1
|
de Water E, Demurie E, Mies GW, Scheres A. Temporal discounting in children and adolescents with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a comparison of four scoring methods. Child Neuropsychol 2024; 30:702-721. [PMID: 37860876 DOI: 10.1080/09297049.2023.2268768] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/31/2023] [Accepted: 10/04/2023] [Indexed: 10/21/2023]
Abstract
Temporal discounting (TD) tasks measure the preference for immediate rewards over larger delayed rewards and have been widely used to study impulsivity in children and adolescents with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Relatively impulsive individuals tend to show high inconsistency in their choices, which makes it difficult to determine commonly used TD outcome measures (e.g., area under the curve, AUC). In this study, we leveraged two published datasets to compare four methods to compute TD outcome measures in children and adolescents (8-17 years) with (n = 107) and without ADHD (n = 128): two predetermined rules methods, a proportion method, and logistic regression. In both datasets, when using the two predetermined rules methods and the proportion method, TD outcomes were highly correlated and group differences in TD were similar. When using logistic regression, a large proportion of AUCs (95% in dataset 1; 33% in dataset 2) could not be computed due to inconsistent choice patterns. These findings indicate that predetermined rules methods (for studies with small sample sizes and experienced raters) and a proportion method (for studies with larger sample sizes or less experienced raters) are recommended over logistic regression when determining subjective reward values for participants with inconsistent choice patterns.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Ellen Demurie
- Research in Developmental Disorders Lab, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
| | - Gabry W Mies
- Karakter Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Anouk Scheres
- Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Duff N, Olsen R, Walsh Z, Salmon K, Hunt M, Macaskill A. A fragile effect: The influence of episodic memory on delay discounting. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2024:17470218241239289. [PMID: 38429230 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241239289] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 03/03/2024]
Abstract
Delay discounting occurs when a reward loses value as a function of delay. Episodic future thinking (EFT) reliably decreases delay discounting. EFT may share cognitive features with recalling episodic memories such as constructive episodic simulation. We therefore explored whether recalling episodic memories also reduces delay discounting. In Experiment 1, participants wrote about episodic memories and recalled those memories before completing a delay discounting task. Episodic memories reduced delay discounting according to one commonly used delay discounting measure (area under the curve) but not another (using the hyperbolic model). Experiment 2 compared the effects of general and episodic memories. Neither general nor episodic memories significantly decreased delay discounting compared with a control "counting" condition, but episodic memories reduced delay discounting compared with general memories under some conditions. In Experiment 3, episodic memories did not decrease delay discounting compared with three other control conditions while EFT did. Experiment 3 therefore found that thinking must be both episodic and future orientated to reduce delay discounting. Together, these results suggest that episodic thinking is not sufficient to reliably decrease delay discounting, rather, features unique to episodic future thinking are required. Episodic memory might reduce delay discounting in some contexts, but this effect is small and fragile.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicky Duff
- School of Psychology, Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand
| | - Rebecca Olsen
- School of Psychology, Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand
| | - Zoe Walsh
- School of Psychology, Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand
| | - Karen Salmon
- School of Psychology, Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand
| | - Maree Hunt
- School of Psychology, Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand
| | - Anne Macaskill
- School of Psychology, Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Lieder F, Chen PZ, Prentice M, Amo V, Tošić M. Gamification of Behavior Change: Mathematical Principle and Proof-of-Concept Study. JMIR Serious Games 2024; 12:e43078. [PMID: 38517466 PMCID: PMC10998180 DOI: 10.2196/43078] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/30/2022] [Revised: 06/12/2023] [Accepted: 08/31/2023] [Indexed: 03/23/2024] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Many people want to build good habits to become healthier, live longer, or become happier but struggle to change their behavior. Gamification can make behavior change easier by awarding points for the desired behavior and deducting points for its omission. OBJECTIVE In this study, we introduced a principled mathematical method for determining how many points should be awarded or deducted for the enactment or omission of the desired behavior, depending on when and how often the person has succeeded versus failed to enact it in the past. We called this approach optimized gamification of behavior change. METHODS As a proof of concept, we designed a chatbot that applies our optimized gamification method to help people build healthy water-drinking habits. We evaluated the effectiveness of this gamified intervention in a 40-day field experiment with 1 experimental group (n=43) that used the chatbot with optimized gamification and 2 active control groups for which the chatbot's optimized gamification feature was disabled. For the first control group (n=48), all other features were available, including verbal feedback. The second control group (n=51) received no feedback or reminders. We measured the strength of all participants' water-drinking habits before, during, and after the intervention using the Self-Report Habit Index and by asking participants on how many days of the previous week they enacted the desired habit. In addition, all participants provided daily reports on whether they enacted their water-drinking intention that day. RESULTS A Poisson regression analysis revealed that, during the intervention, users who received feedback based on optimized gamification enacted the desired behavior more often (mean 14.71, SD 6.57 times) than the active (mean 11.64, SD 6.38 times; P<.001; incidence rate ratio=0.80, 95% CI 0.71-0.91) or passive (mean 11.64, SD 5.43 times; P=.001; incidence rate ratio=0.78, 95% CI 0.69-0.89) control groups. The Self-Report Habit Index score significantly increased in all conditions (P<.001 in all cases) but did not differ between the experimental and control conditions (P>.11 in all cases). After the intervention, the experimental group performed the desired behavior as often as the 2 control groups (P≥.17 in all cases). CONCLUSIONS Our findings suggest that optimized gamification can be used to make digital behavior change interventions more effective. TRIAL REGISTRATION Open Science Framework (OSF) H7JN8; https://osf.io/h7jn8.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Falk Lieder
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, United States
- Max Planck Institute for Intelligent Systems, Tübingen, Germany
| | - Pin-Zhen Chen
- Max Planck Institute for Intelligent Systems, Tübingen, Germany
| | - Mike Prentice
- Max Planck Institute for Intelligent Systems, Tübingen, Germany
| | - Victoria Amo
- Max Planck Institute for Intelligent Systems, Tübingen, Germany
| | - Mateo Tošić
- Max Planck Institute for Intelligent Systems, Tübingen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Fusco G, Scandola M, Lin H, Inzlicht M, Aglioti SM. Modulating preferences during intertemporal choices through exogenous midfrontal transcranial alternating current stimulation: A registered report. Cortex 2024; 171:435-464. [PMID: 38113613 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2023.09.019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/03/2023] [Revised: 08/24/2023] [Accepted: 09/21/2023] [Indexed: 12/21/2023]
Abstract
Decision conflicts may arise when the costs and benefits of choices are evaluated as a function of outcomes predicted along a temporal dimension. Electrophysiology studies suggest that during performance monitoring a typical oscillatory activity in the theta rhythm, named midfrontal theta, may index conflict processing and resolution. In the present within-subject, sham controlled, cross-over preregistered study, we delivered online midfrontal transcranial Alternating Current Stimulation (tACS) to modulate electrocortical activity during intertemporal decisions. Participants were invited to select choice preference between economic offers at three different intermixed levels of conflict (i.e., low, medium, high) while receiving either theta -, gamma-, or sham tACS in separate blocks and sessions. At the end of each stimulation block, a Letter-Flanker task was also administered to measure behavioural aftereffects. We hypothesized that theta-tACS would have acted on the performance monitoring system inducing behavioural changes (i.e., faster decisions and more impulsive choices) in high conflicting trials, rather than gamma- and sham-tACS. Results very partially confirmed our predictions. Unexpectedly, both theta- and gamma-driven neuromodulation speeded-up decisions compared to sham. However, exploratory analyses revealed that such an effect was stronger in the high-conflict decisions during theta-tACS. These findings were independent from the influence of the sensations induced by the electrical stimulation. Moreover, further analyses highlighted a significant association during theta-tACS between the selection of immediate offers in high-conflict trials and attentional impulsiveness, suggesting that individual factors may account for the tACS effects during intertemporal decisions. Finally, we did not capture long-lasting behavioural changes following tACS in the Flanker task. Our findings may inform scholars to improve experimental designs and boost the knowledge toward a more effective application of tACS.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gabriele Fusco
- Sapienza University of Rome and CLNS@SAPIENZA, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Italy; IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy.
| | - Michele Scandola
- NPSY Lab-Vr, Department of Human Sciences, University of Verona, Verona, Italy
| | - Hause Lin
- Sloan School of Management, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA
| | - Michael Inzlicht
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
| | - Salvatore Maria Aglioti
- Sapienza University of Rome and CLNS@SAPIENZA, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Italy; IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Fennessy A, Homer MS, Ensaff H. Nudging food choice in a prison setting: an investigation using food choice data. J Hum Nutr Diet 2024; 37:270-279. [PMID: 37933808 DOI: 10.1111/jhn.13252] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/30/2023] [Accepted: 09/29/2023] [Indexed: 11/08/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND The prison population presents complex health needs and is disproportionately affected by poor health, compared to the general population. Diet has a clear role in prisoner health, and the prison food environment within which food choices are made is relatively under-researched. The aim of this study was to examine whether food choices in a women's prison changed after the introduction of a new menu design by the catering team. METHODS The adjusted menu design incorporated an emoticon (a smiley face) placed next to designated 'Healthy Choice' foods on the menu sheets, which were used to preselect meals. Data comprised all women's (n = 865) food choices (more than 115,000 selections) for a period of 8 weeks (with the new menu) as well as 8 weeks prior (baseline period). The study design was a pre-post intervention study, and food selection was examined using chi-square tests and binary logistic regression models. RESULTS The selection of promoted foods overall significantly increased under the new menu design (with the emoticon nudge strategy) compared to baseline; the effect size, however, was small according to the usual guidelines (21.4% compared to 20% at baseline; χ2 (1) = 32.6, p < 0.001, φ = 0.02). Individuals were 11% more likely (p < 0.001) to select the promoted 'Healthy Choice' foods under the adjusted food choice architecture. A significant effect was found for lunch and evening meal - but not for desserts. A minority of individual food items that were promoted had significant positive changes in selection, and were 1.3-4 times as likely to be selected when emoticons had been introduced, compared to baseline. CONCLUSIONS Further research is needed to examine the potential added benefit of multiple complementary nudge strategies, and the relevance of the preselection of foods in advance of consumption.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A Fennessy
- HM Prison & Probation Service, London, UK
- Nutritional Sciences and Epidemiology, School of Food Science and Nutrition, University of Leeds, Leeds, UK
| | - M S Homer
- School of Education, University of Leeds, Leeds, UK
- School of Medicine, University of Leeds, Leeds, UK
| | - H Ensaff
- Nutritional Sciences and Epidemiology, School of Food Science and Nutrition, University of Leeds, Leeds, UK
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Gelino BW, Graham ME, Strickland JC, Glatter HW, Hursh SR, Reed DD. Using behavioral economics to optimize safer undergraduate late-night transportation. J Appl Behav Anal 2024; 57:117-130. [PMID: 37932923 DOI: 10.1002/jaba.1029] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/16/2022] [Accepted: 09/17/2023] [Indexed: 11/08/2023]
Abstract
Many universities sponsor student-oriented transit services that could reduce alcohol-induced risks but only if services adequately anticipate and adapt to student needs. Human choice data offer an optimal foundation for planning and executing late-night transit services. In this simulated choice experiment, respondents opted to either (a) wait an escalating delay for a free university-sponsored "safe" option, (b) pay an escalating fee for an on-demand rideshare service, or (c) pick a free, immediately available "unsafe" option (e.g., ride with an alcohol-impaired driver). Behavioral-economic nonlinear models of averaged-choice data describe preference across arrangements. Best-fit metrics indicate adequate sensitivity to contextual factors (i.e., wait time, preceding late-night activity). At short delays, students preferred the free transit option. As delays extend beyond 30 min, most students preferred competing alternatives. These data depict a policy-relevant delay threshold to better safeguard undergraduate student safety.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Brett W Gelino
- Department of Applied Behavioral Science, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, USA
| | - Madison E Graham
- Department of Applied Behavioral Science, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, USA
| | - Justin C Strickland
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Hannah W Glatter
- Department of Applied Behavioral Science, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, USA
| | - Steven R Hursh
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
- Institutes for Behavior Resources, Inc., Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Derek D Reed
- Department of Applied Behavioral Science, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, USA
- Cofrin-Logan Center for Addiction Research and Treatment, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, USA
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
File D, Bőthe B, Demetrovics Z. Examining the impact of perceived psychological distances of quitting and continuing tobacco smoking on antismoking intention: a cross-sectional study. Sci Rep 2023; 13:22993. [PMID: 38151498 PMCID: PMC10752891 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-50440-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/06/2023] [Accepted: 12/20/2023] [Indexed: 12/29/2023] Open
Abstract
To date, little research has been conducted to understand the role of psychological distances on smoking behaviour. Construal Level Theory posits that individuals mentally construe events, objects, or ideas based on their perceived distance in terms of spatial, temporal, social, and hypothetical dimensions, influencing their judgments and decision-making processes. The aim of the current study was (1) to provide a comprehensive exploration of psychological distances of costs and benefits of tobacco smoking and antismoking intention and (2) to examine whether smoking can be attributed to rational behaviour based on the psychological distance weighted balance of perceived costs and benefits of quitting and continuing smoking. Mediation models delineating the relationships among temporal and hypothetical psychological distances, personal relevance and antismoking intention were tested on cross-sectional survey data of 1486 smokers (880 men, Mage = 39.9 years, SD = 13.36). Psychological distances were shown to be important factors in the cognitive evaluation process of smoking behaviour. Perceived temporal distance to smoking continuation/cessation was related to personal importance and hypothetical psychological distances, which were associated with anti-smoking intention. Furthermore, antismoking intention was related to the psychological distance-weighted gain-cost balance of quitting and continuing smoking. The current findings enhance our knowledge of the cognitive evaluation of the outcomes of smoking, indicating that the choice of not quitting smoking may be partially based on a biased rational decision-making process.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Domonkos File
- Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary.
