51
|
Aging effect of picture naming in Chinese: The influence of the non-selective inhibition ability. ACTA PSYCHOLOGICA SINICA 2019. [DOI: 10.3724/sp.j.1041.2019.01079] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
52
|
Omata K, Ito S, Takata Y, Ouchi Y. Similar Neural Correlates of Planning and Execution to Inhibit Continuing Actions. Front Neurosci 2018; 12:951. [PMID: 30631263 PMCID: PMC6315197 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00951] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/22/2018] [Accepted: 11/30/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Inhibition of action is involved in stopping a movement, as well as terminating unnecessary movement during performance of a behavior. The inhibition of single actions, known as response inhibition (Inhibition of the urge to respond before or after actions) has been widely investigated using the go/no-go task and stop signal task. However, few studies focused on phase and volition-related inhibition after an action has been initiated. Here, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate the neural correlates of planning and execution underlying the voluntary inhibition of ongoing action. We collected fMRI data while participants performed a continuous finger-tapping task involving voluntary and involuntary (externally directed) inhibition, and during the initiation of movement. The results revealed areas of significantly greater activation during the preparation of inhibition of an ongoing action during voluntary inhibition, compared with involuntary inhibition, in the supplementary (SMA) and pre-supplementary motor areas, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), inferior parietal lobe, bilateral globus pallidus/putamen, bilateral insula and premotor cortex. Focusing on the period of execution of inhibition of ongoing actions, an event-related fMRI analysis revealed significant activation in the SMA, middle cingulate cortex, bilateral insula, right IFG and inferior parietal cortex. Additional comparative analyses suggested that brain activation while participants were planning to inhibit an ongoing action was similar to that during planning to start an action, indicating that the same neural substrates of motor planning may be recruited even when an action is ongoing. The present finding that brain activation associated with inhibiting ongoing actions was compatible with that seen in response inhibition (urge to stop before/after actions) suggests that common inhibitory mechanisms for motor movement are involved in both actual and planned motor action, which makes our behavior keep going seamlessly.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kei Omata
- Department of Biofunctional Imaging, Medical Photonics Research Center, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Hamamatsu, Japan
| | - Shigeru Ito
- Hamamatsu Medical Photonics Foundation, Hamamatsu PET Imaging Center, Hamamatsu, Japan
| | - Youhei Takata
- Hamamatsu Photonics KK, Global Strategic Challenge Center, Hamamatsu, Japan
| | - Yasuomi Ouchi
- Department of Biofunctional Imaging, Medical Photonics Research Center, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Hamamatsu, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
53
|
Dzafic I, Burianová H, Martin AK, Mowry B. Neural correlates of dynamic emotion perception in schizophrenia and the influence of prior expectations. Schizophr Res 2018; 202:129-137. [PMID: 29910121 DOI: 10.1016/j.schres.2018.06.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/17/2017] [Revised: 05/31/2018] [Accepted: 06/09/2018] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
Impaired emotion perception is a well-established and stable deficit in schizophrenia; however, there is limited knowledge about the underlying aberrant cognitive and brain processes that result in emotion perception deficits. Recent influential work has shown that perceptual deficits in schizophrenia may result from aberrant precision in prior expectations, associated with disrupted activity in frontal regions. In the present study, we investigated the perception of dynamic, multisensory emotion, the influence of prior expectations and the underlying aberrant brain processes in schizophrenia. During a functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging scan, participants completed the Dynamic Emotion Perception task, which induces prior expectations with emotion instruction cues. We delineated neural responses and functional connectivity in whole-brain large-scale networks underlying emotion perception. Compared to healthy individuals, schizophrenia patients had lower accuracy specifically for emotions that were congruent with prior expectations. At the neural level, schizophrenia patients had less engagement of right inferior frontal and parietal regions, as well as right amygdala dysconnectivity during discrimination of emotions congruent with prior expectations. The results indicate that individuals with schizophrenia may have aberrant prior expectations about emotional expressions, associated with under-activity in inferior frontoparietal regions and right amygdala dysconnectivity, which results in impaired perception of emotion.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ilvana Dzafic
- Queensland Brain Institute, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia; Centre for Advanced Imaging, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia.
| | - Hana Burianová
- Centre for Advanced Imaging, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia; Department of Psychology, Swansea University, Swansea, United Kingdom
| | - Andrew K Martin
- University of Queensland Centre for Clinical Research, Brisbane, Australia
| | - Bryan Mowry
- Queensland Brain Institute, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia; Queensland Centre for Mental Health Research, Brisbane, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
54
|
Bashore TR, Ally B, van Wouwe NC, Neimat JS, van den Wildenberg WPM, Wylie SA. Exposing an "Intangible" Cognitive Skill Among Collegiate Football Players: II. Enhanced Response Impulse Control. Front Psychol 2018; 9:1496. [PMID: 30186200 PMCID: PMC6113713 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01496] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/25/2018] [Accepted: 07/30/2018] [Indexed: 12/18/2022] Open
Abstract
American football is played in a dynamic environment that places considerable demands on a player’s ability to make fast, precise reactions while controlling premature, impulsive reactions to spatial misinformation. We investigated the hypothesis that collegiate football players are more proficient than their non-athlete counterparts at controlling impulsive motor actions. National Collegiate Athletic Association (NCAA) Division I football players (n = 280) and non-athlete controls (n = 32) completed a variant of the Simon conflict task, which quantifies choice reaction speed and the proficiency of controlling spatially driven response impulses. Overall, the choice reaction times (RTs) and accuracy rates of football players and controls were equivalent. Similarly, football players and controls were equally susceptible to producing incorrect impulsive motor responses. However, the slowing of RT attributed to the activation and successful inhibition of these impulses (i.e., the Simon effect) was reduced significantly among football players compared to controls. Moreover, differences in impulse control varied by position among the players, with the reduction being greater for offensive than for defensive players. Among offensive players, running backs, wide receivers, and offensive linemen had greater impulse control than did controls, whereas among defensive players only linebackers had greater control. Notably, the Simon effect was reduced by 60% in running backs compared to controls. These results contribute to emerging evidence that elite football players possess more proficient executive control over their motor systems than their age counterparts and suggest that the speed of controlling impulsive motor reactions may represent an enhanced cognitive “intangible” among football players.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Theodore R Bashore
- Department of Neurosurgery, University of Louisville, Louisville, KY, United States.,Department of Psychology, University of Northern Colorado, Greeley, CO, United States
| | - Brandon Ally
- Department of Neurosurgery, University of Louisville, Louisville, KY, United States
| | - Nelleke C van Wouwe
- Department of Neurosurgery, University of Louisville, Louisville, KY, United States
| | - Joseph S Neimat
- Department of Neurosurgery, University of Louisville, Louisville, KY, United States
| | | | - Scott A Wylie
- Department of Neurosurgery, University of Louisville, Louisville, KY, United States
| |
Collapse
|
55
|
Tiego J, Testa R, Bellgrove MA, Pantelis C, Whittle S. A Hierarchical Model of Inhibitory Control. Front Psychol 2018; 9:1339. [PMID: 30123154 PMCID: PMC6085548 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01339] [Citation(s) in RCA: 99] [Impact Index Per Article: 16.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/29/2018] [Accepted: 07/12/2018] [Indexed: 02/04/2023] Open
Abstract
Inhibitory control describes the suppression of goal-irrelevant stimuli and behavioral responses. Current developmental taxonomies distinguish between Response Inhibition – the ability to suppress a prepotent motor response, and Attentional Inhibition – the ability to resist interference from distracting stimuli. Response Inhibition and Attentional Inhibition have exhibited moderately strong positive correlations in previous studies, suggesting they are closely related cognitive abilities. These results may reflect the use of cognitive tasks combining Stimulus–Stimulus- and Stimulus–Response-conflict as indicators of both constructs, which may have conflated their empirical association. Additionally, previous statistical modeling studies have not controlled for individual differences in Working Memory Capacity, which may account for some of the empirical overlap between Response Inhibition and Attentional Inhibition. The aim of the current study was to test a hierarchical model of inhibitory control that specifies Working Memory Capacity as a higher-order cognitive construct. Response Inhibition and Attentional Inhibition were conceptualized as lower-order cognitive mechanisms that should be empirically independent constructs apart from their shared reliance on Working Memory Capacity for active maintenance of goal-relevant representations. Measures of performance on modified stimulus–response compatibility tasks, complex memory span, and non-selective stopping tasks were obtained from 136 preadolescent children (M = 11 years, 10 months, SD = 8 months). Consistent with hypotheses, results from Structural Equation Modeling demonstrated that the Response Inhibition and Attentional Inhibition factors were empirically independent constructs that exhibited partial statistical dependence on the Working Memory Capacity factor. These findings have important implications for current theories and models of inhibitory control during development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jeggan Tiego
- Monash Institute of Cognitive and Clinical Neurosciences, School of Psychological Sciences, Monash University, Clayton, VIC, Australia.,Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, Department of Psychiatry, University of Melbourne & Melbourne Health, Carlton South, VIC, Australia
| | - Renee Testa
- Monash Institute of Cognitive and Clinical Neurosciences, School of Psychological Sciences, Monash University, Clayton, VIC, Australia.,Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, Department of Psychiatry, University of Melbourne & Melbourne Health, Carlton South, VIC, Australia
| | - Mark A Bellgrove
- Monash Institute of Cognitive and Clinical Neurosciences, School of Psychological Sciences, Monash University, Clayton, VIC, Australia
| | - Christos Pantelis
- Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, Department of Psychiatry, University of Melbourne & Melbourne Health, Carlton South, VIC, Australia.,Florey Institute for Neuroscience and Mental Health, Parkville, VIC, Australia.,Centre for Neural Engineering, Department of Electrical and Electronic Engineering, University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC, Australia
| | - Sarah Whittle
- Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, Department of Psychiatry, University of Melbourne & Melbourne Health, Carlton South, VIC, Australia.,Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
56
|
Fluchère F, Burle B, Vidal F, van den Wildenberg W, Witjas T, Eusebio A, Azulay JP, Hasbroucq T. Subthalamic nucleus stimulation, dopaminergic treatment and impulsivity in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia 2018; 117:167-177. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.02.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/03/2017] [Revised: 02/10/2018] [Accepted: 02/12/2018] [Indexed: 02/08/2023]
|
57
|
Leong JK, MacNiven KH, Samanez-Larkin GR, Knutson B. Distinct neural circuits support incentivized inhibition. Neuroimage 2018; 178:435-444. [PMID: 29803959 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.055] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/08/2017] [Revised: 05/12/2018] [Accepted: 05/23/2018] [Indexed: 12/11/2022] Open
Abstract
The ability to inhibit responses under high stakes, or "incentivized inhibition," is critical for adaptive impulse control. While previous research indicates that right ventrolateral prefrontal cortical (VLPFC) activity plays a key role in response inhibition, less research has addressed how incentives might influence this circuit. By combining a novel behavioral task, functional magnetic resonance imaging (FMRI), and diffusion-weighted imaging (DWI), we targeted and characterized specific neural circuits that support incentivized inhibition. Behaviorally, large incentives enhanced responses to obtain money, but also reduced response inhibition. Functionally, activity in both right VLPFC and right anterior insula (AIns) predicted successful inhibition for high incentives. Structurally, characterization of a novel white-matter tract connecting the right AIns and VLPFC revealed an association of tract coherence with incentivized inhibition performance. Finally, individual differences in right VLPFC activity statistically mediated the association of right AIns-VLPFC tract coherence with incentivized inhibition performance. These multimodal findings bridge brain structure, brain function, and behavior to clarify how individuals can inhibit impulses, even in the face of high stakes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Josiah K Leong
