1
|
Green CD, Kang S, Harvey EA, Jones HA. ADHD in Black Youth: A Content Analysis of Empirical Research from 1972 to 2023. J Atten Disord 2024; 28:1699-1715. [PMID: 39318151 DOI: 10.1177/10870547241285244] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 09/26/2024]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE The purpose of this content analysis study was to characterize patterns of research on Black youth with ADHD. METHOD Relevant articles were identified through searching psychology and medical databases and cross-referencing citations in previously published review articles. The 204 empirical articles included in this study (1) were conducted in the United States, (2) had a predominantly child or adolescent sample, (3) had more than 80% Black youth/families in the study sample and/or conducted separate analyses for Black youth/families, and (4) examined ADHD symptoms or diagnoses. Articles were categorized into five primary content areas: Assessment, Treatment, Perceptions, Prevalence, and Associated Correlates. Journal characteristics, sample characteristics, and methodological characteristics are presented across these content areas. RESULTS Findings show a relatively low representation of Black youth with ADHD in the literature, with most studies using race comparative approaches and secondary data analyses, and many being published in medical journals. CONCLUSION Based on these results, changes are recommended both at the individual study and broader systems levels (e.g., funding agencies). More research, funding, and publications centering Black youth with ADHD are vital to understanding and correcting long-standing health disparities for this community.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cathrin D Green
- Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA
- Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, OH, USA
| | - Sungha Kang
- University of Massachusetts Amherst, Amherst, MA, USA
- Loyola University Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
2
|
Holcomb L, Hall W, Gardiner-Walsh SJ, Scott J. Challenging the "norm": a critical look at deaf-hearing comparison studies in research. JOURNAL OF DEAF STUDIES AND DEAF EDUCATION 2024:enae048. [PMID: 39496187 DOI: 10.1093/jdsade/enae048] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/13/2024] [Revised: 09/10/2024] [Accepted: 10/09/2024] [Indexed: 11/06/2024]
Abstract
This study critically examines the biases and methodological shortcomings in studies comparing deaf and hearing populations, demonstrating their implications for both the reliability and ethics of research in deaf education. Upon reviewing the 20 most-cited deaf-hearing comparison studies, we identified recurring fallacies such as the presumption of hearing ideological biases, the use of heterogeneously small samples, and the misinterpretation of critical variables. Our research reveals a propensity to based conclusions based on the norms of white, hearing, monolingual English speakers. This dependence upholds eugenics ideas and scientific ableism, which reinforces current power dynamics that marginalize the epistemologies and lived experiences of deaf populations. Going forward, it will be imperative for deaf people to be included in meaningful roles in deaf-related research as active contributors who help define the whole research process. Without this shift, the research risks remaining detached from the very populations it seeks to understand.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Leala Holcomb
- University of Tennessee, Knoxville, Knoxville, TN 37996, United States
| | - Wyatte Hall
- University of Rochester, Medical Center, 265 Crittenden Blvd, Rochester, NY 14642, United States
| | - Stephanie J Gardiner-Walsh
- Commonwealth University of Pennsylvania at Bloomsburg, 400 E 2nd St, Bloomsburg, PA 17815, United States
| | - Jessica Scott
- Georgia State University P.O. Box 3965 Atlanta, GA 30302-3965, United States
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Prein JC, Maurits L, Werwach A, Haun DBM, Bohn M. Variation in gaze following across the life span: A process-level perspective. Dev Sci 2024; 27:e13546. [PMID: 38980169 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13546] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/24/2023] [Revised: 06/05/2024] [Accepted: 06/19/2024] [Indexed: 07/10/2024]
Abstract
Following eye gaze is fundamental for many social-cognitive abilities, for example, when judging what another agent can or cannot know. While the emergence of gaze following has been thoroughly studied on a group level, we know little about (a) the developmental trajectory beyond infancy and (b) the sources of individual differences. In Study 1, we examined gaze following across the lifespan (N = 478 3- to 19-year-olds from Leipzig, Germany; and N = 240 20- to 80-year-old international, remotely tested adults). We found a steep performance improvement during preschool years, in which children became more precise in locating the attentional focus of an agent. Precision levels then stayed comparably stable throughout adulthood with a minor decline toward old age. In Study 2, we formalized the process of gaze following in a computational cognitive model that allowed us to conceptualize individual differences in a psychologically meaningful way (N = 60 3- to 5-year-olds, 50 adults). According to our model, participants estimate pupil angles with varying levels of precision based on observing the pupil location within the agent's eyes. In Study 3, we empirically tested how gaze following relates to vector following in non-social settings and perspective-taking abilities (N = 102 4- to 5-year-olds). We found that gaze following is associated with both of these abilities but less so with other Theory of Mind tasks. This work illustrates how the combination of reliable measurement instruments and formal theoretical models allows us to explore the in(ter)dependence of core social-cognitive processes in greater detail. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: Gaze following develops beyond infancy. The highest precision levels in localizing attentional foci are reached in young adulthood with a slight decrease towards old age. We present a computational model that describes gaze following as a process of estimating pupil angles and the corresponding gaze vectors. The model explains individual differences and recovers signature patterns in the data. To estimate the relation between gaze- and vector following, we designed a non-social vector following task. We found substantial correlations between gaze following and vector following, as well as Level 2 perspective-taking. Other Theory of Mind tasks did not correlate.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julia Christin Prein
- Department of Comparative Cultural Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
- Institute of Psychology in Education, Leuphana University Lüneburg, Lüneburg, Germany
| | - Luke Maurits
- Department of Comparative Cultural Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Annika Werwach
- Department of Comparative Cultural Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
- Center for Lifespan Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
- Max Planck School of Cognition, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Daniel B M Haun
- Department of Comparative Cultural Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Manuel Bohn
- Department of Comparative Cultural Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
- Institute of Psychology in Education, Leuphana University Lüneburg, Lüneburg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Singh L. A vision for a diverse, inclusive, equitable, and representative developmental science. Dev Sci 2024; 27:e13548. [PMID: 39091060 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13548] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/25/2024] [Accepted: 06/29/2024] [Indexed: 08/04/2024]
Affiliation(s)
- Leher Singh
- Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Kiar G, Mumford JA, Xu T, Vogelstein JT, Glatard T, Milham MP. Why experimental variation in neuroimaging should be embraced. Nat Commun 2024; 15:9411. [PMID: 39482294 PMCID: PMC11528113 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-024-53743-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/22/2023] [Accepted: 10/21/2024] [Indexed: 11/03/2024] Open
Abstract
In a perfect world, scientists would develop analyses that are guaranteed to reveal the ground truth of a research question. In reality, there are countless viable workflows that produce distinct, often conflicting, results. Although reproducibility places a necessary bound on the validity of results, it is not sufficient for claiming underlying validity, eventual utility, or generalizability. In this work we focus on how embracing variability in data analysis can improve the generalizability of results. We contextualize how design decisions in brain imaging can be made to capture variation, highlight examples, and discuss how variability capture may improve the quality of results.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gregory Kiar
- Center for Data Analytics, Innovation, and Rigor, Child Mind Institute, New York, NY, USA.
| | | | - Ting Xu
- Center for Data Analytics, Innovation, and Rigor, Child Mind Institute, New York, NY, USA
- Center for Integrative Developmental Neuroscience, Child Mind Institute, New York, NY, USA
| | - Joshua T Vogelstein
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Tristan Glatard
- Krembil Centre for Neuroinformatics, The Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Michael P Milham
- Center for Data Analytics, Innovation, and Rigor, Child Mind Institute, New York, NY, USA
- Center for Integrative Developmental Neuroscience, Child Mind Institute, New York, NY, USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Symeonidou M, Doherty MJ, Ross J. Thinking about thinking: A longitudinal investigation linking developments in metacognition, inhibitory control, and theory of mind. J Exp Child Psychol 2024; 249:106103. [PMID: 39418813 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106103] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/11/2024] [Revised: 09/13/2024] [Accepted: 09/14/2024] [Indexed: 10/19/2024]
Abstract
This longitudinal study tracked the developmental relations linking metacognition, theory of mind, and inhibitory control in 52 children across a 1.5-year interval, beginning at 3 or 4 years of age. Metacognition and inhibitory control emerged before theory of mind and predicted subsequent theory of mind competence. Moreover, there was evidence of developmental mediation, whereby metacognition predicted inhibitory control, which predicted theory of mind. We suggest that metacognitive self-reflection may provide the "developmental enrichment" necessary to think about thinking, and when inhibitory control is sufficiently developed this thinking can be extended to complex reasoning about own and other minds.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mariel Symeonidou
- Psychology, School of Humanities, Social Sciences, and Law, University of Dundee, Dundee DD1 4HN, Scotland, UK
| | - Martin J Doherty
- School of Psychology, University of East Anglia, Norwich NR4 7TJ, UK
| | - Josephine Ross
- Psychology, School of Humanities, Social Sciences, and Law, University of Dundee, Dundee DD1 4HN, Scotland, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Lloyd-Fox S, McCann S, Milosavljevic B, Katus L, Blasi A, Bulgarelli C, Crespo-Llado M, Ghillia G, Fadera T, Mbye E, Mason L, Njai F, Njie O, Perapoch-Amado M, Rozhko M, Sosseh F, Saidykhan M, Touray E, Moore SE, Elwell CE. The Brain Imaging for Global Health (BRIGHT) Project: Longitudinal cohort study protocol. Gates Open Res 2024; 7:126. [PMID: 39372355 PMCID: PMC11452580 DOI: 10.12688/gatesopenres.14795.2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 07/30/2024] [Indexed: 10/08/2024] Open
Abstract
There is a scarcity of prospective longitudinal research targeted at early postnatal life which maps developmental pathways of early-stage processing and brain specialisation in the context of early adversity. Follow up from infancy into the one-five year age range is key, as it constitutes a critical gap between infant and early childhood studies. Availability of portable neuroimaging (functional near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) and electroencephalography (EEG)) has enabled access to rural settings increasing the diversity of our sampling and broadening developmental research to include previously underrepresented ethnic-racial and geographical groups in low- and middle- income countries (LMICs). The primary objective of the Brain Imaging for Global Health (BRIGHT) project was to establish brain function - using longitudinal data from mother - for-age reference curves infant dyads living in the UK and rural Gambia and investigate the association between context-associated moderators and developmental trajectories across the first two years of life in The Gambia. In total, 265 participating families were seen during pregnancy, at 7-14 days, 1-, 5-, 8-, 12-, 18- and 24-months post-partum. An additional visit is now underway at 3-5 years to assess pre-school outcomes. The majority of our Gambian cohort live in poverty, but while resource-poor in many factors they commonly experience a rich and beneficial family and caregiving context with multigenerational care and a close-knit supportive community. Understanding the impact of different factors at play in such an environment ( i.e., detrimental undernutrition versus beneficial multigenerational family support) will (i) improve the representativeness of models of general cognitive developmental pathways from birth, (ii) identify causal pathways of altered trajectories associated with early adversity at both individual and group level, and (iii) identify the context-associated moderators ( i.e. social context) that protect development despite the presence of poverty-associated challenges. This will in turn contribute to the development of targeted interventions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah Lloyd-Fox
- Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, England, UK
- Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck University of London, London, England, UK
| | - Sam McCann
- Women's and Children's Health, Kings College London, London, UK
| | - Bosiljka Milosavljevic
- Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, England, UK
- Department of Biological and Experimental Psychology, Queen Mary University of London, London, England, UK
| | - Laura Katus
- School of Human Sciences, University of Greenwich, London, England, UK
| | - Anna Blasi
- Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering, University College London, London, England, UK
| | - Chiara Bulgarelli
- Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck University of London, London, England, UK
- Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering, University College London, London, England, UK
| | - Maria Crespo-Llado
- Institute of Lifecourse and Medical Sciences, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, England, UK
| | - Giulia Ghillia
- Women's and Children's Health, Kings College London, London, UK
| | - Tijan Fadera
- The Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Banjul, The Gambia
| | - Ebrima Mbye
- The Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Banjul, The Gambia
| | - Luke Mason
- Women's and Children's Health, Kings College London, London, UK
| | - Fabakary Njai
- The Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Banjul, The Gambia
| | - Omar Njie
- The Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Banjul, The Gambia
| | | | - Maria Rozhko
- Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, England, UK
| | - Fatima Sosseh
- The Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Banjul, The Gambia
| | - Mariama Saidykhan
- The Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Banjul, The Gambia
| | - Ebou Touray
- The Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Banjul, The Gambia
| | - Sophie E. Moore
- Women's and Children's Health, Kings College London, London, UK
- The Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Banjul, The Gambia
| | - Clare E. Elwell
- Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering, University College London, London, England, UK
| | - The BRIGHT Project team
- Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, England, UK
- Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck University of London, London, England, UK
- Women's and Children's Health, Kings College London, London, UK
- Department of Biological and Experimental Psychology, Queen Mary University of London, London, England, UK
- School of Human Sciences, University of Greenwich, London, England, UK
- Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering, University College London, London, England, UK
- Institute of Lifecourse and Medical Sciences, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, England, UK
- The Medical Research Council Unit The Gambia at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, Banjul, The Gambia
- Psychology, University of East London, London, England, UK
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Nimphy CA, Mitrou V, Elzinga BM, Van der Does W, Aktar E. The Role of Parental Verbal Threat Information in Children's Fear Acquisition: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 2024; 27:714-731. [PMID: 38789695 PMCID: PMC11486780 DOI: 10.1007/s10567-024-00485-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 04/21/2024] [Indexed: 05/26/2024]
Abstract
Children can acquire fears of novel stimuli as a result of listening to parental verbal threat information about these stimuli (i.e., instructional learning). While empirical studies have shown that learning via parental information occurs, the effect size of parental verbal threat information on child fear of a novel stimulus has not yet been measured in a meta-analysis. We conducted a systematic review and meta analysis to assess the effect of parents' verbal statements on their children's fear acquisition. Additionally, we explored potential moderators of this effect, namely, parent and child anxiety levels, as well as child age. WebOfScience, Pubmed, Medline, and PsycINFO were used to identify eligible studies that assessed children's (30 months to 18 years old) fear of novel stimuli after being exposed to parental verbal threat information. We selected 17 studies for the meta-analysis and 18 for the systematic review. The meta-analysis revealed a significant causal effect of parental verbal threat information on children's fear reaction towards novel stimuli [g = 1.26]. No evidence was found for a moderation of verbal learning effects, neither by child or parent anxiety levels nor by child age. The effect of parents' verbal threat information on children's fear of novel stimuli is large and not dependent on anxiety levels or child age.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cosima Anna Nimphy
- Department of Clinical Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands.
| | - Vasiliki Mitrou
- Department of Clinical Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands
| | - Bernet M Elzinga
- Department of Clinical Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands
- Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition (LIBC), Leiden, The Netherlands
| | - Willem Van der Does
- Department of Clinical Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands
- Leiden University Treatment Center (LUBEC), Leiden, The Netherlands
| | - Evin Aktar
- Department of Clinical Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands
- Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition (LIBC), Leiden, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Singh L, Rajendra SJ. Greater attention to socioeconomic status in developmental research can improve the external validity, generalizability, and replicability of developmental science. Dev Sci 2024; 27:e13521. [PMID: 38661538 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13521] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/08/2023] [Accepted: 04/05/2024] [Indexed: 04/26/2024]
Abstract
Psychological researchers have been criticized for making broad presumptions about human behavior based on limited sampling. In part, presumptive generalizability is reflected in the limited representation of sociodemographic variation in research reports. In this analysis, we examine time-trends in reporting of a key sociodemographic construct relevant to many aspects of child development-socioeconomic status (SES)-across six mainstream developmental journals (Infancy, Child Development, Developmental Science, Developmental Psychology, Infant and Child Development, and Infant Behavior & Development) between 2016 and 2022. Findings point to limited reporting of SES across developmental journals and across time. Reporting rates varied significantly by region and by topic of development. In terms of specific indicators of SES, there was consistent use of income and caregiver education as SES indicators. The epistemic costs of the lack of integration of socio-economic factors in developmental research are addressed. Pathways to greater integration of SES are proposed. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: We analyzed reporting and representation of socioeconomic status in published studies on early child development. A large proportion of published studies did not report any socio-economic information. Suggestions for greater attention to socioeconomic status are proposed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Leher Singh
- Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore
| | - Sarah J Rajendra
- Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Hermida MJ, Narea M, Singh L, Cristia A. Insights into infant behavior and development from Latin America. Infant Behav Dev 2024; 76:101979. [PMID: 39116706 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101979] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 08/10/2024]
Affiliation(s)
- Maria Julia Hermida
- Universidad Nacional de Hurlingham-Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (UNAHUR-CONICET), Argentina.
| | - Marigen Narea
- Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, School of Psychology and Center for Advanced Studies on Educational Justice (CJE), Chile
| | - Leher Singh
- Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore
| | - Alejandrina Cristia
- Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et de Psycholinguistique, Département d'études cognitives, ENS, EHESS, CNRS, PSL University, France
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Haskaraca FN, Ilgaz H. Culturally constituted universals: Evidential basis of belief matters. Dev Sci 2024; 27:e13398. [PMID: 37062958 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13398] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/02/2022] [Revised: 01/16/2023] [Accepted: 03/16/2023] [Indexed: 04/18/2023]
Abstract
Differences in the sequence with which children pass the tasks in Wellman and Liu's (2004) theory of mind (ToM) battery is increasingly bringing into question the universal and cultural specifics of children's developing understanding of others' minds. Children from China, Iran, and Turkey pass the knowledge access (KA) task of the battery earlier than they pass the diverse beliefs (DB) task (e.g., Selcuk et al., 2018). This pattern is the reverse of what has been documented with children from Australia and the US (e.g., Peterson et al., 2005). This paper presents three studies with Turkish samples that explore the possible reasons for developmental sequence and performance differences in the ToM battery. Study 1 investigated Turkish-speaking adults' judgments of appropriateness for different epistemic verbs as used in the DB and false belief (FB) tasks. Study 2 investigated whether adults' performance (i.e., accuracy, reaction time) on FB tasks were affected by culturally preferred uses of these verbs. Collectively these studies showed that adults found different epistemic language ("guess," "think," and the Turkish-specific "falsely think") to be appropriate for different belief-based tasks. However, there was no difference in adults' performance based on epistemic language. In Study 3, Turkish-speaking preschoolers' performance in belief-related tasks based on variations in epistemic language and epistemic features (i.e., presence of evidence) was investigated. Among five modifications, Turkish children benefited only from a modification that involved the manipulation of the epistemological basis for ambiguous beliefs (i.e., visual evidence for belief). RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: Turkish adults find different epistemic language ("guess," "think," and the Turkish-specific "falsely-think") to be appropriate for different belief-based tasks (diverse and false belief tasks). Turkish adults' performance on false belief (FB) tasks is not affected by epistemic language. Turkish children perform better in diverse beliefs (DB) task if an epistemological basis for ambiguous beliefs (i.e., visual evidence for either belief) is provided.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Feride Nur Haskaraca
- Department of Developmental Psychology, University of Göttingen, Göttingen, Germany
| | - Hande Ilgaz
- Psychology Department, Bilkent University, Ankara, Turkey
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Carmiol AM, Castro S, Castro-Rojas MD, Weisleder A, Robalino J. Links between booksharing and early vocabulary development in Costa Rica. Infant Behav Dev 2024; 76:101958. [PMID: 38772284 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101958] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/20/2023] [Revised: 04/24/2024] [Accepted: 05/10/2024] [Indexed: 05/23/2024]
Abstract
Early vocabulary development is crucial for future cognitive and academic outcomes, and parent-child booksharing has been recognized as a powerful home literacy practice to promote word learning. However, evidence about the link between booksharing and language development in diverse cultural and socioeconomic settings is currently limited, hindering the formulation of a broadly applicable framework to understand the favorable conditions for early vocabulary development. This study explores the relationship between booksharing and early receptive and expressive vocabulary in a sample of 183 mothers and their toddlers in Costa Rica, a context where reading is not a common practice and children have limited access to books. Mothers completed an interview about their booksharing practices and reported children's receptive and expressive vocabulary. Results demonstrated a positive link between maternal booksharing and children's expressive vocabulary. Child gender moderated the link between booksharing and receptive vocabulary, exhibiting a stronger association in girls than in boys. Mothers with lower education levels reported higher expressive vocabulary scores for their children than mothers with higher education levels. These findings underscore the significance of booksharing in the home literacy environment, even in cultural contexts with distinct reading practices. Moreover, they highlight the need to incorporate sociocultural factors into comprehensive accounts concerning the role of booksharing in early word learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ana M Carmiol
- Instituto de Investigaciones Psicológicas, Universidad de Costa Rica, Costa Rica.