| | - Beáta Bőthe
- Département de Psychologie, Université de Montréal, Montreal, Canada
| | - Zsolt Demetrovics
- Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary
- Centre of Excellence in Responsible Gaming, University of Gibraltar, Gibraltar, Gibraltar
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Burrell M, Pastor-Bernier A, Schultz W. Worth the Work? Monkeys Discount Rewards by a Subjective Adapting Effort Cost. J Neurosci 2023; 43:6796-6806. [PMID: 37625854 PMCID: PMC10552939 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.0115-23.2023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/20/2023] [Revised: 05/25/2023] [Accepted: 05/30/2023] [Indexed: 08/27/2023] Open
Abstract
All life must solve how to allocate limited energy resources to maximize benefits from scarce opportunities. Economic theory posits decision makers optimize choice by maximizing the subjective benefit (utility) of reward minus the subjective cost (disutility) of the required effort. While successful in many settings, this model does not fully account for how experience can alter reward-effort trade-offs. Here, we test how well the subtractive model of effort disutility explains the behavior of two male nonhuman primates (Macaca mulatta) in a binary choice task in which reward quantity and physical effort to obtain were varied. Applying random utility modeling to independently estimate reward utility and effort disutility, we show the subtractive effort model better explains out-of-sample choice behavior when compared with parabolic and exponential effort discounting. Furthermore, we demonstrate that effort disutility depends on previous experience of effort: in analogy to work from behavioral labor economics, we develop a model of reference-dependent effort disutility to explain the increased willingness to expend effort following previous experience of effortful options in a session. The result of this analysis suggests that monkeys discount reward by an effort cost that is measured relative to an expected effort learned from previous trials. When this subjective cost of effort, a function of context and experience, is accounted for, trial-by-trial choices can be explained by the subtractive cost model of effort. Therefore, in searching for net utility signals that may underpin effort-based decision-making in the brain, careful measurement of subjective effort costs is an essential first step.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT All decision-makers need to consider how much effort they need to expend when evaluating potential options. Economic theories suggest that the optimal way to choose is by cost-benefit analysis of reward against effort. To be able to do this efficiently over many decision contexts, this needs to be done flexibly, with appropriate adaptation to context and experience. Therefore, in aiming to understand how this might be achieved in the brain, it is important to first carefully measure the subjective cost of effort. Here, we show monkeys make reward-effort cost-benefit decisions, subtracting the subjective cost of effort from the subjective value of rewards. Moreover, the subjective cost of effort is dependent on the monkeys' experience of effort in previous trials.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mark Burrell
- Department of Physiology, Development and Neuroscience, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3DY, United Kingdom
| | - Alexandre Pastor-Bernier
- Department of Physiology, Development and Neuroscience, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3DY, United Kingdom
| | - Wolfram Schultz
- Department of Physiology, Development and Neuroscience, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3DY, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
El Haj M, Moustafa AA. "Bitcoin now": temporal discounting in Bitcoin holders. Front Neurosci 2023; 17:1205814. [PMID: 37521703 PMCID: PMC10372628 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2023.1205814] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/14/2023] [Accepted: 07/03/2023] [Indexed: 08/01/2023] Open
Abstract
Introduction Cryptocurrency investment and trading are rapidly growing activities due to the development of applications and platforms that offer fast, continuous, and easy entry into the cryptocurrency world. To understand decision making in cryptocurrency holders, we assessed temporal discounting, that is, whether Bitcoin holders disregard rewards if they are distant in time and overvalue rewards if they are more immediate. Further, we compared performance between short-term investors (i.e., day-traders) vs. long-term investors. Methods Using an online survey, we invited 144 Bitcoin holders to answer temporal discounting questionnaires dealing with money ("Which do you prefer, that you get right now 20 USD in cash or 100 USD in a month?") and Bitcoin ("Which do you prefer, that you get right now 0.1 or 1 Bitcoin in a month?"). Results Analysis demonstrated no significant differences between temporal discounting for money and Bitcoin. However, and critically, higher temporal discounting for both money and Bitcoin was observed in short-term investors compared with long-term investors. In a similar vein, significant positive correlations were observed between day trading and temporal discounting for both money and Bitcoin. Discussion These findings demonstrate how Bitcoin holders with short-term time horizons tend to prioritize immediate rewards over larger but delayed rewards. Future research can assess the neural basis of temporal discounting for cryptocurrencies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mohamad El Haj
- Nantes Université, Univ Angers, Laboratoire de Psychologie des Pays de la Loire (LPPL – EA 4638), Nantes, France
- CHU Nantes, Clinical Gerontology Department, Nantes, France
- Institut Universitaire de France, Paris, France
| | - Ahmed A. Moustafa
- School of Psychology, Faculty of Society and Design, Bond University, Gold Coast, QLD, Australia
- Department of Human Anatomy and Physiology, The Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Johannesburg, Johannesburg, South Africa
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Exum AC, Sutton CA, Bellitti JS, Yi R, Fazzino TL. Delay discounting and substance use treatment outcomes: A systematic review focused on treatment outcomes and discounting methodology. JOURNAL OF SUBSTANCE USE AND ADDICTION TREATMENT 2023; 149:209037. [PMID: 37072099 PMCID: PMC10429418 DOI: 10.1016/j.josat.2023.209037] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/25/2022] [Revised: 02/07/2023] [Accepted: 04/07/2023] [Indexed: 04/20/2023]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Delay discounting-the tendency to choose small, immediate rewards over larger, delayed rewards-is robustly associated with substance use. Delay discounting may present challenges in treatment for substance use disorders, as individuals with elevated discounting may struggle to wait for the long-term rewards that come from abstinence, which may yield poorer treatment outcomes. However, evidence on the role of discounting in treatment outcomes has been inconsistent. The study conducted a systematic review of the literature to characterize the prospective effects of delay discounting measured pre-treatment on substance use treatment outcomes, with a focus on characterizing findings across: 1) type of treatment outcome and 2) methodology used to assess and characterize discounting. METHOD A systematic literature search identified N = 17 studies that examined the association between delay discounting at treatment entry (pre-treatment) and substance use treatment outcomes. Findings were reported across the following substance use treatment outcomes: abstinence, relapse, use frequency and related problems, and treatment adherence. Findings regarding discounting methodology were reported by type of discounting measure (adjusting choice task, fixed choice task, or experiential task) and parameter used to characterize discounting (k, log transformed k (lnk), and area under the curve). RESULTS Delay discounting at treatment entry was not consistently associated with substance use treatment outcomes when examined across all studies overall (47 %) or by treatment outcome (0-40 % for most outcomes). The majority of studies (64 %) that used an adjusting choice, computer-based task reported a significant association between discounting and treatment outcomes, whereas few studies that used a fixed choice or experiential task reported significant associations with treatment outcomes (0-25 %). Most studies (71 %) that used the lnk parameter to characterize discounting reported significant associations between discounting and a range of treatment outcomes. In contrast, few studies that used k or AUC (25-33 %) reported significant associations between discounting and treatment outcomes. CONCLUSION When examined overall and by treatment outcome, evidence did not consistently indicate that delay discounting was prospectively associated with substance use treatment outcomes. However, delay discounting at treatment entry was more commonly associated with a variety of poorer treatment outcomes when researchers used more fine-grained methods to characterize discounting.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alexis C Exum
- Department of Psychology, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, United States of America; Cofrin Logan Center for Addiction Research and Treatment, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, United States of America
| | - Cassandra A Sutton
- Department of Psychology, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, United States of America; Cofrin Logan Center for Addiction Research and Treatment, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, United States of America
| | - Joseph S Bellitti
- Department of Psychology, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, United States of America; Cofrin Logan Center for Addiction Research and Treatment, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, United States of America
| | - Richard Yi
- Department of Psychology, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, United States of America; Cofrin Logan Center for Addiction Research and Treatment, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, United States of America
| | - Tera L Fazzino
- Department of Psychology, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, United States of America; Cofrin Logan Center for Addiction Research and Treatment, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, United States of America.
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Patten CA, Koller KR, King DK, Prochaska JJ, Sinicrope PS, McDonell MG, Decker PA, Lee FR, Fosi JK, Young AM, Sabaque CV, Brown AR, Borah BJ, Thomas TK. Aniqsaaq (To Breathe): Study protocol to develop and evaluate an Alaska Native family-based financial incentive intervention for smoking cessation. Contemp Clin Trials Commun 2023; 33:101129. [PMID: 37091507 PMCID: PMC10120296 DOI: 10.1016/j.conctc.2023.101129] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/02/2023] [Revised: 03/27/2023] [Accepted: 04/01/2023] [Indexed: 04/05/2023] Open
Abstract
Background Alaska Native and American Indian (ANAI) communities in Alaska are disproportionately affected by commercial tobacco use. Financial incentive interventions promote cigarette smoking cessation, but family-level incentives have not been evaluated. We describe the study protocol to adapt and evaluate the effectiveness and implementation of a remotely delivered, family-based financial incentive intervention for cigarette smoking among Alaskan ANAI people. Methods The study has 3 phases: 1) qualitative interviews with ANAI adults who smoke, family members, and stakeholders to inform the intervention, 2) beta-test of the intervention, and 3) randomized controlled trial (RCT) evaluating intervention reach and effectiveness on verified, prolonged smoking abstinence at 6- and 12-months post-treatment. In the RCT, adult dyads (ANAI person who smokes [index participant] and family member) recruited throughout Alaska will be randomized to a no-incentives control condition (n = 328 dyads) or a 6-month incentive intervention (n = 328 dyads). All dyads will receive cessation support and family wellness materials. Smoking status will be assessed weekly for four weeks and at three and six months. Intervention index participants will receive escalating incentives for verified smoking abstinence at each time point (maximum $750 total); the family member will receive rewards of equal value. Results A community advisory committee contributed input on the study design and methods for relevance to ANAI people, particularly emphasizing the involvement of families. Conclusion Our study aligns with the strength and value AIAN people place on family. Findings, processes, and resources will inform how Indigenous family members can support smoking cessation within incentive interventions. Clinical Trials Registry NCT05209451.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christi A. Patten
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychology and Behavioral Health Research Program, Mayo Clinic, 200 1st St SW, Rochester, MN, 55905, USA
| | - Kathryn R. Koller
- Research Services, Division of Community Health Services, Alaska Native Tribal Health Consortium, 4000 Ambassador Dr, Anchorage, AK, 99508, USA
| | - Diane K. King
- Center for Behavioral Health Research and Service, University of Alaska Anchorage, 3211 Providence Dr, SBB 303F, Anchorage, AK, 99508-4614, USA
| | - Judith J. Prochaska
- Stanford Prevention Research Center, Department of Medicine, Stanford University, 1265 Welch Road, Stanford, CA, 94305, USA
| | - Pamela S. Sinicrope
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychology and Behavioral Health Research Program, Mayo Clinic, 200 1st St SW, Rochester, MN, 55905, USA
| | - Michael G. McDonell
- Behavioral Health Innovation, Washington State University, Spokane, WA, 99202, USA
| | - Paul A. Decker
- Division of Clinical Trials and Biostatistics, Department of Quantitative Health Sciences, Mayo Clinic, 200 1st St SW, Rochester, MN, 55905, USA
| | - Flora R. Lee
- Research Services, Division of Community Health Services, Alaska Native Tribal Health Consortium, 4000 Ambassador Dr, Anchorage, AK, 99508, USA
| | - Janessa K. Fosi
- Research Services, Division of Community Health Services, Alaska Native Tribal Health Consortium, 4000 Ambassador Dr, Anchorage, AK, 99508, USA
| | - Antonia M. Young
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychology and Behavioral Health Research Program, Mayo Clinic, 200 1st St SW, Rochester, MN, 55905, USA
| | - Corinna V. Sabaque
- Center for Health Equity and Community Engagement Research, Mayo Clinic, 200 1st St SW, Rochester, MN, 55905, USA
| | - Ashley R. Brown
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychology and Behavioral Health Research Program, Mayo Clinic, 200 1st St SW, Rochester, MN, 55905, USA
| | - Bijan J. Borah
- Health Care Policy and Research, Center for the Science of Health Care Delivery, Mayo Clinic, 200 1st St SW, Rochester, MN, 55905, USA
| | - Timothy K. Thomas
- Research Services, Division of Community Health Services, Alaska Native Tribal Health Consortium, 4000 Ambassador Dr, Anchorage, AK, 99508, USA
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
van de Groep S, Sweijen SW, de Water E, Crone EA. Temporal discounting for self and friends in adolescence: A fMRI study. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2023; 60:101204. [PMID: 36736019 PMCID: PMC9918426 DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2023.101204] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/29/2022] [Revised: 01/20/2023] [Accepted: 01/26/2023] [Indexed: 02/05/2023] Open
Abstract
Adolescence is characterized by impulsivity but also by increased importance of friendships. This study took the novel perspective of testing temporal discounting in a fMRI task where choices could affect outcomes for 96 adolescents (aged 10-20-years) themselves and their best friend. Decisions either benefitted themselves (i.e., the Self Immediate - Self Delay' condition) or their friend (i.e., 'Friend Immediate - Friend Delay' condition); or juxtaposed rewards for themselves and their friends (i.e., the 'Self Immediate - Friend Delay' or 'Friend Immediate - Self Delay' conditions). We observed that younger adolescents were more impulsive; and all participants were more impulsive when this was associated with an immediate benefit for friends. Individual differences analyses revealed increased activity in the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex extending in the ventral striatum for immediate relative to delayed reward choices for self. Temporal choices were associated with activity in the prefrontal cortex, parietal cortex, insula, and ventral striatum, but only activity in the right inferior parietal lobe was associated with age. Finally, temporal delay choices for friends relative to self were associated with increased activity in the temporo-parietal junction and precuneus. Overall, this study shows a unique role of the social context in adolescents' temporal decision making.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Suzanne van de Groep
- Erasmus SYNC Lab, the Netherlands; Erasmus School of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Erasmus University Rotterdam, the Netherlands; Brain and Development Research Center, the Netherlands; Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, the Netherlands.
