51
|
Zuelsdorff M, Okonkwo OC, Norton D, Barnes LL, Graham KL, Clark LR, Wyman MF, Benton SF, Gee A, Lambrou N, Johnson SC, Gleason CE. Stressful Life Events and Racial Disparities in Cognition Among Middle-Aged and Older Adults. J Alzheimers Dis 2020; 73:671-682. [PMID: 31815690 PMCID: PMC7481054 DOI: 10.3233/jad-190439] [Citation(s) in RCA: 70] [Impact Index Per Article: 14.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND It is well-documented that African Americans have elevated risk for cognitive impairment and dementia in late life, but reasons for the racial disparities remain unknown. Stress processes have been linked to premature age-related morbidity, including Alzheimer's and related dementias (ADRD), and plausibly contribute to social disparities in cognitive aging. OBJECTIVE We examined the relationship between stressful life events and cognitive decline among African American and White participants enrolled in the Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer's Prevention (WRAP). METHODS Linear mixed models including demographic, literacy, and health-related covariates were used to estimate (1) relationships between a life event index score and decline in cognitive test performance in two domains of executive function (Speed & Flexibility, Working Memory) and one domain of episodic memory (Verbal Learning & Memory) among 1,241 WRAP enrollees, stratified by race, and (2) contributions of stressful life events to racial differences in cognition within the full sample. RESULTS African Americans (N = 50) reported more stressful life events than Whites (N = 1,191). Higher stress scores associated with poorer Speed & Flexibility performance in both groups, though not with declines across time, and partially explained racial differentials in this domain. Among African Americans only, stressor exposure also associated with age-related decline in Verbal Learning & Memory. Stressor-cognition relationships were independent of literacy and health-related variables. CONCLUSIONS Greater lifetime stress predicted poorer later-life cognition, and, in a small sample of African Americans, faster declines in a key domain of episodic memory. These preliminary findings suggest that future work in large minority aging cohorts should explore stress as an important source of modifiable, socially-rooted risk for impairment and ADRD in African Americans, who are disproportionately exposed to adverse experiences across the life course.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Megan Zuelsdorff
- School of Nursing, University of Wisconsin - Madison, Madison, WI, USA.,Wisconsin Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Madison, WI, USA
| | - Ozioma C Okonkwo
- Wisconsin Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Madison, WI, USA.,Geriatric Research, Education, and Clinical Center, William S. Middleton Memorial Veterans Hospital, Madison, WI, USA
| | - Derek Norton
- Department of Biostatistics and Medical Informatics, University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI, USA
| | - Lisa L Barnes
- Rush Alzheimer's Disease Center, Chicago, IL, USA.,Department of Neurological Sciences, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Karen L Graham
- Rush Alzheimer's Disease Center, Chicago, IL, USA.,Rush University College of Nursing, Chicago IL, USA
| | - Lindsay R Clark
- Wisconsin Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Madison, WI, USA.,Geriatric Research, Education, and Clinical Center, William S. Middleton Memorial Veterans Hospital, Madison, WI, USA
| | - Mary F Wyman
- Wisconsin Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Madison, WI, USA.,Geriatric Research, Education, and Clinical Center, William S. Middleton Memorial Veterans Hospital, Madison, WI, USA
| | - Susan F Benton
- Wisconsin Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Madison, WI, USA.,Department of Family Medicine, University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, Madison, WI, USA
| | - Alexander Gee
- Nehemiah Center for Urban Leadership Development, Madison, WI, USA
| | - Nickolas Lambrou
- Wisconsin Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Madison, WI, USA.,Geriatric Research, Education, and Clinical Center, William S. Middleton Memorial Veterans Hospital, Madison, WI, USA
| | - Sterling C Johnson
- Wisconsin Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Madison, WI, USA.,Geriatric Research, Education, and Clinical Center, William S. Middleton Memorial Veterans Hospital, Madison, WI, USA.,Wisconsin Alzheimer's Institute, University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, Madison, WI, USA
| | - Carey E Gleason
- Wisconsin Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Madison, WI, USA.,Geriatric Research, Education, and Clinical Center, William S. Middleton Memorial Veterans Hospital, Madison, WI, USA
| |
Collapse
|
52
|
Colombo B, Piromalli G, Pins B, Taylor C, Fabio RA. The relationship between cognitive reserve and personality traits: a pilot study on a healthy aging Italian sample. Aging Clin Exp Res 2020; 32:2031-2040. [PMID: 31650502 DOI: 10.1007/s40520-019-01386-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/11/2019] [Accepted: 10/11/2019] [Indexed: 01/12/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Exploring the role of different individual factors in affecting the cognitive reserve levels is crucial step for aging research. Several studies explored the relationship between personality traits and aging, but a specific focus on the cognitive reserve is missing. AIMS This study aimed at collecting more direct evidence about possible relationships between cognitive reserve and personality traits. METHODS A sample of 100 healthy aging participants was involved in the study. They completed the Big Five personality inventory and a test to assess the cognitive reserve. RESULTS Results returned a positive relationship between the personality traits and participants' cognitive reserve. The only factor that did not return a significant correlation was Emotional stability (which overlaps with Neuroticism). DISCUSSION This study provides additional evidence to the existing literature and also adds relevant information and a critical reading regarding the role of personality traits that has been neglected in the aging literature, Friendliness and Conscientiousness. CONCLUSION The ability to measure and identify personality traits could be important in future research for developing interventions or activities that could target specific personality characteristics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Barbara Colombo
- Neuroscience Lab, Champlain College, 163 South Willard Street, 05401, Burlington, VT, USA.
| | - Giulia Piromalli
- Department of Clinical and Medical Medicine, University of Messina, Piazza Pugliatti 1, 98122, Messina, Italy
| | - Brittany Pins
- Neuroscience Lab, Champlain College, 163 South Willard Street, 05401, Burlington, VT, USA
| | - Catherine Taylor
- Neuroscience Lab, Champlain College, 163 South Willard Street, 05401, Burlington, VT, USA
| | - Rosa Angela Fabio
- Department of Clinical and Medical Medicine, University of Messina, Piazza Pugliatti 1, 98122, Messina, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
53
|
Torres JM, Sofrygin O, Rudolph KE, Haan MN, Wong R, Glymour MM. US Migration Status of Adult Children and Cognitive Decline Among Older Parents Who Remain in Mexico. Am J Epidemiol 2020; 189:761-769. [PMID: 31942611 DOI: 10.1093/aje/kwz277] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/20/2019] [Revised: 10/11/2019] [Accepted: 11/07/2019] [Indexed: 12/31/2022] Open
Abstract
Low- and middle-income countries (LMICs) are experiencing rapid aging, a growing dementia burden, and relatively high rates of out-migration among working-age adults. Family member migration status may be a unique societal determinant of cognitive aging in LMIC settings. We aimed to evaluate the association between adult child US migration status and change in cognitive performance scores using data from the Mexican Health and Aging Study, a population-based, national-level cohort study of Mexico adults aged ≥50 years at baseline (2001), with 2-, 12-, and 14-year follow-up waves (2003, 2012, and 2015). Cognitive performance assessments were completed by 5,972 and 4,939 respondents at 11 years and 14 years of follow-up, respectively. For women, having an adult child in the United States was associated with steeper decline in verbal memory scores (e.g., for 9-year change in immediate verbal recall z score, marginal risk difference (RD) = -0.09 (95% confidence interval (CI): -0.16, -0.03); for delayed verbal recall z score, RD = -0.10 (95% CI: -0.17, -0.03)) and overall cognitive performance (for overall cognitive performance z score, RD = -0.04, 95% CI: -0.07, -0.00). There were mostly null associations for men. To our knowledge, this is the first study to have evaluated the association between family member migration status and cognitive decline; future work should be extended to other LMICs facing population aging.
Collapse
|
54
|
Ying G, Vonk JMJ, Sol K, Brickman AM, Manly JJ, Zahodne LB. Family Ties and Aging in a Multiethnic Cohort. J Aging Health 2020; 32:1464-1474. [PMID: 32600080 DOI: 10.1177/0898264320935238] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/08/2023]
Abstract
Objective: Lack of social support is linked to lower cognitive function and cognitive decline. We investigated the effects of a specific type of social relationship, family ties, on cognitive trajectories in non-Hispanic whites, non-Hispanic blacks, and Hispanics. Methods: Using multiple-group latent growth curve models, we analyzed associations between the number of children/grandchildren/siblings/other relatives contacted within the past month at baseline and cognitive trajectories in 1420 older adults who were nondemented at baseline. Language, memory, and visuospatial abilities were assessed at baseline and at 18- and 24-month follow-ups for up to six visits. Inferential analyses assessed the differential effects of sex/gender and race/ethnicity for each family tie. Results: Independent of all covariates, contact with more relatives was associated with better initial memory (b = .01) and language functioning (b = .01) across race/ethnicity and sex/gender. Conclusion: The size of peripheral rather than immediate family networks may be more likely to affect cognitive function in older adults.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gelan Ying
- 171553Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer's Disease and the Aging Brain, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| | - Jet M J Vonk
- 171553Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer's Disease and the Aging Brain, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA.,168086Julius Center for Health Sciences and Primary Care, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, the Netherlands
| | | | - Adam M Brickman
- 171553Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer's Disease and the Aging Brain, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| | - Jennifer J Manly
- 171553Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer's Disease and the Aging Brain, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
55
|
Han SD, Barnes LL, Leurgans S, Yu L, Bennett DA, Boyle PA. Literacy Mediates Racial Differences in Financial and Healthcare Decision Making in Older Adults. J Am Geriatr Soc 2020; 68:1279-1285. [PMID: 32092157 PMCID: PMC8324307 DOI: 10.1111/jgs.16381] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/08/2019] [Revised: 01/21/2020] [Accepted: 01/27/2020] [Indexed: 01/30/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND/OBJECTIVES Decision making in financial and healthcare matters is of critical importance for well-being in old age. Preliminary work suggests racial differences in decision making; however, the factors that drive racial differences in decision making remain unclear. We hypothesized literacy, particularly financial and health literacy, mediates racial differences in decision making. DESIGN Community-based epidemiologic cohort study. SETTING Communities in northeastern Illinois. PARTICIPANTS Nondemented Black participants (N = 138) of the Rush Alzheimer's Disease Center Minority Aging Research Study and the Rush Memory and Aging Project who completed decision-making and literacy measures were matched to White participants (N = 138) according to age, education, sex, and global cognition using Mahalanobis distance (total N = 276). MEASUREMENTS All participants completed clinical assessments, a decision-making measure that resembles real-world materials relevant to finance and healthcare, and a financial and health literacy measure. Regression models were used to examine racial differences in decision making and test the hypothesis that literacy mediates this association. In secondary analyses, we examined the impact of literacy in specific domains of decision making (financial and healthcare). RESULTS In models adjusted for age, education, sex, and global cognition, older Black adults performed lower than older White adults on literacy (β = -8.20; SE = 1.34; 95% CI = -10.82 to -5.57; P < .01) and separately on decision making (β = -.80; SE = .23; 95% CI = -1.25 to -.34; P < .01). However, when decision making was regressed on both race and literacy, the association of race was attenuated and became nonsignificant (β = -.45; SE = .24; 95% CI = -.93 to .02; P = .06), but literacy remained significantly associated with decision making (β = .04; SE = .01; 95% CI = .02-.06; P < .01). In secondary models, a similar pattern was observed for both financial and healthcare decision making. CONCLUSIONS Racial differences in decision making are largely mediated by literacy. These findings suggest that efforts to improve literacy may help reduce racial differences in decision making and improve health and well-being for diverse populations. J Am Geriatr Soc 68:1279-1285, 2020.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S Duke Han
- Department of Family Medicine, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California
- Department of Neurology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California
- Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California
- School of Gerontology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California
- Department of Behavioral Sciences and Psychiatry, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois
- Rush Alzheimer's Disease Center, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois
- Department of Neurological Sciences, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois
| | - Lisa L Barnes
- Department of Behavioral Sciences and Psychiatry, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois
- Rush Alzheimer's Disease Center, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois
- Department of Neurological Sciences, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois
| | - Sue Leurgans
- Rush Alzheimer's Disease Center, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois
- Department of Neurological Sciences, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois
| | - Lei Yu
- Rush Alzheimer's Disease Center, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois
- Department of Neurological Sciences, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois
| | - David A Bennett
- Rush Alzheimer's Disease Center, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois
- Department of Neurological Sciences, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois
| | - Patricia A Boyle
- Department of Behavioral Sciences and Psychiatry, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois
- Rush Alzheimer's Disease Center, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois
| |
Collapse
|
56
|
Seblova D, Berggren R, Lövdén M. Education and age-related decline in cognitive performance: Systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies. Ageing Res Rev 2020; 58:101005. [PMID: 31881366 DOI: 10.1016/j.arr.2019.101005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 109] [Impact Index Per Article: 21.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/09/2019] [Revised: 12/18/2019] [Accepted: 12/23/2019] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
Abstract
Central theories of cognitive aging propose that education is an important protective factor for decline in cognitive performance in older age. We conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis of reported estimates of an association between educational attainment and change in performance in six cognitive domains (episodic memory, processing speed, verbal fluency, crystallized intelligence, fluid intelligence, and global ability) in the general population of older individuals. The systematic search (11th of October 2019) identified 92 eligible articles. The episodic memory domain had the highest number of estimates (37 estimates from 18 articles, n = 109,281) included in the meta-analysis. The fewest estimates (6 estimates from 6 articles, n = 5263) were included for fluid intelligence. Pooled mean estimates from an inverse-variance weighted random effects analysis were not statistically significant and indicated that any association between education and change in cognitive performance is likely of a negligible magnitude. The estimates for education's role (one additional year) for change in cognitive performance ranged from -0.019 (95 % confidence interval, CI = -0.047, 0.010) to 0.004SD (CI = -0.003, 0.012) per decade. Even if the larger positive point estimates (i.e., protective effects) are selectively considered, the influence of education on change is still at least 12 times less important for the cognitive functioning of an older individual than the association between education and level of cognitive performance. Sensitivity analyses did not substantially alter these results. However, heterogeneity was substantial, and remained largely unexplained by mean age, mean educational attainment, Gini coefficient, GDP per capita, maximum follow-up period, and publication year. Overall, education is an important factor in aging due to its robust association with level of performance, but the current base of empirical evidence is not revealing a consistent and substantial association between educational attainment and changes in cognitive performance in the general population. Theories of cognitive aging must be updated to incorporate this pattern of findings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D Seblova
- Aging Research Center, Karolinska Institutet and Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden; Department of Public Health Sciences, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden; Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer's Disease and the Aging Brain, The Gertrude H. Sergievsky Center, Department of Neurology, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA.
