima, Inc., Woburn, Massachusetts, United States of America
| | - Arthur F. Kramer
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
69
|
De Putter LMS, Vanderhasselt MA, Baeken C, De Raedt R, Koster EHW. Combining tDCS and Working Memory Training to Down Regulate State Rumination: A Single-Session Double Blind Sham-Controlled Trial. COGNITIVE THERAPY AND RESEARCH 2015. [DOI: 10.1007/s10608-015-9710-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
70
|
Engelhardt LE, Briley DA, Mann FD, Harden KP, Tucker-Drob EM. Genes Unite Executive Functions in Childhood. Psychol Sci 2015; 26:1151-63. [PMID: 26246520 DOI: 10.1177/0956797615577209] [Citation(s) in RCA: 79] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/02/2014] [Accepted: 02/22/2015] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Individual differences in children's executive functions (EFs) are relevant for a wide range of normal and atypical psychological outcomes across the life span, but the origins of variation in children's EFs are not well understood. We used data from a racially and socioeconomically diverse sample of 505 third- through eighth-grade twins and triplets from the Texas Twin Project to estimate genetic and environmental influences on a Common EF factor and on variance unique to four core EF domains: inhibition, switching, working memory, and updating. As has been previously demonstrated in young adults, the Common EF factor was 100% heritable, which indicates that correlations among the four EF domains are entirely attributable to shared genetic etiology. Nonshared environmental influences were evident for variance unique to individual domains. General EF may thus serve as an early life marker of genetic propensity for a range of functions and pathologies later in life.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Daniel A Briley
- Department of Psychology Population Research Center, University of Texas at Austin
| | | | - K Paige Harden
- Department of Psychology Population Research Center, University of Texas at Austin
| | - Elliot M Tucker-Drob
- Department of Psychology Population Research Center, University of Texas at Austin
| |
Collapse
|
71
|
Fairfield B, Mammarella N, Di Domenico A, Palumbo R. Running with emotion: When affective content hampers working memory performance. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY 2014; 50:161-4. [DOI: 10.1002/ijop.12101] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/28/2014] [Revised: 08/22/2014] [Accepted: 08/29/2014] [Indexed: 11/12/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Beth Fairfield
- Department of Psychological Sciences; University of Chieti; Italy
| | | | | | - Rocco Palumbo
- Department of Psychological Sciences; University of Chieti; Italy
| |
Collapse
|
72
|
Gothe NP, Kramer AF, McAuley E. The effects of an 8-week Hatha yoga intervention on executive function in older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2014; 69:1109-16. [PMID: 25024234 PMCID: PMC4202261 DOI: 10.1093/gerona/glu095] [Citation(s) in RCA: 77] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/19/2013] [Accepted: 05/20/2014] [Indexed: 01/14/2023] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Few scientific studies have examined movement-based embodied contemplative practices such as yoga and their effects on cognition. The purpose of this randomized controlled trial was to examine the effects of an 8-week Hatha yoga intervention on executive function measures of task switching and working memory capacity. METHODS Community-dwelling older adults (N = 118; mean age = 62.0) were randomized to one of two groups: a Hatha yoga intervention or a stretching-strengthening control. Both groups participated in hour-long exercise classes 3×/week over the 8-week study period. All participants completed established tests of executive function including the task switching paradigm, n-back and running memory span at baseline and follow-up. RESULTS Analysis of covariances showed significantly shorter reaction times on the mixed and repeat task switching trials (partial η(2) = .04, p < .05) for the Hatha yoga group. Higher accuracy was recorded on the single trials (partial η(2) = .05, p < .05), the 2-back condition of the n-back (partial η(2) = .08, p < .001), and partial recall scores (partial η(2) = .06, p < .01) of running span task. CONCLUSIONS Following 8 weeks of yoga practice, participants in the yoga intervention group showed significantly improved performance on the executive function measures of working memory capacity and efficiency of mental set shifting and flexibility compared with their stretching-strengthening counterparts. Although the underlying mechanisms need to be investigated, these results demand larger systematic trials to thoroughly examine effects of yoga on executive function as well as across other domains of cognition, and its potential to maintain or improve cognitive functioning in the aging process.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Neha P Gothe
- Department of Kinesiology and Community Health, Present address: Division of Kinesiology, Health and Sport Studies, Wayne State University, Detroit, Michigan.