| | - Sophie W Sweijen
- Erasmus SYNC Lab, the Netherlands; Erasmus School of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Erasmus University Rotterdam, the Netherlands; Brain and Development Research Center, the Netherlands; Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, the Netherlands
| | - Erik de Water
- Great Lakes Neurobehavioral Center, Edina, MN, United States
| | - Eveline A Crone
- Erasmus SYNC Lab, the Netherlands; Erasmus School of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Erasmus University Rotterdam, the Netherlands; Brain and Development Research Center, the Netherlands; Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
El Haj M, Caillaud M, Moustafa A, Prundean A, Scherer C, Verny C, Allain P. "Ten euros now" temporal discounting in Huntington disease. Neurol Sci 2023:10.1007/s10072-023-06775-z. [PMID: 36964316 DOI: 10.1007/s10072-023-06775-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/02/2023] [Accepted: 03/21/2023] [Indexed: 03/26/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND When making decisions, one often faces a trade-off between immediate and long-term rewards. In these situations, people may prefer immediate over later rewards, even if immediate rewards are smaller than later ones; a phenomenon known as temporal discounting. In this study, we, for the first time, assessed temporal discounting in three populations: participants with manifest Huntington disease (HD), participants with premanifest HD, and control participants. METHODS Using the temporal discounting task, we invited participants to choose between small immediate amount of money vs. delayed, but larger amount of money (e.g., "Which do you prefer: you get 10 euros right now or 50 euros in a month?"). We also measured inhibition in order to test if it impacts discounting performance. RESULTS Analysis demonstrated higher temporal discounting (i.e., a preference for the immediate rewards) in participants with manifest HD compared to those with premanifest HD or control participants, but no significant differences were observed in participants with premanifest HD and control participants. Analysis also demonstrated significant correlations between temporal discounting and scores on an inhibition test in participants with manifest HD, but not in those with premanifest HD or in control participants. DISCUSSION We suggest that, when making decisions, patients with manifest HD may have difficulties with suppressing the temptation of smaller, but immediate, rewards.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mohamad El Haj
- Nantes Université, Univ Angers, Laboratoire de Psychologie des Pays de la Loire (LPPL - EA 4638), F-44000, Nantes, France.
- CHU Nantes, Clinical Gerontology Department, 41 rue Pierre et Marie Curie, 44093, Nantes, France.
- Institut Universitaire de France, Paris, France.
- Faculté de Psychologie, LPPL - Laboratoire de Psychologie des Pays de la Loire, Université Nantes, Chemin de la Censive du Tertre, BP 81227, 44312, Nantes Cedex 3, France.
| | - Marie Caillaud
- University of Texas, Clinical Neuroscience Lab, 108 East Dean Keeton St., Austin, TX, 78712, USA
| | - Ahmed Moustafa
- School of Psychology, Faculty of Society and Design, Bond University, Gold Coast, Queensland, Australia
- Department of Human Anatomy and Physiology, the Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Johannesburg, Johannesburg, South Africa
| | | | | | | | - Philippe Allain
- Département de Neurologie, CHU Angers, Angers, France
- Laboratoire de Psychologie des Pays de la Loire, LPPL EA 4638 SFR Confluences, UNIV Angers, Nantes Université, Maison de la recherche Germaine Tillion, 5 bis Boulevard Lavoisier, 49045, Angers Cedex 01, France
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
El Haj M, Moustafa AA. "Ten dollars today or 50 dollars after one month?" Temporal discounting in Korsakoff syndrome. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 2023; 28:116-129. [PMID: 36724487 DOI: 10.1080/13546805.2023.2173059] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/03/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Little research has investigated decision making in patients with Korsakoff syndrome (KS). Specifically, to our knowledge, there is a lack of research investigating whether patients with KS may tend to prefer immediate over future rewards (i.e., temporal discounting). Further, we investigated the relationship between temporal discounting and inhibition. METHODS We, for the first time, invited patients with KS and control participants to perform a temporal discounting task, in which they answered questions probing preferences between an immediate, but smaller amount of money, and a delayed, but larger amount of money (e.g., "would you prefer 10 dollars today or 50 dollars after one month?"). Furthermore, inhibition was measured using the Stroop Colour Word Test. RESULTS Analysis demonstrated higher temporal discounting in patients with KS than in control participants. Temporal discounting in both populations was significantly correlated with inhibition. CONCLUSIONS Patients with KS may have difficulties to suppress the temptation of smaller, but immediate, rewards.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mohamad El Haj
- Laboratoire de Psychologie des Pays de la Loire (LPPL - EA 4638), Nantes Université, Univ Angers Nantes, France
- Unité de Gériatrie, Centre Hospitalier de Tourcoing, Tourcoing, France
- Institut Universitaire de France, Paris, France
| | - Ahmed A Moustafa
- School of Psychology, Faculty of Society and Design, Bond University, Gold Coast, Australia
- Department of Human Anatomy and Physiology, the Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Johannesburg, Johannesburg, South Africa
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Miguel AQ, Smith CL, Rodin NM, Johnson RK, McDonell MG, McPherson SM. Automated Reinforcement Management System: Feasibility study findings of an app-based contingency management treatment for alcohol use disorder. DRUG AND ALCOHOL DEPENDENCE REPORTS 2023; 6:100140. [PMID: 36994367 PMCID: PMC10040325 DOI: 10.1016/j.dadr.2023.100140] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/11/2022] [Revised: 01/26/2023] [Accepted: 01/30/2023] [Indexed: 02/04/2023]
Abstract
Alcohol Use Disorder (AUD) is the most prevalent substance use disorder in the United States and is directly related to 5% of all annually reported deaths worldwide. Contingency Management (CM) is among the most effective interventions for AUD, with recent technological advancements allowing CM to be provided remotely. Objective: To evaluate the feasibility and acceptability of a mobile Automated Reinforcement Management System (ARMS) designed to provide CM for AUD remotely. Methods: Twelve participants with mild or moderate AUD were exposed to ARMS in a A-B-A within-subject experimental design where they were required to submit three breathalyzer samples per day. During the B phase participants could earned rewards with monetary value for submitting negative samples. Feasibility was determined by the proportion of samples submitted and retention in the study and acceptability was based on participants self-reported experience. Results: The mean number of samples submitted per day was 2.02 out of 3. The proportion of samples submitted in each phase was 81.5%, 69.4% and 49.4%, respectively. Participants were retained for a mean of 7.5 (SD=1.1) out of 8 weeks with 10 participants (83.3%) completing the study. All participants found the app easy to use and stated it helped them reduce their alcohol use. Eleven (91.7%) would recommend the app as an adjunct to AUD treatment. Preliminary indicators of efficacy are also presented. Conclusions: ARMS has shown to be feasible and well accepted. If shown effective, ARMS can serve as an adjunctive treatment for AUD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- André Q. Miguel
- Elson S Floyd College of Medicine, Washington State University, Spokane, WA, United States
- Analytics and PsychoPharmacology Laboratory, Spokane, WA, United States
- Program of Excellence in Addictions Research, Spokane, WA, United States
- Corresponding author.
| | - Crystal L. Smith
- Elson S Floyd College of Medicine, Washington State University, Spokane, WA, United States
- Analytics and PsychoPharmacology Laboratory, Spokane, WA, United States
- Program of Excellence in Addictions Research, Spokane, WA, United States
| | - Nicole M. Rodin
- College of Pharmacy and Pharmaceutical Sciences, Washington State University, Spokane, WA, United States
- Analytics and PsychoPharmacology Laboratory, Spokane, WA, United States
- Program of Excellence in Addictions Research, Spokane, WA, United States
| | | | - Michael G. McDonell
- Elson S Floyd College of Medicine, Washington State University, Spokane, WA, United States
- Analytics and PsychoPharmacology Laboratory, Spokane, WA, United States
- Program of Excellence in Addictions Research, Spokane, WA, United States
| | - Sterling M. McPherson
- Elson S Floyd College of Medicine, Washington State University, Spokane, WA, United States
- Analytics and PsychoPharmacology Laboratory, Spokane, WA, United States
- Program of Excellence in Addictions Research, Spokane, WA, United States
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Burrell M, Pastor-Bernier A, Schultz W. Worth the work? Monkeys discount rewards by a subjective adapting effort cost. BIORXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR BIOLOGY 2023:2023.01.10.523384. [PMID: 36712043 PMCID: PMC9882027 DOI: 10.1101/2023.01.10.523384] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/12/2023]
Abstract
All life must solve how to allocate limited energy resources to maximise benefits from scarce opportunities. Economic theory posits decision makers optimise choice by maximising the subjective benefit (utility) of reward minus the subjective cost (disutility) of the required effort. While successful in many settings, this model does not fully account for how experience can alter reward-effort trade-offs. Here we test how well the subtractive model of effort disutility explains the behavior of two non-human primates ( Macaca mulatta ) in a binary choice task in which reward quantity and physical effort to obtain were varied.Applying random utility modelling to independently estimate reward utility and effort disutility, we show the subtractive effort model better explains out-of-sample choice behavior when compared to parabolic and exponential effort discounting. Furthermore, we demonstrate that effort disutility is dependent on previous experience of effort: in analogy to work from behavioral labour economics, we develop a model of reference-dependent effort disutility to explain the increased willingness to expend effort following previous experience of effortful options in a session. The result of this analysis suggests that monkeys discount reward by an effort cost that is measured relative to an expected effort learned from previous trials. When this subjective cost of effort, a function of context and experience, is accounted for, trial-by-trial choice behavior can be explained by the subtractive cost model of effort.Therefore, in searching for net utility signals that may underpin effort-based decision-making in the brain, careful measurement of subjective effort costs is an essential first step. Significance All decision-makers need to consider how much effort they need to expend when evaluating potential options. Economic theories suggest that the optimal way to choose is by cost-benefit analysis of reward against effort. To be able to do this efficiently over many decision contexts, this needs to be done flexibly, with appropriate adaptation to context and experience. Therefore, in aiming to understand how this might be achieved in the brain, it is important to first carefully measure the subjective cost of effort. Here we show monkeys make reward-effort cost-benefit decisions, subtracting the subjective cost of effort from the subjective value of rewards. Moreover, the subjective cost of effort is dependent on the monkeys’ experience of effort in previous trials.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mark Burrell
- Department of Physiology, Development and Neuroscience, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3DY, United Kingdom
| | - Alexandre Pastor-Bernier
- Department of Physiology, Development and Neuroscience, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3DY, United Kingdom
| | - Wolfram Schultz
- Department of Physiology, Development and Neuroscience, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3DY, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Stahlman WD, Catania AC. Faustian bargains: Short-term and long-term contingencies in phylogeny, ontogeny, and sociogeny. J Exp Anal Behav 2023; 119:192-202. [PMID: 36478575 PMCID: PMC10107318 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.812] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/14/2022] [Revised: 11/07/2022] [Accepted: 11/18/2022] [Indexed: 12/13/2022]
Abstract
Rachlin's interpretations of self-control depend on the short-term versus the long-term consequences of behavior. Sometimes these effects support each other (typing an abstract produces a written product now and is later read by others). Sometimes they conflict (procrastination now is incompatible with finishing the abstract by deadline). We usually reserve the language of self-control for human cases where long-term consequences are chosen over short-term ones. Rachlin made this distinction salient in ontogeny, but it also applies to selection in phylogeny (Darwinian evolution) and sociogeny (behavior passed from one organism to another). Our account examines relations between short-term and long-term consequences at each level of selection. For example, sexual selection has adaptive, short-term mating consequences but may drive species to extreme specializations that jeopardize long-term survival. In sociogeny, as in the Tragedy of the Commons, group members may get immediate economic benefits from exploiting resources but exhaust those resources over the long term. Whatever the level, when short-term and long-term consequences have opposing effects, adaptive behavior may depend on whether temporally extended contingencies exert more control than more immediate benefits.