| | - R Berggren
- Aging Research Center, Karolinska Institutet and Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden
| | - M Lövdén
- Aging Research Center, Karolinska Institutet and Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
57
|
Garcia MA, Ortiz K, Arévalo SP, Diminich ED, Briceño E, Vega IE, Tarraf W. Age of Migration and Cognitive Function Among Older Latinos in the United States. J Alzheimers Dis 2020; 76:1493-1511. [PMID: 32651313 PMCID: PMC8061235 DOI: 10.3233/jad-191296] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Age of migration has been shown to have a robust association with Latino immigrant health outcomes; however, the relationship between timing of migration and cognition is less understood. OBJECTIVE To examine associations between race/ethnicity, nativity, age of migration, and cognitive aging among US-born (USB) non-Latino Whites (NLW) and USB and foreign-born Latinos 50 years and older. METHODS We used longitudinal biennial data from the Health and Retirement Study (HRS; 2006-2014) to fit generalized linear and linear latent growth curve models for: 1) global cognition (Modified Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status; TICS-M); 2) memory and attention subdomains of TICS-M; and 3) cognitive dysfunction. We also tested for sex modifications. RESULTS In age and sex adjusted models, all Latino subgroups, independent of nativity and age of migration, had lower global and domain-specific cognitive scores and higher propensity of cognitive impairment classification compared to USB-NLWs. Differences between USB Latinos, but not other Latino subgroups, and USB-NLWs remained after full covariate adjustment. Latinas, independent of nativity or age of migration, had poorer cognitive scores relative to NLW females. Differences between all Latinos and USB-NLWs were principally expressed at baseline. Racial/ethnic, nativity, and age of migration grouping was not associated with slope (nor explained variance) of cognitive decline. CONCLUSION Older US-born Latinos, regardless of sex exhibit poorer cognitive function than older USB-NLWs and foreign-born Latinos. Social determinants that differentially affect cognitive function, particularly those that compensate for education and sex differences among US-born Latinos and foreign-born Latinos, require further exploration.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marc A. Garcia
- Department of Sociology and Institute of Ethnic Studies, University of Nebraska, Lincoln, NE, USA
| | - Kasim Ortiz
- Department of Sociology and Criminology, University of New Mexico, Institute for the Study of “Race” and Social Justice, Center for Participatory Research, Albuquerque, NM, USA
| | - Sandra P. Arévalo
- Department of Human Development, California State University, Long Beach, Long Beach, CA, USA
| | - Erica D. Diminich
- Program in Public Health, Department of Family, Population and Preventive Medicine, Stony Brook, NY, USA
| | - Emily Briceño
- Department of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, University of Michigan, School of Medicine, Ann Arbor, MI, USA
| | - Irving E. Vega
- Department of Translational Neuroscience, Michigan State University, College of Human Medicine, East Lansing, MI, USA
| | - Wassim Tarraf
- Institute of Gerontology and Department of Healthcare Sciences, Wayne State University, MI, USA
| |
Collapse
|
58
|
Beydoun MA, Hossain S, Beydoun HA, Shaked D, Weiss J, Evans MK, Zonderman AB. Red Cell Distribution Width Is Directly Associated with Poor Cognitive Performance among Nonanemic, Middle-Aged, Urban Adults. J Nutr 2020; 150:128-139. [PMID: 31912144 PMCID: PMC6946901 DOI: 10.1093/jn/nxz182] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/28/2019] [Revised: 05/08/2019] [Accepted: 07/15/2019] [Indexed: 12/15/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Epidemiological evidence suggests that both anemia and elevated red cell distribution width (RDW) are associated with cognitive impairment. However, the interplay between these 2 predictors has been understudied. OBJECTIVES We examined sex- and anemia-specific associations between RDW and cognitive performance among urban adults in the United States. METHODS Data from the Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity Across the Life Span Study (Baltimore, MD; participants aged 30-65 y at baseline, ∼59% African-American, 45% men) were used. Participants were selected based on the completion of 11 cognitive tasks at baseline (2004-2009) and follow-up (2009-2013) visits (mean time between visits: 4.64 ± 0.93 y) and availability of exposure and covariate data, yielding a sample of between 1526 and 1646 adults out of the initial 3720 adults recruited at baseline. Multiple linear mixed-effects regression models were conducted with RDW as the main exposure of interest and anemia/sex as the key effect modifiers. RESULTS Overall, high RDWs were linked to poorer baseline performance on the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) List A (per 1 unit increase in RDW %, main effect: γ01 = -0.369 ± 0.114; P = 0.001) and to slower rates of decline on the CVLT Delayed Free Recall (per 1 unit increase in RDW %, RDW × time: γ11 = +0.036 ± 0.013; P = 0.007). Among nonanemic participants, RDWs were consistently associated with poorer baseline performance on the Trailmaking Test, Part A (γ01 = +3.11 ± 0.89; P < 0.001) and on the CVLT List A (γ01 = -0.560 ± 0.158; P < 0.001). Moreover, RDWs were associated with poorer baseline performance on the Brief Test of Attention in the total population (γ01 = -0.123 ± 0.039; P = 0.001) and among men (γ01 = -0.221 ± 0.068; P = 0.001). We did not detect an association between hemoglobin (Hb) and baseline cognitive performance or changes over time. CONCLUSIONS Elevated RDW had a consistent cross-sectional association with poor cognitive performance in the domains of verbal memory and attention among the nonanemic group in a sample of middle-aged, urban adults. Anemia and Hb concentrations were not associated with cognition. More longitudinal studies are needed to replicate our findings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- May A Beydoun
- Laboratory of Epidemiology and Population Sciences, National Institute on Aging, NIH, Intramural Research Program, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Sharmin Hossain
- Laboratory of Epidemiology and Population Sciences, National Institute on Aging, NIH, Intramural Research Program, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Hind A Beydoun
- Department of Research Programs, Fort Belvoir Community Hospital, Fort Belvoir, VA, USA
| | - Danielle Shaked
- Laboratory of Epidemiology and Population Sciences, National Institute on Aging, NIH, Intramural Research Program, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Jordan Weiss
- Population Studies Center and Leonard Davis Institute of Health Economics, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
| | - Michele K Evans
- Laboratory of Epidemiology and Population Sciences, National Institute on Aging, NIH, Intramural Research Program, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Alan B Zonderman
- Laboratory of Epidemiology and Population Sciences, National Institute on Aging, NIH, Intramural Research Program, Baltimore, MD, USA
| |
Collapse
|
59
|
Amariglio RE, Buckley RF, Rabin JS, Papp KV, Quiroz YT, Mormino EC, Sparks KP, Johnson KA, Rentz DM, Sperling RA. Examining Cognitive Decline Across Black and White Participants in the Harvard Aging Brain Study. J Alzheimers Dis 2020; 75:1437-1446. [PMID: 32417775 PMCID: PMC7397774 DOI: 10.3233/jad-191291] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/30/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Black Americans are approximately twice as likely to develop dementia as compared to White Americans and the magnitude of this disparity is often attributed to a variety of factors that include psychosocial and vascular risk factors. However, less is known about the potential contribution of Alzheimer's disease pathological differences. OBJECTIVE To examine potential differences incross-sectional and longitudinal cognitive performance in black and white participants who were clinically normal at baseline. METHODS 296 participants (48 African-American/black participants) underwent MRI and amyloid PET at baseline. Linear mixed models were used to examine the main effects of race, years of education, reading ability, Framingham Heart Study cardiovascular risk score (FHS-CVD), white matter hyperintensities (WMH), and amyloid (Aβ) burden on the Preclinical Alzheimer Cognitive Composite-5 (PACC5). RESULTS Lower levels of educationalattainment and reading ability were found for blacks compared to whites. By contrast, no differences in FHS-CVD, WMH, or Aβ were found by racial group. Baseline differences in PACC5 score were attenuated after adjusting for educationalfactors, vascular factors, and Aβ, but remained lower for blacks compared to whites (β= -0.24, p = 0.014). Further, blacks demonstrated a faster rate of PACC5 decline longitudinally compared to whites (β = -0.055, p = 0.025) after adjusting for covariates. CONCLUSION Accounting for educationalfactors, vascular factors, and Aβ burden diminished, but did not eliminate, racial differences in PACC5 performance longitudinally. Understanding potential differences in longitudinal cognitive outcomes by race may be important for upcoming secondary prevention trials.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rebecca E. Amariglio
- Center for Alzheimer Research and Treatment, Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02115
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02114
| | - Rachel F. Buckley
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02114
- Florey Institute, University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
- Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
| | - Jennifer S. Rabin
- Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02114
| | - Kathryn V. Papp
- Center for Alzheimer Research and Treatment, Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02115
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02114
| | - Yakeel T. Quiroz
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02114
- Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02114
| | - Elizabeth C. Mormino
- Department of Neurology and Neurological Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Palo Alto, CA, USA
| | - Kathryn P. Sparks
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02114
| | - Keith A. Johnson
- Center for Alzheimer Research and Treatment, Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02115
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02114
- Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02114
| | - Dorene M. Rentz
- Center for Alzheimer Research and Treatment, Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02115
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02114
| | - Reisa A. Sperling
- Center for Alzheimer Research and Treatment, Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02115
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02114
| |
Collapse
|
60
|
Forrester SN, Gallo JJ, Whitfield KE, Thorpe RJ. A Framework of Minority Stress: From Physiological Manifestations to Cognitive Outcomes. THE GERONTOLOGIST 2019; 59:1017-1023. [PMID: 30169640 PMCID: PMC6858824 DOI: 10.1093/geront/gny104] [Citation(s) in RCA: 73] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/25/2018] [Indexed: 12/31/2022] Open
Abstract
Cognitive impairment and dementia continue to threaten the aging population. Although no one is immune, certain groups, namely black older persons, are more likely to have a diagnosis of certain dementias. Because researchers have not found a purely biological reason for this disparity, they have turned to a biopsychosocial model. Specifically, black persons in the United States are more likely to live with social conditions that affect their stress levels which in turn affect physiological regulation leading to conditions that result in higher levels of cognitive impairment or dementia. Here we discuss some of these social conditions such as discrimination, education, and socioeconomic status, and how physiological dysregulation, namely allostatic load that can lead to cognitive impairment and dementia in black persons especially.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah N Forrester
- Quantitative Health Sciences, University of Massachusetts Medical School, Worcester
| | - Joseph J Gallo
- Department of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, Maryland
| | | | - Roland J Thorpe
- Departmnet of Health, Behavior, and Society, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, Maryland
| |
Collapse
|
61
|
Abstract
The concept of cognitive reserve (CR) was proposed to account for the discrepancy between levels of brain pathologic process or damage and clinical and cognitive function. We provide a detailed review of prospective longitudinal studies that have investigated the interaction between CR and Alzheimer disease (AD) biomarkers on clinical and cognitive outcomes among individuals with normal cognition at baseline. Current evidence is consistent with the view that higher levels of CR are associated with a delay in the onset of symptoms of mild cognitive impairment and that there may be multiple pathways by which CR exerts its protective effects.