| | - Arthur F Kramer
- Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, and Department of Psychology, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign
| | - Edward McAuley
- Department of Kinesiology and Community Health, Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, and
| |
Collapse
|
73
|
Lucidi A, Loaiza V, Camos V, Barrouillet P. Assessing Working Memory Capacity Through Time-Constrained Elementary Activities. The Journal of General Psychology 2014; 141:98-112. [DOI: 10.1080/00221309.2013.870121] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
74
|
Haier RJ. Increased intelligence is a myth (so far). Front Syst Neurosci 2014; 8:34. [PMID: 24659957 PMCID: PMC3950413 DOI: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00034] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/29/2014] [Accepted: 02/23/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Richard J Haier
- Emeritus, Pediatrics, School of Medicine, University of California Irvine, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
75
|
Abstract
Cognitive symptoms of depression, such as rumination, have shown to be associated with deficits in working memory functioning. More precisely, the capacity to expel irrelevant negative information from working memory seems to be affected. Even though these associations have repeatedly been demonstrated, the nature and causal direction of this association is still unclear. Therefore, within an experimental design, we tried to manipulate working memory functioning of participants with heightened rumination scores in two similar experiments (n = 72 and n = 45) using a six day working memory training compared to active and passive control groups. Subsequently the effects on the processing of non-emotional and emotional information in working memory were monitored. In both experiments, performance during the training task significantly increased, but this performance gain did not transfer to the outcome working memory tasks or rumination and depression measures. Possible explanations for the failure to find transfer effects are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas Onraedt
- Department of Experimental-Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
| | - Ernst H. W. Koster
- Department of Experimental-Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
76
|
Abstract
A total of 1,734 adults performed two running memory tasks and a battery of cognitive tests representing four cognitive abilities. Simultaneous analyses were used to identify unique relations of each cognitive ability, including fluid intelligence, on the running memory measures. The large sample size allowed powerful analyses of the relations at the level of individual trials, separate list lengths, and different serial positions. The results indicated that the relations of running memory performance with cognitive abilities were remarkably constant from the first to the last trial, across different list lengths, and on successive input positions. It is proposed that an important aspect of fluid intelligence is the ability to cope with novelty and complexity, and that running memory tasks may merely be one of many ways in which those processes can be operationalized.
Collapse
|
77
|
Roll M, Gosselke S, Lindgren M, Horne M. Time-driven effects on processing grammatical agreement. Front Psychol 2014; 4:1004. [PMID: 24416027 PMCID: PMC3874852 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.01004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/29/2013] [Accepted: 12/17/2013] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
“Agreement” is a grammatical relation between words; e.g., the verbal suffix –s reflects agreement with a singular subject (He run-s). Previous studies with time intervals under 2.5 s between disagreeing words have found a left-lateralized negative brain potential, arguably reflecting detection of the morphosyntactic violation. We tested the neurophysiological effects of number agreement between the first and last word in sentences at temporal distances between 1.75 and 3.25 s. Distances were varied by visually presenting sentences word by word at different rates. For distances under 2.5 s, a left-lateralized negativity was observed. At a 3.25-s interval, an anterior, slightly right-lateralized negativity was found. At an intermediate distance of 2.75 s, the difference between disagreement and agreement at left electrodes correlated with participants' working memory span. Results indicate that different brain processes occur when agreement involves agreement domains approaching and exceeding 3 s than when the agreement dependency involves shorter temporal intervals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mikael Roll
- Department of Linguistics and Phonetics, Lund University Lund, Sweden
| | - Sabine Gosselke
- Department of Linguistics and Phonetics, Lund University Lund, Sweden
| | | | - Merle Horne
- Department of Linguistics and Phonetics, Lund University Lund, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
78
|