Collapse
|
18
|
Bulley A, Lempert KM, Conwell C, Irish M, Schacter DL. Intertemporal choice reflects value comparison rather than self-control: insights from confidence judgements. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2022; 377:20210338. [PMID: 36314145 PMCID: PMC9619231 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2021.0338] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/23/2021] [Accepted: 05/24/2022] [Indexed: 12/21/2023] Open
Abstract
Intertemporal decision-making has long been assumed to measure self-control, with prominent theories treating choices of smaller, sooner rewards as failed attempts to override immediate temptation. If this view is correct, people should be more confident in their intertemporal decisions when they 'successfully' delay gratification than when they do not. In two pre-registered experiments with built-in replication, adult participants (n = 117) made monetary intertemporal choices and rated their confidence in having made the right decisions. Contrary to assumptions of the self-control account, confidence was not higher when participants chose delayed rewards. Rather, participants were more confident in their decisions when possible rewards were further apart in time-discounted subjective value, closer to the present, and larger in magnitude. Demonstrating metacognitive insight, participants were more confident in decisions that better aligned with their separate valuation of possible rewards. Decisions made with less confidence were more prone to changes-of-mind and more susceptible to a patience-enhancing manipulation. Together, our results establish that confidence in intertemporal choice tracks uncertainty in estimating and comparing the value of possible rewards-just as it does in decisions unrelated to self-control. Our findings challenge self-control views and instead cast intertemporal choice as a form of value-based decision-making about future possibilities. This article is part of the theme issue 'Thinking about possibilities: mechanisms, ontogeny, functions and phylogeny'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Adam Bulley
- The University of Sydney School of Psychology and Brain and Mind Centre, Sydney, NSW 2050, Australia
- Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA
| | - Karolina M. Lempert
- Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA
| | - Colin Conwell
- Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA
| | - Muireann Irish
- The University of Sydney School of Psychology and Brain and Mind Centre, Sydney, NSW 2050, Australia
| | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Munuera J, Burguière E. Can we tackle climate change by behavioral hacking of the dopaminergic system? Front Behav Neurosci 2022; 16:996955. [PMID: 36311863 PMCID: PMC9606619 DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.996955] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/18/2022] [Accepted: 09/20/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Climate change is an undeniable fact that will certainly affect millions of people in the following decades. Despite this danger threatening our economies, wellbeing and our lives in general, there is a lack of immediate response at both the institutional and individual level. How can it be that the human brain cannot interpret this threat and act against it to avoid the immense negative consequences that may ensue? Here we argue that this paradox could be explained by the fact that some key brain mechanisms are potentially poorly tuned to take action against a threat that would take full effect only in the long-term. We present neuro-behavioral evidence in favor of this proposal and discuss the role of the dopaminergic (DA) system in learning accurate prediction of the value of an outcome, and its consequences regarding the climate issue. We discuss how this system discounts the value of delayed outcomes and, consequently, does not favor action against the climate crisis. Finally, according to this framework, we suggest that this view may be reconsidered and, on the contrary, that the DA reinforcement learning system could be a powerful ally if adapted to short-term incentives which promote climate-friendly behaviors. Additionally, the DA system interacts with multiple brain systems, in particular those related to higher cognitive functions, which can adjust its functions depending on psychological, social, or other complex contextual information. Thus, we propose several generic action plans that could help to hack these neuro-behavioral processes to promote climate-friendly actions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jérôme Munuera
- Sorbonne Université, Institut du Cerveau–Paris Brain Institute–ICM, Inserm, CNRS, AP-HP, Hôpital de la Pitié Salpêtrière, Paris, France
- Institut Jean Nicod, Département d’Études Cognitives, École Normale Supérieure (ENS), EHESS, CNRS, PSL University, Paris, France
- *Correspondence: Jérôme Munuera,
| | - Eric Burguière
- Sorbonne Université, Institut du Cerveau–Paris Brain Institute–ICM, Inserm, CNRS, AP-HP, Hôpital de la Pitié Salpêtrière, Paris, France
- Eric Burguière,
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Ruggeri K, Panin A, Vdovic M, Većkalov B, Abdul-Salaam N, Achterberg J, Akil C, Amatya J, Amatya K, Andersen TL, Aquino SD, Arunasalam A, Ashcroft-Jones S, Askelund AD, Ayacaxli N, Sheshdeh AB, Bailey A, Barea Arroyo P, Mejía GB, Benvenuti M, Berge ML, Bermaganbet A, Bibilouri K, Bjørndal LD, Black S, Lyshol JKB, Brik T, Buabang EK, Burghart M, Bursalıoğlu A, Buzayu NM, Čadek M, de Carvalho NM, Cazan AM, Çetinçelik M, Chai VE, Chen P, Chen S, Clay G, D'Ambrogio S, Damnjanović K, Duffy G, Dugue T, Dwarkanath T, Envuladu EA, Erceg N, Esteban-Serna C, Farahat E, Farrokhnia RA, Fawad M, Fedryansyah M, Feng D, Filippi S, Fonollá MA, Freichel R, Freira L, Friedemann M, Gao Z, Ge S, Geiger SJ, George L, Grabovski I, Gracheva A, Gracheva A, Hajian A, Hasan N, Hecht M, Hong X, Hubená B, Ikonomeas AGF, Ilić S, Izydorczyk D, Jakob L, Janssens M, Jarke H, Kácha O, Kalinova KN, Kapingura FM, Karakasheva R, Kasdan DO, Kemel E, Khorrami P, Krawiec JM, Lagidze N, Lazarević A, Lazić A, Lee HS, Lep Ž, Lins S, Lofthus IS, Macchia L, Mamede S, Mamo MA, Maratkyzy L, Mareva S, Marwaha S, McGill L, McParland S, Melnic A, Meyer SA, Mizak S, Mohammed A, Mukhyshbayeva A, Navajas J, Neshevska D, Niazi SJ, Nieves AEN, Nippold F, Oberschulte J, Otto T, Pae R, Panchelieva T, Park SY, Pascu DS, Pavlović I, Petrović MB, Popović D, Prinz GM, Rachev NR, Ranc P, Razum J, Rho CE, Riitsalu L, Rocca F, Rosenbaum RS, Rujimora J, Rusyidi B, Rutherford C, Said R, Sanguino I, Sarikaya AK, Say N, Schuck J, Shiels M, Shir Y, Sievert EDC, Soboleva I, Solomonia T, Soni S, Soysal I, Stablum F, Sundström FTA, Tang X, Tavera F, Taylor J, Tebbe AL, Thommesen KK, Tobias-Webb J, Todsen AL, Toscano F, Tran T, Trinh J, Turati A, Ueda K, Vacondio M, Vakhitov V, Valencia AJ, Van Reyn C, Venema TAG, Verra SE, Vintr J, Vranka MA, Wagner L, Wu X, Xing KY, Xu K, Xu S, Yamada Y, Yosifova A, Zupan Z, García-Garzon E. The globalizability of temporal discounting. Nat Hum Behav 2022; 6:1386-1397. [PMID: 35817934 PMCID: PMC9584811 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-022-01392-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/03/2021] [Accepted: 05/17/2022] [Indexed: 01/08/2023]
Abstract
Economic inequality is associated with preferences for smaller, immediate gains over larger, delayed ones. Such temporal discounting may feed into rising global inequality, yet it is unclear whether it is a function of choice preferences or norms, or rather the absence of sufficient resources for immediate needs. It is also not clear whether these reflect true differences in choice patterns between income groups. We tested temporal discounting and five intertemporal choice anomalies using local currencies and value standards in 61 countries (N = 13,629). Across a diverse sample, we found consistent, robust rates of choice anomalies. Lower-income groups were not significantly different, but economic inequality and broader financial circumstances were clearly correlated with population choice patterns.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kai Ruggeri
- Columbia University, New York, NY, USA.
- Centre for Business Research, Judge Business School, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK.