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, 94305-2025, USA
| | - Kelly H MacNiven
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, 94305-2025, USA; Stanford Neuroscience Institute, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, 94305-2025, USA
| | | | - Brian Knutson
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, 94305-2025, USA; Stanford Neuroscience Institute, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, 94305-2025, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
58
|
Yamasaki T, Ogawa A, Osada T, Jimura K, Konishi S. Within-Subject Correlation Analysis to Detect Functional Areas Associated With Response Inhibition. Front Hum Neurosci 2018; 12:208. [PMID: 29872386 PMCID: PMC5972214 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00208] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/30/2017] [Accepted: 05/04/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Functional areas in fMRI studies are often detected by brain-behavior correlation, calculating across-subject correlation between the behavioral index and the brain activity related to a function of interest. Within-subject correlation analysis is also employed in a single subject level, which utilizes cognitive fluctuations in a shorter time period by correlating the behavioral index with the brain activity across trials. In the present study, the within-subject analysis was applied to the stop-signal task, a standard task to probe response inhibition, where efficiency of response inhibition can be evaluated by the stop-signal reaction time (SSRT). Since the SSRT is estimated, by definition, not in a trial basis but from pooled trials, the correlation across runs was calculated between the SSRT and the brain activity related to response inhibition. The within-subject correlation revealed negative correlations in the anterior cingulate cortex and the cerebellum. Moreover, the dissociation pattern was observed in the within-subject analysis when earlier vs. later parts of the runs were analyzed: negative correlation was dominant in earlier runs, whereas positive correlation was dominant in later runs. Regions of interest analyses revealed that the negative correlation in the anterior cingulate cortex, but not in the cerebellum, was dominant in earlier runs, suggesting multiple mechanisms associated with inhibitory processes that fluctuate on a run-by-run basis. These results indicate that the within-subject analysis compliments the across-subject analysis by highlighting different aspects of cognitive/affective processes related to response inhibition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tomoko Yamasaki
- Department of Neurophysiology, Juntendo University School of Medicine, Tokyo, Japan
| | - Akitoshi Ogawa
- Department of Neurophysiology, Juntendo University School of Medicine, Tokyo, Japan
| | - Takahiro Osada
- Department of Neurophysiology, Juntendo University School of Medicine, Tokyo, Japan
| | - Koji Jimura
- Department of Biosciences and Informatics, Keio University School of Science and Technology, Yokohama, Japan
| | - Seiki Konishi
- Department of Neurophysiology, Juntendo University School of Medicine, Tokyo, Japan.,Research Institute for Diseases of Old Age, Juntendo University School of Medicine, Tokyo, Japan.,Sportology Center, Juntendo University School of Medicine, Tokyo, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
59
|
Vromans RD, Jongman SR. The interplay between selective and nonselective inhibition during single word production. PLoS One 2018; 13:e0197313. [PMID: 29746594 PMCID: PMC5945011 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0197313] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/09/2017] [Accepted: 04/29/2018] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
The present study investigated the interplay between selective inhibition (the ability to suppress specific competing responses) and nonselective inhibition (the ability to suppress any inappropriate response) during single word production. To this end, we combined two well-established research paradigms: the picture-word interference task and the stop-signal task. Selective inhibition was assessed by instructing participants to name target pictures (e.g., dog) in the presence of semantically related (e.g., cat) or unrelated (e.g., window) distractor words. Nonselective inhibition was tested by occasionally presenting a visual stop-signal, indicating that participants should withhold their verbal response. The stop-signal was presented early (250 ms) aimed at interrupting the lexical selection stage, and late (325 ms) to influence the word-encoding stage of the speech production process. We found longer naming latencies for pictures with semantically related distractors than with unrelated distractors (semantic interference effect). The results further showed that, at both delays, stopping latencies (i.e., stop-signal RTs) were prolonged for naming pictures with semantically related distractors compared to pictures with unrelated distractors. Taken together, our findings suggest that selective and nonselective inhibition, at least partly, share a common inhibitory mechanism during different stages of the speech production process.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ruben D. Vromans
- Tilburg center for Cognition and Communication (TiCC), Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands
| | | |
Collapse
|
60
|
Phillips HN, Cope TE, Hughes LE, Zhang J, Rowe JB. Monitoring the past and choosing the future: the prefrontal cortical influences on voluntary action. Sci Rep 2018; 8:7247. [PMID: 29739978 PMCID: PMC5940796 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-018-25127-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/15/2018] [Accepted: 04/10/2018] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Choosing between equivalent response options requires the resolution of ambiguity. One could facilitate such decisions by monitoring previous actions and implementing transient or arbitrary rules to differentiate response options. This would reduce the entropy of chosen actions. We examined voluntary action decisions during magnetoencephalography, identifying the spatiotemporal correlates of stimulus- and choice-entropy. Negative correlations between frontotemporal activity and entropy of past trials were observed after participants’ responses, reflecting sequential monitoring of recent events. In contrast, choice entropy correlated negatively with prefrontal activity, before and after participants’ response, consistent with transient activation of latent response-sets ahead of a decision and updating the monitor of recent decisions after responding. Individual differences in current choices were related to the strength of the prefrontal signals that reflect monitoring of the statistical regularities in previous events. Together, these results explain individual expressions of voluntary action, through differential engagement of prefrontal areas to guide sequential decisions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- H N Phillips
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 0SZ, UK. .,Medical Research Council, Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, CB2 7EF, UK.
| | - T E Cope
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 0SZ, UK
| | - L E Hughes
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 0SZ, UK.,Medical Research Council, Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, CB2 7EF, UK
| | - J Zhang
- School of Psychology, University of Cardiff, Cardiff, CF2 2AT, UK
| | - J B Rowe
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 0SZ, UK.,Medical Research Council, Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, CB2 7EF, UK.,Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| |
Collapse
|
61
|
Agnew ZK, McGettigan C, Banks B, Scott SK. Group and individual variability in speech production networks during delayed auditory feedback. THE JOURNAL OF THE ACOUSTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA 2018; 143:3009. [PMID: 29857719 PMCID: PMC5963950 DOI: 10.1121/1.5026500] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/21/2016] [Revised: 02/05/2018] [Accepted: 02/12/2018] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
Altering reafferent sensory information can have a profound effect on motor output. Introducing a short delay [delayed auditory feedback (DAF)] during speech production results in modulations of voice and loudness, and produces a range of speech dysfluencies. The ability of speakers to resist the effects of delayed feedback is variable yet it is unclear what neural processes underlie differences in susceptibility to DAF. Here, susceptibility to DAF is investigated by looking at the neural basis of within and between subject changes in speech fluency under 50 and 200 ms delay conditions. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, networks involved in producing speech under two levels of DAF were identified, lying largely within networks active during normal speech production. Independent of condition, fluency ratings were associated with midbrain activity corresponding to periaqueductal grey matter. Across subject variability in ability to produce normal sounding speech under a 200 ms delay was associated with activity in ventral sensorimotor cortices, whereas ability to produce normal sounding speech under a 50 ms delay was associated with left inferior frontal gyrus activity. These data indicate whilst overlapping cortical mechanisms are engaged for speaking under different delay conditions, susceptibility to different temporal delays in speech feedback may involve different processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Z K Agnew
- Institute for Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, 17 Queen Square, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom
| | - C McGettigan
- Institute for Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, 17 Queen Square, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom
| | - B Banks
- Institute for Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, 17 Queen Square, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom
| | - S K Scott
- Institute for Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, 17 Queen Square, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
62
|
Vissers ME, Ridderinkhof KR, Cohen MX, Slagter HA. Oscillatory Mechanisms of Response Conflict Elicited by Color and Motion Direction: An Individual Differences Approach. J Cogn Neurosci 2018; 30:468-481. [DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_01222] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Goal-directed behavior requires control over automatic behavior, for example, when goal-irrelevant information from the environment captures an inappropriate response and conflicts with the correct, goal-relevant action. Neural oscillations in the theta band (∼6 Hz) measured at midfrontal electrodes are thought to form an important substrate of the detection and subsequent resolution of response conflict. Here, we examined the extent to which response conflict and associated theta-band activity depend on the visual stimulus feature dimension that triggers the conflict. We used a feature-based Simon task to manipulate conflict by motion direction and stimulus color. Analyses were focused on individual differences in behavioral response conflict elicited across different stimulus dimensions and their relationship to conflict-related midfrontal theta. We first confirmed the presence of response conflict elicited by task-irrelevant motion and stimulus color, demonstrating the usefulness of our modified version of the Simon task to assess different sensory origins of response conflict. Despite titrating overall task performance, we observed large individual differences in the behavioral manifestations of response conflict elicited by the different visual dimensions. These behavioral conflict effects were mirrored in a dimension-specific relationship with conflict-related midfrontal theta power, such that, for each dimension, individual midfrontal theta power was generally higher when experienced response conflict was high. Finally, exploratory analyses of interregional functional connectivity suggested a role for phase synchronization between frontal and parietal scalp sites in modulating experienced conflict when color was the task-relevant visual dimension. Highlighting the importance of an individual differences approach in cognitive neuroscience, these results reveal large individual differences in experienced response conflict depending on the source of visual interference, which are predicted by conflict-related midfrontal theta power.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Michael X. Cohen
- Radboud University Nijmegen
- Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Center
| | | |
Collapse
|
63
|
Dopaminergic medication shifts the balance between going and stopping in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia 2018; 109:262-269. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.12.032] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/01/2017] [Revised: 12/12/2017] [Accepted: 12/17/2017] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
|
64
|
Li Q, Yang G, Li Z, Qi Y, Cole MW, Liu X. Conflict detection and resolution rely on a combination of common and distinct cognitive control networks. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2017; 83:123-131. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.09.032] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/26/2017] [Revised: 09/26/2017] [Accepted: 09/29/2017] [Indexed: 12/23/2022]
|
65
|
Sachs M, Kaplan J, Der Sarkissian A, Habibi A. Increased engagement of the cognitive control network associated with music training in children during an fMRI Stroop task. PLoS One 2017; 12:e0187254. [PMID: 29084283 PMCID: PMC5662181 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0187254] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/05/2017] [Accepted: 10/17/2017] [Indexed: 12/01/2022] Open
Abstract