Collapse
|
62
|
Arce Rentería M, Vonk JMJ, Felix G, Avila JF, Zahodne LB, Dalchand E, Frazer KM, Martinez MN, Shouel HL, Manly JJ. Illiteracy, dementia risk, and cognitive trajectories among older adults with low education. Neurology 2019; 93:e2247-e2256. [PMID: 31722961 DOI: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000008587] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/01/2018] [Accepted: 06/26/2019] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE To investigate whether illiteracy was associated with greater risk of prevalent and incident dementia and more rapid cognitive decline among older adults with low education. METHODS Analyses included 983 adults (≥65 years old, ≤4 years of schooling) who participated in a longitudinal community aging study. Literacy was self-reported ("Did you ever learn to read or write?"). Neuropsychological measures of memory, language, and visuospatial abilities were administered at baseline and at follow-ups (median [range] 3.49 years [0-23]). At each visit, functional, cognitive, and medical data were reviewed and a dementia diagnosis was made using standard criteria. Logistic regression and Cox proportional hazards models evaluated the association of literacy with prevalent and incident dementia, respectively, while latent growth curve models evaluated the effect of literacy on cognitive trajectories, adjusting for relevant demographic and medical covariates. RESULTS Illiterate participants were almost 3 times as likely to have dementia at baseline compared to literate participants. Among those who did not have dementia at baseline, illiterate participants were twice as likely to develop dementia. While illiterate participants showed worse memory, language, and visuospatial functioning at baseline than literate participants, literacy was not associated with rate of cognitive decline. CONCLUSION We found that illiteracy was independently associated with higher risk of prevalent and incident dementia, but not with a more rapid rate of cognitive decline. The independent effect of illiteracy on dementia risk may be through a lower range of cognitive function, which is closer to diagnostic thresholds for dementia than the range of literate participants.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Miguel Arce Rentería
- From the Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer's Disease and the Aging Brain (M.A.R., J.M.J.V., G.F., E.D., K.M.F., M.N.M., H.L.S., J.J.M.), Department of Neurology, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons, New York, NY; Center for Health Policy (J.F.A.), University of New Mexico, Albuquerque; and Department of Psychology (L.B.Z.), University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
| | - Jet M J Vonk
- From the Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer's Disease and the Aging Brain (M.A.R., J.M.J.V., G.F., E.D., K.M.F., M.N.M., H.L.S., J.J.M.), Department of Neurology, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons, New York, NY; Center for Health Policy (J.F.A.), University of New Mexico, Albuquerque; and Department of Psychology (L.B.Z.), University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
| | - Gloria Felix
- From the Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer's Disease and the Aging Brain (M.A.R., J.M.J.V., G.F., E.D., K.M.F., M.N.M., H.L.S., J.J.M.), Department of Neurology, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons, New York, NY; Center for Health Policy (J.F.A.), University of New Mexico, Albuquerque; and Department of Psychology (L.B.Z.), University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
| | - Justina F Avila
- From the Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer's Disease and the Aging Brain (M.A.R., J.M.J.V., G.F., E.D., K.M.F., M.N.M., H.L.S., J.J.M.), Department of Neurology, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons, New York, NY; Center for Health Policy (J.F.A.), University of New Mexico, Albuquerque; and Department of Psychology (L.B.Z.), University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
| | - Laura B Zahodne
- From the Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer's Disease and the Aging Brain (M.A.R., J.M.J.V., G.F., E.D., K.M.F., M.N.M., H.L.S., J.J.M.), Department of Neurology, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons, New York, NY; Center for Health Policy (J.F.A.), University of New Mexico, Albuquerque; and Department of Psychology (L.B.Z.), University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
| | - Elizabeth Dalchand
- From the Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer's Disease and the Aging Brain (M.A.R., J.M.J.V., G.F., E.D., K.M.F., M.N.M., H.L.S., J.J.M.), Department of Neurology, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons, New York, NY; Center for Health Policy (J.F.A.), University of New Mexico, Albuquerque; and Department of Psychology (L.B.Z.), University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
| | - Kirsten M Frazer
- From the Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer's Disease and the Aging Brain (M.A.R., J.M.J.V., G.F., E.D., K.M.F., M.N.M., H.L.S., J.J.M.), Department of Neurology, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons, New York, NY; Center for Health Policy (J.F.A.), University of New Mexico, Albuquerque; and Department of Psychology (L.B.Z.), University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
| | - Michelle N Martinez
- From the Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer's Disease and the Aging Brain (M.A.R., J.M.J.V., G.F., E.D., K.M.F., M.N.M., H.L.S., J.J.M.), Department of Neurology, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons, New York, NY; Center for Health Policy (J.F.A.), University of New Mexico, Albuquerque; and Department of Psychology (L.B.Z.), University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
| | - Heather L Shouel
- From the Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer's Disease and the Aging Brain (M.A.R., J.M.J.V., G.F., E.D., K.M.F., M.N.M., H.L.S., J.J.M.), Department of Neurology, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons, New York, NY; Center for Health Policy (J.F.A.), University of New Mexico, Albuquerque; and Department of Psychology (L.B.Z.), University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
| | - Jennifer J Manly
- From the Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer's Disease and the Aging Brain (M.A.R., J.M.J.V., G.F., E.D., K.M.F., M.N.M., H.L.S., J.J.M.), Department of Neurology, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons, New York, NY; Center for Health Policy (J.F.A.), University of New Mexico, Albuquerque; and Department of Psychology (L.B.Z.), University of Michigan, Ann Arbor. jjm71@
| |
Collapse
|
63
|
Chandler MC, McGowan AL, Payne BR, Hampton Wray A, Pontifex MB. Aerobic fitness relates to differential attentional but not language-related cognitive processes. BRAIN AND LANGUAGE 2019; 198:104681. [PMID: 31514088 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandl.2019.104681] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/10/2019] [Revised: 08/07/2019] [Accepted: 08/22/2019] [Indexed: 06/10/2023]
Abstract
Compelling evidence supports an association between the attribute of aerobic fitness and achievement scores on standardized tests of reading. However, such standardized assessments provide only a broad valuation of a complex network of language related sub-processes that contribute to reading and are heavily confounded by other attention-related processes. The present investigation sought to clarify the nature of the association between aerobic fitness and language processing in a sample of college-aged adults. Participants were bifurcated based on aerobic fitness level and on a separate day were asked to complete a lexical decision task while neuroelectric activity was recorded. Analysis of word-level language-related ERP components revealed no fitness differences. However, lower aerobically-fit individuals elicited smaller amplitude for attention-related ERP components relative to the higher aerobically-fit group. These data provide initial evidence to suggest that fitness-related differences in reading achievement may result from attentional processes rather than acting upon specific language-related processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Madison C Chandler
- Department of Kinesiology, Michigan State University, 308 W. Circle Drive, East Lansing, MI 48824, United States.
| | - Amanda L McGowan
- Department of Kinesiology, Michigan State University, 308 W. Circle Drive, East Lansing, MI 48824, United States.
| | - Brennan R Payne
- Department of Psychology, University of Utah, 380 S 1530 E Beh S 502, Salt Lake City, UT 84112, United States.
| | - Amanda Hampton Wray
- Department of Communication Science & Disorders, University of Pittsburgh, 6035 Forbes Tower, Pittsburgh, PA 15260, United States.
| | - Matthew B Pontifex
- Department of Kinesiology, Michigan State University, 308 W. Circle Drive, East Lansing, MI 48824, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
64
|
Carmichael O, Newton R. Brain MRI findings related to Alzheimer's disease in older African American adults. PROGRESS IN MOLECULAR BIOLOGY AND TRANSLATIONAL SCIENCE 2019; 165:3-23. [PMID: 31481168 DOI: 10.1016/bs.pmbts.2019.04.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/06/2023]
Abstract
Although a substantial body of research has identified brain MRI measures as important markers of Alzheimer's disease (AD) risk, progression, and treatment response, most of that research has been performed in non-Hispanic white American populations, leading to questions about the utility of the brain MRI measures among individuals of other races or ethnicities. African American individuals in particular are under-represented in AD research, and may exhibit differences in prevalence of AD risk factors, prevalence of AD, incidence of AD, the clinical course of cognitive decline, and AD neuropathology, each of which could influence the utility of brain MRI markers. Unfortunately, while current evidence suggests that African Americans exhibit poorer brain health late in life based on brain MRI measurements, several other aspects of brain MRI markers in this population are unclear, including trajectories of brain MRI markers leading up to old age, relationships between traditional brain health risk factors and brain MRI findings, and the status of brain MRI markers as correlates of cognitive impairment. This unclear state of affairs highlights the urgency of future research in which large numbers of older African American adults contribute longitudinal brain MRI measurements concurrent with clinical, cognitive, and molecular biomarker measurements, ideally in the context of AD preventive or therapeutic trials.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Owen Carmichael
- Pennington Biomedical Research Center, Baton Rouge, LA, United States.