| | | | - Milica Vdovic
- Faculty of Media and Communications, Belgrade, Serbia
| | | | | | - Jascha Achterberg
- University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
- MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK
| | - Carla Akil
- American University of Beirut, Beirut, Lebanon
| | - Jolly Amatya
- UN Major Group for Children and Youth (UNMGCY), Kathmandu, Nepal
| | - Kanchan Amatya
- United Nations Children's Fund (UNICEF), Kathmandu, Nepal
| | | | - Sibele D Aquino
- Pontifical Catholic University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
- Laboratory of Research in Social Psychology, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | - Nathalia Melo de Carvalho
- Pontifical Catholic University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
- Estácio de Sá University, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
| | | | - Melis Çetinçelik
- Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, the Netherlands
| | | | - Patricia Chen
- National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore
| | - Shiyi Chen
- The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China
| | - Georgia Clay
- Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | - Eman Farahat
- Ain Shams University, Cairo, Egypt
- International Socioeconomics Laboratory, New York, NY, USA
| | | | | | | | - David Feng
- Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
- London School of Economics and Political Science, London, UK
| | | | | | | | - Lucia Freira
- Universidad Torcuato Di Tella, Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | | | - Ziwei Gao
- University College London, London, UK
| | - Suwen Ge
- Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| | | | | | | | | | - Anastasia Gracheva
- Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
- The Wharton School of the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | | | - Nida Hasan
- Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
- Sciences Po, Paris, France
| | - Marlene Hecht
- Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
- Humboldt University of Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | | | | | | | | | | | - Lea Jakob
- Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic
- National Institute of Mental Health, Klecany, Czech Republic
| | | | | | - Ondřej Kácha
- University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
- Green Dock, Hostivice, Czech Republic
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | - Jakub M Krawiec
- SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Warsaw, Poland
| | | | | | | | | | - Žan Lep
- University of Ljubljana, Ljubljana, Slovenia
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | - Lucy McGill
- University of Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands
| | | | | | | | - Szymon Mizak
- SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Warsaw, Poland
| | | | | | - Joaquin Navajas
- Universidad Torcuato Di Tella, Buenos Aires, Argentina
- National Scientific and Technical Research Council, Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | - Riinu Pae
- University College London, London, UK
| | | | | | | | | | | | - Dora Popović
- Ivo Pilar Institute of Social Sciences, Zagreb, Croatia
| | | | | | - Pika Ranc
- University of Ljubljana, Ljubljana, Slovenia
| | - Josip Razum
- Ivo Pilar Institute of Social Sciences, Zagreb, Croatia
| | | | | | | | - R Shayna Rosenbaum
- York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
- Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| | | | | | | | - Rand Said
- Queen's University Belfast, Belfast, UK
| | | | | | - Nicolas Say
- Prague University of Economics and Business, Prague, Czech Republic
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | - Siddhant Soni
- Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, Netherlands
| | - Irem Soysal
- Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
- University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| | - Federica Stablum
- University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
- University of Trento, Trento, Italy
| | | | | | | | | | - Anna-Lena Tebbe
- Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| | | | | | | | | | - Tran Tran
- Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, Netherlands
| | | | - Alice Turati
- Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
- Sciences Po, Paris, France
| | | | | | | | - Adrianna J Valencia
- Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
- University of Central Florida, Orlando, FL, USA
| | | | | | | | - Jáchym Vintr
- Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic
- Green Dock, Hostivice, Czech Republic
| | | | | | - Xue Wu
- Kyushu University, Fukuoka, Japan
| | | | - Kailin Xu
- Queen's University Belfast, Belfast, UK
| | - Sonya Xu
- Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
- University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
21
|
Borissova A, Soni S, Aston ER, Lees R, Petrilli K, Wall MB, Bloomfield MAP, Mertzani E, Paksina A, Freeman TP, Mokrysz C, Lawn W, Curran HV. Age differences in the behavioural economics of cannabis use: Do adolescents and adults differ on demand for cannabis and discounting of future reward? Drug Alcohol Depend 2022; 238:109531. [PMID: 35809475 DOI: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2022.109531] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/13/2021] [Revised: 04/28/2022] [Accepted: 04/29/2022] [Indexed: 11/03/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Adolescence is a period of psychological and neural development in which harms associated with cannabis use may be heightened. We hypothesised that adolescent who use cannabis (adolescentsWUC) would have steeper delay discounting (preference for immediate over future rewards) and greater demand (relative valuation) for cannabis than adults who use cannabis (adultsWUC). METHODS This cross-sectional study, part of the 'CannTeen' project, compared adultsWUC (n = 71, 26-29 years old) and adolescentsWUC (n = 76, 16-17 years old), and gender- and age-matched adolescent (n = 63) and adult (n = 64) controls. AdolescentsWUC and adultsWUC used cannabis 1-7 days/week and were matched on cannabis use frequency (4 days/week). The Monetary Choice Questionnaire assessed delay discounting. A modified Marijuana Purchase Task (MPT) assessed cannabis demand in adolescentsWUC and adultsWUC. The MPT yielded five indices: intensity (amount of cannabis used at zero cost), Omax (total peak expenditure), Pmax (price at peak expenditure), breakpoint (cost at which cannabis demand is suppressed to zero) and elasticity (degree to which cannabis use decreases with increasing price). Analyses were adjusted for covariates of gender, socioeconomic status, other illicit drug use. RESULTS Both adolescentsWUC and adultsWUC had steeper delay discounting than controls (F, (1,254)= 9.13, p = 0.003, ηp2= 0.04), with no significant age effect or interaction. AdolescentsWUC showed higher intensity (F, (1,138)= 9.76, p = 0.002, ηp2= 0.07) and lower elasticity (F, (1,138)= 15.25, p < 0.001, ηp2= 0.10) than adultsWUC. There were no significant differences in Pmax, Omax or breakpoint. CONCLUSION Individuals who use cannabis prefer immediate rewards more than controls. AdolescentsWUC, compared to adultsWUC, may be in a high-risk category with diminished sensitivity to cannabis price increases and a greater consumption of cannabis when it is free.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A Borissova
- Clinical Psychopharmacology Unit, University College London, London, United Kingdom; Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, United Kingdom; NIHR University College London Hospitals Biomedical Research Centre, University College Hospital, London, United Kingdom.
| | - S Soni
- Clinical Psychopharmacology Unit, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - E R Aston
- Center for Alcohol and Addiction Studies, Department of Behavioral and Social Sciences, Brown University School of Public Health, Providence, RI, USA
| | - R Lees
- Addiction and Mental Health Group (AIM), Department of Psychology, University of Bath, Bath, United Kingdom
| | - K Petrilli
- Addiction and Mental Health Group (AIM), Department of Psychology, University of Bath, Bath, United Kingdom
| | - M B Wall
- Clinical Psychopharmacology Unit, University College London, London, United Kingdom; Invicro London, Burlington Danes Building, Hammersmith Hospital, Du Cane Road, London, United Kingdom
| | - M A P Bloomfield
- Clinical Psychopharmacology Unit, University College London, London, United Kingdom; NIHR University College London Hospitals Biomedical Research Centre, University College Hospital, London, United Kingdom; Translational Psychiatry Research Group, Research Department of Mental Health Neuroscience, Division of Psychiatry, University College London, London, United Kingdom; Psychiatric Imaging Group, Medical Research Council London Institute of Medical Sciences, Imperial College London, Hammersmith Hospital, London, United Kingdom
| | - E Mertzani
- Clinical Psychopharmacology Unit, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - A Paksina
- Clinical Psychopharmacology Unit, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - T P Freeman
- Clinical Psychopharmacology Unit, University College London, London, United Kingdom; Addiction and Mental Health Group (AIM), Department of Psychology, University of Bath, Bath, United Kingdom
| | - C Mokrysz
- Clinical Psychopharmacology Unit, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - W Lawn
- Clinical Psychopharmacology Unit, University College London, London, United Kingdom; National Addiction Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, United Kingdom; Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - H V Curran
- Clinical Psychopharmacology Unit, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Contreras-Huerta LS, Lockwood PL, Bird G, Apps MAJ, Crockett MJ. Prosocial behavior is associated with transdiagnostic markers of affective sensitivity in multiple domains. Emotion 2022; 22:820-835. [PMID: 32718171 PMCID: PMC9301775 DOI: 10.1037/emo0000813] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Prosocial behaviors-actions that benefit others-fundamentally shape our interpersonal interactions. Psychiatric disorders have been suggested to be related to prosocial disturbances, which may underlie many of their social impairments. However, broader affective traits, present to different degrees in both psychiatric and healthy populations, also have been linked to variability in prosociality. Therefore, it is unclear to what extent prosocial variability is explained by specific psychiatric disorders relative to broad affective traits. Using a computational, transdiagnostic approach in two online studies, we found that participants who reported being more affectively reactive across a broad cluster of traits manifested greater frequencies of prosocial actions in two different contexts: They reported being more averse to harming others for profit, and they were more willing to exert effort to benefit others. These findings help illuminate the profile of prosociality across psychiatric conditions as well as the architecture of prosocial behavior in healthy individuals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
Collapse
|
23
|
Furrebøe EF. Qualitative variations in delay discounting: A brief review and future directions. Behav Processes 2022; 200:104666. [PMID: 35680101 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2022.104666] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/11/2021] [Revised: 04/29/2022] [Accepted: 05/28/2022] [Indexed: 11/02/2022]
Abstract
The discounting paradigm has been challenged by an increasing number of studies presenting qualitative variations in the individual discount function. Particularly, the subjective value of a loss does not necessarily systematically decrease with delay to the outcome. Qualitative variation refers to variations in shape rather than steepness of the discount function, such as positive discounting, zero discounting, unsystematic discounting, and negative discounting. Data from three previous studies were analysed in terms of qualitative variations observed in delay discounting patterns. Attention was also given to methods used and to the relationship between the results from the various levels of investigation. We found qualitative differences between discounting of monetary gains and losses on an individual level. While discounting of gains mainly took the form of conventional positive discounting, discounting of losses often took the form of zero discounting or unsystematic discounting. Further, there were more qualitative variations in discounting of both gains and losses among adolescents compared to adults. By examining verbal reports and single choices, we identified some of the rules and consequences involved in these delay discounting patterns. The different rules and consequences observed for the gain and loss scenarios, support that discounting of gains and losses may involve different combinations of reinforcing contingencies. These results point towards a possible way to explain the influences of qualitative variations in delay discounting.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elise Frølich Furrebøe
- School of Business and Law, University of Agder, Postboks 422, 4604 Kristiansand, Norway.
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Zhu X. Proximal language predicts crowdfunding success: Behavioral and experimental evidence. COMPUTERS IN HUMAN BEHAVIOR 2022. [DOI: 10.1016/j.chb.2022.107213] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
|
25
|
Basgol H, Ayhan I, Ugur E. Time Perception: A Review on Psychological, Computational, and Robotic Models. IEEE Trans Cogn Dev Syst 2022. [DOI: 10.1109/tcds.2021.3059045] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Hamit Basgol
- Department of Cognitive Science, Bogazici University, Istanbul, Turkey
| | - Inci Ayhan
- Department of Psychology, Bogazici University, Istanbul, Turkey
| | - Emre Ugur
- Department of Computer Engineering, Bogazici University, Istanbul, Turkey
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Sedgwick-Müller JA, Müller-Sedgwick U, Adamou M, Catani M, Champ R, Gudjónsson G, Hank D, Pitts M, Young S, Asherson P. University students with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a consensus statement from the UK Adult ADHD Network (UKAAN). BMC Psychiatry 2022; 22:292. [PMID: 35459116 PMCID: PMC9027028 DOI: 10.1186/s12888-022-03898-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/29/2021] [Accepted: 03/15/2022] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is associated with poor educational outcomes that can have long-term negative effects on the mental health, wellbeing, and socio-economic outcomes of university students. Mental health provision for university students with ADHD is often inadequate due to long waiting times for access to diagnosis and treatment in specialist National Health Service (NHS) clinics. ADHD is a hidden and marginalised disability, and within higher education in the UK, the categorisation of ADHD as a specific learning difference (or difficulty) may be contributing to this. AIMS This consensus aims to provide an informed understanding of the impact of ADHD on the educational (or academic) outcomes of university students and highlight an urgent need for timely access to treatment and management. METHODS The UK Adult ADHD Network (UKAAN) convened a meeting of practitioners and experts from England, Wales, and Scotland, to discuss issues that university students with ADHD can experience or present with during their programme of studies and how best to address them. A report on the collective analysis, evaluation, and opinions of the expert panel and published literature about the impact of ADHD on the educational outcomes of university students is presented. RESULTS A consensus was reached that offers expert advice, practical guidance, and recommendations to support the medical, education, and disability practitioners working with university students with ADHD. CONCLUSIONS Practical advice, guidance, and recommendations based on expert consensus can inform the identification of ADHD in university students, personalised interventions, and educational support, as well as contribute to existing research in this topic area. There is a need to move away from prevailing notions within higher education about ADHD being a specific learning difference (or difficulty) and attend to the urgent need for university students with ADHD to have timely access to treatment and support. A multimodal approach can be adapted to support university students with ADHD. This approach would view timely access to treatment, including reasonable adjustments and educational support, as having a positive impact on the academic performance and achievement of university students with ADHD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jane A. Sedgwick-Müller
- Health and Community Services, Government of Jersey, St Helier, Jersey. Social, Genetic & Developmental Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience (IoPPN) & Florence Nightingale Faculty of Nursing, Midwifery & Palliative Care (FNFNM), King’s College London, London, UK
| | - Ulrich Müller-Sedgwick
- Adult Neurodevelopmental Service, Health and Community Services, Government of Jersey, St Helier, Jersey. Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Marios Adamou