| | - Susan Castro
- Instituto de Investigaciones Psicológicas, Universidad de Costa Rica, Costa Rica
| | - María Dolores Castro-Rojas
- Instituto de Investigaciones Psicológicas, Universidad de Costa Rica, Costa Rica; Escuela de Psicología, Universidad Nacional de Costa Rica, Costa Rica
| | - Adriana Weisleder
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Northwestern University, USA
| | - Juan Robalino
- Instituto de Investigaciones en Ciencias Económicas, Universidad de Costa Rica, Costa Rica
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Singh L, Bortfeld H. Towards a global developmental science. Dev Sci 2024; 27:e13555. [PMID: 39075676 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13555] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 07/31/2024]
Affiliation(s)
- Leher Singh
- Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Merced, Singapore
| | | |
Collapse
|
14
|
Symeonidou M, Mizokawa A, Kabaya S, Doherty MJ, Ross J. Contrasting one's share of the shared life space: Comparing the roles of metacognition and inhibitory control in the development of theory of mind among Scottish and Japanese children. Dev Sci 2024; 27:e13417. [PMID: 37408284 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13417] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/23/2022] [Revised: 04/24/2023] [Accepted: 05/12/2023] [Indexed: 07/07/2023]
Abstract
Cultural comparisons suggest that an understanding of other minds may develop sooner in independent versus interdependent settings, and vice versa for inhibitory control. From a western lens, this pattern might be considered paradoxical, since there is a robust positive relationship between theory of mind (ToM) and inhibitory control in western samples. In independent cultures, an emphasis on one's own mind offers a clear route to 'simulate' other minds, and inhibitory control may be required to set aside one's own perspective to represent the perspective of others. However, in interdependent cultures, social norms are considered the key catalyst for behaviour, and metacognitive reflection and/or suppression of one's own perspective may not be necessary. The cross-cultural generalizability of the western developmental route to ToM is therefore questionable. The current study used an age-matched cross-sectional sample to contrast 56 Japanese and 56 Scottish 3-6-year-old's metacognition, ToM and inhibitory control skills. We replicated the expected cultural patterns for ToM (Scotland > Japan) and inhibitory control (Japan > Scotland). Supporting western developmental enrichment theories, we find that inhibitory control and metacognition predict theory of mind competence in Scotland. However, these variables cannot be used to predict Japanese ToM. This confirms that individualistic mechanisms do not capture the developmental mechanism underlying ToM in Japan, highlighting a bias in our understanding of ToM development. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: We replicate an independent cultural advantage for theory of mind (Scotland > Japan) and interdependent advantage for inhibitory control (Japan > Scotland). From a western lens, this pattern might be considered paradoxical, since there is a robust positive relationship between theory of mind and inhibitory control. Supporting western developmental enrichment theories, we find that the development of inhibitory control mediates the link between metacognition and theory of mind in Scotland. However, this model does not predict Japanese theory of mind, highlighting an individualistic bias in our mechanistic understanding of theory of mind development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mariel Symeonidou
- Psychology, School of Humanities, Social Sciences and Law, University of Dundee, Dundee, Scotland
| | - Ai Mizokawa
- Graduate School of Education and Human Development, Nagoya University, Nagoya, Japan
| | - Shinsuke Kabaya
- Department of Psychology, Aichi Shukutoku University, Nagakute, Aichi, Japan
| | | | - Josephine Ross
- Psychology, School of Humanities, Social Sciences and Law, University of Dundee, Dundee, Scotland
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Stengelin R, Maurits L, Hepach R, Haun DBM. Children's risk preferences vary across sexes, social contexts, and cultures. COMMUNICATIONS PSYCHOLOGY 2024; 2:79. [PMID: 39242761 PMCID: PMC11343856 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00127-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/02/2023] [Accepted: 08/07/2024] [Indexed: 09/09/2024]
Abstract
People exhibit more risk-prone behaviors when together with peers than when in private. The interplay of social context effects and other variables that alter human risk preferences (i.e., age, sex, or culture) remains poorly understood. Here, we explored risk preferences among Namibian Hai||om and Ovambo children (N = 144; AgeRange = 6-10 years). Participants chose between risky and safe options in private or during peer presence. In a third condition, children collaborated with peers before their risk preferences were assessed in those peers' presence. Children from both societies were risk-averse, but Hai||om children showed greater risk aversion than their Ovambo counterparts. Across cultures and ages, boys were less averse to risks than girls. This effect was most pronounced during peer presence, whereas collaboration did not additionally affect risk preferences. These results suggest a dynamic interplay of individual, social, and cultural factors shaping children's risk preferences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Roman Stengelin
- Department of Comparative Cultural Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany.
- Department of Psychology and Social Work, University of Namibia, Windhoek, Namibia.
| | - Luke Maurits
- Department of Comparative Cultural Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Robert Hepach
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
| | - Daniel Benjamin Moritz Haun
- Department of Comparative Cultural Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Ma S, Cui YK, Wan S, Chen EE, Corriveau KH. Children consider informants' explanation quality with their social dominance in seeking novel explanations. Child Dev 2024. [PMID: 39099094 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14148] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 08/06/2024]
Abstract
Identifying high-quality causal explanations is key to scientific understanding. This research (N = 202; 50% girls; Mage: 5.82 years; 64% Asian, 33% White, and 3% multiracial; data collected from 2018 to 2024) examined how explanation circularity and informants' social dominance impact children's learning preferences for causal explanations. Raised in a culture valuing circular logic, Chinese children still preferred non-circular explanations and learning from informants providing non-circular explanations (d ≥ 0.50). When informants with non-circular explanations were subordinate to those with circular explanations, Chinese and American children preferred non-circular over circular explanations (d = 1.10), but did not prefer learning new information from either informant. Although children weigh explanation quality over informant dominance when seeking explanations for given questions, they consider both cues when evaluating informants' credibility.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shaocong Ma
- Hong Kong University of Science and Technology, Hong Kong, China
| | - Yixin K Cui
- Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
| | - Shan Wan
- Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
| | - Eva E Chen
- Hong Kong University of Science and Technology, Hong Kong, China
- National Tsing Hua University, Hsinchu City, Taiwan
| | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Lo CH, Hermes J, Kartushina N, Mayor J, Mani N. e-Babylab: An open-source browser-based tool for unmoderated online developmental studies. Behav Res Methods 2024; 56:4530-4552. [PMID: 37620744 PMCID: PMC11289032 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-023-02200-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 07/12/2023] [Indexed: 08/26/2023]
Abstract
The COVID-19 pandemic massively changed the context and feasibility of developmental research. This new reality, as well as considerations about sample diversity and naturalistic settings for developmental research, highlights the need for solutions for online studies. In this article, we present e-Babylab, an open-source browser-based tool for unmoderated online studies targeted for young children and babies. e-Babylab offers an intuitive graphical user interface for study creation and management of studies, users, participant data, and stimulus material, with no programming skills required. Various kinds of audiovisual media can be presented as stimuli, and possible measures include webcam recordings, audio recordings, key presses, mouse-click/touch coordinates, and reaction times. An additional feature of e-Babylab is the possibility to administer short adaptive versions of MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories (Chai et al. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 63, 3488-3500, 2020). Information pages, consent forms, and participant forms are customizable. e-Babylab has been used with a variety of measures and paradigms in over 12 studies with children aged 12 months to 8 years (n = 1516). We briefly summarize some results of these studies to demonstrate that data quality, participant engagement, and overall results are comparable between laboratory and online settings. Finally, we discuss helpful tips for using e-Babylab and present plans for upgrades.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chang Huan Lo
- School of Psychology, University of Nottingham Malaysia, Semenyih, Malaysia
| | - Jonas Hermes
- Department of Developmental Psychology, Institute of Psychology, University of Göttingen, Waldweg 26, D-37073, Göttingen, Germany
| | - Natalia Kartushina
- Center for Multilingualism in Society across the Lifespan (Multiling), University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
- Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
| | - Julien Mayor
- Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
| | - Nivedita Mani
- Leibniz ScienceCampus Primate Cognition, Kellnerweg 4, D-37077, Göttingen, Germany.
- Department of Psychology of Language, Institute of Psychology, University of Göttingen, Goßlerstr. 14, D-37073, Göttingen, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Anlló H, Bavard S, Benmarrakchi F, Bonagura D, Cerrotti F, Cicue M, Gueguen M, Guzmán EJ, Kadieva D, Kobayashi M, Lukumon G, Sartorio M, Yang J, Zinchenko O, Bahrami B, Silva Concha J, Hertz U, Konova AB, Li J, O'Madagain C, Navajas J, Reyes G, Sarabi-Jamab A, Shestakova A, Sukumaran B, Watanabe K, Palminteri S. Comparing experience- and description-based economic preferences across 11 countries. Nat Hum Behav 2024; 8:1554-1567. [PMID: 38877287 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-024-01894-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/23/2023] [Accepted: 04/19/2024] [Indexed: 06/16/2024]
Abstract
Recent evidence indicates that reward value encoding in humans is highly context dependent, leading to suboptimal decisions in some cases, but whether this computational constraint on valuation is a shared feature of human cognition remains unknown. Here we studied the behaviour of n = 561 individuals from 11 countries of markedly different socioeconomic and cultural makeup. Our findings show that context sensitivity was present in all 11 countries. Suboptimal decisions generated by context manipulation were not explained by risk aversion, as estimated through a separate description-based choice task (that is, lotteries) consisting of matched decision offers. Conversely, risk aversion significantly differed across countries. Overall, our findings suggest that context-dependent reward value encoding is a feature of human cognition that remains consistently present across different countries, as opposed to description-based decision-making, which is more permeable to cultural factors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hernán Anlló
- Human Reinforcement Learning Team, Laboratory of Cognitive and Computational Neuroscience, Paris, France.
- Faculty of Science and Engineering, Waseda University, Tokyo, Japan.
- Intercultural Cognitive Network, Paris, France.
| | - Sophie Bavard
- Human Reinforcement Learning Team, Laboratory of Cognitive and Computational Neuroscience, Paris, France
- Intercultural Cognitive Network, Paris, France
- General Psychology Lab, Hamburg University, Hamburg, Germany
| | - FatimaEzzahra Benmarrakchi
- Intercultural Cognitive Network, Paris, France
- School of Collective Intelligence, Université Mohammed VI Polytechnique, Rabat, Morocco
| | - Darla Bonagura
- Intercultural Cognitive Network, Paris, France
- Department of Psychiatry, University Behavioral Health Care and Brain Health Institute, Rutgers University-New Brunswick, Piscataway, NJ, USA
| | - Fabien Cerrotti
- Human Reinforcement Learning Team, Laboratory of Cognitive and Computational Neuroscience, Paris, France
- Intercultural Cognitive Network, Paris, France
| | - Mirona Cicue
- Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
| | - Maelle Gueguen
- Intercultural Cognitive Network, Paris, France
- Department of Psychiatry, University Behavioral Health Care and Brain Health Institute, Rutgers University-New Brunswick, Piscataway, NJ, USA
| | - Eugenio José Guzmán
- Facultad de Psicología, Universidad del Desarrollo, Santiago de Chile, Chile
| | - Dzerassa Kadieva
- International Laboratory for Social Neurobiology, Institute for Cognitive Neuroscience, HSE University, Moscow, Russia
| | - Maiko Kobayashi
- Faculty of Science and Engineering, Waseda University, Tokyo, Japan
| | - Gafari Lukumon
- School of Collective Intelligence, Université Mohammed VI Polytechnique, Rabat, Morocco
| | - Marco Sartorio
- Laboratorio de Neurociencia, Universidad Torcuato Di Tella, Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Jiong Yang
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Oksana Zinchenko
- Intercultural Cognitive Network, Paris, France
- Centre for Cognition and Decision Making, Institute for Cognitive Neuroscience, HSE University, Moscow, Russia
| | - Bahador Bahrami
- Intercultural Cognitive Network, Paris, France
- Department of Psychology, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany
| | - Jaime Silva Concha
- Intercultural Cognitive Network, Paris, France
- Facultad de Psicología, Universidad del Desarrollo, Santiago de Chile, Chile
| | - Uri Hertz
- Intercultural Cognitive Network, Paris, France
- Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
| | - Anna B Konova
- Intercultural Cognitive Network, Paris, France
- Department of Psychiatry, University Behavioral Health Care and Brain Health Institute, Rutgers University-New Brunswick, Piscataway, NJ, USA
| | - Jian Li
- Intercultural Cognitive Network, Paris, France
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
- IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Cathal O'Madagain
- Intercultural Cognitive Network, Paris, France
- School of Collective Intelligence, Université Mohammed VI Polytechnique, Rabat, Morocco
| | - Joaquin Navajas
- Intercultural Cognitive Network, Paris, France
- Laboratorio de Neurociencia, Universidad Torcuato Di Tella, Buenos Aires, Argentina
- Escuela de Negocios, Universidad Torcuato Di Tella, Buenos Aires, Argentina
- Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas, Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Gabriel Reyes
- Intercultural Cognitive Network, Paris, France
- Facultad de Psicología, Universidad del Desarrollo, Santiago de Chile, Chile
| | - Atiye Sarabi-Jamab
- Intercultural Cognitive Network, Paris, France
- School of Cognitive Sciences, Institute for Research in Fundamental Sciences, Tehran, Iran
| | - Anna Shestakova
- Intercultural Cognitive Network, Paris, France
- Centre for Cognition and Decision Making, Institute for Cognitive Neuroscience, HSE University, Moscow, Russia
| | - Bhasi Sukumaran
- Intercultural Cognitive Network, Paris, France
- Department of Clinical Psychology, SRM Medical College Hospital and Research Centre, Chennai, India
| | - Katsumi Watanabe
- Faculty of Science and Engineering, Waseda University, Tokyo, Japan
- Intercultural Cognitive Network, Paris, France
| | - Stefano Palminteri
- Human Reinforcement Learning Team, Laboratory of Cognitive and Computational Neuroscience, Paris, France.