Playing a musical instrument engages various sensorimotor processes and draws on cognitive capacities collectively termed executive functions. However, while music training is believed to associated with enhancements in certain cognitive and language abilities, studies that have explored the specific relationship between music and executive function have yielded conflicting results. As part of an ongoing longitudinal study, we investigated the effects of music training on executive function using fMRI and several behavioral tasks, including the Color-Word Stroop task. Children involved in ongoing music training (N = 14, mean age = 8.67) were compared with two groups of comparable general cognitive abilities and socioeconomic status, one involved in sports ("sports" group, N = 13, mean age = 8.85) and another not involved in music or sports ("control" group, N = 17, mean age = 9.05). During the Color-Word Stroop task, children with music training showed significantly greater bilateral activation in the pre-SMA/SMA, ACC, IFG, and insula in trials that required cognitive control compared to the control group, despite no differences in performance on behavioral measures of executive function. No significant differences in brain activation or in task performance were found between the music and sports groups. The results suggest that systematic extracurricular training, particularly music-based training, is associated with changes in the cognitive control network in the brain even in the absence of changes in behavioral performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matthew Sachs
- Brain and Creativity Institute, Dornsife College of Letters, Arts and Sciences, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California, United States of America
| | - Jonas Kaplan
- Brain and Creativity Institute, Dornsife College of Letters, Arts and Sciences, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California, United States of America
| | - Alissa Der Sarkissian
- Brain and Creativity Institute, Dornsife College of Letters, Arts and Sciences, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California, United States of America
| | - Assal Habibi
- Brain and Creativity Institute, Dornsife College of Letters, Arts and Sciences, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
66
|
van Campen AD, Kunert R, van den Wildenberg WPM, Ridderinkhof KR. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over inferior frontal cortex impairs the suppression (but not expression) of action impulses during action conflict. Psychophysiology 2017; 55. [DOI: 10.1111/psyp.13003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/04/2015] [Revised: 05/10/2017] [Accepted: 07/06/2017] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- A. Dilene van Campen
- Department of Psychology; University of Amsterdam; Amsterdam The Netherlands
- The Amsterdam Brain and Cognition Center (ABC), University of Amsterdam; Amsterdam The Netherlands
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University; Nijmegen The Netherlands
| | - Richard Kunert
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University; Nijmegen The Netherlands
- Max Planck Institut für Psycholinguistik; Nijmegen The Netherlands
| | - Wery P. M. van den Wildenberg
- Department of Psychology; University of Amsterdam; Amsterdam The Netherlands
- The Amsterdam Brain and Cognition Center (ABC), University of Amsterdam; Amsterdam The Netherlands
| | - K. Richard Ridderinkhof
- Department of Psychology; University of Amsterdam; Amsterdam The Netherlands
- The Amsterdam Brain and Cognition Center (ABC), University of Amsterdam; Amsterdam The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
67
|
Curley LB, Newman E, Thompson WK, Brown TT, Hagler DJ, Akshoomoff N, Reuter C, Dale AM, Jernigan TL. Cortical morphology of the pars opercularis and its relationship to motor-inhibitory performance in a longitudinal, developing cohort. Brain Struct Funct 2017; 223:211-220. [PMID: 28756486 PMCID: PMC5772141 DOI: 10.1007/s00429-017-1480-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/27/2017] [Accepted: 07/18/2017] [Indexed: 10/29/2022]
Abstract
This study investigates the relationship between variability in cortical surface area and thickness of the pars opercularis of the inferior frontal gyrus and motor-inhibitory performance on a stop-signal task in a longitudinal, typically developing cohort of children and adolescents. Linear mixed-effects models were used to investigate the hypotheses that (1) cortical thinning and (2) a relatively larger cortical surface area of the bilateral pars opercularis of the inferior frontal gyrus would predict better performance on the stop-signal task in a cohort of 110 children and adolescents 4-13 years of age, with one to four observations (totaling 232 observations). Cortical thickness of the bilateral opercular region was not related to inhibitory performance. However, independent of age, gender, and total cortical surface area, relatively larger cortical surface area of the bilateral opercular region of the inferior frontal gyrus was associated with better motor-inhibitory performance. Follow-up analyses showed a significant effect of surface area of the right pars opercularis, but no evidence for an effect of area of left pars opercularis, on motor-inhibitory performance. These findings are consistent with the previous work in adults showing that cortical morphology of the pars opercularis is related to inhibitory functioning. It also expands upon this literature by showing that, in contrast to earlier work highlighting the importance of cortical thickness of this region in adults, relative cortical surface area of the pars opercularis may be related to developing motor-inhibitory functions during childhood and adolescence. Relationships between cortical phenotypes and individual differences in behavioral measures may vary across the lifespan.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lauren B Curley
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, USA
| | - Erik Newman
- Center for Human Development, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive, La Jolla, CA, 92093-0115, USA
| | - Wesley K Thompson
- Center for Human Development, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive, La Jolla, CA, 92093-0115, USA.,Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego School of Medicine, La Jolla, USA
| | - Timothy T Brown
- Center for Human Development, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive, La Jolla, CA, 92093-0115, USA.,Center for Multimodal Imaging and Genetics, University of California, San Diego School of Medicine, La Jolla, USA.,Department of Neurosciences, University of California, San Diego School of Medicine, La Jolla, USA
| | - Donald J Hagler
- Center for Multimodal Imaging and Genetics, University of California, San Diego School of Medicine, La Jolla, USA.,Department of Radiology, University of California, San Diego School of Medicine, La Jolla, USA
| | - Natacha Akshoomoff
- Center for Human Development, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive, La Jolla, CA, 92093-0115, USA.,Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego School of Medicine, La Jolla, USA
| | - Chase Reuter
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego School of Medicine, La Jolla, USA
| | - Anders M Dale
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, USA.,Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego School of Medicine, La Jolla, USA.,Center for Multimodal Imaging and Genetics, University of California, San Diego School of Medicine, La Jolla, USA.,Department of Neurosciences, University of California, San Diego School of Medicine, La Jolla, USA.,Department of Radiology, University of California, San Diego School of Medicine, La Jolla, USA
| | - Terry L Jernigan
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, USA. .,Center for Human Development, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive, La Jolla, CA, 92093-0115, USA. .,Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego School of Medicine, La Jolla, USA. .,Department of Radiology, University of California, San Diego School of Medicine, La Jolla, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
68
|
Boekel W, Forstmann BU, Keuken MC. A test-retest reliability analysis of diffusion measures of white matter tracts relevant for cognitive control. Psychophysiology 2017; 54:24-33. [PMID: 28000260 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12769] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/29/2015] [Accepted: 08/10/2016] [Indexed: 02/02/2023]
Abstract
Recent efforts to replicate structural brain-behavior correlations have called into question the replicability of structural brain measures used in cognitive neuroscience. Here, we report an evaluation of test-retest reliability of diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) measures, including fractional anisotropy, mean diffusivity, axial diffusivity, and radial diffusivity, in several white matter tracts previously shown to be involved in cognitive control. In a data set consisting of 34 healthy participants scanned twice on a single day, we observe overall stability of DTI measures. This stability remained in a subset of participants who were also scanned a third time on the same day as well as in a 2-week follow-up session. We conclude that DTI measures in these tracts show relative stability, and that alternative explanations for the recent failures of replication must be considered.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- W Boekel
- Amsterdam Brain & Cognition Centre, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, and Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - B U Forstmann
- Amsterdam Brain & Cognition Centre, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, and Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - M C Keuken
- Amsterdam Brain & Cognition Centre, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, and Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
69
|
Relationship between symptom dimensions and white matter alterations in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Acta Neuropsychiatr 2017; 29:153-163. [PMID: 27620171 DOI: 10.1017/neu.2016.45] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE To investigate the relationship between the severities of symptom dimensions in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) and white matter alterations. METHODS We applied tract-based spatial statistics for diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) acquired by 3T magnetic resonance imaging. First, we compared fractional anisotropy (FA) between 20 OCD patients and 30 healthy controls (HC). Then, applying whole brain analysis, we searched the brain regions showing correlations between the severities of symptom dimensions assessed by Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory-Revised and FA in all participants. Finally, we calculated the correlations between the six symptom dimensions and multiple DTI measures [FA, axial diffusivity (AD), radial diffusivity (RD), mean diffusivity (MD)] in a region-of-interest (ROI) analysis and explored the differences between OCD patients and HC. RESULTS There were no between-group differences in FA or brain region correlations between the severities of symptom dimensions and FA in any of the participants. ROI analysis revealed negative correlations between checking severity and left inferior frontal gyrus white matter and left middle temporal gyrus white matter and a positive correlation between ordering severity and right precuneus in FA in OCD compared with HC. We also found negative correlations between ordering severity and right precuneus in RD, between obsessing severities and right supramarginal gyrus in AD and MD, and between hoarding severity and right insular gyrus in AD. CONCLUSION Our study supported the hypothesis that the severities of respective symptom dimensions are associated with different patterns of white matter alterations.
Collapse
|
70
|
Sensory neural pathways revisited to unravel the temporal dynamics of the Simon effect: A model-based cognitive neuroscience approach. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2017; 77:48-57. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.02.023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/16/2016] [Revised: 01/23/2017] [Accepted: 02/22/2017] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
|
71
|
van Wouwe NC, Pallavaram S, Phibbs FT, Martinez-Ramirez D, Neimat JS, Dawant BM, D'Haese PF, Kanoff KE, van den Wildenberg WPM, Okun MS, Wylie SA. Focused stimulation of dorsal subthalamic nucleus improves reactive inhibitory control of action impulses. Neuropsychologia 2017; 99:37-47. [PMID: 28237741 PMCID: PMC5493526 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.02.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/12/2016] [Revised: 02/13/2017] [Accepted: 02/22/2017] [Indexed: 01/23/2023]
Abstract
Frontal-basal ganglia circuitry dysfunction caused by Parkinson's disease impairs important executive cognitive processes, such as the ability to inhibit impulsive action tendencies. Subthalamic Nucleus Deep Brain Stimulation in Parkinson's disease improves the reactive inhibition of impulsive actions that interfere with goal-directed behavior. An unresolved question is whether this effect depends on stimulation of a particular Subthalamic Nucleus subregion. The current study aimed to 1) replicate previous findings and additionally investigate the effect of chronic versus acute Subthalamic Nucleus stimulation on inhibitory control in Parkinson's disease patients off dopaminergic medication 2) test whether stimulating Subthalamic Nucleus subregions differentially modulate proactive response control and the proficiency of reactive inhibitory control. In the first experiment, twelve Parkinson's disease patients completed three sessions of the Simon task, Off Deep brain stimulation and medication, on acute Deep Brain Stimulation and on chronic Deep Brain Stimulation. Experiment 2 consisted of 11 Parkinson's disease patients with Subthalamic Nucleus Deep Brain Stimulation (off medication) who completed two testing sessions involving of a Simon task either with stimulation of the dorsal or the ventral contact in the Subthalamic Nucleus. Our findings show that Deep Brain Stimulation improves reactive inhibitory control, regardless of medication and regardless of whether it concerns chronic or acute Subthalamic Nucleus stimulation. More importantly, selective stimulation of dorsal and ventral subregions of the Subthalamic Nucleus indicates that especially the dorsal Subthalamic Nucleus circuitries are crucial for modulating the reactive inhibitory control of motor actions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- N C van Wouwe
- Department of Neurology, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville, TN, USA.