| | - Robert Newton
- Pennington Biomedical Research Center, Baton Rouge, LA, United States
| |
Collapse
|
65
|
Medina LD, Sadler M, Yeh M, Filoteo JV, Woods SP, Gilbert PE. Collectivism Is Associated With Greater Neurocognitive Fluency in Older Adults. Front Hum Neurosci 2019; 13:122. [PMID: 31031611 PMCID: PMC6470262 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00122] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/30/2018] [Accepted: 03/22/2019] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Neuropsychological research has been limited in the representation of cultural diversity due to various issues, raising questions regarding the applicability of findings to diverse populations. Nonetheless, culture-dependent differences in fundamental psychological processes have been demonstrated. One of the most basic of these, self-construal (individualism, collectivism), is central to how many other differences are interpreted. Self-construals may have possible consequences on social interactions, emotions, motivation, and cognition. This study aimed to evaluate the impact of self-construal on neurocognitive functions in older adults. A total of 86 community-dwelling older adults 60 years and older were assessed with three common self-report measures of self-construal along individualism and collectivism (IC). A cognitive battery was administered to assess verbal and non-verbal fluency abilities. Latent profile analysis (LPA) was used to categorize individuals according to IC, and one-way analyses of covariance (ANCOVA), including relevant covariates (e.g., ethnicity, gender, linguistic abilities), were used to compare neurocognitive functions between individualists and collectivists. Collectivists outperformed individualists on left frontally-mediated measures of verbal fluency (action, phonemic) after controlling for relevant covariates, F(1,77) = 6.942, p = 0.010, η2 = 0.061. Groups did not differ on semantic fluency, non-verbal fluency, or attention/working memory (all ps > 0.05). These findings suggest a cognitive advantage in collectivists for verbal processing speed with an additional contribution of left frontal processes involved in lexicosemantic retrieval. Self-construal may provide a meaningful descriptor for diverse samples in neuropsychological research and may help explain other cross-cultural differences.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Luis D Medina
- Department of Psychology, University of Houston, Houston, TX, United States
| | - Melody Sadler
- Department of Psychology, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA, United States
| | - May Yeh
- Department of Psychology, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA, United States.,Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, San Diego CA, United States
| | - J Vincent Filoteo
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, San Diego CA, United States
| | - Steven Paul Woods
- Department of Psychology, University of Houston, Houston, TX, United States
| | - Paul E Gilbert
- Department of Psychology, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA, United States
| |
Collapse
|
66
|
Wright RS, Waldstein SR, Gerassimakis CS, Sprung MR, Moody DLB, Taylor AD, Al'Najjar E, McNeely JM, Zhang Z, Evans MK, Zonderman AB. Multiple Influences on Cognitive Function Among Urban-Dwelling African Americans. J Racial Ethn Health Disparities 2019; 6:851-860. [PMID: 30915683 DOI: 10.1007/s40615-019-00584-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/10/2019] [Revised: 03/14/2019] [Accepted: 03/18/2019] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
This study examined multiple influences on cognitive function among African Americans, including education, literacy, poverty status, substance use, depressive symptoms, and cardiovascular disease (CVD) risk factors. Baseline data were analyzed from the Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS) study. Participants were 987 African Americans (mean age 48.5 years, SD = 9.17) who completed cognitive measures assessing verbal learning and memory, nonverbal memory, working memory, verbal fluency, perceptuo-motor speed, attention, and cognitive flexibility. Using preplanned hierarchical regression, cognitive performance was regressed on the following: (1) age, sex, education, poverty status; (2) literacy; (3) cigarette smoking, illicit substance use; (4) depressive symptoms; and (5) number of CVD risk factors. Results indicated that literacy eliminated the influence of education and poverty status in select instances, but added predictive utility in others. In fully adjusted models, results showed that literacy was the most important influence on cognitive performance across all cognitive domains (p < .001); however, education and poverty status were related to attention and cognitive flexibility. Depressive symptoms and substance use were significant predictors of multiple cognitive outcomes, and CVD risk factors were not associated with cognitive performance. Overall, findings underscore the need to develop cognitive supports for individuals with low literacy, educational attainment, and income, and the importance of treating depressive symptoms and thoroughly examining the role of substance use in this population.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- R S Wright
- School of Nursing, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, USA.
| | - S R Waldstein
- Department of Psychology, University of Maryland Baltimore County, Baltimore, MD, USA.,Geriatric Research Education & Clinical Center, Baltimore Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - C S Gerassimakis
- Department of Behavioral Health and Nutrition, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, USA
| | - M R Sprung
- Department of Psychology, University of Maryland Baltimore County, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - D L Beatty Moody
- Department of Psychology, University of Maryland Baltimore County, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - A D Taylor
- Department of Psychology, University of Maryland Baltimore County, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - E Al'Najjar
- Department of Psychology, University of Maryland Baltimore County, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - J M McNeely
- Department of Psychology, University of Maryland Baltimore County, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Z Zhang
- Christiana Care Value Institute, Newark, DE, USA
| | - M K Evans
- Laboratory of Epidemiology and Population Sciences, National Institute on Aging Intramural Research Program, National Institutes of Health, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - A B Zonderman
- Laboratory of Epidemiology and Population Sciences, National Institute on Aging Intramural Research Program, National Institutes of Health, Baltimore, MD, USA
| |
Collapse
|
67
|
Cognitive Reserve Protects Against Memory Decrements Associated With Neuropathology in Traumatic Brain Injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil 2019; 34:E57-E65. [PMID: 30829821 DOI: 10.1097/htr.0000000000000472] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE To evaluate whether cognitive reserve (CR) moderates the relationship between neuropathology and cognitive outcomes after traumatic brain injury (TBI). SETTING Outpatient research organization. PARTICIPANTS Patients with complicated mild (n = 8), moderate (n = 9), and severe (n = 44) TBI. DESIGN Prospective, cross-sectional study. MAIN MEASURES Cognitive reserve was estimated using a test of word reading (Wechsler Test of Adult Reading). Diffusion tensor imaging (functional anisotropy) was used to quantify neuropathology. Neuropsychological test scores were submitted to principal components analyses to create cognitive composites for memory, attention, executive function, and processing speed domains. RESULTS At lower levels of neuropathology, people with higher CR exhibited better memory than those with lower CR. This benefit diminished as neuropathology increased and disappeared at the highest levels of neuropathology. Cognitive reserve ceased exerting a protective effect at premorbid intelligence levels below average. CONCLUSION Cognitive reserve may differentially protect some cognitive domains against neuropathology relative to others. A clinical cutoff below which CR is no longer protective, together with a possible neuropathology ceiling effect, may be instructive for prognostication and clinical decision-making in cognitive rehabilitation.
Collapse
|
68
|
Tometich DB, Small BJ, Carroll JE, Zhai W, Luta G, Zhou X, Kobayashi LC, Ahles T, Saykin AJ, Clapp JD, Jim HSL, Jacobsen PB, Hurria A, Graham D, McDonald BC, Denduluri N, Extermann M, Isaacs C, Dilawari A, Root J, Rini C, Mandelblatt JS. Pretreatment Psychoneurological Symptoms and Their Association With Longitudinal Cognitive Function and Quality of Life in Older Breast Cancer Survivors. J Pain Symptom Manage 2019; 57:596-606. [PMID: 30472317 PMCID: PMC6382533 DOI: 10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2018.11.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/24/2018] [Revised: 11/12/2018] [Accepted: 11/14/2018] [Indexed: 12/22/2022]
Abstract
CONTEXT Symptoms affect quality of life (QOL), functional status, and cognitive function in cancer survivors, but older survivors are understudied. OBJECTIVES The objectives of this study were to identify prototypical presystemic therapy psychoneurological symptom clusters among older breast cancer survivors and determine whether these symptom clusters predicted cognition and QOL over time. METHODS Women with newly diagnosed nonmetastatic breast cancer (n = 319) and matched noncancer controls (n = 347) aged 60+ years completed questionnaires and neuropsychological tests before systemic therapy and 12 and 24 months later. Latent class analysis identified clusters of survivors based on their pretherapy depression, anxiety, fatigue, sleep disturbance, and pain. Linear mixed-effects models examined changes in objective cognition, perceived cognition, and functional status (Instrumental Activities of Daily Living disability, functional well-being, and breast cancer-specific QOL) by group, controlling for covariates. RESULTS Nearly one-fifth of older survivors were classified as having high pretherapy symptoms (n = 51; 16%); the remainder had low symptoms (n = 268; 84%); both groups improved over time on all outcomes. However, compared to the low symptom group and controls, survivors with high symptoms had lower baseline objective cognition and lower perceived cognition at baseline and 24 months, lower functional well-being at baseline and 12 months, greater Instrumental Activities of Daily Living disability at baseline, and lower breast cancer-specific QOL at all time points (all P < 0.05). CONCLUSION Nearly one-fifth of older breast cancer survivors had high psychoneurological symptoms at diagnosis, which predicted clinically meaningful decrements in perceived cognition and function in the first 24 months after diagnosis. Pretreatment psychoneurological symptom clusters could identify survivors for monitoring or intervention.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Danielle B Tometich
- Indiana University-Purdue University Indianapolis, Indianapolis, Indiana, USA.