- Intercultural Cognitive Network, Paris, France.
- Departement d'études cognitives, Ecole normale supérieure, PSL Research University, Paris, France.
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Ünlütabak B, Trujillo Hernandez G, Velioğlu İ, Menendez D, Rosengren KS. Children's questions and teachers' responses about COVID-19 in Türkiye and the US. PLoS One 2024; 19:e0307475. [PMID: 39037981 PMCID: PMC11262656 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0307475] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/22/2024] [Accepted: 07/06/2024] [Indexed: 07/24/2024] Open
Abstract
Question-asking is a crucial tool for acquiring information about unseen entities, such as viruses; thus, examining children's questions within the context of COVID-19 is particularly important for understanding children's learning about the coronavirus. The study examined 3-12-year-old children's questions and teachers' responses about the COVID-19 pandemic in Türkiye, a non-Western developing context, and the United States, a Western cultural context. A total of 119 teachers from Türkiye and 95 teachers from the US participated in the study. Teachers completed an online survey consisting of a demographic form and a questionnaire asking them to report three questions about COVID-19 asked by children in their classrooms and their responses to these questions. We analyzed children's questions and teachers' responses for their type and content and examined demographic factors associated with children's questions and teachers' responses. Consistent with the literature, children from Türkiye asked fewer explanation-seeking (i.e., why/how) questions than children from the United States. Children asked questions about viruses and precautions. Teachers responded to children's questions realistically in both countries. The findings have important implications for how children gain knowledge from teachers when discussing health, disease, and virus topics in two countries.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Burcu Ünlütabak
- Department of Psychology, Yeditepe University, Istanbul, Türkiye
| | | | - İlayda Velioğlu
- Department of Psychology, Kadir Has University, Istanbul, Türkiye
| | - David Menendez
- Department of Psychology, University of California-Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz, California, United States of America
| | - Karl S. Rosengren
- Department of Psychology, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, United States of America
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Kick L, Schleicher D, Ecker A, Kandsperger S, Brunner R, Jarvers I. Alexithymia as a mediator between adverse childhood events and the development of psychopathology: a meta-analysis. Front Psychiatry 2024; 15:1412229. [PMID: 39011338 PMCID: PMC11246998 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1412229] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/04/2024] [Accepted: 06/07/2024] [Indexed: 07/17/2024] Open
Abstract
Introduction Victims of child abuse have an elevated risk of developing mental health issues later in life. Several variables have been suggested as mediators of this correlation, but little is known about the possible influence of alexithymia. Alexithymia is a sub-clinical personality trait that manifests as difficulties recognizing and verbalizing emotions. Methods In this study, two separate meta-analyses were conducted using questionnaire data, and Pearson correlations for overall effects were estimated. Results The correlation between child abuse and alexithymia showed to be significant (r = .26), as did the correlation between alexithymia and general psychopathology (r = .44). Further analyses revealed no indication for possible publication bias. When investigating differences between various subtypes of child maltreatment, each subtype significantly correlated with alexithymia. Emotional abuse, emotional neglect, and physical neglect had stronger correlations than physical and sexual abuse. Discussion These results suggest that alexithymia plays a mediating role, at least in part, in the relationship between experiences of child abuse and general psychopathology in adulthood. Therefore, alexithymia may be relevant to further research and deserves attention in the prevention of and therapy for mental health issues in victims of child abuse.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lorenz Kick
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany
| | - Daniel Schleicher
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany
| | - Angelika Ecker
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany
| | - Stephanie Kandsperger
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany
| | - Romuald Brunner
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany
| | - Irina Jarvers
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Cvencek D, Brečić R, Sanders EA, Gaćeša D, Skala D, Meltzoff AN. Am I a good person? Academic correlates of explicit and implicit self-esteem during early childhood. Child Dev 2024; 95:1047-1062. [PMID: 38148568 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14052] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/01/2022] [Revised: 10/26/2023] [Accepted: 11/25/2023] [Indexed: 12/28/2023]
Abstract
Implicit and explicit self-esteem are not commonly measured in the same children. Using a cross-sectional design, data from 354 Croatian children (184 girls) in Grade 1 (Mage = 7.55 years) and Grade 5 (Mage = 11.58 years) were collected in Spring 2019. All children completed explicit and implicit self-esteem measures; math and language grades were obtained. For the explicit measure, older children showed lower self-esteem than younger children, and girls showed lower self-esteem than boys. For the implicit measure, there were no age effects, and girls showed higher self-esteem than boys. Although both types of self-esteem were positively associated with academic achievement, implicit self-esteem was associated more strongly with language than with math achievement. Discussion is provided about why self-esteem relates to academic achievement during childhood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dario Cvencek
- Institute for Learning & Brain Sciences, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington, USA
| | - Ružica Brečić
- Department of Marketing, Faculty of Economics and Business, University of Zagreb, Zagreb, Croatia
| | - Elizabeth A Sanders
- Measurement and Statistics, College of Education, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington, USA
| | - Dora Gaćeša
- Department of Marketing, Faculty of Economics and Business, University of Zagreb, Zagreb, Croatia
| | - David Skala
- Department of Marketing, Faculty of Economics and Business, University of Zagreb, Zagreb, Croatia
| | - Andrew N Meltzoff
- Institute for Learning & Brain Sciences, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington, USA
- Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington, USA
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
van der Miesen AIR, Shi SY, Lei HC, Ngan CL, VanderLaan DP, Wong WI. Gender diversity in a Chinese community sample and its associations with autism traits. Autism Res 2024; 17:1407-1416. [PMID: 38100234 DOI: 10.1002/aur.3075] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/30/2023] [Accepted: 11/27/2023] [Indexed: 07/26/2024]
Abstract
Emerging evidence suggests that gender dysphoria or gender diversity (GD) intersects frequently with autism spectrum disorder or autism traits. However, the magnitude and interpretation of this link continue to be debated. Most child studies on this topic were performed in clinical populations, and little is known about the generalizability of this co-occurrence to the broader community, especially to non-Western samples. Also, little is known about whether specific subdomains of autism are more strongly associated with GD. Therefore, we investigated GD and its association with autism traits in a Chinese community sample of 4-12-year-olds (N = 379; 51% birth-assigned girls). Parents provided information about GD characteristics using the standardized Gender Identity Questionnaire for Children and autism traits using the Chinese version of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient-Children. In addition, broader behavioral and emotional challenges were measured by the Behavior Problem Index (BPI) to account for psychological challenges other than autism traits. In this community sample of Chinese children, increased GD was associated with increased autism traits, even after accounting for the BPI. Of the four subscales, the Imagination and Patterns subscales in birth-assigned girls and the Imagination subscale in birth-assigned boys were especially associated with GD. These findings indicate that the association between GD and autism traits generalizes to a nonclinical, non-Western sample. Clinicians and researchers working with clinical as well as community children should thus pay attention to the co-occurrence of GD and autism traits, in and outside the West.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anna I R van der Miesen
- Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Center of Expertise on Gender Dysphoria, VU University Medical Center, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Sylvia Yun Shi
- Gender Studies Programme and Department of Psychology, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Shatin, Hong Kong
- Department of Psychology, The University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam, Hong Kong
| | - Hoi Ching Lei
- Department of Psychology, The University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam, Hong Kong
| | - Cho Lam Ngan
- Department of Psychology, The University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam, Hong Kong
| | - Doug P VanderLaan
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Mississauga, Ontario, Canada
- Child and Youth Psychiatry, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| | - Wang Ivy Wong
- Gender Studies Programme and Department of Psychology, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Shatin, Hong Kong
- Department of Psychology, The University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam, Hong Kong
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Kim MH, Buford K, Ellis A, Davis-Kean PE, Antony C, Braun C, Hurst T, Todd J. A metascience investigation of inclusive, open, and reproducible science practices in research posters at the 2021 SRCD biennial meeting. Child Dev 2024; 95:1109-1123. [PMID: 38102780 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14059] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/17/2023]
Abstract
Over the past decade, there has been a growing appreciation of metascience issues in psychological science. Using data collected from 2615 posters presented at the 2021 biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, this article examines the use of transparent research practices to increase rigor and reproducibility as well as generalizability through greater inclusivity of diverse samples. Research presented through poster presentations was heavily skewed toward quantitative studies featuring American researchers using Western hemisphere samples. Sharing of data/materials, preregistrations, and replications were uncommon. During a time when governments are increasingly requiring more open practices and access, this research provides an important baseline by which developmental science can benchmark progress toward the goals of greater inclusivity and openness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matthew H Kim
- Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky, USA
| | - Kristen Buford
- Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky, USA
| | - Alexa Ellis
- Department of Human Development and Family Studies, University of Alabama, Tuscaloosa, Alabama, USA
| | - Pamela E Davis-Kean
- Department of Psychology and the Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan, USA
| | - Chellam Antony
- Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky, USA
| | - Claire Braun
- Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky, USA
| | - Tabetha Hurst
- Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky, USA
| | - Julia Todd
- Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky, USA
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Chuey A, Boyce V, Cao A, Frank MC. Conducting Developmental Research Online vs. In-Person: A Meta-Analysis. Open Mind (Camb) 2024; 8:795-808. [PMID: 38957506 PMCID: PMC11219065 DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00147] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/21/2023] [Accepted: 05/01/2024] [Indexed: 07/04/2024] Open
Abstract
An increasing number of psychological experiments with children are being conducted using online platforms, in part due to the COVID-19 pandemic. Individual replications have compared the findings of particular experiments online and in-person, but the general effect of data collection method on data collected from children is still unknown. Therefore, the goal of the current meta-analysis is to estimate the average difference in effect size for developmental studies conducted online compared to the same studies conducted in-person. Our pre-registered analysis includes 211 effect sizes calculated from 30 papers with 3282 children, ranging in age from four months to six years. The estimated effect size for studies conducted online was slightly smaller than for their counterparts conducted in-person, a difference of d = -.05, but this difference was not significant, 95% CI = [-.17, .07]. We examined several potential moderators of the effect of online testing, including the role of dependent measure (looking vs verbal), online study method (moderated vs unmoderated), and age, but none of these were significant. The literature to date thus suggests-on average-small differences in results between in-person and online experimentation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aaron Chuey
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
| | - Veronica Boyce
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