| | - S Pallavaram
- Department of Engineering, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA
| | - F T Phibbs
- Department of Neurology, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville, TN, USA
| | - D Martinez-Ramirez
- Department of Neurology, University of Florida Medical Center, Gainesville, Florida, USA
| | - J S Neimat
- Department of Neurosurgery, University of Louisville Medical Center, Louisville, KY, USA
| | - B M Dawant
- Department of Engineering, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA
| | - P F D'Haese
- Department of Engineering, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA
| | - K E Kanoff
- Department of Neurology, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville, TN, USA
| | - W P M van den Wildenberg
- Cognitive Science Center Amsterdam and Psychology Department, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - M S Okun
- Department of Neurology, University of Florida Medical Center, Gainesville, Florida, USA
| | - S A Wylie
- Department of Neurosurgery, University of Louisville Medical Center, Louisville, KY, USA
| |
Collapse
|
72
|
Van Wouwe NC, Claassen DO, Neimat JS, Kanoff KE, Wylie SA. Dopamine Selectively Modulates the Outcome of Learning Unnatural Action-Valence Associations. J Cogn Neurosci 2017; 29:816-826. [PMID: 28129053 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_01099] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Learning the contingencies between stimulus, action, and outcomes is disrupted in disorders associated with altered dopamine (DA) function in the BG, such as Parkinson disease (PD). Although the role of DA in learning to act has been extensively investigated in PD, the role of DA in "learning to withhold" (or inhibit) action to influence outcomes is not as well understood. The current study investigated the role of DA in learning to act or to withhold action to receive rewarding, or avoid punishing outcomes, in patients with PD tested "off" and "on" dopaminergic medication (n = 19) versus healthy controls (n = 30). Participants performed a reward-based learning task that orthogonalized action and outcome valence (action-reward, inaction-reward, action-punishment, inaction-punishment). We tested whether DA would bias learning toward action, toward reward, or to particular action-outcome interactions. All participants demonstrated inherent learning biases preferring action with reward and inaction to avoid punishment, and this was unaffected by medication. Instead, DA produced a complex modulation of learning less natural action-outcome associations. "Off" DA medication, patients demonstrated impairments in learning to withhold action to gain reward, suggesting a difficulty to overcome a bias toward associating inaction with punishment avoidance. On DA medication, these patterns changed, and patients showed a reduced ability to learn to act to avoid punishment, indicating a bias toward action and reward. The current findings suggest that DA in PD has a complex influence on the formation of action-outcome associations, particularly those involving less natural linkages between action and outcome valence.
Collapse
|
73
|
Duprez J, Houvenaghel JF, Argaud S, Naudet F, Robert G, Drapier D, Vérin M, Sauleau P. Impulsive oculomotor action selection in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia 2016; 95:250-258. [PMID: 28039058 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.12.027] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/01/2016] [Revised: 12/23/2016] [Accepted: 12/27/2016] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
The effects of Parkinson's disease (PD) on the dynamics of impulsive action selection and suppression have recently been studied using distributional analyses, but with mixed results, especially for selection. Furthermore, some authors have suggested that impulsivity, regarded as a personality trait, shares common features with behavioral tasks' measures. The current study was designed to clarify the impact of PD on impulsive action selection and suppression, and investigate the link between cognitive action control and self-reported impulsivity. We administered an oculomotor version of the Simon task to 32 patients with PD and 32 matched healthy controls (HC), and conducted distributional analyses in accordance with the activation-suppression model. Patients and HC also filled out the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS) questionnaire. Results showed that patients with PD were faster overall and exhibited a greater congruence effect than HC. They also displayed enhanced impulsive action selection. By contrast, the suppression of impulsive responses was similar across both groups. Furthermore, patients had higher impulsivity scores, which were correlated with higher impulsive action selection and higher suppression. Our study yielded two interesting findings. First, PD resulted in a higher number of fast errors. The activation-suppression model suggests that patients with PD are more susceptible to the impulsive action selection induced by the irrelevant stimulus dimension. Second, impulsive action selection and suppression were both associated with trait impulsivity, as measured by the BIS, indicating that these two aspects of impulsivity share common features.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joan Duprez
- "Behavior and Basal Ganglia" research unit (EA 4712), University of Rennes 1, Avenue du Professeur Léon Bernard, 35000 Rennes, France.
| | - Jean-François Houvenaghel
- "Behavior and Basal Ganglia" research unit (EA 4712), University of Rennes 1, Avenue du Professeur Léon Bernard, 35000 Rennes, France; Department of Neurology, Rennes University Hospital, Rue Henri Le Guilloux, 35033 Rennes, France
| | - Soizic Argaud
- "Behavior and Basal Ganglia" research unit (EA 4712), University of Rennes 1, Avenue du Professeur Léon Bernard, 35000 Rennes, France; "Neuroscience of Emotion and Affective Dynamics" Lab, Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, 40 boulevard du Pont d'Arve, 1205 Geneva, Switzerland
| | - Florian Naudet
- "Behavior and Basal Ganglia" research unit (EA 4712), University of Rennes 1, Avenue du Professeur Léon Bernard, 35000 Rennes, France; Department of Psychiatry, Rennes University Hospital, 108 Avenue du Général Leclerc, 35703 Rennes, France; Clinical Investigation Center (INSERM 0203), Department of Pharmacology, Rennes University Hospital, Rue Henri Le Guilloux, 35033 Rennes, France
| | - Gabriel Robert
- "Behavior and Basal Ganglia" research unit (EA 4712), University of Rennes 1, Avenue du Professeur Léon Bernard, 35000 Rennes, France; Department of Psychiatry, Rennes University Hospital, 108 Avenue du Général Leclerc, 35703 Rennes, France
| | - Dominique Drapier
- "Behavior and Basal Ganglia" research unit (EA 4712), University of Rennes 1, Avenue du Professeur Léon Bernard, 35000 Rennes, France; Department of Psychiatry, Rennes University Hospital, 108 Avenue du Général Leclerc, 35703 Rennes, France
| | - Marc Vérin
- "Behavior and Basal Ganglia" research unit (EA 4712), University of Rennes 1, Avenue du Professeur Léon Bernard, 35000 Rennes, France; Department of Neurology, Rennes University Hospital, Rue Henri Le Guilloux, 35033 Rennes, France
| | - Paul Sauleau
- "Behavior and Basal Ganglia" research unit (EA 4712), University of Rennes 1, Avenue du Professeur Léon Bernard, 35000 Rennes, France; Department of Neurophysiology, Rennes University Hospital, Rue Henri Le Guilloux, 35033 Rennes, France
| |
Collapse
|
74
|
Abstract
A number of studies have shown that training on action video games improves various aspects of visual cognition including selective attention and inhibitory control. Here, we demonstrate that action video game play can also reduce the Simon Effect, and, hence, may have the potential to improve response selection during the planning and execution of goal-directed action. Non-game-players were randomly assigned to one of four groups; two trained on a first-person-shooter game (Call of Duty) on either Microsoft Xbox or Nintendo DS, one trained on a visual training game for Nintendo DS, and a control group who received no training. Response times were used to contrast performance before and after training on a behavioral assay designed to manipulate stimulus-response compatibility (the Simon Task). The results revealed significantly faster response times and a reduced cost of stimulus-response incompatibility in the groups trained on the first-person-shooter game. No benefit of training was observed in the control group or the group trained on the visual training game. These findings are consistent with previous evidence that action game play elicits plastic changes in the neural circuits that serve attentional control, and suggest training may facilitate goal-directed action by improving players' ability to resolve conflict during response selection and execution.