Baniqued PL, Kranz MB, Voss MW, Lee H, Cosman JD, Severson J, Kramer AF. Cognitive training with casual video games: points to consider. Front Psychol 2014; 4:1010. [PMID: 24432009 PMCID: PMC3882717 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.01010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 64] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/29/2013] [Accepted: 12/17/2013] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Brain training programs have proliferated in recent years, with claims that video games or computer-based tasks can broadly enhance cognitive function. However, benefits are commonly seen only in trained tasks. Assessing generalized improvement and practicality of laboratory exercises complicates interpretation and application of findings. In this study, we addressed these issues by using active control groups, training tasks that more closely resemble real-world demands and multiple tests to determine transfer of training. We examined whether casual video games can broadly improve cognition, and selected training games from a study of the relationship between game performance and cognitive abilities. A total of 209 young adults were randomized into a working memory–reasoning group, an adaptive working memory–reasoning group, an active control game group, and a no-contact control group. Before and after 15 h of training, participants completed tests of reasoning, working memory, attention, episodic memory, perceptual speed, and self-report measures of executive function, game experience, perceived improvement, knowledge of brain training research, and game play outside the laboratory. Participants improved on the training games, but transfer to untrained tasks was limited. No group showed gains in reasoning, working memory, episodic memory, or perceptual speed, but the working memory–reasoning groups improved in divided attention, with better performance in an attention-demanding game, a decreased attentional blink and smaller trail-making costs. Perceived improvements did not differ across training groups and those with low reasoning ability at baseline showed larger gains. Although there are important caveats, our study sheds light on the mixed effects in the training and transfer literature and offers a novel and potentially practical training approach. Still, more research is needed to determine the real-world benefits of computer programs such as casual games.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pauline L Baniqued
- Department of Psychology, Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign Urbana, IL, USA
| | - Michael B Kranz
- Department of Psychology, Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign Urbana, IL, USA
| | - Michelle W Voss
- Department of Psychology, University of Iowa Iowa City, IA, USA
| | - Hyunkyu Lee
- Brain Plasticity Institute San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Joshua D Cosman
- Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University Nashville, TN, USA
| | | | - Arthur F Kramer
- Department of Psychology, Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign Urbana, IL, USA
| |
Collapse
|
79
|
Cinan S, Doğan A. Working Memory, Mental Prospection, Time Orientation, and Cognitive Insight. JOURNAL OF INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2013. [DOI: 10.1027/1614-0001/a000111] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/15/2022]
Abstract
This research is new in its attempt to take future time orientation, morningness orientation, and prospective memory as measures of mental prospection, and to examine a three-factor model that assumes working memory, mental prospection, and cognitive insight are independent but related higher-order cognitive constructs by using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). The three-factor model produced a good fit to the data. An alternative one-factor model was tested and rejected. The results suggest that working memory and cognitive insight are distinguishable, related constructs, and that both are distinct from, but negatively associated with, mental prospection. In addition, structural equation modeling (SEM) showed that working memory had a strong positive effect on cognitive insight and a moderate negative effect on mental prospection.
Collapse
|
80
|
|
81
|
|
82
|
Uittenhove K, Lemaire P. Strategy sequential difficulty effects vary with working-memory and response-stimulus-intervals: a study in arithmetic. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2013; 143:113-8. [PMID: 23558154 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.02.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/25/2012] [Revised: 01/30/2013] [Accepted: 02/22/2013] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
Abstract
Strategy sequential difficulty effects are the findings that when participants execute strategies, performance is worse after a difficult strategy than after an easy strategy (Uittenhove & Lemaire, 2012). Strategy sequential difficulty effects are hypothesized to result from decreased working-memory resources following difficult strategy execution. In the present study we found a correlation between individuals' working memory and strategy sequential difficulty effects in arithmetic, supporting a working-memory account of these effects. Furthermore, we varied response-stimulus intervals, and we found decreased strategy sequential difficulty effects with increasing response-stimulus intervals. Implications of these findings for further understanding of strategic variations in human cognition are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kim Uittenhove
- Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique and Aix-Marseille Université, Marseille, France
| | | |
Collapse
|
83
|
|
84
|
Cogmed working memory training: Does the evidence support the claims? JOURNAL OF APPLIED RESEARCH IN MEMORY AND COGNITION 2012. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2012.06.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 168] [Impact Index Per Article: 14.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
|
85
|