| | - Anjie Cao
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
| | - Michael C. Frank
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Gumančík J, Cornelissen PL, Brokjøb LG, Ridley BJ, McCarty K, Tovée MJ, Cornelissen KK. Testing the validity of online psychophysical measurement of body image perception. PLoS One 2024; 19:e0302747. [PMID: 38857270 PMCID: PMC11164378 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0302747] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/16/2024] [Accepted: 04/10/2024] [Indexed: 06/12/2024] Open
Abstract
This body image study tests the viability of transferring a complex psychophysical paradigm from a controlled in-person laboratory task to an online environment. 172 female participants made online judgements about their own body size when viewing images of computer-generated female bodies presented in either in front-view or at 45-degrees in a method of adjustment (MOA) paradigm. The results of these judgements were then compared to the results of two laboratory-based studies (with 96 and 40 female participants respectively) to establish three key findings. Firstly, the results show that the accuracy of online and in-lab estimates of body size are comparable, secondly that the same patterns of visual biases in judgements are shown both in-lab and online, and thirdly online data shows the same view-orientation advantage in accuracy in body size judgements as the laboratory studies. Thus, this study suggests that that online sampling potentially represents a rapid and accurate way of collecting reliable complex behavioural and perceptual data from a more diverse range of participants than is normally sampled in laboratory-based studies. It also offers the potential for designing stratified sampling strategies to construct a truly representative sample of a target population.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jiří Gumančík
- Department of Psychology, Northumbria University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom
| | - Piers L. Cornelissen
- Department of Psychology, Northumbria University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom
| | - Lise Gulli Brokjøb
- Department of Psychology, The Artic University of Norway, Tromsø, Norway
| | - Bethany J. Ridley
- Department of Psychology, Northumbria University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom
| | - Kristofor McCarty
- Department of Psychology, Northumbria University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom
| | - Martin J. Tovée
- Department of Psychology, Northumbria University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom
| | - Katri K. Cornelissen
- Department of Psychology, Northumbria University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Casillas M, Casey K. Daylong egocentric recordings in small- and large-scale language communities: A practical introduction. ADVANCES IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT AND BEHAVIOR 2024; 66:29-53. [PMID: 39074924 DOI: 10.1016/bs.acdb.2024.05.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 07/31/2024]
Abstract
Daylong egocentric (i.e., participant-centered) recordings promise an unprecedented view into the experiences that drive early language learning, impacting both assumptions and theories about how learning happens. Thanks to recent advances in technology, collecting long-form audio, photo, and video recordings with child-worn devices is cheaper and more convenient than ever. These recording methods can be similarly deployed across small- and large-scale language communities around the world, opening up enormous possibilities for comparative research on early language development. However, building new high-quality naturalistic corpora is a massive investment of time and money. In this chapter, we provide a practical look into considerations relevant for developing and managing daylong egocentric recording projects: Is it possible to re-use existing data? How much time will manual annotation take? Can automated tools sufficiently tackle the questions at hand? We conclude by outlining two exciting directions for future naturalistic child language research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marisa Casillas
- Comparative Human Development Department, University of Chicago.
| | | |
Collapse
|
27
|
Foushee R, Srinivasan M. Infants who are rarely spoken to nevertheless understand many words. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2024; 121:e2311425121. [PMID: 38814865 PMCID: PMC11161804 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2311425121] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/06/2023] [Accepted: 02/16/2024] [Indexed: 06/01/2024] Open
Abstract
Theories of language development-informed largely by studies of Western, middleclass infants-have highlighted the language that caregivers direct to children as a key driver of language learning. However, some have argued that language development unfolds similarly across environmental contexts, including those in which childdirected language is scarce. This raises the possibility that children are able to learn from other sources of language in their environments, particularly the language directed to others in their environment. We explore this hypothesis with infants in an indigenous Tseltal-speaking community in Southern Mexico who are rarely spoken to, yet have the opportunity to overhear a great deal of other-directed language by virtue of being carried on their mothers' backs. Adapting a previously established gaze-tracking method for detecting early word knowledge to our field setting, we find that Tseltal infants exhibit implicit knowledge of common nouns (Exp. 1), analogous to their US peers who are frequently spoken to. Moreover, they exhibit comprehension of Tseltal honorific terms that are exclusively used to greet adults in the community (Exp. 2), representing language that could only have been learned through overhearing. In so doing, Tseltal infants demonstrate an ability to discriminate words with similar meanings and perceptually similar referents at an earlier age than has been shown among Western children. Together, these results suggest that for some infants, learning from overhearing may be an important path toward developing language.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ruthe Foushee
- Department of Psychology, New School for Social Research, New York, NY10011
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, CA94705
| | - Mahesh Srinivasan
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, CA94705
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Begum Ali J, Holman R, Goodwin AL, Heraty S, Jones EJ. Parent attitudes towards data sharing in developmental science. OPEN RESEARCH EUROPE 2024; 3:182. [PMID: 39005631 PMCID: PMC11245672 DOI: 10.12688/openreseurope.16516.2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 05/15/2024] [Indexed: 07/16/2024]
Abstract
Background Data sharing in developmental science is increasingly encouraged, supported by funder and publisher mandates for open data access. Data sharing can accelerate discovery, link researchers with high quality analytic expertise to researchers with large datasets and democratise the research landscape to enable researchers with limited funding to access large sample sizes. However, there are also significant privacy and security concerns, in addition to conceptual and ethical considerations. These are particularly acute for developmental science, where child participants cannot consent themselves. As we move forward into a new era of data openness, it is essential that we adequately represent the views of stakeholder communities in designing data sharing efforts. Methods We conducted a comprehensive survey of the opinions of 195 parents on data sharing in developmental science. Survey themes included how widely parents are willing to share their child's data, which type of organisations they would share the data with and the type of consent they would be comfortable providing. Results Results showed that parents were generally supportive of curated, but not open, data sharing. In addition to individual privacy and security concerns, more altruistic considerations around the purpose of research were important. Parents overwhelmingly supported nuanced consenting models in which preferences for particular types of data sharing could be changed over time. This model is different to that implemented in the vast majority of developmental science research and is contrary to many funder or publisher mandates. Conclusions The field should look to create shared repositories that implement features such as dynamic consent and mechanisms for curated sharing that allow consideration of the scientific questions addressed. Better communication and outreach are required to build trust in data sharing, and advanced analytic methods will be required to understand the impact of selective sharing on reproducibility and representativeness of research datasets.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jannath Begum Ali
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck University of London, London, England, UK
| | - Rebecca Holman
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck University of London, London, England, UK
| | - Amy L. Goodwin
- Department of Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, King's College London, London, England, UK
| | - Siofra Heraty
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck University of London, London, England, UK
| | - Emily J.H. Jones
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck University of London, London, England, UK
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Santos JA, Giovannetti F, Smulski MC, Hermida MJ, Petetta DR, Segretin MS, Lipina S. A systematic review of the concept of self-regulation in infants between 0 and 36 months in Latin America. Infant Behav Dev 2024; 75:101954. [PMID: 38763014 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101954] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/16/2023] [Revised: 04/23/2024] [Accepted: 04/23/2024] [Indexed: 05/21/2024]
Abstract
This paper aims to identify how cognitive and emotional self-regulation (SR) processes in infants from 0 to 36 months are defined within the Latin American academic context. A systematic review based on the PRISMA methodology was implemented to review the conceptual and operational definition of SR, the type of study, the country of origin of the authors, and the reference to the adequacy of the research to the specific cultural context of Latin America. Twenty-two papers that met the selection criteria were selected. The study identified four types of conceptual definitions for SR, each associated with different constructs or sets of constructs: executive functions, temperament, the integration of executive functions and temperament, and physiological homeostasis. These definitions were based on mainstream approaches to SR rather than being specific to the Latin American region. The study also found compatibility between the sample and some observed trends. On one hand, there was an underrepresentation of the Latin American population in high-impact publications on the subject. However, from 2010 to the present, there is evidence of growth in publications on SR in the analyzed sample. On the other hand, the sample also indicates a disparate representation of the countries of Latin America and the Caribbean in existing publications. Finally, concerning the adaptation to the cultural context of the research, a small number of studies addressed this variable in a specific and significant way. However, even in these cases, the approach is based on models and hypotheses that are limited to understanding the Latin American region's cultural, socioeconomic, and demographic diversity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jorge Alejandro Santos
- Unidad de Neurobiología Aplicada (UNA, CEMIC-CONICET), Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires, Argentina.
| | - Federico Giovannetti
- Unidad de Neurobiología Aplicada (UNA, CEMIC-CONICET), Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | | | - Maria Julia Hermida
- Universidad Nacional de Hurlingham, Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (UNAHUR, CONICET), Villa Tesei, Pcia de Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Daniel Roberto Petetta
- Instituto Nacional de Estadísticas y Censos (INDEC), Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - María Soledad Segretin
- Unidad de Neurobiología Aplicada (UNA, CEMIC-CONICET), Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Sebastián Lipina
- Unidad de Neurobiología Aplicada (UNA, CEMIC-CONICET), Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires, Argentina
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Nakamichi K, Nakamichi N, Nakazawa J. Longitudinal Relations Among Executive Function, Theory of Mind, and Japanese Language Skills Achievement in Elementary School: A 4-year Longitudinal Study. Psychol Rep 2024; 127:1336-1354. [PMID: 36250541 DOI: 10.1177/00332941221133010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/18/2024]
Abstract
This longitudinal study investigated the roles of elementary schoolers' executive function (EF) and theory of mind (ToM) as predictors in their academic achievements in Japanese language skills (e.g., speaking and listening ability, writing ability, reading ability, and linguistic knowledge) from first through fifth grade. We assessed the EF, ToM, and academic achievements of 85 Japanese elementary schoolers, when they were grade 1 (M = 90.19 months) and grade 3 (M = 114.34 months). Moreover, academic achievements of same children tested in grade 5 (M = 137.92 months). A path analysis revealed that EF abilities in Grades 1 and 3 predicted Japanese language achievement via ToM ability, and EF and ToM in Grade 1 had an indirect effect on achievements in Grades 3 and 5. Further, Japanese language skills achievement in Grade 1 predicted EF ability in Grade 3 and EF predicted achievement in Grade 5. These findings indicate the vital role of elementary schoolers' EF on academic achievement in Japan.