| | - Brent J Small
- Moffitt Cancer Center, Tampa, Florida, USA; University of South Florida, Tampa, Florida, USA
| | - Judith E Carroll
- University of California Los Angeles and Jonsson Comprehensive Cancer Center, Los Angeles, California, USA
| | - Wanting Zhai
- Georgetown University and Lombardi Comprehensive Cancer Center, Washington, District of Columbia, USA
| | - George Luta
- Georgetown University and Lombardi Comprehensive Cancer Center, Washington, District of Columbia, USA
| | - Xingtao Zhou
- Georgetown University and Lombardi Comprehensive Cancer Center, Washington, District of Columbia, USA
| | - Lindsay C Kobayashi
- Georgetown University and Lombardi Comprehensive Cancer Center, Washington, District of Columbia, USA
| | - Tim Ahles
- Memorial Sloan Kettering Cancer Center, New York City, New York, USA
| | - Andrew J Saykin
- Indiana University School of Medicine and Indiana University Melvin and Bren Simon Cancer Center, Indianapolis, Indiana, USA
| | - Jonathan D Clapp
- University of South Florida, Tampa, Florida, USA; Georgetown University and Lombardi Comprehensive Cancer Center, Washington, District of Columbia, USA
| | | | | | - Arti Hurria
- City of Hope Comprehensive Cancer Center, Duarte, California, USA
| | - Deena Graham
- John Theurer Cancer Center, Hackensack University Medical Center, Hackensack, New Jersey, USA
| | - Brenna C McDonald
- Indiana University School of Medicine and Indiana University Melvin and Bren Simon Cancer Center, Indianapolis, Indiana, USA
| | | | | | - Claudine Isaacs
- Georgetown University and Lombardi Comprehensive Cancer Center, Washington, District of Columbia, USA
| | - Asma Dilawari
- Georgetown University and Lombardi Comprehensive Cancer Center, Washington, District of Columbia, USA
| | - James Root
- Memorial Sloan Kettering Cancer Center, New York City, New York, USA
| | - Christine Rini
- Georgetown University and Lombardi Comprehensive Cancer Center, Washington, District of Columbia, USA; John Theurer Cancer Center, Hackensack University Medical Center, Hackensack, New Jersey, USA
| | - Jeanne S Mandelblatt
- Georgetown University and Lombardi Comprehensive Cancer Center, Washington, District of Columbia, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
69
|
Babulal GM, Quiroz YT, Albensi BC, Arenaza-Urquijo E, Astell AJ, Babiloni C, Bahar-Fuchs A, Bell J, Bowman GL, Brickman AM, Chételat G, Ciro C, Cohen AD, Dilworth-Anderson P, Dodge HH, Dreux S, Edland S, Esbensen A, Evered L, Ewers M, Fargo KN, Fortea J, Gonzalez H, Gustafson DR, Head E, Hendrix JA, Hofer SM, Johnson LA, Jutten R, Kilborn K, Lanctôt KL, Manly JJ, Martins RN, Mielke MM, Morris MC, Murray ME, Oh ES, Parra MA, Rissman RA, Roe CM, Santos OA, Scarmeas N, Schneider LS, Schupf N, Sikkes S, Snyder HM, Sohrabi HR, Stern Y, Strydom A, Tang Y, Terrera GM, Teunissen C, Melo van Lent D, Weinborn M, Wesselman L, Wilcock DM, Zetterberg H, O'Bryant SE. Perspectives on ethnic and racial disparities in Alzheimer's disease and related dementias: Update and areas of immediate need. Alzheimers Dement 2019; 15:292-312. [PMID: 30555031 PMCID: PMC6368893 DOI: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.09.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 380] [Impact Index Per Article: 63.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/19/2018] [Revised: 07/12/2018] [Accepted: 09/13/2018] [Indexed: 02/07/2023]
Abstract
Alzheimer's disease and related dementias (ADRDs) are a global crisis facing the aging population and society as a whole. With the numbers of people with ADRDs predicted to rise dramatically across the world, the scientific community can no longer neglect the need for research focusing on ADRDs among underrepresented ethnoracial diverse groups. The Alzheimer's Association International Society to Advance Alzheimer's Research and Treatment (ISTAART; alz.org/ISTAART) comprises a number of professional interest areas (PIAs), each focusing on a major scientific area associated with ADRDs. We leverage the expertise of the existing international cadre of ISTAART scientists and experts to synthesize a cross-PIA white paper that provides both a concise "state-of-the-science" report of ethnoracial factors across PIA foci and updated recommendations to address immediate needs to advance ADRD science across ethnoracial populations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ganesh M Babulal
- Department of Neurology and Knight Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
| | - Yakeel T Quiroz
- Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA; Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Benedict C Albensi
- Division of Neurodegenerative Disorders, St. Boniface Hospital Albrechtsen Research Centre, Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canada; Department of Pharmacology & Therapeutics, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canada
| | | | - Arlene J Astell
- Department of Occupational Sciences & Occupational Therapy, University of Toronto, CA; School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, UK
| | - Claudio Babiloni
- Department of Physiology and Pharmacology "V. Erspamer", Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy; Department of Neuroscience, IRCCS-Hospital San Raffaele Pisana of Rome and Cassino, Rome and Cassino, Italy
| | - Alex Bahar-Fuchs
- Academic Unit for Psychiatry of Old Age, Department of Psychiatry, the University of Melbourne, Australia
| | | | - Gene L Bowman
- Nutrition and Brain Health Laboratory, Nestlé Institute of Health Sciences, Lausanne, Switzerland; Department of Neurology, Layton Aging & Alzheimer's Disease Center, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, OR, USA
| | - Adam M Brickman
- Taub Institute for Research in Alzheimer's Disease and the Aging Brain, The Gertrude H. Sergievsky Center, Department of Neurology, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| | - Gaël Chételat
- Inserm, Inserm UMR-S U1237, Université de Caen-Normandie, GIP Cyceron, Caen, France
| | - Carrie Ciro
- Department of Occupational Therapy Education, University of Kansas Medical Center, Kansas City, KS, USA
| | - Ann D Cohen
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
| | | | - Hiroko H Dodge
- Department of Neurology, Layton Aging and Alzheimer's Disease Center, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, OR, USA
| | - Simone Dreux
- Undergraduate Program of History and Science, Harvard College, Cambridge, MA, USA
| | - Steven Edland
- Department of Family Medicine and Public Health, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA
| | - Anna Esbensen
- Department of Pediatrics, University of Cincinnati College of Medicine & Division of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, OH, USA
| | - Lisbeth Evered
- Melbourne Medical School, University of Melbourne, Australia
| | - Michael Ewers
- Institute for Stroke and Dementia Research, Klinikum der Universität München, Munich, Germany
| | - Keith N Fargo
- Medical & Scientific Relations, Alzheimer's Association, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Juan Fortea
- Memory Unit, Department of Neurology, Hospital de la Santa Creu i Sant Pau, Biomedical Research Institute Sant Pau, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain; Barcelona Down Medical Center, Fundació Catalana de Síndrome de Down, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Hector Gonzalez
- Department of Neurosciences and Shiley-Marcos Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, University of San Diego, CA, USA
| | - Deborah R Gustafson
- Department of Neurology, Section for NeuroEpidemiology, State University of New York - Downstate Medical Center, Brooklyn, NY, USA
| | - Elizabeth Head
- Sanders Brown Center on Aging, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY, USA
| | - James A Hendrix
- Medical & Scientific Relations, Alzheimer's Association, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Scott M Hofer
- Adult Development and Aging, University of Victoria, British Columbia, CA, USA
| | - Leigh A Johnson
- Department of Pharmacology & Neuroscience, University of North Texas Health Science Center, Fort Worth, TX, USA
| | - Roos Jutten
- VU University Medical Center, Department of Neurology, Amsterdam Neuroscience, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | - Kerry Kilborn
- Department of Psychology, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, Scotland, UK
| | - Krista L Lanctôt
- Sunnybrook Research Institute of Psychiatry and Pharmacology, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Jennifer J Manly
- Taub Institute for Research in Alzheimer's Disease and the Aging Brain, The Gertrude H. Sergievsky Center, Department of Neurology, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| | - Ralph N Martins
- Aging and Alzheimer's Disease, School of Medical and Health Sciences, Edith Cowan University, Joondalup, Australia
| | - Michelle M Mielke
- Department of Epidemiology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, MN, USA; Department of Neurology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, MN, USA
| | | | | | - Esther S Oh
- Department of Medicine, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Mario A Parra
- School of Social Sciences, Department of Psychology, Heriot-Watt University, UK; Universidad Autónoma del Caribe, Barranquilla, Colombia; Neuroprogressive and Dementia Network, UK
| | - Robert A Rissman
- Department of Neurosciences, University of California San Diego School of Medicine, CA, USA
| | - Catherine M Roe
- Department of Neurology and Knight Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
| | - Octavio A Santos
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Mayo Clinic, Jacksonville, FL, USA
| | - Nikolaos Scarmeas
- Taub Institute for Research in Alzheimer's Disease and the Aging Brain, The Gertrude H. Sergievsky Center, Department of Neurology, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA; Aiginition Hospital, 1st Neurology Clinic, Department of Social Medicine, Psychiatry and Neurology, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Athens, Greece
| | - Lon S Schneider
- Department of Psychiatry and The Behavioral Sciences, University of Southern California, CA, USA
| | - Nicole Schupf
- Department of Epidemiology, Mailman School of Public Health Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| | - Sietske Sikkes
- Massachusetts General Hospital, Department of Neurology, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Heather M Snyder
- Medical & Scientific Relations, Alzheimer's Association, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Hamid R Sohrabi
- Aging and Alzheimer's Disease, School of Medical and Health Sciences, Edith Cowan University, Joondalup, Australia
| | - Yaakov Stern
- Department of Neurology, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA; Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| | - Andre Strydom
- Department of Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Science, Institute of Psychiatry Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
| | - Yi Tang
- Department of Neurology, Xuan Wu Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
| | - Graciela Muniz Terrera
- Centers for Clinical Brain Sciences and Dementia Prevention, University in Edinburgh, Scotland, UK
| | - Charlotte Teunissen
- Neurochemistry Laboratory and Biobank, Department of Clinical Chemistry, Amsterdam Neuroscience, Vrije Universiteit University Medical Center, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | - Debora Melo van Lent
- Department of Clinical Research, German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Bonn, Germany
| | - Michael Weinborn
- Aging and Alzheimer's Disease, School of Medical and Health Sciences, Edith Cowan University, Joondalup, Australia
| | | | - Donna M Wilcock
- Neurochemistry Laboratory and Biobank, Department of Clinical Chemistry, Amsterdam Neuroscience, Vrije Universiteit University Medical Center, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
| | - Henrik Zetterberg
- UK Dementia Research Institute at UCL, London, UK; Department of Molecular Neuroscience, UCL Institute of Neurology, Queen Square, London, UK; Clinical Neurochemistry Laboratory, Sahlgrenska University Hospital, Mölndal, Sweden; Department of Psychiatry and Neurochemistry, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology, the Sahlgrenska Academy at the University of Gothenburg, Mölndal, Sweden
| | - Sid E O'Bryant
- Department of Pharmacology & Neuroscience, University of North Texas Health Science Center, Fort Worth, TX, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
70
|
Abstract
OBJECTIVE The appropriateness of the current neuropsychological tests for the assessment of different populations has been questioned. In this issue several articles have proposed the modernization of neuropsychological testing by incorporating modern technological devices such as computers, mobile phones, or tablets. This is a commentary on the articles contained in the special issue about the inclusion of modern assessment methods that can be properly used around the world and not only in technologically/economically advanced countries. METHODS The author of this article reviewed the articles included in the special issue with the goal of commenting on whether the new approaches proposed would have global relevance and applicability. RESULTS The use of biomedical databases and modern technological devices such as computers, wearables and tablets might not be available in many countries, thus leading to a situation in which modern neuropsychological tests might be available only in some world regions. In addition, because in many countries there are large percentages of population with low education these modern neuropsychological tests might not be appropriate for them. Finally, tests developed with culturally specific stimuli will not be applicable in some cultures. CONCLUSION This article provides an overview of several issues that should be considered to make neuropsychological tests both modern and accessible around the world, more specifically the cross-cultural appropriateness and accessibility for low educated individuals and availability in low/middle income regions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alberto Luis Fernandez
- a Universidad Católica de Córdoba - Cortex Foundation-Universidad Nacional de Córdoba , Córdoba , Argentina
| |
Collapse
|
71
|
Abstract
PURPOSE OF REVIEW The aim of this review is to summarize current conceptual models of cognitive reserve (CR) and related concepts and to discuss evidence for these concepts within the context of aging and Alzheimer's disease. RECENT FINDINGS Evidence to date supports the notion that higher levels of CR, as measured by proxy variables reflective of lifetime experiences, are associated with better cognitive performance, and with a reduced risk of incident mild cognitive impairment/dementia. However, the impact of CR on longitudinal cognitive trajectories is unclear and may be influenced by a number of factors. Although there is promising evidence that some proxy measures of CR may influence structural brain measures, more research is needed. The protective effects of CR may provide an important mechanism for preserving cognitive function and cognitive well-being with age, in part because it can be enhanced throughout the lifespan. However, more research on the mechanisms by which CR is protective is needed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Corinne Pettigrew
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, 1620 McElderry St., Reed Hall 1-West, Baltimore, MD, 21205, USA
| | - Anja Soldan
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, 1620 McElderry St., Reed Hall 1-West, Baltimore, MD, 21205, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
72
|