Shipstead Z, Redick TS, Hicks KL, Engle RW. The scope and control of attention as separate aspects of working memory. Memory 2012; 20:608-28. [PMID: 22734653 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2012.691519] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
Abstract
The present study examines two varieties of working memory (WM) capacity task: visual arrays (i.e., a measure of the amount of information that can be maintained in working memory) and complex span (i.e., a task that taps WM-related attentional control). Using previously collected data sets we employ confirmatory factor analysis to demonstrate that visual arrays and complex span tasks load on separate, but correlated, factors. A subsequent series of structural equation models and regression analyses demonstrate that these factors contribute both common and unique variance to the prediction of general fluid intelligence (Gf). However, while visual arrays does contribute uniquely to higher cognition, its overall correlation to Gf is largely mediated by variance associated with the complex span factor. Thus we argue that visual arrays performance is not strictly driven by a limited-capacity storage system (e.g., the focus of attention; Cowan, 2001), but may also rely on control processes such as selective attention and controlled memory search.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zach Shipstead
- School of Psychology, Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, GA 30332, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
86
|
Drummond SPA, Anderson DE, Straus LD, Vogel EK, Perez VB. The effects of two types of sleep deprivation on visual working memory capacity and filtering efficiency. PLoS One 2012; 7:e35653. [PMID: 22530064 PMCID: PMC3329471 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0035653] [Citation(s) in RCA: 68] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/14/2011] [Accepted: 03/22/2012] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
Sleep deprivation has adverse consequences for a variety of cognitive functions. The exact effects of sleep deprivation, though, are dependent upon the cognitive process examined. Within working memory, for example, some component processes are more vulnerable to sleep deprivation than others. Additionally, the differential impacts on cognition of different types of sleep deprivation have not been well studied. The aim of this study was to examine the effects of one night of total sleep deprivation and 4 nights of partial sleep deprivation (4 hours in bed/night) on two components of visual working memory: capacity and filtering efficiency. Forty-four healthy young adults were randomly assigned to one of the two sleep deprivation conditions. All participants were studied: 1) in a well-rested condition (following 6 nights of 9 hours in bed/night); and 2) following sleep deprivation, in a counter-balanced order. Visual working memory testing consisted of two related tasks. The first measured visual working memory capacity and the second measured the ability to ignore distractor stimuli in a visual scene (filtering efficiency). Results showed neither type of sleep deprivation reduced visual working memory capacity. Partial sleep deprivation also generally did not change filtering efficiency. Total sleep deprivation, on the other hand, did impair performance in the filtering task. These results suggest components of visual working memory are differentially vulnerable to the effects of sleep deprivation, and different types of sleep deprivation impact visual working memory to different degrees. Such findings have implications for operational settings where individuals may need to perform with inadequate sleep and whose jobs involve receiving an array of visual information and discriminating the relevant from the irrelevant prior to making decisions or taking actions (e.g., baggage screeners, air traffic controllers, military personnel, health care providers).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sean P A Drummond
- Psychology Service, VA San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, California, United States of America.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
87
|
Redick TS, Broadway JM, Meier ME, Kuriakose PS, Unsworth N, Kane MJ, Engle RW. Measuring Working Memory Capacity With Automated Complex Span Tasks. EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL ASSESSMENT 2012. [DOI: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000123] [Citation(s) in RCA: 245] [Impact Index Per Article: 20.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
Individual differences in working memory capacity are related to a variety of behaviors both within and outside of the lab. Recently developed automated complex span tasks have contributed to increasing our knowledge concerning working memory capacity by making valid and reliable assessments freely available for use by researchers. Combining the samples from three testing locations yielded data from over 6,000 young adult participants who performed at least one of three such tasks (Operation, Symmetry, and Reading Span). Normative data are presented here for researchers interested in applying cutoffs for their own applications, and information on the validity and reliability of the tasks is also reported. In addition, the data were analyzed as a function of sex and college status. While automated complex span tasks are just one way to measure working memory capacity, the use of a standardized procedure for administration and scoring greatly facilitates comparison across studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Matt E. Meier
- University of North Carolina Greensboro, Greensboro, NC, USA