Collapse
|
31
|
Pollet TV, Bovet J, Buhaenko R, Cornelissen PL, Tovée MJ. Sample characteristics for quantitative analyses in Body Image: Issues of generalisability. Body Image 2024; 49:101714. [PMID: 38744196 DOI: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2024.101714] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/09/2023] [Revised: 04/02/2024] [Accepted: 04/09/2024] [Indexed: 05/16/2024]
Abstract
Psychological research frequently encounters criticism regarding the representativeness of the samples under study, highlighting concerns about the external validity of the obtained results. Here, we conducted a comprehensive survey of all the quantitative samples from the journal Body Image for 2021 (n = 149 samples). Our primary objective was to examine the extent to which the sampled populations deviated from the population at large, which could potentially compromise the generalizability of findings. We identified that a substantial number of these samples came from student populations (n = 44) and the majority were from the United States, United Kingdom, and Australia. Only a small number of samples (n = 9) employed direct measurements of body mass index (BMI), while the majority relied on self-reported data (n = 93). For a subset of samples in the journal, which were drawn from the general population, we compared whether these differed from population reference values in terms of age and BMI. Using Monte Carlo simulations, we found that samples tended to be younger and score lower on BMI than reference values obtained from the broader population. Samples drawn from female university students also tended to be lower on BMI than age-matched reference samples. We discuss the implications of our findings and make recommendations on sampling and inference. We conclude that a clearer specification of the parameters or conditions under which findings are expected to generalise has the potential to enhance the overall rigor and validity of this field of research.
Collapse
|
32
|
Abubakar A, Brandelli Costa A, Cui L, Koller SH, Nwafor CE, Raval VV. Towards a decolonial developmental science: Adolescent development in the Majority World taking center stage. JOURNAL OF RESEARCH ON ADOLESCENCE : THE OFFICIAL JOURNAL OF THE SOCIETY FOR RESEARCH ON ADOLESCENCE 2024; 34:246-256. [PMID: 38773708 DOI: 10.1111/jora.12956] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/15/2024] [Accepted: 04/16/2024] [Indexed: 05/24/2024]
Abstract
While aspiring to be a diverse and global science, developmental science continues to be dominated by EuroAmerican epistemologies, researchers, and communities in its published scholarship. Adolescents in communities across Africa, Asia, the Middle East, and Latin America comprise 85% of the world's adolescent population, and yet their experiences and perspectives are marginalized in our science. Adolescents in the Majority World live in highly diverse social, cultural, political, economic, educational and healthcare contexts that contribute to their development, and we have much to learn from their experiences. This article situates the marginalization of the global majority within coloniality embedded in developmental science. The article describes the impetus for this special issue Towards a decolonial developmental science and the process of putting it together, along with providing an overview of the 18 articles in this collection that push us towards decoloniality. The special issue serves as a call to transform developmental science to be decolonial by empowering adolescent development in Majority World communities to take center stage. Adolescent development research from Majority World communities has the potential to challenge the knowledge base generated from Minority World samples, contributing to a science that is comprehensive, inclusive, and can inform prevention and intervention efforts to support the well-being of adolescents globally.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | - Silvia H Koller
- Universidade Federal do Rio Grande, Porto Alegre, Brazil
- North West University, Potchefstroom, South Africa
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
33
|
Marfo K. Toward a globally inclusive knowledge base on adolescent development: A charge to the Majority World and a plea for epistemic and paradigmatic pluralism. JOURNAL OF RESEARCH ON ADOLESCENCE : THE OFFICIAL JOURNAL OF THE SOCIETY FOR RESEARCH ON ADOLESCENCE 2024; 34:507-512. [PMID: 38803300 DOI: 10.1111/jora.12979] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/14/2024] [Accepted: 05/14/2024] [Indexed: 05/29/2024]
Abstract
Ongoing internal dialog on the limitations of Euro-American developmental science has opened up space to explore how best to work toward a knowledge base that is adequately representative of the values, cultures, epistemic traditions, and lived experiences of peoples, nations, and regions around the world. So far, recommendations for the advancement of a global developmental science have focused preponderantly on (1) methodological considerations and (2) an architecture to support cross-disciplinary international collaborative inquiry and/or enhance research capacity building for Majority World scholars and institutions. In this commentary, instead of focusing on specific contributions to the Special Issue, I make a case for an explicit commitment to field-building within Majority World contexts as the primary gap-closing path toward the cultivation of a global developmental science knowledge base. I begin with a worldwide population analysis to demonstrate the magnitude of geopolitical, eco-cultural, and epistemic imbalances inherent in the shaping of Euro-American developmental science. In tandem with the Special Issue's central theme, I draw on scholarship from the fields of history, sociology, and political economy to link decolonial theory to the advancement of a global developmental science. Finally, I explore ways in which exemplary research establishments already engaged in prolific inquiry and research training may be ideal candidates to support field-building and help to advance multidisciplinary inquiry within an ethos of epistemic and methodological pluralism.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kofi Marfo
- Aga Khan University (South-Central Asia, East Africa & United Kingdom), Nairobi, Kenya
- University of South Florida, Tampa, Florida, USA
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Fannin DK, Williams EDG, Fuller M, Pearson JN, Boyd BA, Drame E, Taylor J, Dickerson AS, Spinks-Franklin A, Coles-White D. Unpacking the prevalence: A warning against overstating the recently narrowed gap for Black autistic youth. Autism Res 2024; 17:1072-1082. [PMID: 38804591 PMCID: PMC11186720 DOI: 10.1002/aur.3168] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/18/2024] [Accepted: 05/02/2024] [Indexed: 05/29/2024]
Abstract
Recent findings from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention's (CDC) Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring (ADDM) Network's 2020 prevalence report indicate that disparities in autism diagnoses between Black and White youth have narrowed, reflecting improved screening, awareness, and access to services (Maenner et al., 2023. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. Surveillance Summaries (Washington, D.C.: 2002), 72, 1-14.). Claims of reducing disparities beyond prevalence rates, however, are not fully supported, as indicated by the reality that Black youth whose screenings indicate autistic traits are still not being referred for full evaluation or early intervention services at the same rate as their White peers (Major et al., 2020. Autism, 24, 1629-1638; Smith et al., 2020. Pediatrics, 145, S35-S46.). Black 8-year-olds identified as autistic still experience disparate educational placements (Waitoller et al., 2010. The Journal of Special Education. 44, 29-49.) where services may not be autism-specific or have Individual Education Plan goals only focused on "behavior problems" (Severini et al., 2018. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48, 3261-3272.), are served in the most restrictive environments (Skiba et al., 2006. Exceptional Children, 72, 411-424.) and lack consistent augmentative and alternative communication support (Pope et al., 2022. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 31, 2159-2174.). Additionally, ADMM researchers report consistent disparities in the identification of co-occurring intellectual disability where Black autistic children have significantly more co-occurrences than White autistic children. The purpose of this commentary is to first examine the assertion that the narrowed gap indicates, "…improved…access to services among historically underserved groups," (p. 9) (Maenner et al., 2023. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. Surveillance Summaries (Washington, D.C.: 2002), 72, 1-14.). We will then recommend strategies to address the ongoing disparities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Danai Kasambira Fannin
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, North Carolina Central University, Durham, NC
- Department of Head and Neck Surgery and Communication Sciences, Duke University School of Medicine, Durham, NC
| | | | - Marcus Fuller
- Department of Education, University of Maryland Eastern Shore, Princess Anne, MD
| | - Jamie N. Pearson
- Department of Teacher Education and Learning Sciences, North Carolina State University, Raleigh, NC
| | - Brian A. Boyd
- School of Education, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC
- Frank Porter Graham Child Development Institute, Chapel Hill, NC
| | - Elizabeth Drame
- Department of Teaching and Learning, University of Wisconsin, Milwaukee, WI
- Division of Diversity, Equity, and Inclusion, University of Wisconsin, Milwaukee, WI
| | - Jonte’ Taylor
- Department of Educational Psychology, Counseling, and Special Education, Penn State University, University Park, PA
| | - Aisha S. Dickerson
- Department of Epidemiology, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD
| | | | - D’Jaris Coles-White
- Department of Speech, Language, Hearing Science, Western Michigan University, Kalamazoo, MI
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
Fernández-Theoduloz G. Research in Latin America from a decolonial perspective: Challenges of producing socially situated knowledge. JOURNAL OF RESEARCH ON ADOLESCENCE : THE OFFICIAL JOURNAL OF THE SOCIETY FOR RESEARCH ON ADOLESCENCE 2024; 34:517-520. [PMID: 38831583 DOI: 10.1111/jora.12951] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/09/2024] [Accepted: 04/13/2024] [Indexed: 06/05/2024]
Abstract
The developmental science literature predominantly originates from WEIRD (Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic) countries. This bias perpetuates colonial power imbalances and marginalizes non-WEIRD societies' knowledge. This special issue addresses this gap by focusing on Latin America, emphasizing the region's diverse socioeconomic, cultural, and political contexts. This commentary contextualizes research in Latin America, and then presents and discusses the articles. Finally, it presents some of the challenges researchers in Latin America face.