Collapse
|
75
|
Salas CE, Castro O, Yuen KS, Radovic D, d'Avossa G, Turnbull OH. 'Just can't hide it': a behavioral and lesion study on emotional response modulation after right prefrontal damage. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2016; 11:1528-40. [PMID: 27317928 PMCID: PMC5040916 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsw075] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/12/2015] [Revised: 05/16/2016] [Accepted: 05/18/2016] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Historically, emotion regulation problems have been reported as a common consequence of right prefrontal cortex (rPFC) damage. It has been proposed that the rPFC, particularly the rIFG, has a key role inhibiting prepotent reflexive actions, thus contributing to emotion regulation and self-regulation. This study is the first to directly explore this hypothesis, by testing whether damage to the rIFG compromises the voluntary modulation of emotional responses, and whether performance on inhibition tasks is associated with emotion regulation. METHOD 10 individuals with unilateral right prefrontal damage and 15 matched healthy controls were compared on a well-known response modulation task. During the task participants had to amplify and suppress their facial emotional expressions, while watching film clips eliciting amusement. Measures of executive control, emotion regulation strategies usage and symptomatology were also collected. RESULTS As a group, individuals with rPFC damage presented a significantly reduced range of response modulation compared with controls. In addition, performance in the suppression task was associated with measures of cognitive inhibition and suppression usage. Interestingly, these effects were driven primarily by a subgroup of individuals with rPFC damage, all of whom also had damage to the right posterior insula, and who presented a marked impairment in suppressing facial emotional expressions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christian E Salas
- Laboratorio de Neurociencia Cognitiva y Social (LaNCyS), Facultad de Psicologia, Universidad Diego Portales, Vergara 275, Santiago, Chile School of Psychology, Brigantia Building, Bangor University, Bangor, Wales LL57 2AS, UK Gwynedd
| | - Osvaldo Castro
- Escuela De Terapia Ocupacional, Universidad Autonoma De Chile, Ricardo Morales, San Miguel 3369, Chile, Santiago
| | - Kenneth Sl Yuen
- Neuroimaging Centre, Johannes Gutenberg University Medical Center, Langenbeckstr. 1 Geb. 701, EG, R. 0.035, Mainz 55131, Germany
| | | | - Giovanni d'Avossa
- School of Psychology, Brigantia Building, Bangor University, Bangor, Wales LL57 2AS, UK Gwynedd
| | - Oliver H Turnbull
- School of Psychology, Brigantia Building, Bangor University, Bangor, Wales LL57 2AS, UK Gwynedd
| |
Collapse
|
76
|
Leng Y, Shi Y, Yu Q, Van Horn JD, Tang H, Li J, Xu W, Ge X, Tang Y, Han Y, Zhang D, Xiao M, Zhang H, Pang Z, Toga AW, Liu S. Phenotypic and Genetic Correlations Between the Lobar Segments of the Inferior Fronto-occipital Fasciculus and Attention. Sci Rep 2016; 6:33015. [PMID: 27597294 PMCID: PMC5011720 DOI: 10.1038/srep33015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/30/2016] [Accepted: 08/18/2016] [Indexed: 01/14/2023] Open
Abstract
Attention deficits may present dysfunctions in any one or two components of attention (alerting, orienting, and executive control (EC)). However, these various forms of attention deficits generally have abnormal microstructure integrity of inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (IFOF). In this work, we aim to deeply explore: (1) associations between microstructure integrities of IFOF (including frontal, parietal, temporal, occipital, and insular segments) and attention by means of structural equation models and multiple regression analyses; (2) genetic/environmental effects on IFOF, attention, and their correlations using bivariate genetic analysis. EC function was attributed to the fractional anisotropy (FA) of left (correlation was driven by genetic and environmental factors) and right IFOF (correlation was driven by environmental factors), especially to left frontal part and right occipital part (correlation was driven by genetic factors). Alerting was associated with FA in parietal and insular parts of left IFOF. No significant correlation was found between orienting and IFOF. This study revealed the advantages of lobar-segmental analysis in structure-function correlation study and provided the anatomical basis for kinds of attention deficits. The common genetic/environmental factors implicated in the certain correlations suggested the common physiological mechanisms for two traits, which should promote the discovery of single-nucleotide polymorphisms affecting IFOF and attention.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yuan Leng
- Research Center for Sectional Imaging Anatomy, Shandong Provincial Key Laboratory of Mental Disorders, Shandong University School of Medicine, 44 Wen-hua Xi Road, 250012 Jinan, Shandong, China
- Laboratory of Neuro Imaging, USC Mark and Mary Stevens Neuroimaging and Informatics Institute, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90033, USA
| | - Yonggang Shi
- Laboratory of Neuro Imaging, USC Mark and Mary Stevens Neuroimaging and Informatics Institute, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90033, USA
| | - Qiaowen Yu
- Research Center for Sectional Imaging Anatomy, Shandong Provincial Key Laboratory of Mental Disorders, Shandong University School of Medicine, 44 Wen-hua Xi Road, 250012 Jinan, Shandong, China
| | - John Darrell Van Horn
- Laboratory of Neuro Imaging, USC Mark and Mary Stevens Neuroimaging and Informatics Institute, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90033, USA
| | - Haiyan Tang
- Research Center for Sectional Imaging Anatomy, Shandong Provincial Key Laboratory of Mental Disorders, Shandong University School of Medicine, 44 Wen-hua Xi Road, 250012 Jinan, Shandong, China
| | - Junning Li
- Laboratory of Neuro Imaging, USC Mark and Mary Stevens Neuroimaging and Informatics Institute, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90033, USA
| | - Wenjian Xu
- Department of Radiology, Affiliated Hospital of Medical College, Qingdao University, 266003 Qingdao, Shandong, China
| | - Xinting Ge
- Research Center for Sectional Imaging Anatomy, Shandong Provincial Key Laboratory of Mental Disorders, Shandong University School of Medicine, 44 Wen-hua Xi Road, 250012 Jinan, Shandong, China
| | - Yuchun Tang
- Research Center for Sectional Imaging Anatomy, Shandong Provincial Key Laboratory of Mental Disorders, Shandong University School of Medicine, 44 Wen-hua Xi Road, 250012 Jinan, Shandong, China
| | - Yan Han
- Department of Radiology, Affiliated Hospital of Medical College, Qingdao University, 266003 Qingdao, Shandong, China
| | - Dong Zhang
- Department of Epidemiology, Qingdao Municipal Center for Disease Control and Prevention, 266033 Qingdao, Shandong, China
| | - Min Xiao
- Research Center for Sectional Imaging Anatomy, Shandong Provincial Key Laboratory of Mental Disorders, Shandong University School of Medicine, 44 Wen-hua Xi Road, 250012 Jinan, Shandong, China
| | - Huaqiang Zhang
- Department of Epidemiology, Qingdao Municipal Center for Disease Control and Prevention, 266033 Qingdao, Shandong, China
| | - Zengchang Pang
- Department of Epidemiology, Qingdao Municipal Center for Disease Control and Prevention, 266033 Qingdao, Shandong, China
| | - Arthur W. Toga
- Laboratory of Neuro Imaging, USC Mark and Mary Stevens Neuroimaging and Informatics Institute, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90033, USA
| | - Shuwei Liu
- Research Center for Sectional Imaging Anatomy, Shandong Provincial Key Laboratory of Mental Disorders, Shandong University School of Medicine, 44 Wen-hua Xi Road, 250012 Jinan, Shandong, China
| |
Collapse
|
77
|
Häuser KI, Titone DA, Baum SR. The role of the ventro-lateral prefrontal cortex in idiom comprehension: An rTMS study. Neuropsychologia 2016; 91:360-370. [PMID: 27609125 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.09.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/12/2016] [Revised: 08/22/2016] [Accepted: 09/04/2016] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Previous research is equivocal with respect to the neural substrates of idiom processing. Particularly elusive is the role of the left ventro-lateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC), a region implicated in semantic control generally. Although fMRI studies have shown that the left VLPFC is active during idiom processing (see Rapp et al. (2012), for review), rTMS studies have failed to corroborate a clear role of this prefrontal region (e.g., Oliveri et al., 2004). We investigated this issue using a semantic meaningfulness judgment task that compared idiom comprehension following rTMS-stimulation to the left VLPFC relative to a control site (vertex). We also investigated whether individual differences in general cognitive capacity among comprehenders modulated the effects of rTMS. The results indicate that left VLPFC stimulation particularly affected the processing of low-familiar idioms, possibly because these items involve a maximal semantic conflict between a salient literal and less-known figurative meaning. Of note, this pattern only emerged for comprehenders with higher cognitive control capacity, possibly because they were more likely to activate or maintain multiple semantic representations during idiom processing, which required VLPFC integrity. Taken together, the results support the importance of the left VLPFC to idiom processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katja I Häuser
- School of Communication Sciences and Disorders, McGill University, Montreal, Canada; Centre for Research on Brain, Language and Music, McGill University, Montreal, Canada.
| | - Debra A Titone
- Centre for Research on Brain, Language and Music, McGill University, Montreal, Canada; Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
| | - Shari R Baum
- School of Communication Sciences and Disorders, McGill University, Montreal, Canada; Centre for Research on Brain, Language and Music, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
78
|
Sikora K, Roelofs A, Hermans D, Knoors H. Executive control in spoken noun-phrase production: Contributions of updating, inhibiting, and shifting. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2016; 69:1719-40. [DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1093007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
The present study examined how the updating, inhibiting, and shifting abilities underlying executive control influence spoken noun-phrase production. Previous studies provided evidence that updating and inhibiting, but not shifting, influence picture-naming response time (RT). However, little is known about the role of executive control in more complex forms of language production like generating phrases. We assessed noun-phrase production using picture description and a picture–word interference procedure. We measured picture description RT to assess length, distractor, and switch effects, which were assumed to reflect, respectively, the updating, inhibiting, and shifting abilities of adult participants. Moreover, for each participant we obtained scores on executive control tasks that measured verbal and nonverbal updating, nonverbal inhibiting, and nonverbal shifting. We found that both verbal and nonverbal updating scores correlated with the overall mean picture description RTs. Furthermore, the length effect in the RTs correlated with verbal but not nonverbal updating scores, while the distractor effect correlated with inhibiting scores. We did not find a correlation between the switch effect in the mean RTs and the shifting scores. However, the shifting scores correlated with the switch effect in the normal part of the underlying RT distribution. These results suggest that updating, inhibiting, and shifting each influence the speed of phrase production, thereby demonstrating a contribution of all three executive control abilities to language production.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katarzyna Sikora
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Centre for Cognition, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Ardi Roelofs
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Centre for Cognition, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Daan Hermans
- Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
- Royal Dutch Kentalis, Sint-Michielsgestel, The Netherlands
| | - Harry Knoors
- Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
- Royal Dutch Kentalis, Sint-Michielsgestel, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
79
|
Combined DTI-fMRI Analysis for a Quantitative Assessment of Connections Between WM Bundles and Their Peripheral Cortical Fields in Verbal Fluency. Brain Topogr 2016; 29:814-823. [PMID: 27509899 DOI: 10.1007/s10548-016-0516-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/19/2016] [Accepted: 08/03/2016] [Indexed: 12/17/2022]
Abstract
Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) tractography and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) are powerful techniques to elucidate the anatomical and functional aspects of brain connectivity. However, integrating these approaches to describe the precise link between structure and function within specific brain circuits remains challenging. In this study, a novel DTI-fMRI integration method is proposed, to provide the topographical characterization and the volumetric assessment of the functional and anatomical connections within the language circuit. In a group of 21 healthy elderly subjects (mean age 68.5 ± 5.8 years), the volume of connection between the cortical activity elicited by a verbal fluency task and the cortico-cortical fiber tracts associated with this function are mapped and quantified. An application of the method to a case study in neuro-rehabilitation context is also presented. Integrating structural and functional data within the same framework, this approach provides an overall view of white and gray matter when studying specific brain circuits.