- School of Human and Health Sciences, University of Huddersfield, Huddersfield, UK
| | - Marco Catani
- Natbrainlab, Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience (IoPPN), King’s College London, London, UK
| | - Rebecca Champ
- School of Human and Health Sciences, University of Huddersfield, Huddersfield, UK
| | - Gísli Gudjónsson
- Psychology Department, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience (IoPPN), King’s College London, London, UK
| | - Dietmar Hank
- Adult ADHD Service, Avon and Wiltshire Mental Health Partnership NHS Trust, Bristol, UK
| | - Mark Pitts
- Adult ADHD and Autism Outpatient Service, South London & Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust, London, UK
| | - Susan Young
- Psychology Services Limited, Department of Psychology, Reykjavik University, Reykjavik, Iceland
| | - Philip Asherson
- Social, Genetic & Developmental Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience (IoPPN), King’s College London, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
van Hoorn J, de Water E, Dekkers TJ, Pollak Y, Popma A, Jansen BRJ, Huizenga HM, van Duijvenvoorde ACK. Peer feedback decreases impulsive choice in adolescents with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JCPP ADVANCES 2022; 2:e12065. [PMID: 37431500 PMCID: PMC10242953 DOI: 10.1002/jcv2.12065] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/30/2022] [Accepted: 02/03/2022] [Indexed: 01/02/2023] Open
Abstract
Background Impulsivity is a core feature of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Previous work using the delay discounting task to assess impulsivity reveals that adolescents with ADHD tend to prefer a smaller-immediate reward over a larger-delayed reward, and this relates to problematic choices in daily life. To gain a better understanding of daily decision-making in adolescence, it is important to examine the social context, as peers have a major influence on decisions. Peer influence often has a negative connotation, but also provides an opportunity to promote positive outcomes. To date, it is unclear if peers affect impulsive decision-making in adolescents with ADHD, for better or for worse. Methods The aim of this preregistered study was to examine the effect of peer feedback on impulsive choice in male adolescents with and without ADHD (ages 13-23; N = 113). We utilized an adapted delay discounting task that was administered alone, in a social condition, and alone again. In the social condition, adolescents received either (between-subjects) manipulated impulsive or non-impulsive peer feedback. Impulsive peer feedback consisted of likes for choosing the smaller immediate reward, whereas non-impulsive peers endorsed choosing the larger delayed reward. Results Preregistered analyses showed that non-impulsive peer feedback resulted in decreased impulsive choice, whereas impulsive peer feedback did not alter decision-making in adolescents with and without ADHD. Explorative analyses of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms in the total sample, irrespective of diagnosis, showed that lower hyperactivity-impulsivity and more inattention symptoms were associated with increased susceptibility to non-impulsive peer feedback. Conclusions Together, these findings indicate that peers may provide an opportunity to decrease impulsivity and emphasize individual differences in susceptibility to non-impulsive peer feedback related to inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity. Therefore, peer feedback may be a promising component in behavioral peer-supported interventions in adolescents with ADHD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jorien van Hoorn
- Institute of Psychology, Department of Developmental and Educational PsychologyLeiden UniversityLeidenThe Netherlands
- Leiden Institute for Brain and CognitionLeidenThe Netherlands
- LevvelAcademic Center for Child‐ and Adolescent PsychiatryAmsterdamThe Netherlands
| | | | - Tycho J. Dekkers
- LevvelAcademic Center for Child‐ and Adolescent PsychiatryAmsterdamThe Netherlands
- Department of PsychologyUniversity of AmsterdamAmsterdamThe Netherlands
- Department of Child‐ and Adolescent PsychiatryUniversity Medical Center GroningenGroningenThe Netherlands
- Department of Child‐ and Adolescent PsychiatryAmsterdam University Medical CentersAmsterdamThe Netherlands
| | - Yehuda Pollak
- Seymour Fox School of EducationHebrew University of JerusalemJerusalemIsrael
| | - Arne Popma
- LevvelAcademic Center for Child‐ and Adolescent PsychiatryAmsterdamThe Netherlands
- Department of Child‐ and Adolescent PsychiatryAmsterdam University Medical CentersAmsterdamThe Netherlands
| | - Brenda R. J. Jansen
- Department of PsychologyUniversity of AmsterdamAmsterdamThe Netherlands
- Amsterdam Brain and Cognition CenterAmsterdamThe Netherlands
- Research Priority Area YieldAmsterdamThe Netherlands
| | - Hilde M. Huizenga
- Department of PsychologyUniversity of AmsterdamAmsterdamThe Netherlands
- Amsterdam Brain and Cognition CenterAmsterdamThe Netherlands
- Research Priority Area YieldAmsterdamThe Netherlands
| | - Anna C. K. van Duijvenvoorde
- Institute of Psychology, Department of Developmental and Educational PsychologyLeiden UniversityLeidenThe Netherlands
- Leiden Institute for Brain and CognitionLeidenThe Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Rehfeldt RA, Tyndall I. Why We Are Not Acting to Save Ourselves: ACT, Health, and Culture. Behav Anal Pract 2022; 15:55-70. [PMID: 34306541 PMCID: PMC8280594 DOI: 10.1007/s40617-021-00592-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 04/03/2021] [Indexed: 12/02/2022] Open
Abstract
Chronic health conditions are increasing at an alarming rate worldwide, and many could be prevented if people were to engage in specific lifestyle behaviors. Intervening on lifestyle behaviors is challenging due to the fact that the consequences associated with unhealthy behaviors are temporally distant and probabilistic, and the aversive functions of covert stimuli may interfere with people's engagement in healthy, preventative behaviors. This article explores the role of relational framing in the promotion of healthy lifestyle behaviors and summarizes research supporting the use of acceptance and commitment training (ACT) as a framework for prevention and intervention. We explore how ACT alters the context in which rigid patterns of rule following occur. ACT loosens the literal functions of stimuli so that experiential-avoidance behaviors are weakened, and healthy, values-consistent behaviors are strengthened. We propose culture-wide interventions inspired by contextual behavior science so that healthier societies can be cultivated.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ruth Anne Rehfeldt
- The Chicago School of Professional Psychology, Chicago, 325 N. Wells St, Chicago, IL 60654 USA
| | - Ian Tyndall
- Department of Psychology, University of Chichester, Chichester, West Sussex UK
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Oh H, DeVylder JE, Koyanagi A. Psychotic experiences as a health indicator: A provisional framework. Int J Soc Psychiatry 2022; 68:244-252. [PMID: 33554709 DOI: 10.1177/0020764021992809] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Hans Oh
- Suzanne Dworak Peck School of Social Work, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
| | - Jordan E DeVylder
- Graduate School of Social Service, Fordham University, New York, NY, USA
| | - Ai Koyanagi
- Parc Sanitari Sant Joan de Deu, Universitat de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain.,Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM)
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Medrano RL, Thomas GC, Rouse EJ. Can humans perceive the metabolic benefit provided by augmentative exoskeletons? J Neuroeng Rehabil 2022; 19:26. [PMID: 35219335 PMCID: PMC8881941 DOI: 10.1186/s12984-022-01002-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/06/2021] [Accepted: 02/15/2022] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND The purpose of augmentative exoskeletons is to help people exceed the limitations of their human bodies, but this cannot be realized unless people choose to use these exciting technologies. Although human walking efficiency has been highly optimized over generations, exoskeletons have been able to consistently improve this efficiency by 10-15%. However, despite these measurable improvements, exoskeletons today remain confined to the laboratory. To achieve widespread adoption, exoskeletons must not only exceed the efficiency of human walking, but also provide a perceivable benefit to their wearers. METHODS In this study, we quantify the perceptual threshold of the metabolic efficiency benefit provided during exoskeleton-assisted locomotion. Ten participants wore bilateral ankle exoskeletons during continuous walking. The assistance provided by the exoskeletons was varied in 2 min intervals while participants provided feedback on their metabolic rate. These data were aggregated and used to estimate the perceptual threshold. RESULTS Participants were able to detect a change in their metabolic rate of 22.7% (SD: 17.0%) with 75% accuracy. This indicates that in the short term and on average, wearers cannot yet reliably perceive the metabolic benefits of today's augmentative exoskeletons. CONCLUSIONS If wearers cannot perceive the benefits provided by these technologies, it will negatively affect their impact, including long-term adoption and product viability. Future exoskeleton researchers and designers can use these methods and results to inform the development of exoskeletons that reach their potential.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Roberto Leo Medrano
- Department of Mechanical Engineering, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, 48109 USA
- Robotics Institute, University of Michigan, 48109 Ann Arbor, USA
| | - Gray Cortright Thomas
- Department of Mechanical Engineering, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, 48109 USA
- Department of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, 48109 USA
- Robotics Institute, University of Michigan, 48109 Ann Arbor, USA
| | - Elliott J. Rouse
- Department of Mechanical Engineering, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, 48109 USA
- Robotics Institute, University of Michigan, 48109 Ann Arbor, USA
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Adams MA, Todd M, Angadi SS, Hurley JC, Stecher C, Berardi V, Phillips CB, McEntee ML, Hovell MF, Hooker SP. Adaptive Goals and Reinforcement Timing to Increase Physical Activity in Adults: A Factorial Randomized Trial. Am J Prev Med 2022; 62:e57-e68. [PMID: 35000693 PMCID: PMC8820277 DOI: 10.1016/j.amepre.2021.09.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/30/2020] [Revised: 08/24/2021] [Accepted: 09/26/2021] [Indexed: 02/03/2023]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Potent lifestyle interventions to increase moderate-to-vigorous physical activity are urgently needed for population-level chronic disease prevention. This trial tested the independent and joint effects of a mobile health system automating adaptive goal setting and immediate financial reinforcement for increasing daily walking among insufficiently active adults. STUDY DESIGN Participants were randomized into a 2 (adaptive versus static goal setting) X 2 (immediate versus delayed financial incentive timing) condition factorial trial to increase walking. SETTINGS/PARTICIPANTS Participants (N=512 adults) were recruited between 2016 and 2018 and were 64.5% female, aged 18-60 years, 18.8% Hispanic, 6.1% African American, and 83% White. INTERVENTION Principles of reinforcement and behavioral economics directed intervention design. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES Participants wore accelerometers daily (133,876 day-level observations) that remotely measured moderate-to-vigorous physical activity bout minutes of ≥3 minutes/day for 1 year. Primary outcomes were between-condition differences in (1) engaging ≥1 bout of moderate-to-vigorous physical activity on each day and (2) on days with ≥1 bout, daily total moderate-to-vigorous physical activity minutes. RESULTS Mixed-effects hurdle models tested treatment group X phase (time) interactions using an intent-to-treat approach in 2021. Engaging in any ambulatory moderate-to-vigorous physical activity was greater for Adaptive than for Static Goal groups (OR=2.34, 95% CI=2.10, 2.60 vs OR=1.66, 95% CI=1.50, 1.84; p<0.001) and for Immediate than for Static Reinforcement groups (OR=2.16 95% CI=1.94, 2.40 vs OR=1.77, 95% CI=1.59, 1.97; p<0.01). The Immediate Reinforcement group increased by 16.54 moderate-to-vigorous physical activity minutes/day, whereas the Delayed Reinforcement group increased by 9.91 minutes/day (p<0.001). The combined Adaptive Goals + Immediate Reinforcement group increased by 16.52 moderate-to-vigorous physical activity minutes/day, significantly more than that of either Delayed Reinforcement group. CONCLUSIONS This study offers automated and scalable-behavior change strategies for increasing walking among adults most at-risk for chronic diseases attributed to sedentary lifestyles. TRIAL REGISTRATION This study is registered at www.clinicaltrials.gov (ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT02717663).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marc A Adams
- College of Health Solutions, Arizona State University, Phoenix, Arizona.
| | - Michael Todd
- Edson College of Nursing and Health Innovation, Arizona State University, Phoenix, Arizona
| | - Siddhartha S Angadi
- College of Health Solutions, Arizona State University, Phoenix, Arizona; Department of Kinesiology, School of Education and Human Development, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, Virginia
| | - Jane C Hurley
- College of Health Solutions, Arizona State University, Phoenix, Arizona
| | - Chad Stecher
- College of Health Solutions, Arizona State University, Phoenix, Arizona
| | - Vincent Berardi
- Department of Psychology, Crean College of Health and Behavioral Sciences, Chapman University, Orange, California
| | | | - Mindy L McEntee
- College of Health Solutions, Arizona State University, Phoenix, Arizona
| | - Melbourne F Hovell
- School of Public Health, San Diego State University, San Diego, California
| | - Steven P Hooker
- College of Health and Human Services, San Diego State University, San Diego, California
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Broulidakis MJ, Golm D, Cortese S, Fairchild G, Sonuga-Barke E. ssDefault mode network connectivity and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adolescence: Associations with delay aversion and temporal discounting, but not mind wandering. Int J Psychophysiol 2022; 173:38-44. [PMID: 35032471 DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2022.01.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/17/2021] [Revised: 11/27/2021] [Accepted: 01/09/2022] [Indexed: 12/21/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) has been associated with reduced resting state connectivity in the core subsystem of the default mode network (DMN; medial prefrontal cortex - posterior cingulate cortex). However, the neuropsychological consequences of this hypoconnectivity remain to be determined. Building on recent theoretical models of DMN function, we tested the association between DMN hypo-connectivity and three neuropsychological processes previously implicated in ADHD: (i) excessive task-unrelated spontaneous thought (i.e., mind-wandering); (ii) sub-optimal decision-making due to exaggerated temporal discounting; and (iii) delay aversion - a heightened emotional response to the imposition or experience of delay. METHODS Twenty male adolescents with a clinical diagnosis of ADHD and 18 typically developing adolescents (all aged 11-16 years) underwent a resting-state fMRI scan to assess DMN connectivity. An experimental paradigm was used to assess temporal discounting and self-report questionnaires were used to measure mind wandering and delay aversion. RESULTS ADHD was significantly associated with DMN hypo-connectivity specifically in the core subsystem, elevated levels of mind-wandering, delay aversion, and temporal discounting. Mediation analysis suggested that DMN hypoconnectivity mediated the link between ADHD and delay aversion. CONCLUSION The results provide initial evidence that disturbances in the DMN may impair ability to regulate delay-related negative affect in adolescents with ADHD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M John Broulidakis
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Southampton, United States of America; Department of psychology, College of Science, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, United States of America
| | - Dennis Golm
- Department of Psychology, University of Southampton, United States of America
| | - Samuele Cortese
- Department of Psychology, University of Southampton, United States of America; Solent NHS Trust, Southampton, United States of America; Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Hassenfeld Children's Hospital at NYU Langone, New York, United States of America; Division of Psychiatry and Applied Psychology, School of Medicine, University of Nottingham, UK
| | | | - Edmund Sonuga-Barke
- Department of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, UK; Department of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, Aarhus University, Denmark.