Collapse
|
36
|
Tang Y, Gonzalez MR, Deák GO. The slow emergence of gaze- and point-following: A longitudinal study of infants from 4 to 12 months. Dev Sci 2024; 27:e13457. [PMID: 37941084 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13457] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/29/2022] [Revised: 09/14/2023] [Accepted: 10/02/2023] [Indexed: 11/10/2023]
Abstract
Acquisition of visual attention-following skills, notably gaze- and point-following, contributes to infants' ability to share attention with caregivers, which in turn contributes to social learning and communication. However, the development of gaze- and point-following in the first 18 months remains controversial, in part because of different testing protocols and standards. To address this, we longitudinally tested N = 43 low-risk, North American middle-class infants' tendency to follow gaze direction, pointing gestures, and gaze-and-point combinations. Infants were tested monthly from 4 to 12 months of age. To control motivational differences, infants were taught to expect contingent reward videos in the target locations. No-cue trials were included to estimate spontaneous target fixation rates. A comparison sample (N = 23) was tested at 9 and 12 months to estimate practice effects. Results showed gradual increases in both gaze- and point-following starting around 7 months, and modest month-to-month individual stability from 8 to 12 months. However, attention-following did not exceed chance levels until after 6 months. Infants rarely followed cues to locations behind them, even at 12 months. Infants followed combined gaze-and-point cues more than gaze alone, and followed points at intermediate levels (not reliably different from the other cues). The comparison group's results showed that practice effects did not explain the age-related increase in attention-following. The results corroborate and extend previous findings that North American middle-class infants' attention-following in controlled laboratory settings increases slowly and incrementally between 6 and 12 months of age. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: A longitudinal experimental study documented the emergence and developmental trajectories of North American middle-class infants' visual attention-following skills, including gaze-following, point-following, and gaze-and-point-following. A new paradigm controlled for factors including motivation, attentiveness, and visual-search baserates. Motor development was ruled out as a predictor or limiter of the emergence of attention-following. Infants did not follow attention reliably until after 6 months, and following increased slowly from 7 to 12 months. Infants' individual trajectories showed modest month-to-month stability from 8 to 12 months of age.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yueyan Tang
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, California, USA
| | - Marybel Robledo Gonzalez
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, California, USA
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Health, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio, USA
| | - Gedeon O Deák
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, California, USA
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
Figueroa M. Language development, linguistic input, and linguistic racism. WILEY INTERDISCIPLINARY REVIEWS. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 2024; 15:e1673. [PMID: 38297101 DOI: 10.1002/wcs.1673] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/26/2021] [Revised: 11/29/2023] [Accepted: 11/30/2023] [Indexed: 02/02/2024]
Abstract
Language development is both remarkable and unremarkable. It is remarkable because children learn the language(s) around them, signed or spoken, without explicit instruction or correction. It is unremarkable because children have done this for thousands of years without worldwide incident or catastrophe. Yet, much research on this organic developmental phenomenon relies on an empirical falsehood: "quality" linguistic input is necessary to facilitate language development. "Quality" is a value judgment, not a structural feature of any human language. I argue selectively legitimizing some linguistic input as "quality" is possible only through mischaracterizing what language is. This falsehood is also linguistic racism because it is based on a deficit perspective of the early linguistic experiences of a subset of children, specifically racialized children. I explore how linguistic racism stalls our collective understanding of language development and promotes an environment of bad science. This article is categorized under: Linguistics > Language Acquisition Psychology > Language Neuroscience > Development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Megan Figueroa
- Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona, USA
| |
Collapse
|
38
|
Xia M, Poorthuis AMG, Thomaes S. Children's overestimation of performance across age, task, and historical time: A meta-analysis. Child Dev 2024; 95:1001-1022. [PMID: 37997449 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14042] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/02/2023] [Revised: 10/01/2023] [Accepted: 10/30/2023] [Indexed: 11/25/2023]
Abstract
Children tend to overestimate their performance on a variety of tasks and activities. The present meta-analysis examines the specificity of this phenomenon across age, tasks, and more than five decades of historical time (1968-2021). Self-overestimation was operationalized as the ratio between children's prospective self-estimates of task performance and their actual (i.e., objectively measured) task performance. A total of 246 effect sizes from 43 published articles were analyzed (4277 participants; 49.6% girls; sample mean ages range from 4 to 12; 86.0% of studies conducted in North America or Europe). Children's self-overestimation was robust across tasks, with their estimates of performance being 1.3 times their actual performance. In addition, children's self-overestimation decreased with sample age and increased with the year of data collection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mengtian Xia
- Department of Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | | | - Sander Thomaes
- Department of Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
Roberts SO. Dealing With Diversity in Psychology: Science and Ideology. PERSPECTIVES ON PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2024; 19:590-601. [PMID: 38652780 DOI: 10.1177/17456916241240743] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 04/25/2024]
Abstract
In the spirit of America's Shakespeare, August Wilson (1997), I have written this article as a testimony to the conditions under which I, and too many others, engage in scholarly discourse. I hope to make clear from the beginning that although the ideas presented here are not entirely my own-as they have been inherited from the minority of scholars who dared and managed to bring the most necessary, unpalatable, and unsettling truths about our discipline to the broader scientific community-I do not write for anyone but myself and those scholars who have felt similarly marginalized, oppressed, and silenced. And I write as a race scholar, meaning simply that I believe that race-and racism-affects the sociopolitical conditions in which humans, and scholars, develop their thoughts, feelings, and actions. I believe that it is important for all scholars to have a basic understanding of these conditions, as well as the landmines and pitfalls that define them, as they shape how research is conducted, reviewed, and disseminated. I also believe that to evolve one's discipline into one that is truly robust and objective, it must first become diverse and self-aware. Any effort to suggest otherwise, no matter how scholarly it might present itself, is intellectually unsound.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Steven Othello Roberts
- Department of Psychology, Center for the Comparative Study of Race and Ethnicity, Stanford University
| |
Collapse
|
40
|
Macias C, Persaud K. From silos to synergy: Integrating approaches to investigate the role of prior knowledge and expectations on episodic memory. Psychon Bull Rev 2024:10.3758/s13423-024-02505-4. [PMID: 38691223 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02505-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 03/19/2024] [Indexed: 05/03/2024]
Abstract
Significant progress in the investigation of how prior knowledge influences episodic memory has been made using three sometimes isolated (but not mutually exclusive) approaches: strictly adult behavioral investigations, computational models, and investigations into the development of the system. Here we point out that these approaches are complementary, each approach informs and is informed by the other. Thus, a natural next step for research is to combine all three approaches to further our understanding of the role of prior knowledge in episodic memory. Here we use studies of memory for expectation-congruent and incongruent information from each of these often disparate approaches to illustrate how combining approaches can be used to test and revise theories from the other. This domain is particularly advantageous because it highlights important features of more general memory processes, further differentiates models of memory, and can shed light on developmental change in the memory system. We then present a case study to illustrate the progress that can be made from integrating all three approaches and highlight the need for more endeavors in this vein. As a first step, we also propose a new computational model of memory that takes into account behavioral and developmental factors that can influence prior knowledge and episodic memory interactions. This integrated approach has great potential for offering novel insights into the relationship between prior knowledge and episodic memory, and cognition more broadly.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carla Macias
- Psychology Department, Rutgers University - Newark, Smith Hall, 101 Warren Street, Newark, NJ, 07102, USA.
| | - Kimele Persaud
- Psychology Department, Rutgers University - Newark, Smith Hall, 101 Warren Street, Newark, NJ, 07102, USA
| |
Collapse
|
41
|
Hughes J, Roberts R, Tarver J, Warters-Louth C, Zhang B, Southward E, Shaw R, Edwards G, Waite J, Pearson E. 'It wasn't the strategies on their own': Exploring caregivers' experiences of accessing services in the development of interventions for autistic people with intellectual disability. AUTISM : THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF RESEARCH AND PRACTICE 2024; 28:1231-1244. [PMID: 37712611 PMCID: PMC11067391 DOI: 10.1177/13623613231196084] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 09/16/2023]
Abstract
LAY ABSTRACT Many autistic individuals with intellectual disability experience anxiety, and for those who use few or no words, anxiety may present as behaviour that challenges, such as self-injury and avoiding anxiety-provoking situations. Families report difficulty accessing support from services for autistic individuals experiencing anxiety. Moreover, once receiving support, effective interventions for autistic people with intellectual disability are limited. We completed individual and group discussions with 16 caregivers of autistic people with intellectual disability, to (a) explore their experiences of accessing services for anxiety and/or behaviour that challenges for their child; and (b) understand what matters to caregivers when developing interventions that have been designed for them and the autistic individual with intellectual disability that they support. Caregivers reported that services, in their experience, did not deliver the support that they expected, and that they often needed to 'fight' for support. Caregivers considered services and families working together, the inclusion of peer support, and families being offered interventions that are flexible to individual circumstances to be important. These considerations are valuable for clinicians and researchers developing interventions and aiming to improve outcomes for autistic people with intellectual disability and their families.
Collapse
|
42
|
Sui Z, Wang Q, Xu J. Modeling children's moral development in postwar Taiwan through naturalistic observations preserved in historical texts. Sci Rep 2024; 14:9140. [PMID: 38644443 PMCID: PMC11033267 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-59985-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/16/2023] [Accepted: 04/17/2024] [Indexed: 04/23/2024] Open
Abstract
A core issue in the interdisciplinary study of human morality is its ontogeny in diverse cultures, but systematic, naturalistic data in specific cultural contexts are rare to find. This study conducts a novel analysis of 213 children's socio-moral behavior in a historical, non-Western, rural setting, based on a unique dataset of naturalistic observations from the first field research on Han Chinese children. Using multilevel multinomial modeling, we examined a range of proactive behaviors in 0-to-12-year-old children's peer cooperation and conflict in an entire community in postwar Taiwan. We modeled the effects of age, sex, kinship, and behavioral roles, and revealed complex interactions between these four variables in shaping children's moral development. We discovered linkages between coercive and non-coercive behaviors as children strategically negotiated leadership dynamics. We identified connections between prosocial and aggressive behaviors, illuminating the nuances of morality in real life. Our analysis also revealed gendered patterns and age-related trends that deviated from cultural norms and contradicted popular assumptions about Chinese family values. These findings highlight the importance of naturalistic observations in cultural contexts for understanding how we become moral persons. This re-analysis of historically significant fieldnotes also enriches the interdisciplinary study of child development across societies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zhining Sui
- Department of Biostatistics, University of Washington, 1410 NE Campus Parkway, Seattle, WA, 98195, USA
- Department of Biostatistics and Computational Biology, University of Rochester, 265 Crittenden Boulevard, Rochester, NY, 14642, USA
| | - Qinyan Wang
- Department of Linguistics, University of Washington, 1410 NE Campus Parkway, Seattle, WA, 98195, USA
- Amazon.com, Inc., 400 9th Ave N, Seattle, WA, 98109, USA
| | - Jing Xu
- Department of Anthropology and eScience Institute, University of Washington, 1410 NE Campus Parkway, Seattle, WA, 98195, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
43
|
Schneider JM, Behboudi MH, Maguire MJ. The Necessity of Taking Culture and Context into Account When Studying the Relationship between Socioeconomic Status and Brain Development. Brain Sci 2024; 14:392. [PMID: 38672041 PMCID: PMC11048655 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci14040392] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/25/2024] [Revised: 04/11/2024] [Accepted: 04/12/2024] [Indexed: 04/28/2024] Open
Abstract