Sherwood JJ, Inouye C, Webb SL, Zhou A, Anderson EA, Spink NS. Relationship between physical and cognitive performance in community dwelling, ethnically diverse older adults: a cross-sectional study. PeerJ 2019; 7:e6159. [PMID: 30643695 PMCID: PMC6327882 DOI: 10.7717/peerj.6159] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/25/2018] [Accepted: 11/26/2018] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Regular exercise training stimulates physiological adaptations to improve physical performance, reduce chronic disease risk, and slow age-related cognitive decline. Since the physiological mechanisms responsible for aging-associated cognitive decline are not yet fully understood, and training-induced physiological adaptations responsible for performance measure improvements are specific to the type (aerobic vs. strength) and intensity of training, studies are needed to assess the relationships between physical performance measures and cognitive performance in older adults. These results could be used to guide exercise prescriptions with the goal of improving age-related cognitive performance. The purpose of this study was to investigate the relationship between physical performance measures and cognitive performance in a population of community dwelling, ethnically diverse older adults. Methods The cognitive performance of ninety independent, community dwelling participants (69 female, 21 male), aged 75 ± 9.5 years (mean ± SD) was measured with the Modified Mini-Mental State Test (3MS), Trailmaking Tests A and B (TMT A & B), and the Animal Naming test. Sociodemographic (age, sex, ethnicity, medication use, years of education) and anthropometric data were collected, physical activity was assessed with the Physical Activity Scale for the Elderly (PASE), peak hand-grip strength, distance walked in the 6MWT, and heart rate pre-, during, and up to 5 min. post- 6MWT were measured. Forward stepwise multiple regression analyses were performed with each cognitive measure as a dependent variable. Results and Discussion Controlling for sociodemographic covariates, peak heart rate during the 6MWT (6MWT HRPEAK) was positively correlated with performance in the 3MS (p < 0.017), and TMT A (p < 0.001) and B (p < 0.029). Controlling for sociodemographic covariates, PASE was positively (p = 0.001), and β-blocker use negatively (p = 0.035), correlated with performance on the Animal Naming test. Also, controlling for sociodemographic covariates, PASE was positively correlated with performance on the TMT A (p = 0.017). Here we show that higher peak heart rate during the 6MWT is positively correlated with cognitive performance in a population of community dwelling, ethnically diverse older adults (ages 60-95 years). Conclusion Higher peak heart rate during the 6MWT was found to be independently and positively correlated with cognitive function in community-dwelling older adults. Although additional work is needed, these results are promising and suggest that physicians, exercise professionals, and/or fitness/fall prevention programs may use peak heart rate during the 6MWT to easily monitor exercise intensity to support cognitive health.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jennifer J Sherwood
- Department of Kinesiology, California State University, East Bay, Hayward, CA, United States of America
| | - Cathy Inouye
- Department of Kinesiology, California State University, East Bay, Hayward, CA, United States of America
| | - Shannon L Webb
- Department of Kinesiology, California State University, East Bay, Hayward, CA, United States of America
| | - Ange Zhou
- Department of Statistics and Biostatistics, California State University, East Bay, Hayward, CA, United States of America
| | - Erik A Anderson
- Department of Kinesiology, California State University, East Bay, Hayward, CA, United States of America
| | - Nicole S Spink
- Department of Kinesiology, California State University, East Bay, Hayward, CA, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
73
|
Aging, neurocognitive reserve, and the healthy brain. PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 2019. [DOI: 10.1016/bs.plm.2019.07.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
|
74
|
Neuropsychological Evaluation of Culturally/Linguistically Diverse Older Adults. HANDBOOK ON THE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OF AGING AND DEMENTIA 2019. [DOI: 10.1007/978-3-319-93497-6_3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/13/2022]
|
75
|
The impact of cognitive reserve on neurocognitive performance in Major Depressive Disorder. Psychiatry Res 2018; 270:211-218. [PMID: 30267985 DOI: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.09.031] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/07/2018] [Revised: 08/01/2018] [Accepted: 09/14/2018] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Cognitive reserve may mitigate the degree of cognitive deficit observed in Major Depressive Disorder (MDD), confounding attempts to fully characterize the nature of these deficits. In this study, cognitive reserve was examined as a potential moderator of neurocognitive deficits in MDD. Unmedicated, currently depressed patients with MDD (n = 269), and healthy volunteers (n = 143) were compared on measures assessing psychomotor speed, interference processing, verbal memory, visual memory, and executive functioning. Moderating effects of education level and estimated intelligence level were examined as interactions, along with age, in a regression model for each test. Differences between patients and non-patients were found with most measures, and sustained in regression models as main effects. However, the interaction of estimated intelligence and patient status was significant for processing speed, verbal memory, visual memory, and executive functioning, with patient/non-patient differences diminishing with higher estimated intelligence. Neither estimated intelligence nor education level impacted interference processing differences, which were reduced with increasing age. Better intellectual ability moderates the effect of MDD on neurocognitive test performance. This effect may confound attempts to characterize these deficits in higher functioning samples. More challenging tasks may be needed, given the potential predictive value of neurocognition for differential therapeutic and clinical outcomes.
Collapse
|
76
|
Milani SA, Marsiske M, Cottler LB, Chen X, Striley CW. Optimal cutoffs for the Montreal Cognitive Assessment vary by race and ethnicity. ALZHEIMER'S & DEMENTIA (AMSTERDAM, NETHERLANDS) 2018; 10:773-781. [PMID: 30505927 PMCID: PMC6247398 DOI: 10.1016/j.dadm.2018.09.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 148] [Impact Index Per Article: 21.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/03/2022]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), scored from 0 to 30, is used as a screening tool for mild cognitive impairment (MCI). The current cutoff (26) may not be optimal among minorities. METHODS Data from the National Alzheimer's Coordinating Center Uniform Data Set March 2018 data freeze was used to calculate optimal cutoffs for detection of MCI and dementia by race/ethnic group and education. RESULTS Of the 3895 individuals included, 80.7% were non-Hispanic White, 15.0% were non-Hispanic Black, and 4.2% were Hispanic. Optimal cutoffs for detection of MCI were 25 among non-Hispanic Whites, 24 among Hispanics, and 23 among non-Hispanic Blacks. Optimal cutoffs for detection of dementia were 19 among non-Hispanic Whites and 16 for both non-Hispanic Blacks and Hispanics. Lower educational attainment produced lower optimal cutoffs. DISCUSSION Our findings suggest cutoffs may need to be stratified by race/ethnicity and education to ensure detecting MCI from normal and MCI from dementia.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sadaf Arefi Milani
- Department of Epidemiology, College of Public Health and Health Professions & College of Medicine, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
| | - Michael Marsiske
- Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, College of Public Health and Health Professions, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
| | - Linda B. Cottler
- Department of Epidemiology, College of Public Health and Health Professions & College of Medicine, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
| | - Xinguang Chen
- Department of Epidemiology, College of Public Health and Health Professions & College of Medicine, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
| | - Catherine W. Striley
- Department of Epidemiology, College of Public Health and Health Professions & College of Medicine, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
| |
Collapse
|
77
|
Cognitive Aging in Black and White Americans: Cognition, Cognitive Decline, and Incidence of Alzheimer Disease Dementia. Epidemiology 2018; 29:151-159. [PMID: 28863046 DOI: 10.1097/ede.0000000000000747] [Citation(s) in RCA: 256] [Impact Index Per Article: 36.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND US-based studies have reported that older blacks perform worse than older whites on cognitive tests and have higher risk of Alzheimer disease dementia (AD). It is unclear whether these findings reflect differences in cognitive decline. METHODS The Chicago Health and Aging Project followed individuals, 65+ years old (64% black, 36% white), for up to 18 years. Participants underwent triennial cognitive assessments; stratified randomized samples underwent assessments for AD. We compared black and white participants' cognitive performance, cognitive decline rate (N = 7,735), and AD incidence (N = 2,144), adjusting for age and sex. RESULTS Black participants performed worse than white participants on the cognitive tests; 441 participants developed AD. Black participants' incident AD risk was twice that of whites (RR = 1.9; 95% CI, 1.4, 2.7), with 58 excess cases/1,000 occurring among blacks (95% CI, 28, 88). Among noncarriers of APOE ε4, blacks had 2.3 times the AD risk (95% CI, 1.5, 3.6), but among carriers, race was not associated with risk (RR = 1.1; 95% CI, 0.6, 2.0; Pinteraction = 0.05). However, cognitive decline was not faster among blacks: the black-white difference in 5-year change in global cognitive score was 0.007 standard unit (95% CI, -0.034, 0.047). Years of education accounted for a sizable portion of racial disparities in cognitive level and AD risk, in analyses using a counterfactual approach. CONCLUSIONS The higher risk of AD among blacks may stem from lower level of cognitive test performance persisting throughout the observation period rather than faster rate of late-life cognitive decline. Disparities in educational attainment may contribute to these performance disparities. See video abstract at, http://links.lww.com/EDE/B299.
Collapse
|
78
|
Colombo B, Antonietti A, Daneau B. The Relationships Between Cognitive Reserve and Creativity. A Study on American Aging Population. Front Psychol 2018; 9:764. [PMID: 29875716 PMCID: PMC5974109 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00764] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/28/2018] [Accepted: 04/30/2018] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
Abstract
The Cognitive Reserve (CR) hypothesis suggests that the brain actively attempts to cope with neural damages by using pre-existing cognitive processing approaches or by enlisting compensatory approaches. This would allow an individual with high CR to better cope with aging than an individual with lower CR. Many of the proxies used to assess CR indirectly refer to the flexibility of thought. The present paper aims at directly exploring the relationships between CR and creativity, a skill that includes flexible thinking. We tested a sample of 72 adults (aged between 45 and 78) assessing both their level of CR and their creativity. To evaluate CR we used the proxies commonly used in literature, namely, three subtests from the WAIS (vocabulary, similarities, and digit span) and the years of education. We also used an ad-hoc test asking people to report how frequently they tend to perform activities that are believed to increase CR. We used verbal creativity tasks (alternative uses and generation of acronyms) to assess individual levels of creativity. We asked participants to describe their main occupation (present or past) and coded each occupation as creative or not creative. Results (controlling for age-related differences) showed that scores from the WAIS correlated positively with creativity performance, even though correlations varied across the subtests. Focusing on the frequency and type of activities that people perform, and comparing individuals who have or had a creative job to those with a routine job, a clear relationship between creativity and CR emerged. This effect was more relevant than the level of job complexity. Implications for the study of CR and aging are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Barbara Colombo
- Neuroscience Lab, Champlain College, Burlington, VT, United States
| | | | - Brendan Daneau
- Neuroscience Lab, Champlain College, Burlington, VT, United States
| |
Collapse
|
79
|
Zheng J, Yu H. Assessing the Readability of Medical Documents: A Ranking Approach. JMIR Med Inform 2018; 6:e17. [PMID: 29572199 PMCID: PMC5889493 DOI: 10.2196/medinform.8611] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/03/2017] [Revised: 01/12/2018] [Accepted: 01/31/2018] [Indexed: 02/02/2023] Open
Abstract
Background The use of electronic health record (EHR) systems with patient engagement capabilities, including viewing, downloading, and transmitting health information, has recently grown tremendously. However, using these resources to engage patients in managing their own health remains challenging due to the complex and technical nature of the EHR narratives. Objective Our objective was to develop a machine learning–based system to assess readability levels of complex documents such as EHR notes. Methods We collected difficulty ratings of EHR notes and Wikipedia articles using crowdsourcing from 90 readers. We built a supervised model to assess readability based on relative orders of text difficulty using both surface text features and word embeddings. We evaluated system performance using the Kendall coefficient of concordance against human ratings. Results Our system achieved significantly higher concordance (.734) with human annotators than did a baseline using the Flesch-Kincaid Grade Level, a widely adopted readability formula (.531). The improvement was also consistent across different disease topics. This method’s concordance with an individual human user’s ratings was also higher than the concordance between different human annotators (.658). Conclusions We explored methods to automatically assess the readability levels of clinical narratives. Our ranking-based system using simple textual features and easy-to-learn word embeddings outperformed a widely used readability formula. Our ranking-based method can predict relative difficulties of medical documents. It is not constrained to a predefined set of readability levels, a common design in many machine learning–based systems. Furthermore, the feature set does not rely on complex processing of the documents. One potential application of our readability ranking is personalization, allowing patients to better accommodate their own background knowledge.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jiaping Zheng
- College of Information and Computer Sciences, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA, United States
| | - Hong Yu
- College of Information and Computer Sciences, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA, United States.,Center for Healthcare Organization and Implementation Research, Bedford Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Bedford, MA, United States.,Department of Computer Science, University of Massachusetts, Lowell, MA, United States.,Department of Medicine, University of Massachusetts Medical School, Worcester, MA, United States
| |
Collapse
|
80
|
Soldan A, Pettigrew C, Albert M. Evaluating Cognitive Reserve Through the Prism of Preclinical Alzheimer Disease. Psychiatr Clin North Am 2018; 41:65-77. [PMID: 29412849 PMCID: PMC5806143 DOI: 10.1016/j.psc.2017.10.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
The concept of cognitive reserve (CR) was proposed to account for the discrepancy between levels of brain pathologic features or damage and clinical and cognitive function. This article provides a detailed review of prospective longitudinal studies that have investigated the interaction between CR and Alzheimer's disease (AD) biomarkers on clinical and cognitive outcomes among individuals with preclinical AD. Current evidence shows that higher levels of CR are associated with a delay in the onset of symptoms of mild cognitive impairment and that there may be multiple pathways by which CR exerts its protective effects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anja Soldan
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, 1620 McElderry Street, Reed Hall West - 1, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA.