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Palomo-Vélez G, van Vugt M. The evolutionary psychology of climate change behaviors: Insights and applications. Curr Opin Psychol 2021; 42:54-59. [PMID: 33915462 DOI: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.03.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/29/2020] [Accepted: 03/20/2021] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
We examine climate-related activities through an evolutionary psychology lens, zooming in on factors that motivate or discourage people to behave sustainably to mitigate climate change. Complementing current knowledge, we discuss five core ancestral psychological motivations that shape people's environmental decisions in fundamental ways. We review recent studies that explore how evolved psychological mechanisms related to self-interest, status, sensing, discounting tendencies, and social imitation can be used to promote proenvironmental behavior. We discuss the potential strengths and limitations of evolutionary-based behavioral interventions and briefly reflect on outstanding research questions that can further the integration of evolutionary approaches into mainstream environmental psychology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gonzalo Palomo-Vélez
- Expertise Group on Environmental Psychology, Rijksuniversiteit Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands.
| | - Mark van Vugt
- Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, the Netherlands; Department of Politics and International Relations, University of Oxford, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Shawler LA, Blair BJ. A Proposed Functional Analysis of Transmission Prevention Behaviors for a Respiratory Virus (SARS-CoV-2). BEHAVIOR AND SOCIAL ISSUES 2021; 30:666-691. [PMID: 38624752 PMCID: PMC8412977 DOI: 10.1007/s42822-021-00055-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 03/24/2021] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
The coronavirus (SARS-CoV-2) pandemic has had a global impact on lives everywhere and has led to the disruption of, and interference with, virtually every aspect of life. In response, leading experts, political leaders, doctors, and scientists have released guidelines that attempt to prevent and mitigate the exponential rate of infection. The response to these safety recommendations has produced tremendous behavior variability as a society. Although a plethora of factors are likely relevant, a more complete analysis of human behavior during this time might help explain this disparity. The principles of applied behavior analysis allow for a functional analysis of an individual's use of transmission prevention behaviors (TPBs) during the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic. Thus, the purpose of this discussion is to provide a conceptual analysis for some possible explanations for why individuals might or might not engage in virus TPBs, as well as some recommendations to help combat the current pandemic, as well as those in the future.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lesley A. Shawler
- Department of Behavioral Psychology, Kennedy Krieger Institute and Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, 9810 Patuxent Woods Dr., Ste. C., Columbia, MD 21046 USA
| | - Bryan J. Blair
- School of Education, Long Island University, Orangeburg, NY USA
| |
Collapse
|
35
|
Brassard SL, Balodis IM. A review of effort-based decision-making in eating and weight disorders. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2021; 110:110333. [PMID: 33905755 DOI: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2021.110333] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/01/2020] [Revised: 03/19/2021] [Accepted: 04/21/2021] [Indexed: 12/26/2022]
Abstract
Effort-based decision-making provides a framework to understand the mental computations estimating the amount of work ("effort") required to obtain a reward. The aim of the current review is to systematically synthesize the available literature on effort-based decision-making across the spectrum of eating and weight disorders. More specifically, the current review summarises the literature examining whether 1) individuals with eating disorders and overweight/obesity are willing to expend more effort for rewards compared to healthy controls, 2) if particular components of effort-based decision-making (i.e. risk, discounting) relate to specific binge eating conditions, and 3) how individual differences in effort and reward -processing measures relate to eating pathology and treatment measures. A total of 96 studies were included in our review, following PRISMA guidelines. The review suggests that individuals with binge eating behaviours 1) are more likely to expend greater effort for food rewards, but not monetary rewards, 2) demonstrate greater decision-making impairments under risk and uncertainty, 3) prefer sooner rather than delayed rewards for both food and money, and 4) demonstrate increased implicit 'wanting' for high fat sweet foods. Finally, individual differences in effort and reward -processing measures relating to eating pathology and treatment measures are also discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah L Brassard
- Department of Neuroscience, McMaster University, Canada; Peter Boris Center for Addictions Research, St. Joseph's Healthcare Hamilton, Canada
| | - Iris M Balodis
- Department of Neuroscience, McMaster University, Canada; Peter Boris Center for Addictions Research, St. Joseph's Healthcare Hamilton, Canada; Department of Psychiatry and Behavioural Neuroscience, McMaster University, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Malesza M. Relationship between emotion regulation, negative affect, gender and delay discounting. CURRENT PSYCHOLOGY 2021. [DOI: 10.1007/s12144-019-00366-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
37
|
Ilagan GS, Heatherington L. Advancing the understanding of factors that influence client preferences for race and gender matching in psychotherapy. COUNSELLING PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY 2021. [DOI: 10.1080/09515070.2021.1960274] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
|
38
|
Wong CA, Hakimi S, Santanam TS, Madanay F, Fridman I, Ford C, Patel M, Ubel PA. Applying Behavioral Economics to Improve Adolescent and Young Adult Health: A Developmentally-Sensitive Approach. J Adolesc Health 2021; 69:17-25. [PMID: 33288458 PMCID: PMC8175460 DOI: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2020.10.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/29/2020] [Revised: 09/25/2020] [Accepted: 10/05/2020] [Indexed: 12/28/2022]
Abstract
Each day, adolescents and young adults (AYAs) choose to engage in behaviors that impact their current and future health. Behavioral economics represents an innovative lens through which to explore decision-making among AYAs. Behavioral economics outlines a diverse set of phenomena that influence decision-making and can be leveraged to develop interventions that may support behavior change. Up to this point, behavioral economic interventions have predominantly been studied in adults. This article provides an integrative review of how behavioral economic phenomena can be leveraged to motivate health-related behavior change among AYAs. We contextualize these phenomena in the physical and social environments unique to AYAs and the neurodevelopmental changes they undergo, highlighting opportunities to intervene in AYA-specific contexts. Our review of the literature suggests behavioral economic phenomena leveraging social choice are particularly promising for AYA health. Behavioral economic interventions that take advantage of AYA learning and development have the potential to positively impact youth health and well-being over the lifespan.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Charlene A Wong
- Division of Primary Care, Department of Pediatrics, Duke University School of Medicine, Durham, North Carolina; Duke-Robert J. Margolis, MD, Center for Health Policy, Durham, North Carolina; Duke Clinical Research Institute, Durham, North Carolina; Duke Sanford School of Public Policy, Durham, North Carolina
| | - Shabnam Hakimi
- Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University, Levine Science Research Center, Durham, North Carolina.
| | - Taruni S Santanam
- Duke-Robert J. Margolis, MD, Center for Health Policy, Durham, North Carolina
| | - Farrah Madanay
- Duke Sanford School of Public Policy, Durham, North Carolina
| | - Ilona Fridman
- Duke-Robert J. Margolis, MD, Center for Health Policy, Durham, North Carolina
| | - Carol Ford
- Division of Adolescent Medicine, Department of Pediatrics, University of Pennsylvania Perelman School of Medicine and the Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania
| | - Mitesh Patel
- Perelman Center for Advanced Medicine, University of Pennsylvania Perelman School of Medicine, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania
| | - Peter A Ubel
- Duke Sanford School of Public Policy, Durham, North Carolina; Fuqua School of Business, Durham North Carolina
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
Leurcharusmee S, Bokati L, Kosheleva O. Why linear expressions in discounting and in empathy: a symmetry-based explanation. Soft comput 2021. [DOI: 10.1007/s00500-020-05153-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
40
|
Improving Beverage Choice in Adults with Developmental Disabilities: Implementation of a Token Reinforcement System in a Community Residential Setting. J Autism Dev Disord 2021; 52:1523-1535. [PMID: 33961181 DOI: 10.1007/s10803-021-05051-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 04/25/2021] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
Individuals with developmental disabilities (DD) are twice as likely to have obesity than non-disabled individuals. Replacing the consumption of sugar sweetened beverages (SSBs) with water has many health benefits, including weight reduction. In this study, a token reinforcement system was implemented to increase water consumption and decrease the consumption of SSBs with 14 adult participants with DD living in a community-based independent supported living (ISL) center. Token reinforcement reduced the consumption of SSBs, with associated reductions in calorie consumption and body weight. Findings are especially important for treatment settings where resources for individualized meal planning and staffing to support comprehensive behavioral interventions may be limited.