Decades of research has revealed a relationship between childhood socioeconomic status (SES) and brain development at the structural and functional levels. Of particular note is the distinction between income and maternal education, two highly correlated factors which seem to influence brain development through distinct pathways. Specifically, while a families' income-to-needs ratio is linked with physiological stress and household chaos, caregiver education influences the day-to-day language environment a child is exposed to. Variability in either one of these environmental experiences is related to subsequent brain development. While this work has the potential to inform public policies in a way that benefits children, it can also oversimplify complex factors, unjustly blame low-SES parents, and perpetuate a harmful deficit perspective. To counteract these shortcomings, researchers must consider sociodemographic differences in the broader cultural context that underlie SES-based differences in brain development. This review aims to address these issues by (a) identifying how sociodemographic mechanisms associated with SES influence the day-to-day experiences of children, in turn, impacting brain development, while (b) considering the broader cultural contexts that may differentially impact this relationship.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julie M. Schneider
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Louisiana State University, 72 Hatcher Hall, Field House Drive, Baton Rouge, LA 70803, USA;
| | - Mohammad Hossein Behboudi
- Callier Center for Communication Disorders, The University of Texas at Dallas, 1966 Inwood Road, Dallas, TX 75235, USA;
| | - Mandy J. Maguire
- Callier Center for Communication Disorders, The University of Texas at Dallas, 1966 Inwood Road, Dallas, TX 75235, USA;
- Center for Children and Families, The University of Texas at Dallas, 800 W Campbell Road, Dallas, TX 75080, USA
| |
Collapse
|
44
|
Maria Guzmán EM, LeDuc MK, Cha CB, Goger P, Ng MY, Huang X, Ribeiro JD, Fox KR. Accounting for diversity in the treatment of suicide and self-injury: A systematic review of the past 50 years of randomized controlled trials. Suicide Life Threat Behav 2024; 54:250-262. [PMID: 38193589 DOI: 10.1111/sltb.13037] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/13/2023] [Revised: 11/07/2023] [Accepted: 11/27/2023] [Indexed: 01/10/2024]
Abstract
PURPOSE Patients receiving treatment for self-injurious thoughts and behaviors (SITBs) have diverse backgrounds, yet it remains unclear exactly who is represented in the current SITB treatment literature. METHODS We conducted a systematic review of the past 50 years of randomized controlled trials (RCTs) testing SITB treatments to evaluate sampling practices and reporting of sample characteristics, as well as inclusion of global populations across the included 525 papers. We also assessed changes over the past five decades in these three domains. RESULTS SITB RCTs frequently reported age and sex (98.6%-95.1%), less frequently reported race (83.4%-38.6%), socioeconomic status (48.1%-46.1%) and ethnicity (41.9%-8.1%), and rarely reported LGBTQ+ status (3.7%-1.6%). U.S.-based RCTs featured predominantly White, non-Hispanic, and non-LGBTQ+ samples. Most RCTs were conducted in high-income North American or European countries. Sample reporting practices, sample representativeness, and inclusion of global populations modestly and inconsistently improved over time. CONCLUSIONS There has not been substantial improvement in reporting practices, sample representativeness, or inclusion of global populations in SITB RCTs over the past 50 years. Acknowledging who is being studied and representing diverse populations in SITB treatment research is key to connecting research advances with those who may need it most.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eleonora M Maria Guzmán
- Department of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, Teachers College, Columbia University, New York, New York, USA
| | - Michael K LeDuc
- Department of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, Teachers College, Columbia University, New York, New York, USA
| | - Christine B Cha
- Department of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, Teachers College, Columbia University, New York, New York, USA
| | - Pauline Goger
- Department of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, Teachers College, Columbia University, New York, New York, USA
| | - Mei Yi Ng
- Department of Psychology, Center for Children and Families, Florida International University, Miami, Florida, USA
| | - Xieyining Huang
- Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida, USA
| | - Jessica D Ribeiro
- Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida, USA
| | - Kathryn R Fox
- Department of Psychology, University of Denver, Denver, Colorado, USA
| |
Collapse
|
45
|
Prystauka Y, Altmann GTM, Rothman J. Online eye tracking and real-time sentence processing: On opportunities and efficacy for capturing psycholinguistic effects of different magnitudes and diversity. Behav Res Methods 2024; 56:3504-3522. [PMID: 37528290 PMCID: PMC11133053 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-023-02176-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 06/15/2023] [Indexed: 08/03/2023]
Abstract
Online research methods have the potential to facilitate equitable accessibility to otherwise-expensive research resources, as well as to more diverse populations and language combinations than currently populate our studies. In psycholinguistics specifically, webcam-based eye tracking is emerging as a powerful online tool capable of capturing sentence processing effects in real time. The present paper asks whether webcam-based eye tracking provides the necessary granularity to replicate effects-crucially both large and small-that tracker-based eye tracking has shown. Using the Gorilla Experiment Builder platform, this study set out to replicate two psycholinguistic effects: a robust one, the verb semantic constraint effect, first reported in Altmann and Kamide, Cognition 73(3), 247-264 (1999), and a smaller one, the lexical interference effect, first examined by Kukona et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(2), 326 (2014). Webcam-based eye tracking was able to replicate both effects, thus showing that its functionality is not limited to large effects. Moreover, the paper also reports two approaches to computing statistical power and discusses the differences in their outputs. Beyond discussing several important methodological, theoretical, and practical implications, we offer some further technical details and advice on how to implement webcam-based eye-tracking studies. We believe that the advent of webcam-based eye tracking, at least in respect of the visual world paradigm, will kickstart a new wave of more diverse studies with more diverse populations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yanina Prystauka
- Department of Language and Culture, UiT The Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø, Norway.
| | - Gerry T M Altmann
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT, USA
| | - Jason Rothman
- Department of Language and Culture, UiT The Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø, Norway
- Centro de Investigación Nebrija en Cognición (CINC), University Nebrija, Madrid, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
46
|
Fong FTK, Puebla G, Nielsen M. The role of conventionality and design in children's function judgments about malfunctioning artifacts. J Exp Child Psychol 2024; 240:105835. [PMID: 38176258 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2023.105835] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/11/2023] [Revised: 12/05/2023] [Accepted: 12/05/2023] [Indexed: 01/06/2024]
Abstract
This study investigated the individual influences of conventionality and designer's intent on function judgments of possibly malfunctioning artifacts. Children aged 4 and 5 years and 6 to 8 years were presented with stories about an artifact with two equally plausible functions, one labeled as either conventional or designed. Subsequently, a character attempted to use the artifact for the cued function, which resulted in either malfunction or successful use. The children's task was to identify the real function of the artifact. When the use attempt succeeded, 4- and 5-year-olds preferred conventional functions to the alternative (but did not show a clear preference between design functions and the alternative), and 6- to 8-year-olds preferred conventional and designed functions to the alternative. In case of malfunction, children's choices were at chance, where the effect of either conventional or design cues was less salient. This contrasts with a baseline condition where children avoided the malfunctioning alternatives. Presenting additional cues about an artifact's function can affect function judgments in cases of malfunction.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Frankie T K Fong
- Department of Comparative Cultural Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, 04103 Leipzig, Germany; Early Cognitive Development Centre, School of Psychology, University of Queensland, St. Lucia, Queensland 4067, Australia.
| | - Guillermo Puebla
- Instituto de Alta Investigación, Universidad de Tarapacá, Casilla 7D, Arica 1000000, Chile
| | - Mark Nielsen
- Early Cognitive Development Centre, School of Psychology, University of Queensland, St. Lucia, Queensland 4067, Australia; Faculty of Humanities, University of Johannesburg, Auckland Park, Johannesburg 2092, South Africa
| |
Collapse
|
47
|
Robinson JA. Editorial: Research priorities concerning formal and informal learning in low- and middle-income countries. Front Psychol 2024; 15:1391089. [PMID: 38566945 PMCID: PMC10986789 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1391089] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/24/2024] [Accepted: 03/05/2024] [Indexed: 04/04/2024] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Julie Ann Robinson
- College of Education, Psychology and Social Work, Flinders University, Adelaide, SA, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
48
|
Wojcik EH, Pierce MC, Stevens G, Goulding SJ. Referent-oriented interactions in infancy: A naturalistic, longitudinal case study from an English-speaking household. Infant Behav Dev 2024; 74:101911. [PMID: 38056189 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101911] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/05/2023] [Revised: 10/14/2023] [Accepted: 11/16/2023] [Indexed: 12/08/2023]
Abstract
Caregivers use a of combination labeling, pointing, object grasping, and gaze to communicate with infants about referents in their environment. By two years of age, children reliably use these referent-oriented cues to communicate and learn. While there is some evidence from lab-based studies that younger infants attend to and use referent-oriented cues during communication, some more naturalistic studies have found that in the first year of life, infants do not robustly leverage these cues during dyadic interactions. The current study examined parent and infant gaze, touching, pointing, and reaching to referents for a wide range of nouns, verbs, adjectives, and other early-learned words during 59 one-hour head-camera recordings sampled from one English-learning infants' life between 6 and 12 months of age. We found substantial variability across individual words for all cues. Some variability was explained by referent concreteness and the grammatical category of the label. The parent's touching of labeled referents increased across months, suggesting that parent-infant-referent interactions may change with development. Future studies should investigate the trajectories of specific types of words and contexts, rather than attempting to discover possibly non-existent universal trajectories of parent and infant referent-oriented behaviors.
Collapse
|
49
|
Lan X, Ma C. Narcissism Moderates the Association Between Autonomy-Supportive Parenting and Adolescents' Prosocial Behavior. J Youth Adolesc 2024; 53:632-655. [PMID: 38147189 PMCID: PMC10838263 DOI: 10.1007/s10964-023-01933-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/22/2023] [Accepted: 12/07/2023] [Indexed: 12/27/2023]
Abstract
Prior research has separately investigated the associations of autonomy-supportive parenting and narcissism with adolescents' prosocial behavior, but their joint relationships with prosocial behavior have been rarely examined. The present research aimed to expand the existing literature by scrutinizing the main and interactive associations of autonomy-supportive parenting and narcissism with adolescents' prosocial behavior. In so doing, a series of four studies (collectively N = 2023), combining cross-sectional, longitudinal, and experimental designs, were conducted. The adolescents' mean age varied from 12.42 to 15.70 years, with a balanced representation of the sexes in those studies. Converging results across four studies showed that high narcissism magnified the positive association between autonomy-supportive parenting and adolescents' prosocial behavior. The interaction pattern presented also suggested adolescents with high narcissism scores were more affected than others-both for better and for worse-by autonomy-supportive parenting, although this interaction might be specific to particular facets of prosocial behavior. These results were robust after adjusting for a few key covariates and survived a set of additional analyses. The present findings provide a novel avenue to explain individual differences linking prosocial behavior with those two factors and further advance precise, individualized strategies to promote adolescents' prosocial behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xiaoyu Lan
- Promenta Research Center, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway.
| | - Chunhua Ma
- College of Educational Science and Technology, Northwest Minzu University, Lanzhou, China
| |
Collapse
|
50
|
Medvedev D, Davenport D, Talhelm T, Li Y. The motivating effect of monetary over psychological incentives is stronger in WEIRD cultures. Nat Hum Behav 2024; 8:456-470. [PMID: 38191844 PMCID: PMC10963269 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01769-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/20/2022] [Accepted: 10/25/2023] [Indexed: 01/10/2024]
Abstract
Motivating effortful behaviour is a problem employers, governments and nonprofits face globally. However, most studies on motivation are done in Western, educated, industrialized, rich and democratic (WEIRD) cultures. We compared how hard people in six countries worked in response to monetary incentives versus psychological motivators, such as competing with or helping others. The advantage money had over psychological interventions was larger in the United States and the United Kingdom than in China, India, Mexico and South Africa (N = 8,133). In our last study, we randomly assigned cultural frames through language in bilingual Facebook users in India (N = 2,065). Money increased effort over a psychological treatment by 27% in Hindi and 52% in English. These findings contradict the standard economic intuition that people from poorer countries should be more driven by money. Instead, they suggest that the market mentality of exchanging time and effort for material benefits is most prominent in WEIRD cultures.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Danila Medvedev
- University of Chicago, Booth School of Business, Chicago, IL, USA.
| | - Diag Davenport
- Princeton University, School of Public and International Affairs, Princeton, NJ, USA
| | - Thomas Talhelm
- University of Chicago, Booth School of Business, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Yin Li
- Yale University, Yale School of Management, New Haven, CT, USA
| |
Collapse
|