Collapse
|
80
|
Filley CM, Fields RD. White matter and cognition: making the connection. J Neurophysiol 2016; 116:2093-2104. [PMID: 27512019 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00221.2016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 260] [Impact Index Per Article: 32.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/14/2016] [Accepted: 08/04/2016] [Indexed: 12/14/2022] Open
Abstract
Whereas the cerebral cortex has long been regarded by neuroscientists as the major locus of cognitive function, the white matter of the brain is increasingly recognized as equally critical for cognition. White matter comprises half of the brain, has expanded more than gray matter in evolution, and forms an indispensable component of distributed neural networks that subserve neurobehavioral operations. White matter tracts mediate the essential connectivity by which human behavior is organized, working in concert with gray matter to enable the extraordinary repertoire of human cognitive capacities. In this review, we present evidence from behavioral neurology that white matter lesions regularly disturb cognition, consider the role of white matter in the physiology of distributed neural networks, develop the hypothesis that white matter dysfunction is relevant to neurodegenerative disorders, including Alzheimer's disease and the newly described entity chronic traumatic encephalopathy, and discuss emerging concepts regarding the prevention and treatment of cognitive dysfunction associated with white matter disorders. Investigation of the role of white matter in cognition has yielded many valuable insights and promises to expand understanding of normal brain structure and function, improve the treatment of many neurobehavioral disorders, and disclose new opportunities for research on many challenging problems facing medicine and society.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christopher M Filley
- Behavioral Neurology Section, Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, University of Colorado School of Medicine, Aurora, Colorado; .,Denver Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Denver, Colorado; and
| | - R Douglas Fields
- Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland
| |
Collapse
|
81
|
Yang AC, Tsai SJ, Liu ME, Huang CC, Lin CP. The Association of Aging with White Matter Integrity and Functional Connectivity Hubs. Front Aging Neurosci 2016; 8:143. [PMID: 27378915 PMCID: PMC4908139 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00143] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/28/2016] [Accepted: 05/30/2016] [Indexed: 12/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Normal aging is associated with reduced cerebral structural integrity and altered functional brain activity, yet the association of aging with the relationship between structural and functional brain changes remains unclear. Using combined diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) modalities, we hypothesized that aging-related changes in white matter integrity (i.e., fractional anisotropy) was associated with the short- or long-range functional connectivity density (FCD) in hub regions. We tested this hypothesis by using a healthy aging cohort comprised of 140 younger adults aged 20–39 years and 109 older adults aged 60–79 years. Compared with the younger group, older adults exhibited widespread reductions in white matter integrity with selective preservation in brain stem tracts and the cingulum connected to the hippocampus and cingulate cortex, whereas FCD mapping in older adults showed a reduced FCD in the visual, somatosensory, and motor functional networks and an increased FCD in the default mode network. The older adults exhibited significantly increased short- or long-range FCD in functional hubs of the precuneus, posterior, and middle cingulate, and thalamus, hippocampus, fusiform, and inferior temporal cortex. Furthermore, DTI-fMRI relationship were predominantly identified in older adults in whom short- and long-range FCD in the left precuneus was negatively correlated to structural integrity of adjacent and nonadjacent white matter tracts, respectively. We also found that long-range FCD in the left precuneus was positively correlated to cognitive function. These results support the compensatory hypothesis of neurocognitive aging theory and reveal the DTI-fMRI relationship associated with normal aging.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Albert C Yang
- Division of Interdisciplinary Medicine and Biotechnology, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center/Harvard Medical SchoolBoston, MA, USA; Department of Psychiatry, Taipei Veterans General HospitalTaipei, Taiwan; Division of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, National Yang-Ming UniversityTaipei, Taiwan; Center for Dynamical Biomarkers and Translational Medicine, National Central UniversityChungli, Taiwan
| | - Shih-Jen Tsai
- Department of Psychiatry, Taipei Veterans General HospitalTaipei, Taiwan; Division of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, National Yang-Ming UniversityTaipei, Taiwan
| | - Mu-En Liu
- Department of Psychiatry, Taipei Veterans General HospitalTaipei, Taiwan; Division of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, National Yang-Ming UniversityTaipei, Taiwan
| | - Chu-Chung Huang
- Institute of Brain Science, National Yang-Ming University Taipei, Taiwan
| | - Ching-Po Lin
- Institute of Brain Science, National Yang-Ming University Taipei, Taiwan
| |
Collapse
|
82
|
Hassa T, de Jel E, Tuescher O, Schmidt R, Schoenfeld MA. Functional networks of motor inhibition in conversion disorder patients and feigning subjects. NEUROIMAGE-CLINICAL 2016; 11:719-727. [PMID: 27330971 PMCID: PMC4900693 DOI: 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.05.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/29/2016] [Revised: 04/24/2016] [Accepted: 05/16/2016] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
The neural correlates of motor inhibition leading to paresis in conversion disorder are not well known. The key question is whether they are different of those of normal subjects feigning the symptoms. Thirteen conversion disorder patients with hemiparesis and twelve healthy controls were investigated using functional magnetic resonance tomography under conditions of passive motor stimulation of the paretic/feigned paretic and the non-paretic hand. Healthy controls were also investigated in a non-feigning condition. During passive movement of the affected right hand conversion disorder patients exhibited activations in the bilateral triangular part of the inferior frontal gyri (IFG), with a left side dominance compared to controls in non-feigning condition. Feigning controls revealed for the same condition a weak unilateral activation in the right triangular part of IFG and an activity decrease in frontal midline areas, which couldn't be observed in patients. The results suggest that motor inhibition in conversion disorder patients is mediated by the IFG that was also involved in inhibition processes in normal subjects. The activity pattern in feigning controls resembled that of conversion disorder patients but with a clear difference in the medial prefrontal cortex. Healthy controls showed decreased activity in this region during feigning compared to non-feigning conditions suggesting a reduced sense of self-agency during feigning. Remarkably, no activity differences could be observed in medial prefrontal cortex for patients vs healthy controls in feigning or non-feigning conditions suggesting self-agency related activity in patients to be in between those of non-feigning and feigning healthy subjects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas Hassa
- Lurija Institute for Rehabilitation and Health Sciences, Allensbach, Germany; Neurological Rehabilitation Center Kliniken Schmieder, Allensbach, Germany.
| | - Esther de Jel
- Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz, Germany
| | - Oliver Tuescher
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Center of the Johannes Gutenberg University Mainz, Germany
| | - Roger Schmidt
- Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz, Germany; Department of Psychotherapeutic Neurology, Neurological Rehabilitation Center Kliniken Schmieder, Konstanz and Gailingen, Germany
| | - Mircea Ariel Schoenfeld
- Neurological Rehabilitation Center Kliniken Schmieder, Allensbach, Germany; Department of Neurology, Otto-von-Guericke-University Magdeburg, Germany; Leibniz-Institute for Neurobiology, Magdeburg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
83
|
Cook PF, Spivak M, Berns G. Neurobehavioral evidence for individual differences in canine cognitive control: an awake fMRI study. Anim Cogn 2016; 19:867-78. [DOI: 10.1007/s10071-016-0983-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/30/2015] [Revised: 04/01/2016] [Accepted: 04/04/2016] [Indexed: 12/22/2022]
|
84
|
Duprez J, Houvenaghel JF, Naudet F, Dondaine T, Auffret M, Robert G, Drapier D, Argaud S, Vérin M, Sauleau P. Evaluating Cognitive Action Control Using Eye-Movement Analysis: An Oculomotor Adaptation of the Simon Task. Front Hum Neurosci 2016; 10:84. [PMID: 26973499 PMCID: PMC4773592 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00084] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/11/2016] [Accepted: 02/18/2016] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Cognitive action control has been extensively studied using conflict tasks such as the Simon task. In most recent studies, this process has been investigated in the light of the dual route hypothesis and more specifically of the activation-suppression model using distributional analyses. Some authors have suggested that cognitive action control assessment is not specific to response modes. In this study we adapted the Simon task, using oculomotor responses instead of manual responses, in order to evaluate whether the resolution of conflict induced by a two-dimensional stimulus yielded similar results to what is usually reported in tasks with manual responses. Results obtained from 43 young healthy participants revealed the typical congruence effect, with longer reaction times (RT) and lesser accuracy in the incongruent condition. Conditional accuracy functions (CAF) also revealed a higher proportion of fast errors in the incongruent condition and delta plots confirmed that conflict resolution was easier, as the time taken to respond increased. These results are very similar to what has been reported in the literature. Furthermore, our observations are in line with the assumptions of the activation-suppression model, in which automatic activation in conflict situations is captured in the fastest responses and selective inhibition of cognitive action control needs time to build up. Altogether, our results suggest that conflict resolution has core mechanisms whatever the response mode, manual or oculomotor. Using oculomotor responses in such tasks could be of interest when investigating cognitive action control in patients with severe motor disorders.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joan Duprez
- "Behavior and Basal Ganglia" Research Unit (EA 4712), University of Rennes 1 Rennes, France
| | - Jean-François Houvenaghel
- "Behavior and Basal Ganglia" Research Unit (EA 4712), University of Rennes 1Rennes, France; Neurology Department, Rennes University HospitalRennes, France
| | - Florian Naudet
- "Behavior and Basal Ganglia" Research Unit (EA 4712), University of Rennes 1Rennes, France; Adult Psychiatry Department, Rennes University HospitalRennes, France; Clinical Investigation Center (INSERM 0203), Department of Pharmacology, Rennes University HospitalRennes, France
| | - Thibaut Dondaine
- "Behavior and Basal Ganglia" Research Unit (EA 4712), University of Rennes 1Rennes, France; Adult Psychiatry Department, Rennes University HospitalRennes, France
| | - Manon Auffret
- "Behavior and Basal Ganglia" Research Unit (EA 4712), University of Rennes 1 Rennes, France
| | - Gabriel Robert
- "Behavior and Basal Ganglia" Research Unit (EA 4712), University of Rennes 1Rennes, France; Adult Psychiatry Department, Rennes University HospitalRennes, France
| | - Dominique Drapier
- "Behavior and Basal Ganglia" Research Unit (EA 4712), University of Rennes 1Rennes, France; Adult Psychiatry Department, Rennes University HospitalRennes, France
| | - Soizic Argaud
- "Behavior and Basal Ganglia" Research Unit (EA 4712), University of Rennes 1Rennes, France; Neuroscience of Emotion and Affective Dynamics Lab, Swiss Center for Affective SciencesGeneva, Switzerland
| | - Marc Vérin
- "Behavior and Basal Ganglia" Research Unit (EA 4712), University of Rennes 1Rennes, France; Neurology Department, Rennes University HospitalRennes, France
| | - Paul Sauleau
- "Behavior and Basal Ganglia" Research Unit (EA 4712), University of Rennes 1Rennes, France; Neurophysiology Department, Rennes University HospitalRennes, France
| |
Collapse
|
85
|
Ponzo V, Picazio S, Benussi A, Di Lorenzo F, Brusa L, Caltagirone C, Koch G. Altered inhibitory interaction among inferior frontal and motor cortex inl-dopa-induced dyskinesias. Mov Disord 2016; 31:755-9. [DOI: 10.1002/mds.26520] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/21/2015] [Revised: 11/16/2015] [Accepted: 12/02/2015] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Viviana Ponzo
- Non-invasive Brain Stimulation Unit, Santa Lucia Foundation; Rome Italy