| | | | | | - Michael J. Kane
- University of North Carolina Greensboro, Greensboro, NC, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
88
|
Fiore F, Borella E, Mammarella IC, De Beni R. Age differences in verbal and visuo-spatial working memory updating: evidence from analysis of serial position curves. Memory 2011; 20:14-27. [PMID: 22133192 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2011.628320] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/14/2022]
Abstract
Memory updating is the ability to select and update relevant information and suppress no-longer-relevant data. The few studies in this area, targeting mainly the verbal domain, have investigated and confirmed an age-related decline in working memory updating ability (De Beni & Palladino, 2004; Van der Linden, Bredart, & Beerten, 1994). The present research examines the ability of younger and older adults to update information in verbal and visuo-spatial running memory tasks. Results showed that the participants' performance was higher in the verbal than in the visuo-spatial task. Nonetheless, independently of the task domain, an age-related decline in updating performance was found. Moreover, analysis of serial positions suggested that, in the updating procedure, the participants were not attempting to actively maintain items, preferring to adopt a low-effort, "recency-based" strategy. The use of this type of strategy is more evident in older participants, as shown in both the accuracy performance and the proportion of intrusion errors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Felicia Fiore
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, Padua, Italy.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
89
|
Broadway JM, Engle RW. Individual differences in working memory capacity and temporal discrimination. PLoS One 2011; 6:e25422. [PMID: 22003391 PMCID: PMC3189201 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0025422] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/14/2011] [Accepted: 09/05/2011] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Temporal judgment in the milliseconds-to-seconds range depends on consistent attention to time and robust working memory representation. Individual differences in working memory capacity (WMC) predict a wide range of higher-order and lower-order cognitive abilities. In the present work we examined whether WMC would predict temporal discrimination. High-WMC individuals were more sensitive than low-WMC at discriminating the longer of two temporal intervals across a range of temporal differences. WMC-related individual differences in temporal discrimination were not eliminated by including a measure of fluid intelligence as a covariate. Results are discussed in terms of attention, working memory and other psychological constructs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- James M Broadway
- Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, Georgia, United States of America.
| | | |
Collapse
|
90
|
Abstract
Some evidence suggests that positive mood influences cognitive control. The current research investigated whether positive mood has differential effects on two aspects of cognitive control, working memory and prepotent response inhibition. In Study 1, following either a positive or neutral mood induction, participants completed the Running Memory Span (RMS), a measure primarily of working memory storage capacity, and the Stroop task, a measure of prepotent response inhibition. Results were that the positive mood group performed worse on the RMS task but not on the Stroop task. In Study 2, participants completed the RMS and another measure of prepotent response inhibition, the Flanker task. Results were that when in a positive mood state participants performed worse on the RMS but not on the Flanker task. Overall, this research suggests that positive mood has differential effects on cognitive control, impairing working memory but having no effect on prepotent response inhibition.
Collapse
|
91
|
Broadway JM, Engle RW. Lapsed attention to elapsed time? Individual differences in working memory capacity and temporal reproduction. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2011; 137:115-26. [PMID: 21470583 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.03.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/05/2010] [Revised: 03/08/2011] [Accepted: 03/14/2011] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
Working memory capacity (WMC) predicts individual differences in a wide range of mental abilities. In three experiments we examined whether WMC would predict temporal judgment. Low-WMC temporal reproductions were consistently too long for the shortest duration and too short for the longest, but were accurate (unbiased) for the intermediate. In contrast, high-WMC temporal reproductions were more accurate (unbiased) across the range. Thus low-WMC showed a classic "migration effect" (Vierordt's Law) to a greater extent than high-WMC. Furthermore reproduction errors depended more on temporal context than the absolute durations of "shortest," "longest," and "intermediate." Low-WMC reproductions were overall more variable than high-WMC. General fluid intelligence (gF) was also related to temporal bias and variability. However, WMC-related timing differences were only attenuated and not eliminated with gF as covariate. Results are discussed in terms of attention, memory, and other psychological constructs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- James M Broadway
- School of Psychology, Georgia Institute of Technology, United States.
| | | |
Collapse
|