Collapse
|
41
|
Scheres A, Solanto MV. Do ADHD Symptoms, Executive Function, and Study Strategies Predict Temporal Reward Discounting in College Students with Varying Levels of ADHD Symptoms? A Pilot Study. Brain Sci 2021; 11:brainsci11020181. [PMID: 33540665 PMCID: PMC7912943 DOI: 10.3390/brainsci11020181] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/20/2020] [Revised: 01/20/2021] [Accepted: 01/27/2021] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
The purpose of this study was to examine the relationship between temporal reward discounting and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms in college students. Additionally, we examined whether temporal reward discounting was associated with executive functioning in daily life and with learning and study strategies in this group. Thirty-nine college students (19 with ADHD and 20 controls) participated after meeting criteria for ADHD or non-ADHD based on standardized assessment. Strong preferences for small immediate rewards were specifically associated with the ADHD symptom domain hyperactivity–impulsivity. Additionally, these preferences were associated with daily life executive function problems and with weak learning and study strategies. This suggests that steep temporal discounting may be a key mechanism playing a role in the daily life challenges that college students with ADHD symptoms face. If these findings are replicated in larger samples, then intervention strategies may profitably be developed to counteract this strong preference for small immediate rewards in college students with ADHD symptoms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anouk Scheres
- Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University, 6525 GA Nijmegen, The Netherlands
- Correspondence:
| | - Mary V. Solanto
- Zucker School of Medicine at Hofstra-Northwell, New York, NY 11042, USA;
| |
Collapse
|
42
|
de Cola B, Dallery J. Delay discounting rate by a surrogate decision maker depends on the smoking status of the recipient. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2021; 29:73-81. [PMID: 32105134 PMCID: PMC7483137 DOI: 10.1037/pha0000356] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The tendency to devalue future rewards is known as delay discounting. Discounting is measured using a series of intertemporal choices between smaller, sooner outcomes and larger, later outcomes. We used a surrogate delay discounting task to explore whether such choices would differ if a hypothetical recipient was a smoker or was an individual with good health habits. Across three studies, the descriptions of the recipient included only information about smoking status (n = 66), smoking status and equal annual income (n = 47), and smoking status and equal weekly expenditures (n = 42). Higher rates of delay discounting for the smoker recipient compared to the nonsmoker recipient were observed across all three studies. These results parallel previous findings showing group differences in discounting between actual smokers and nonsmokers. We discuss the similarities between the present results and previous studies in light of an extension of Bem's (1967) self-perception theory, which posits that choices in laboratory-based delay discounting tasks are informed by observation of real-world intertemporal choice. The theory asserts that there is no fundamental difference between a first-person account of such knowledge and a third-person account. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
Collapse
|
43
|
Robayo-Pinzon O, Foxall GR, Montoya-Restrepo LA, Rojas-Berrio S. Does excessive use of smartphones and apps make us more impulsive? An approach from behavioural economics. Heliyon 2021; 7:e06104. [PMID: 33644439 PMCID: PMC7887400 DOI: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2021.e06104] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/01/2020] [Revised: 11/24/2020] [Accepted: 01/22/2021] [Indexed: 12/19/2022] Open
Abstract
PURPOSE Problematic smartphone use has been associated with negative effects in work and school environments. This study proposes the application of a behavioural economics perspective to establish whether heavy smartphone users show a tendency to devalue the consequences of their behaviour in the long term. To address this proposition, the study sought to establish how an objective measurement of usage time of smartphones and apps might help to predict, firstly, participants' choice behaviour and, secondly, their perceived dependence levels. DESIGN/METHODOLOGY/APPROACH An objective measurement of the usage time of smartphones and apps was conducted over four weeks (N = 560 data points), and a computer-based intertemporal choice task and the Spanish version of the Smartphone Addiction Inventory (SPAI) were applied. The participants were twenty undergraduate college students. FINDINGS Although the usage time of devices and apps failed to predict the choice behaviour, a correlation was found between the total usage time of smartphones and WhatsApp and Facebook apps and users' dependence level. On the other hand, dependence had a positive effect on the average selection of the impulsive choice. ORIGINALITY/VALUE This paper proposes the application of a behavioural economics perspective to explore the relationship between objectively measured usage time of smartphone and apps, choice behaviours in an intertemporal task and users' perceived dependence levels. This allows us to consider an alternative to the traditional psychiatric approach in an environment of increasing access to and use of mobile digital platforms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Oscar Robayo-Pinzon
- School of Marketing and Branding, Institucion Universitaria Politecnico Grancolombiano, Bogota, Colombia
- School of Business Administration, Universidad del Rosario, Bogota, Colombia
| | - Gordon R. Foxall
- Cardiff Business School, Cardiff University, Cardiff CF10 3EU, UK
- School of Management, Reykjavik University, Reykjavik, Iceland
| | | | - Sandra Rojas-Berrio
- Department of Economics, Universidad Nacional de Colombia, Branch of Bogota, Carrera 45 No. 26-85 Edificio 311, Cuarto Piso Oficina 10B Bogota, Colombia
| |
Collapse
|
44
|
Mobekk H, Hessen DO, Fagerstrøm A, Jacobsen H. For Your Eyes Only: A Field Experiment on Nudging Hygienic Behavior. Front Psychol 2020; 11:603440. [PMID: 33343474 PMCID: PMC7746819 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.603440] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/06/2020] [Accepted: 11/17/2020] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
These days many gyms and fitness centers are closed to reduce transmission of the SARS-CoV-2 virus in society. The gym is an environment rich in microorganisms, and careful hygiene is a necessity to keep infections at bay. Exercise centers strive for better hygiene compliance among their members. This effort has become essential in light of the current pandemic. Several experimental studies show that others’ physical presence, or the “illusion” of being watched, may alter behavior. This article reports on a natural field experiment testing one specific social nudge intended to increase gym members’ hygienic behavior. The study was conducted before the SARS-COV-2 pandemic. A picture of “observing eyes” was attached to paper dispensers and cleanser spray bottles at two different gyms in Norway. A reversal design, also called an ABA design, with and without the nudge’s presence, was used to investigate the impact on gym members’ hygienic behavior. A follow-up study was conducted in one of the centers to investigate whether the nudge stimuli would function over time. The study included 254 individual choice situations during nine observation sessions conducted over 9 weeks. The results from both centers provide evidence of a strong effect of the nudge. However, the effect decreased during the follow-up study. These findings support previous research indicating that human behavior is influenced by the presence of implicit observation cues – in this case – observing eyes. However, insights into the long-term effect of implicit observation cues are still needed since the salience of the stimuli faded over time.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hilde Mobekk
- Department of Behavioural Sciences, OsloMet - Oslo Metropolitan University, Oslo, Norway
| | | | - Asle Fagerstrøm
- Department of Technology, Kristiania University College, Oslo, Norway
| | - Hanne Jacobsen
- Department of Behavioural Sciences, OsloMet - Oslo Metropolitan University, Oslo, Norway
| |
Collapse
|
45
|
Couto KC, Moura Lorenzo F, Tagliabue M, Henriques MB, Freitas Lemos R. Underlying Principles of a Covid-19 Behavioral Vaccine for a Sustainable Cultural Change. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF ENVIRONMENTAL RESEARCH AND PUBLIC HEALTH 2020; 17:E9066. [PMID: 33291718 PMCID: PMC7729613 DOI: 10.3390/ijerph17239066] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/22/2020] [Revised: 11/28/2020] [Accepted: 11/30/2020] [Indexed: 12/22/2022]
Abstract
Until pharmacological measures are effective at containing the COVID-19 outbreak, adopting protective behaviors is paramount. In this work, we aim at informing interventions to limit the spread of the contagion and prepare against any future outbreaks by developing a behavioral framework to interpret and prescribe both the individual and large-scale uptake of non-pharmaceutical measures. First, we analyze the barriers and facilitators to adherence to protective behaviors according to a three-term contingency by exploring potential gaps in terms of setting stimuli, motivating operations, delayed consequences, and positive or negative consequences. We explore their roles in the likelihood of individual compliance to protective behaviors, taking physical distancing as an example of functional analysis. Second, we interpret contagion control as the cumulative effect of large-scale adherence to protective behaviors. We explore the interrelations between societal problems caused or amplified by similar behaviors presented by many individuals and the coordination of agents or agencies aiming at promoting large-scale behavioral change. Then, we highlight the potential of developing a behavioral vaccine, and practical steps for applying it to promote sustainable cultural change that may protect against health, social, and economic losses in future outbreaks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kalliu Carvalho Couto
- Department of Behavioural Sciences, Faculty of Health Sciences, OsloMet—Oslo Metropolitan University, 0167 Oslo, Norway;
| | - Flora Moura Lorenzo
- Department of Basic Psychological Processes, Psychology Institute, Campus Universitário Darcy Ribeiro, University of Brasília, Brasília 70910-900, Brazil;
| | - Marco Tagliabue
- Department of Behavioural Sciences, Faculty of Health Sciences, OsloMet—Oslo Metropolitan University, 0167 Oslo, Norway;
| | | | - Roberta Freitas Lemos
- Fralin Biomedical Research Institute at Virginia Tech Carilion, Roanoke, VA 24016, USA;
| |
Collapse
|
46
|
Toplak ME, Flora DB. Resistance to cognitive biases: Longitudinal trajectories and associations with cognitive abilities and academic achievement across development. JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DECISION MAKING 2020. [DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2214] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Maggie E. Toplak
- Department of Psychology, LaMarsh Centre for Child and Youth Research York University Toronto Ontario Canada
| | - David B. Flora
- Department of Psychology York University Toronto Ontario Canada
| |
Collapse
|
47
|
Biglan A, Johansson M, Van Ryzin M, Embry D. Scaling up and scaling out: Consilience and the evolution of more nurturing societies. Clin Psychol Rev 2020; 81:101893. [PMID: 32858377 PMCID: PMC7403031 DOI: 10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101893] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/23/2019] [Revised: 07/24/2020] [Accepted: 07/29/2020] [Indexed: 01/13/2023]
Abstract
This paper argues that diverse disciplines within the human sciences have converged in identifying the conditions that human beings need to thrive and the programs, policies, and practices that are needed to foster well-being. In the interest of promoting this view, we suggest that this convergence might usefully be labeled "The Nurture Consilience." We review evidence from evolutionary biology, developmental, clinical, and social psychology, as well as public health and prevention science indicating that, for evolutionary reasons, coercive environments promote a "fast" life strategy that favors limited self-regulation, immediate gratification, and early childbearing. However, this trajectory can be prevented through programs, practices, and policies that (a) minimize toxic social and biological conditions, (b) limit opportunities and influences for problem behavior, (c) richly reinforce prosocial behavior, and (d) promote psychological flexibility. The recognition of these facts has prompted research on the adoption, implementation, and maintenance of evidence-based interventions. To fully realize the fruits of this consilience, it is necessary to reform every sector of society. We review evidence that free-market advocacy has promoted the view that if individuals simply pursue their own economic well-being it will benefit everyone, and trace how that view led business, health care, education, criminal justice, and government to adopt practices that have benefited a small segment of the population but harmed the majority. We argue that the first step in reforming each sector of society would be to promote the value of ensuring everyone's well-being. The second step will be to create contingencies that select beneficial practices and minimizes harmful ones.
Collapse
|
48
|
Blair RJR, Bashford-Largo J, Zhang R, Lukoff J, Elowsky JS, Leibenluft E, Hwang S, Dobbertin M, Blair KS. Temporal Discounting Impulsivity and Its Association with Conduct Disorder and Irritability. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol 2020; 30:542-548. [PMID: 32882144 PMCID: PMC7699000 DOI: 10.1089/cap.2020.0001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/20/2023]
Abstract
Objectives: Temporal reward discounting impulsivity (TDI) reflects a propensity to choose smaller immediate rather than larger delayed rewards relative to age/IQ-matched peers. Previous work with adults has linked TDI to an increased risk for antisocial behavior but also psychopathology in general. However, little work has examined TDI in adolescents with conduct disorder (CD), or considered whether TDI might be associated dimensionally with traits associated with antisocial behavior, that is, impulsivity, irritability, and/or callous-unemotional traits. In this study TDI was investigated in a large adolescent group with varying levels of antisocial behavior. Methods: Participants consisted of 195 adolescents (67 with CD, 77 in a psychiatric comparison group and 51 typically developing adolescents). Participants performed a temporal discounting task and individual differences were measured through the Connors rating scale for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (impulsivity), Affective Reactivity Index (irritability), and Inventory of Callous-Unemotional traits. Results: The adolescents with CD and those in the psychiatric comparison group showed significantly greater TDI than typically developing adolescents. However, these group differences were abolished when dimensional covariates were included. Irritability was significantly associated with TDI. Conclusions: We conclude that TDI reflects a transdiagnostic form of dysfunction that particularly manifests in adolescents with increased irritability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- R. James R. Blair
- Center for Neurobehavioral Research, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, Nebraska, USA.,Address correspondence to: R. James R. Blair, PhD, Center for Neurobehavioral Research, Boys Town National Research Hospital, 14100 Crawford Street, Boys Town, NE 68154, USA
| | - Johannah Bashford-Largo
- Center for Neurobehavioral Research, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, Nebraska, USA
| | - Ru Zhang
- Center for Neurobehavioral Research, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, Nebraska, USA
| | - Jennie Lukoff
- Center for Neurobehavioral Research, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, Nebraska, USA
| | - Jamie S. Elowsky
- Center for Neurobehavioral Research, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, Nebraska, USA
| | - Ellen Leibenluft
- Emotion and Development Branch, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland, USA
| | - Soonjo Hwang
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, Nebraska, USA
| | - Matthew Dobbertin
- Center for Neurobehavioral Research, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, Nebraska, USA
| | - Karina S. Blair
- Center for Neurobehavioral Research, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, Nebraska, USA
| |
Collapse
|
49
|
Is the Value of Future Rewards Independent of Gains and Losses Concurrent with Intertemporal Choices? PSYCHOLOGICAL RECORD 2020. [DOI: 10.1007/s40732-020-00424-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
50
|
Contreras-Huerta LS, Pisauro MA, Apps MAJ. Effort shapes social cognition and behaviour: A neuro-cognitive framework. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2020; 118:426-439. [PMID: 32818580 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.08.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/15/2020] [Revised: 07/13/2020] [Accepted: 08/08/2020] [Indexed: 01/10/2023]
Abstract
Theoretical accounts typically posit that variability in social behaviour is a function of capacity limits. We argue that many social behaviours are goal-directed and effortful, and thus variability is not just a function of capacity, but also motivation. Leveraging recent work examining the cognitive, computational and neural basis of effort processing, we put forward a framework for motivated social cognition. We argue that social cognition is demanding, people avoid its effort costs, and a core-circuit of brain areas that guides effort-based decisions in non-social situations may similarly evaluate whether social behaviours are worth the effort. Thus, effort sensitivity dissociates capacity limits from social motivation, and may be a driver of individual differences and pathological impairments in social cognition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Luis Sebastian Contreras-Huerta
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, OX2 6GG, UK; Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, University of Oxford, UK.
| | - M Andrea Pisauro
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, OX2 6GG, UK; Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, University of Oxford, UK; Centre for Human Brain Health, School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, UK.
| | - Matthew A J Apps
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, OX2 6GG, UK; Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, University of Oxford, UK; Centre for Human Brain Health, School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, UK; Christ Church College, University of Oxford, UK.
| |
Collapse
|