| | - Silvia Picazio
- Non-invasive Brain Stimulation Unit, Santa Lucia Foundation; Rome Italy
| | - Alberto Benussi
- Centre for Ageing Brain and Neurodegenerative Disorders, Department of Clinical and Experimental Sciences; Neurology Unit, University of Brescia; Brescia Italy
| | | | - Livia Brusa
- Neurology Department, S. Eugenio Hospital; Rome Italy
| | - Carlo Caltagirone
- Non-invasive Brain Stimulation Unit, Santa Lucia Foundation; Rome Italy
- System Medicine Department; Tor Vergata University; Rome Italy
| | - Giacomo Koch
- Non-invasive Brain Stimulation Unit, Santa Lucia Foundation; Rome Italy
- Stroke Unit, Department of Neuroscience; Policlinico Tor Vergata; Rome Italy
| |
Collapse
|
86
|
Ho TC, Zhang S, Sacchet MD, Weng H, Connolly CG, Henje Blom E, Han LKM, Mobayed NO, Yang TT. Fusiform Gyrus Dysfunction is Associated with Perceptual Processing Efficiency to Emotional Faces in Adolescent Depression: A Model-Based Approach. Front Psychol 2016; 7:40. [PMID: 26869950 PMCID: PMC4740953 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00040] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/18/2015] [Accepted: 01/10/2016] [Indexed: 01/27/2023] Open
Abstract
While the extant literature has focused on major depressive disorder (MDD) as being characterized by abnormalities in processing affective stimuli (e.g., facial expressions), little is known regarding which specific aspects of cognition influence the evaluation of affective stimuli, and what are the underlying neural correlates. To investigate these issues, we assessed 26 adolescents diagnosed with MDD and 37 well-matched healthy controls (HCL) who completed an emotion identification task of dynamically morphing faces during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). We analyzed the behavioral data using a sequential sampling model of response time (RT) commonly used to elucidate aspects of cognition in binary perceptual decision making tasks: the Linear Ballistic Accumulator (LBA) model. Using a hierarchical Bayesian estimation method, we obtained group-level and individual-level estimates of LBA parameters on the facial emotion identification task. While the MDD and HCL groups did not differ in mean RT, accuracy, or group-level estimates of perceptual processing efficiency (i.e., drift rate parameter of the LBA), the MDD group showed significantly reduced responses in left fusiform gyrus compared to the HCL group during the facial emotion identification task. Furthermore, within the MDD group, fMRI signal in the left fusiform gyrus during affective face processing was significantly associated with greater individual-level estimates of perceptual processing efficiency. Our results therefore suggest that affective processing biases in adolescents with MDD are characterized by greater perceptual processing efficiency of affective visual information in sensory brain regions responsible for the early processing of visual information. The theoretical, methodological, and clinical implications of our results are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tiffany C Ho
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco CA, USA
| | - Shunan Zhang
- Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla CA, USA
| | - Matthew D Sacchet
- Neuroscience Graduate Program and Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Palo Alto CA, USA
| | - Helen Weng
- The Osher Center for Integrative Medicine, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco CA, USA
| | - Colm G Connolly
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco CA, USA
| | - Eva Henje Blom
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco, San FranciscoCA, USA; Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska InstitutetStockhold, Sweden
| | - Laura K M Han
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco CA, USA
| | - Nisreen O Mobayed
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco CA, USA
| | - Tony T Yang
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
87
|
Price CC, Tanner J, Nguyen PT, Schwab NA, Mitchell S, Slonena E, Brumback B, Okun MS, Mareci TH, Bowers D. Gray and White Matter Contributions to Cognitive Frontostriatal Deficits in Non-Demented Parkinson's Disease. PLoS One 2016; 11:e0147332. [PMID: 26784744 PMCID: PMC4718544 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0147332] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/08/2015] [Accepted: 01/01/2016] [Indexed: 01/09/2023] Open
Abstract
Objective This prospective investigation examined: 1) processing speed and working memory relative to other cognitive domains in non-demented medically managed idiopathic Parkinson’s disease, and 2) the predictive role of cortical/subcortical gray thickness/volume and white matter fractional anisotropy on processing speed and working memory. Methods Participants completed a neuropsychological protocol, Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale, brain MRI, and fasting blood draw to rule out vascular contributors. Within group a priori anatomical contributors included bilateral frontal thickness, caudate nuclei volume, and prefrontal white matter fractional anisotropy. Results Idiopathic Parkinson’s disease (n = 40; Hoehn & Yahr stages 1–3) and non-Parkinson’s disease ‘control’ peers (n = 40) matched on demographics, general cognition, comorbidity, and imaging/blood vascular metrics. Cognitively, individuals with Parkinson’s disease were significantly more impaired than controls on tests of processing speed, secondary deficits on working memory, with subtle impairments in memory, abstract reasoning, and visuoperceptual/spatial abilities. Anatomically, Parkinson’s disease individuals were not statistically different in cortical gray thickness or subcortical gray volumes with the exception of the putamen. Tract Based Spatial Statistics showed reduced prefrontal fractional anisotropy for Parkinson’s disease relative to controls. Within Parkinson’s disease, prefrontal fractional anisotropy and caudate nucleus volume partially explained processing speed. For controls, only prefrontal white matter was a significant contributor to processing speed. There were no significant anatomical predictors of working memory for either group. Conclusions Caudate nuclei volume and prefrontal fractional anisotropy, not frontal gray matter thickness, showed unique and combined significance for processing speed in Parkinson’s disease. Findings underscore the relevance for examining gray-white matter interactions and also highlight clinical processing speed metrics as potential indicators of early cognitive impairment in PD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Catherine C. Price
- Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida, United States of America
- University of Florida Center for Movement Disorders and Neurorestoration, Gainesville, Florida, United States of America
- * E-mail:
| | - Jared Tanner
- Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida, United States of America
| | - Peter T. Nguyen
- Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida, United States of America
| | - Nadine A. Schwab
- Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida, United States of America
| | - Sandra Mitchell
- Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida, United States of America
| | - Elizabeth Slonena
- Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida, United States of America
| | - Babette Brumback
- Department of Biostatistics, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida, United States of America
| | - Michael S. Okun
- Department of Neurology, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida, United States of America
- University of Florida Center for Movement Disorders and Neurorestoration, Gainesville, Florida, United States of America
| | - Thomas H. Mareci
- Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida, United States of America
| | - Dawn Bowers
- Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida, United States of America
- University of Florida Center for Movement Disorders and Neurorestoration, Gainesville, Florida, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
88
|
Deng Y, Wang Y, Ding X, Tang YY. The relevance of fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation to interference effect. Behav Brain Res 2016; 296:401-407. [DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.08.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/26/2015] [Revised: 07/25/2015] [Accepted: 08/17/2015] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
|
89
|
Enhancing switching abilities: Improving practice effect by stimulating the dorsolateral pre frontal cortex. Neuroscience 2016; 313:92-8. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.11.050] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/03/2015] [Revised: 10/26/2015] [Accepted: 11/20/2015] [Indexed: 02/02/2023]
|
90
|
Debey E, Ridderinkhof RK, De Houwer J, De Schryver M, Verschuere B. Suppressing the truth as a mechanism of deception: Delta plots reveal the role of response inhibition in lying. Conscious Cogn 2015; 37:148-59. [PMID: 26397036 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.09.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/16/2014] [Revised: 08/26/2015] [Accepted: 09/09/2015] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
|
91
|
Droutman V, Bechara A, Read SJ. Roles of the Different Sub-Regions of the Insular Cortex in Various Phases of the Decision-Making Process. Front Behav Neurosci 2015; 9:309. [PMID: 26635559 PMCID: PMC4658437 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00309] [Citation(s) in RCA: 83] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/20/2014] [Accepted: 11/02/2015] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
This paper presents a coherent account of the role of the insular cortex (IC) in decision-making. We follow a conceptualization of decision-making that is very close to one previously proposed by Ernst and Paulus (2005): that the decision process is a progression of four phases: (1) re-focusing attention; (2) evaluation; (3) action; and (4) outcome processing, and we present evidence for the insula’s role in all these phases. We review the existing work on insula’s functional anatomy that subdivides the IC into posterior, dorsal anterior and ventral anterior regions. We re-map the results provided by the existing literature into these subdivisions wherever possible, to identify the components’ role in each decision making phase. In addition, we identify a self-regulating quality of the IC focused on harm avoidance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Vita Droutman
- Department of Psychology, University of Southern California Los Angeles, CA, USA
| | - Antoine Bechara
- Department of Psychology, University of Southern California Los Angeles, CA, USA
| | - Stephen J Read
- Department of Psychology, University of Southern California Los Angeles, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
92
|
Easy to learn, hard to suppress: The impact of learned stimulus-outcome associations on subsequent action control. Brain Cogn 2015; 101:17-34. [PMID: 26554843 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2015.10.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/06/2015] [Revised: 10/13/2015] [Accepted: 10/29/2015] [Indexed: 12/26/2022]
Abstract
The inhibition of impulsive response tendencies that conflict with goal-directed action is a key component of executive control. An emerging literature reveals that the proficiency of inhibitory control is modulated by expected or unexpected opportunities to earn reward or avoid punishment. However, less is known about how inhibitory control is impacted by the processing of task-irrelevant stimulus information that has been associated previously with particular outcomes (reward or punishment) or response tendencies (action or inaction). We hypothesized that stimulus features associated with particular action-valence tendencies, even though task irrelevant, would modulate inhibitory control processes. Participants first learned associations between stimulus features (color), actions, and outcomes using an action-valence learning task that orthogonalizes action (action, inaction) and valence (reward, punishment). Next, these stimulus features were embedded in a Simon task as a task-irrelevant stimulus attribute. We analyzed the effects of action-valence associations on the Simon task by means of distributional analysis to reveal the temporal dynamics. Learning patterns replicated previously reported biases; inherent, Pavlovian-like mappings (action-reward, inaction-punishment avoidance) were easier to learn than mappings conflicting with these biases (action-punishment avoidance, inaction-reward). More importantly, results from two experiments demonstrated that the easier to learn, Pavlovian-like action-valence associations interfered with the proficiency of inhibiting impulsive actions in the Simon task. Processing conflicting associations led to more proficient inhibitory control of impulsive actions, similar to Simon trials without any association. Fast impulsive errors were reduced for trials associated with punishment in comparison to reward trials or trials without any valence association. These findings provide insight into the temporal dynamics of task irrelevant information associated with action and valence modulating cognitive control. We discuss putative mechanisms that might explain these interactions.