| | - Corinne Pettigrew
- Research Associate, Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Marilyn Albert
- Professor, Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| |
Collapse
|
81
|
Suh SW, Han JW, Park JY, Hong JW, Kim K, Kim T, Lee KH, Han G, Jeong H, Seo J, Kim TH, Lee DY, Lee DW, Ryu SH, Kim SG, Youn JC, Jhoo JH, Kim JL, Lee SB, Lee JJ, Kwak KP, Kim BJ, Moon SW, Park JH, Kim KW. Impacts of Illiteracy on the Risk of Dementia: A Global Health Perspective. J Alzheimers Dis 2018; 53:731-41. [PMID: 27232216 DOI: 10.3233/jad-160108] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Despite its significance as a contributing factor for late-life dementia risk, illiteracy is frequently underappreciated in the management of dementia. In this study, we estimated the proportion of dementia cases attributable to illiteracy using the population attributable fraction (PAF), and calculated to what extent the monetary cost of dementia could be saved in the future by reducing illiteracy from the South Korean, Latin American, South Asian/Middle Eastern, and African populations. We collected necessary data from the 2011 United Nations Human Development Report and prevalence studies conducted in these regions. Additional variables not included in the above sources were estimated using a logit model under a "trend scenario"-based assumption. Around 16% of the total number of dementia cases in South Korea in 2015 can be attributed to illiteracy, with this figure predicted to decline to around 2% by 2050. This translates to a saving in dementia care costs of approximately 52 billion USD, providing we are successful in theoretically eradicating illiteracy as of 2015, in the population aged 65 years or under. Likewise, reducing illiteracy to 50% in Latin America, South Asia/The Middle East, and Africa by 2050 could generate further cost savings of between 71 and 244 billion, 13 and 94 billion, and 17 and 78 billion USD, respectively. Even public policies aimed solely at reducing illiteracy in the childhood, adolescent, or middle-aged population could potentially have a role in the primary prevention of dementia. Moving forward, governments will need to address this issue in a purposeful and systematic manner.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Seung Wan Suh
- Department of Neuropsychiatry, Seoul National University Hospital, Seoul, Korea
| | - Ji Won Han
- Department of Neuropsychiatry, Seoul National University Bundang Hospital, Seongnam, Korea
| | - Jae Young Park
- Department of Neuropsychiatry, Seoul National University Bundang Hospital, Seongnam, Korea
| | - Jong Woo Hong
- Department of Neuropsychiatry, Seoul National University Bundang Hospital, Seongnam, Korea
| | - Kayoung Kim
- Department of Neuropsychiatry, Seoul National University Bundang Hospital, Seongnam, Korea
| | - Taehyun Kim
- Department of Neuropsychiatry, Seoul National University Bundang Hospital, Seongnam, Korea
| | - Kyoung Hwan Lee
- Department of Neuropsychiatry, Seoul National University Bundang Hospital, Seongnam, Korea
| | - Guehee Han
- Department of Neuropsychiatry, Seoul National University Bundang Hospital, Seongnam, Korea
| | - Hyeon Jeong
- Department of Psychiatry, Woori-Seongsim Psychiatric Clinic, Siheung, Korea
| | - Jiyeong Seo
- Department of Psychiatry, Gyeongsang National University Hospital, Jinju, Korea
| | - Tae Hui Kim
- Department of Psychiatry, Yonsei University Wonju Severance Christian Hospital, Wonju, Korea
| | - Dong Young Lee
- Department of Neuropsychiatry, Seoul National University Hospital, Seoul, Korea.,Department of Psychiatry, Seoul National University, College of Medicine, Seoul, Korea
| | - Dong Woo Lee
- Department of Neuropsychiatry, Inje University Sanggye Paik Hospital, Seoul, Korea
| | - Seung-Ho Ryu
- Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Konkuk University, Konkuk University Medical Center, Seoul, Korea
| | - Shin-Gyeom Kim
- Department of Neuropsychiatry, Soonchunhyang University Bucheon Hospital, Bucheon, Korea
| | - Jong Chul Youn
- Department of Neuropsychiatry, Gyeonggi Provincial Hospital for the Elderly, Yongin, Korea
| | - Jin Hyeong Jhoo
- Department of Neuropsychiatry, Kangwon National University Hospital, Chuncheon, Korea
| | - Jeong Lan Kim
- Department of Psychiatry, Chungnam National University Hospital, Daejeon, Korea
| | - Seok Bum Lee
- Department of Psychiatry, Dankook University Hospital, Cheonan, Korea
| | - Jung Jae Lee
- Department of Psychiatry, Dankook University Hospital, Cheonan, Korea
| | - Kyung Phil Kwak
- Department of Psychiatry, Dongguk University Gyeongju Hospital, Gyeongju, Korea
| | - Bong-Jo Kim
- Department of Psychiatry, Gyeongsang National University, School of Medicine, Jinju, Korea
| | - Seok Woo Moon
- Department of Psychiatry, Konkuk University, School of Medicine, Chungju, Korea
| | - Joon Hyuk Park
- Department of Neuropsychiatry, Jeju National University Hospital, Jeju, Korea
| | - Ki Woong Kim
- Department of Neuropsychiatry, Seoul National University Bundang Hospital, Seongnam, Korea.,Department of Psychiatry, Seoul National University, College of Medicine, Seoul, Korea.,Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Seoul National University, College of Natural Sciences, Seoul, Korea
| |
Collapse
|
82
|
Comparing Alternative Effect Decomposition Methods: The Role of Literacy in Mediating Educational Effects on Mortality. Epidemiology 2018; 27:670-6. [PMID: 27280331 DOI: 10.1097/ede.0000000000000517] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Inverse odds ratio weighting, a newly proposed tool to evaluate mediation in exposure-disease associations, may be valuable for a host of research questions, but little is known about its performance in real data. We compare this approach to a more conventional Baron and Kenny type of decomposition on an additive hazards scale to estimate total, direct, and indirect effects using the example of the role of literacy in mediating the effects of education on mortality. METHODS Health and Retirement Study participants born in the United States between 1900 and 1947 were interviewed biennially for up to 12 years (N = 17,054). Literacy was measured with a brief vocabulary assessment. Decomposition estimates were derived based on Aalen additive hazards models. RESULTS A 1 standard deviation difference in educational attainment (3 years) was associated with 6.7 fewer deaths per 1000 person-years (β = -6.7, 95% confidence interval [CI]: -7.9, -5.4). Of this decrease, 1.3 fewer deaths (β = -1.3, 95% CI: -4.0, 1.2) were attributed to the literacy pathway (natural indirect), representing 19% of the total effect. Baron and Kenny estimates were consistent with inverse odds ratio weighting estimates but were less variable (natural indirect effect: -1.2 [95% CI: -1.7, -0.69], representing 18% of total effect). CONCLUSION In a cohort of older Americans, literacy partially mediated the effect of education on mortality. See Video Abstract at http://links.lww.com/EDE/B78.
Collapse
|
83
|
Frankenmolen NL, Fasotti L, Kessels RP, Oosterman JM. The influence of cognitive reserve and age on the use of memory strategies. Exp Aging Res 2018; 44:117-134. [DOI: 10.1080/0361073x.2017.1422472] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Nikita L. Frankenmolen
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Centre for Cognition, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
- Rehabilitation Centre Klimmendaal, Arnhem, The Netherlands
| | - Luciano Fasotti
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Centre for Cognition, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
- Rehabilitation Centre Klimmendaal, Arnhem, The Netherlands
| | - Roy P.C. Kessels
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Centre for Cognition, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
- Department of Medical Psychology, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Joukje M. Oosterman
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Centre for Cognition, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
84
|
Choi SK, Rose ID, Friedman DB. How Is Literacy Being Defined and Measured in Dementia Research? A Scoping Review. Gerontol Geriatr Med 2018; 4:2333721418812246. [PMID: 30505890 PMCID: PMC6256312 DOI: 10.1177/2333721418812246] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/15/2018] [Revised: 10/14/2018] [Accepted: 10/18/2018] [Indexed: 01/22/2023] Open
Abstract
Literacy plays an important role in Alzheimer's disease and related dementias (ADRD); however, less is known about how literacy is being used and defined in ADRD studies. This study reviewed terminology being used to describe types and definitions of literacy and instruments used to assess literacy in ADRD research. Among the 219 studies retrieved from 3 databases, 50 met our inclusion criteria. Literacy terms used in ADRD studies varied: literacy (n = 28), health literacy (n = 9), and dementia literacy (n = 7) were the most often used terms, followed by financial literacy (n = 4), dementia knowledge (n = 3), AD knowledge (n = 2), mental health literacy (n = 2), AD literacy, digital literacy, health literacy about incontinence, and financial knowledge (n = 1 each). Thirty studies did not define literacy terms used. Among the 20 studies defining literacy, definitions were inconsistent across studies even when they used the same term. Surveys (n = 30), open-ended questions, vignettes, or focus groups (n = 10), self-perceived (n = 3) or interviewer assessed (n = 1) literacy levels were used to assess literacy. Ten studies did not specify literacy measurement. Various literacies have been examined in ADRD research with unclear definitions and some inadequate measures. Well-defined terms with valid measures are needed to better understand the role of literacies in ADRD research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - India D. Rose
- ICF, Health Research Informatics and Technology Division, Atlanta, GA, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
85
|
Abstract
There is a consistent association between education and depressive symptoms, but research on the mechanisms to explain this association remains limited. No study has formally evaluated the extent to which the association between education and depressive symptoms is mediated through a foundational skill such as literacy. Inverse odds ratio weighting (IORW) was used to estimate total, natural direct, and natural indirect effects in examining literacy as a mediator of the association between education and depressive symptoms. Health and Retirement Study participants born in the U.S. between 1900 and 1947 were interviewed biennially for up to 12 years (N = 16,718). Literacy was assessed with a brief vocabulary measure. Depressive symptoms were measured using the 8-item Centers for Epidemiologic Studies-Depression (CES-D) scale. Decomposition estimates were derived using regression analyses of repeated measures of depressive symptoms. Standard errors were obtained using a nonparametric bootstrap with the individual as the independent unit to account for dependence of observations within an individual. In a large cohort of older Americans, a one standard deviation difference in educational attainment (~ 3 years) was associated with a 0.35-point decrement in CES-D score (95% CI: -0.38, -0.32). This decrement represents a 0.22 standard deviation difference in depressive symptoms. Using IORW, the estimated effect of education on depressive symptoms mediated through literacy was -0.10 (95% CI: -0.18, -0.01), which represents 28% of the total effect. Education confers many benefits; as demonstrated by this study for depressive symptoms, one important benefit is literacy.