Collapse
|
93
|
Hughes ME, Fulham WR, Michie PT. Electrophysiological signatures of the race model in human primary motor cortex. Psychophysiology 2015; 53:229-36. [DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12562] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/03/2015] [Revised: 08/27/2015] [Accepted: 09/04/2015] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Matthew E. Hughes
- Brain and Psychological Sciences Centre; Swinburne University of Technology; Melbourne Australia
- Schizophrenia Research Institute; Sydney Australia
| | - W. Ross Fulham
- Schizophrenia Research Institute; Sydney Australia
- Centre for Translational Neuroscience and Mental Health; University of Newcastle; Callaghan Australia
- Functional Neuroimaging Laboratory, School of Psychology; University of Newcastle; Callaghan Australia
- Hunter Medical Research Institute; Newcastle Australia
| | - Patricia T. Michie
- Schizophrenia Research Institute; Sydney Australia
- Centre for Translational Neuroscience and Mental Health; University of Newcastle; Callaghan Australia
- Functional Neuroimaging Laboratory, School of Psychology; University of Newcastle; Callaghan Australia
- Hunter Medical Research Institute; Newcastle Australia
| |
Collapse
|
94
|
Hartwigsen G, Siebner HR. Joint Contribution of Left Dorsal Premotor Cortex and Supramarginal Gyrus to Rapid Action Reprogramming. Brain Stimul 2015; 8:945-52. [DOI: 10.1016/j.brs.2015.04.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/12/2014] [Revised: 04/28/2015] [Accepted: 04/29/2015] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
|
95
|
Liu C, Chen Z, Wang T, Tang D, Hitchman G, Sun J, Zhao X, Wang L, Chen A. Predicting stroop effect from spontaneous neuronal activity: a study of regional homogeneity. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0124405. [PMID: 25938442 PMCID: PMC4418763 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0124405] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/13/2014] [Accepted: 03/13/2015] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
The Stroop effect is one of the most robust and well-studied phenomena in cognitive psychology and cognitive neuroscience. However, little is known about the relationship between intrinsic brain activity and the individual differences of this effect. In the present study, we explored this issue by examining whether resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) signals could predict individual differences in the Stroop effect of healthy individuals. A partial correlation analysis was calculated to examine the relationship between regional homogeneity (ReHo) and Stroop effect size, while controlling for age, sex, and framewise displacement (FD). The results showed positive correlations in the left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG), the left insula, the ventral anterior cingulate cortex (vACC), and the medial frontal gyrus (MFG), and negative correlation in the left precentral gyrus (LPG). These results indicate the possible influences of the LIFG, the left insula, and the LPG on the efficiency of cognitive control, and demonstrate that the key nodes of default mode network (DMN) may be important in goal-directed behavior and/or mental effort during cognitive control tasks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Congcong Liu
- Key laboratory of Cognition and Personality of Ministry of Education, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China
| | - Zhencai Chen
- Key laboratory of Cognition and Personality of Ministry of Education, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China
| | - Ting Wang
- Key laboratory of Cognition and Personality of Ministry of Education, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China
- Laboratory of Cognition and Mental Health, Chongqing University of Arts and Sciences, Chongqing, China
| | - Dandan Tang
- Key laboratory of Cognition and Personality of Ministry of Education, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China
| | - Glenn Hitchman
- Key laboratory of Cognition and Personality of Ministry of Education, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China
| | - Jiangzhou Sun
- Key laboratory of Cognition and Personality of Ministry of Education, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China
| | - Xiaoyue Zhao
- Key laboratory of Cognition and Personality of Ministry of Education, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China
| | - Lijun Wang
- Key laboratory of Cognition and Personality of Ministry of Education, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China
| | - Antao Chen
- Key laboratory of Cognition and Personality of Ministry of Education, Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
96
|
Benedetti F, Bollettini I, Poletti S, Locatelli C, Lorenzi C, Pirovano A, Smeraldi E, Colombo C. White matter microstructure in bipolar disorder is influenced by the serotonin transporter gene polymorphism 5-HTTLPR. GENES BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR 2015; 14:238-50. [PMID: 25704032 DOI: 10.1111/gbb.12206] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/27/2014] [Revised: 02/04/2015] [Accepted: 02/05/2015] [Indexed: 12/24/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- F. Benedetti
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Scientific Institute; Milano Italy
- C.E.R.M.A.C. (Centro di Eccellenza Risonanza Magnetica ad Alto Campo); University Vita-Salute San Raffaele; Milano Italy
| | - I. Bollettini
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Scientific Institute; Milano Italy
- C.E.R.M.A.C. (Centro di Eccellenza Risonanza Magnetica ad Alto Campo); University Vita-Salute San Raffaele; Milano Italy
| | - S. Poletti
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Scientific Institute; Milano Italy
- C.E.R.M.A.C. (Centro di Eccellenza Risonanza Magnetica ad Alto Campo); University Vita-Salute San Raffaele; Milano Italy
| | - C. Locatelli
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Scientific Institute; Milano Italy
- C.E.R.M.A.C. (Centro di Eccellenza Risonanza Magnetica ad Alto Campo); University Vita-Salute San Raffaele; Milano Italy
| | - C. Lorenzi
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Scientific Institute; Milano Italy
| | - A. Pirovano
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Scientific Institute; Milano Italy
| | - E. Smeraldi
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Scientific Institute; Milano Italy
- C.E.R.M.A.C. (Centro di Eccellenza Risonanza Magnetica ad Alto Campo); University Vita-Salute San Raffaele; Milano Italy
| | - C. Colombo
- Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Scientific Institute; Milano Italy
| |
Collapse
|
97
|
Chavan CF, Mouthon M, Draganski B, van der Zwaag W, Spierer L. Differential patterns of functional and structural plasticity within and between inferior frontal gyri support training-induced improvements in inhibitory control proficiency. Hum Brain Mapp 2015; 36:2527-43. [PMID: 25801718 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.22789] [Citation(s) in RCA: 49] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/27/2015] [Accepted: 03/04/2015] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Ample evidence indicates that inhibitory control (IC), a key executive component referring to the ability to suppress cognitive or motor processes, relies on a right-lateralized fronto-basal brain network. However, whether and how IC can be improved with training and the underlying neuroplastic mechanisms remains largely unresolved. We used functional and structural magnetic resonance imaging to measure the effects of 2 weeks of training with a Go/NoGo task specifically designed to improve frontal top-down IC mechanisms. The training-induced behavioral improvements were accompanied by a decrease in neural activity to inhibition trials within the right pars opercularis and triangularis, and in the left pars orbitalis of the inferior frontal gyri. Analyses of changes in brain anatomy induced by the IC training revealed increases in grey matter volume in the right pars orbitalis and modulations of white matter microstructure in the right pars triangularis. The task-specificity of the effects of training was confirmed by an absence of change in neural activity to a control working memory task. Our combined anatomical and functional findings indicate that differential patterns of functional and structural plasticity between and within inferior frontal gyri enhanced the speed of top-down inhibition processes and in turn IC proficiency. The results suggest that training-based interventions might help overcoming the anatomic and functional deficits of inferior frontal gyri manifesting in inhibition-related clinical conditions. More generally, we demonstrate how multimodal neuroimaging investigations of training-induced neuroplasticity enable revealing novel anatomo-functional dissociations within frontal executive brain networks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Camille F Chavan
- Neurology Unit, Department of Medicine, Faculty of Sciences, University of Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland
| | - Michael Mouthon
- Neurology Unit, Department of Medicine, Faculty of Sciences, University of Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland
| | - Bogdan Draganski
- LREN, Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Vaudois Hospital University Center, University of Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland
| | - Wietske van der Zwaag
- Center for Biomedical Imaging, École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland
| | - Lucas Spierer
- Neurology Unit, Department of Medicine, Faculty of Sciences, University of Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
98
|
Cognitive performances associate with measures of white matter integrity in bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord 2015; 174:342-52. [PMID: 25553397 DOI: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.12.030] [Citation(s) in RCA: 71] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/15/2014] [Revised: 12/05/2014] [Accepted: 12/10/2014] [Indexed: 12/13/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Neuropsychological deficits constitute enduring trait-like features in bipolar disorder (BD), and persist in euthymia. White matter (WM) abnormalities are one of the most consistently reported findings in neuroimaging studies of BD. We hypothesized that neuropsychological performances could correlate with WM integrity in a sample of bipolar patients in core WM tracts. METHODS Seventy-eight patients affected by BD were evaluated for verbal memory, working memory, psychomotor coordination, executive functions, attention and information processing, and verbal fluency through the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia. White matter integrity was evaluated using DTI and tract-based spatial statistics with threshold free cluster enhancement (p>0.949). RESULTS We observed that cognitive performances in attention and information processing, working memory, executive functions and psychomotor coordination were associated with DTI measures of WM integrity in several association fibres: inferior and superior longitudinal fasciculus, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus, cingulum bundle, corpus callosum, and corona radiata. LIMITATION The drug treatments administered during the course of the illness could have influenced DTI measures and neurocognitive function. Other limitations include issues such as generalizability due to the lack of a control group, possible undetected past comorbidities, population stratification, and the presence of a 28% of patients which previously experienced delusions. CONCLUSIONS This is the first study to use a validated cognitive battery to investigate the principal cognitive domains in BD. Our data confirm the importance of WM integrity as a neurobiological underpinning of cognitive deficits.
Collapse
|
99
|
Dupuy O, Gauthier CJ, Fraser SA, Desjardins-Crèpeau L, Desjardins M, Mekary S, Lesage F, Hoge RD, Pouliot P, Bherer L. Higher levels of cardiovascular fitness are associated with better executive function and prefrontal oxygenation in younger and older women. Front Hum Neurosci 2015; 9:66. [PMID: 25741267 PMCID: PMC4332308 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00066] [Citation(s) in RCA: 124] [Impact Index Per Article: 13.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/04/2014] [Accepted: 01/27/2015] [Indexed: 01/10/2023] Open
Abstract
AIM Many studies have suggested that physical exercise training improves cognition and more selectively executive functions. There is a growing interest to clarify the neurophysiological mechanisms that underlie this effect. The aim of the current study was to evaluate the neurophysiological changes in cerebral oxygenation associated with physical fitness level and executive functions. METHOD In this study, 22 younger and 36 older women underwent a maximal graded continuous test (i.e., [Formula: see text]O2max ) in order to classify them into a fitness group (higher vs. lower fit). All participants completed neuropsychological paper and pencil testing and a computerized Stroop task (which contained executive and non-executive conditions) in which the change in prefrontal cortex oxygenation was evaluated with near infrared spectroscopy (NIRS). RESULTS Our findings revealed a Fitness × Condition interaction (p < 0.05) such that higher fit women scored better on measures of executive functions than lower fit women. In comparison to lower fit women, higher fit women had faster reaction times in the Executive condition of the computerized Stroop task. No significant effect was observed in the non-executive condition of the test and no interactions were found with age. In measures of cerebral oxygenation (ΔHbT and ΔHbO2), we found a main effect of fitness on cerebral oxygenation during the Stroop task such that only high fit women demonstrated a significant increase in the right inferior frontal gyrus. DISCUSSION/CONCLUSION Higher fit individuals who demonstrate better cardiorespiratory functions (as measured by [Formula: see text]O2max ) show faster reaction times and greater cerebral oxygenation in the right inferior frontal gyrus than women with lower fitness levels. The lack of interaction with age, suggests that good cardiorespiratory functions can have a positive impact on cognition, regardless of age.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Olivier Dupuy
- Centre PERFORM, Université ConcordiaMontreal, QC, Canada
- Centre de Recherche de l'Institut de Gériatrie de MontréalMontreal, QC, Canada
- Laboratoire MOVE (EA6314), Faculté des Sciences du Sport de Poitiers, Université de PoitiersPoitiers, France
| | - Claudine J. Gauthier
- Centre de Recherche de l'Institut de Gériatrie de MontréalMontreal, QC, Canada
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain SciencesLeipzig, Germany
| | - Sarah A. Fraser
- Centre de Recherche de l'Institut de Gériatrie de MontréalMontreal, QC, Canada
- Department of Social Work, McGill UniversityMontreal, QC, Canada
| | | | - Michèle Desjardins
- Centre de Recherche de l'Institut de Gériatrie de MontréalMontreal, QC, Canada
- Departement de Physiologie, Université de MontréalMontreal, QC, Canada
| | - Said Mekary
- Centre de Recherche de l'Institut de Gériatrie de MontréalMontreal, QC, Canada
| | | | - Rick D. Hoge
- Centre de Recherche de l'Institut de Gériatrie de MontréalMontreal, QC, Canada
- Departement de Physiologie, Université de MontréalMontreal, QC, Canada
| | | | - Louis Bherer
- Centre PERFORM, Université ConcordiaMontreal, QC, Canada
- Centre de Recherche de l'Institut de Gériatrie de MontréalMontreal, QC, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
100
|
|