Collapse
|
86
|
Fleck JI, Kuti J, Mercurio J, Mullen S, Austin K, Pereira O. The Impact of Age and Cognitive Reserve on Resting-State Brain Connectivity. Front Aging Neurosci 2017; 9:392. [PMID: 29249962 PMCID: PMC5716980 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00392] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/01/2017] [Accepted: 11/13/2017] [Indexed: 12/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Cognitive reserve (CR) is a protective mechanism that supports sustained cognitive function following damage to the physical brain associated with age, injury, or disease. The goal of the research was to identify relationships between age, CR, and brain connectivity. A sample of 90 cognitively normal adults, ages 45–64 years, had their resting-state brain activity recorded with electroencephalography (EEG) and completed a series of memory and executive function assessments. CR was estimated using years of education and verbal IQ scores. Participants were divided into younger and older age groups and low- and high-CR groups. We observed greater left- than right-hemisphere coherence in younger participants, and greater right- than left-hemisphere coherence in older participants. In addition, greater coherence was observed under eyes-closed than eyes-open recording conditions for both low-CR and high-CR participants, with a more substantial difference between recording conditions in individuals high in CR regardless of age. Finally, younger participants low in CR exhibited greater mean coherence than younger participants high in CR, whereas the opposite pattern was observed in older participants, with greater coherence in older participants high in CR. Together, these findings suggest the possibility of a shift in the relationship between CR and brain connectivity during aging.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jessica I Fleck
- School of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Stockton University, Galloway Township, NJ, United States
| | - Julia Kuti
- School of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Stockton University, Galloway Township, NJ, United States
| | - Jeffrey Mercurio
- Department of Cell Biology and Molecular Biology, National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, Bethesda, MD, United States
| | - Spencer Mullen
- School of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Stockton University, Galloway Township, NJ, United States
| | - Katherine Austin
- School of Graduate Studies, Stockton University, Galloway Township, NJ, United States
| | - Olivia Pereira
- Department of Biomedical Research, Nemours Hospital for Children, Wilmington, DE, United States
| |
Collapse
|
87
|
Jackson JD, Rentz DM, Aghjayan SL, Buckley RF, Meneide TF, Sperling RA, Amariglio RE. Subjective cognitive concerns are associated with objective memory performance in Caucasian but not African-American persons. Age Ageing 2017; 46:988-993. [PMID: 29088363 DOI: 10.1093/ageing/afx077] [Citation(s) in RCA: 42] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/18/2016] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Objective subjective cognitive concerns (SCC) have been proposed as a means of identifying individuals at risk for Alzheimer's disease (AD). However, the utility of SCCs has not been well-explored for African-Americans, who are twice as likely to develop AD dementia as Caucasians. We investigated whether race affects the association between SCCs and objective memory performance. Methods we used a composite of three SCC questionnaires, and three challenging episodic memory tests. We studied 289 (61% female; African-American n = 47) clinically normal older individuals. Two hierarchical linear regressions assessed the modifying role of race on the association between SCC and objective memory performance. The first regression was conducted on the full sample, while the second matched the racial groups on age, estimated verbal IQ and socioeconomic status. Results in the full sample, both groups reported similar levels of SCCs, P = 0.10, although African-Americans performed worse on the memory tasks, P < 0.001. No group differences were observed in the matched sample. The SCC × race interaction term was nonsignificant in the full sample, β = 0.109, P = 0.09, but was significant in the matched sample, β = 0.422, P = 0.037. While a significant correlation was observed between SCCs and memory among Caucasians, r = -0.401, the correlation was not found among African-Americans, r = -0.052. Conclusions results suggest that the dissociation between SCCs and memory performance in African-Americans may indicate qualitative differences in how diverse groups endorse cognitive concerns, even after considering socioeconomic and educational factors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jonathan D Jackson
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA 02114-2621, USA
- Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115,
USA
| | - Dorene M Rentz
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA 02114-2621, USA
- Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115,
USA
- Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women's Hospital—Center for Alzheimer Research and Treatment, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Sarah L Aghjayan
- Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women's Hospital—Center for Alzheimer Research and Treatment, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Rachel F Buckley
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA 02114-2621, USA
- Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115,
USA
- Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
| | - Tamy-Fee Meneide
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA 02114-2621, USA
| | - Reisa A Sperling
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA 02114-2621, USA
- Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115,
USA
- Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women's Hospital—Center for Alzheimer Research and Treatment, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Rebecca E Amariglio
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA 02114-2621, USA
- Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115,
USA
- Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women's Hospital—Center for Alzheimer Research and Treatment, Boston, MA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
88
|
Soldan A, Pettigrew C, Cai Q, Wang J, Wang MC, Moghekar A, Miller MI, Albert M. Cognitive reserve and long-term change in cognition in aging and preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging 2017; 60:164-172. [PMID: 28968586 DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.09.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 129] [Impact Index Per Article: 16.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/02/2017] [Revised: 08/30/2017] [Accepted: 09/04/2017] [Indexed: 12/19/2022]
Abstract
We examined if baseline levels of cognitive reserve (CR) and of Alzheimer's disease (AD) biomarkers modify the rate of change in cognition among individuals with normal cognition at baseline (n = 303, mean baseline age = 57 years, mean follow-up = 12 years); 66 participants subsequently developed mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or dementia due to AD. CR was indexed by years of education, reading, and vocabulary measures. AD biomarkers were measured with a composite score composed of measures of amyloid, phosphorylated tau, and neurodegeneration. Higher CR scores were associated with better cognitive performance but did not modify the rate of change in cognition among those who remained cognitively normal, nor among those who progressed to MCI before symptom onset, independent of baseline biomarker levels. However, higher CR scores were associated with faster cognitive decline after symptom onset of MCI. These results suggest that the mechanism by which CR mediates the relationship between pathology and cognitive function is by delaying the onset of symptoms rather than reducing the rate of cognitive decline.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anja Soldan
- Department of Neurology, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA.
| | - Corinne Pettigrew
- Department of Neurology, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Qing Cai
- Department of Biostatistics, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Jiangxia Wang
- Department of Biostatistics, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Mei-Cheng Wang
- Department of Biostatistics, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Abhay Moghekar
- Department of Neurology, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Michael I Miller
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | - Marilyn Albert
- Department of Neurology, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
89
|
Ingber AP, Hassenstab J, Fagan AM, Benzinger TLS, Grant EA, Holtzman DM, Morris JC, Roe CM. Cerebrospinal Fluid Biomarkers and Reserve Variables as Predictors of Future "Non-Cognitive" Outcomes of Alzheimer's Disease. J Alzheimers Dis 2017; 52:1055-64. [PMID: 27104893 DOI: 10.3233/jad-150478] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND The influence of reserve variables and Alzheimer's disease (AD) biomarkers on cognitive test performance has been fairly well-characterized. However, less is known about the influence of these factors on "non-cognitive" outcomes, including functional abilities and mood. OBJECTIVE We examined whether cognitive and brain reserve variables mediate how AD biomarker levels in cognitively normal persons predict future changes in function, mood, and neuropsychiatric behavior. METHODS Non-cognitive outcomes were examined in 328 individuals 50 years and older enrolled in ongoing studies of aging and dementia at the Knight Alzheimer Disease Research Center (ADRC). All participants were cognitively normal at baseline (Clinical Dementia Rating [CDR] 0), completed cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) and structural neuroimaging studies within one year of baseline, and were followed for an average of 4.6 annual visits. Linear mixed effects models explored how cognitive reserve and brain reserve variables mediate the relationships between AD biomarker levels and changes in function, mood, and neuropsychiatric behavior in cognitively normal participants. RESULTS Education levels did not have a significant effect on predicting non-cognitive decline. However, participants with smaller brain volumes exhibited the worst outcomes on measures of mood, functional abilities, and behavioral disturbance. This effect was most pronounced in individuals who also had abnormal CSF biomarkers. CONCLUSIONS The findings suggest that brain reserve plays a stronger, or earlier, role than cognitive reserve in protecting against non-cognitive impairment in AD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Adam P Ingber
- Department of Biological Statistics and Computational Biology, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, USA
| | - Jason Hassenstab
- Knight Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA.,Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
| | - Anne M Fagan
- Knight Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA.,Hope Center for Neurological Disorders, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA.,Department of Neurology, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
| | - Tammie L S Benzinger
- Knight Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA.,Department of Radiology, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA.,Department of Neurological Surgery, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
| | - Elizabeth A Grant
- Knight Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
| | - David M Holtzman
- Knight Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA.,Hope Center for Neurological Disorders, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA.,Department of Neurology, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA.,Department of Molecular Biology and Pharmacology, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
| | - John C Morris
- Knight Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA.,Department of Neurology, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA.,Department of Pathology and Immunology, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA.,Department of Physical Therapy, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA.,Department of Occupational Therapy, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
| | - Catherine M Roe
- Knight Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA.,Department of Neurology, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
| |
Collapse
|
90
|
Tsang S, Sperling SA, Park MH, Helenius IM, Williams IC, Manning C. Health Variables Are Informative in Screening for Mild Cognitive Impairment Among Elderly African Americans. J Appl Gerontol 2017; 38:1421-1444. [PMID: 28554264 DOI: 10.1177/0733464817711961] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
To aid primary care providers in identifying people at increased risk for cognitive decline, we explored the relative importance of health and demographic variables in detecting potential cognitive impairment using the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE). Participants were 94 older African Americans coming to see their primary care physicians for reasons other than cognitive complaints. Education was strongly associated with cognitive functioning. Among those with at least 9 years of education, patients with more vascular risk factors were at greater risk for mild cognitive impairment. For patients with fewer than 9 years of education, those with fewer prescribed medications were at increased risk for dementia. These results suggest that in addition to the MMSE, primary care physicians can make use of patients' health information to improve identification of patients at increased risk for cognitive impairment. With improved identification, physicians can implement strategies to mitigate the progression and impact of cognitive difficulties.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Siny Tsang
- 1 Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| | | | - Moon-Ho Park
- 3 Korea University College of Medicine, Republic of Korea
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
91
|
Lifshitz-Vahav H, Shrira A, Bodner E. The reciprocal relationship between participation in leisure activities and cognitive functioning: the moderating effect of self-rated literacy level. Aging Ment Health 2017; 21:524-531. [PMID: 26745357 DOI: 10.1080/13607863.2015.1124838] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Participation in leisure activities is beneficial for cognitive functioning of older adults, but it is less known whether it is also beneficial for those with low basic cognitive level. This study examined the reciprocal relationship between participating in leisure activities and cognitive functioning among low and higher literacy level older adults. METHOD Respondents aged 60 years and older who participated in both first waves (2005-2006 and 2009-2010) of the Israeli component of the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE-Israel) were divided into low (n = 139) and higher literacy level respondents (n = 714). They reported participation in leisure activities and completed measures of cognitive functioning at both waves. RESULTS Cross-lagged models showed that participation in leisure activities predicted higher cognitive functioning four years later only among older adults with low literacy level. On the other hand, cognitive functioning predicted more participation in leisure activities four years later only among higher literacy level older adults. CONCLUSIONS Participating in leisure activities may be especially beneficial to cognitive functioning among older adults with low literacy level, as their initial low cognitive level allows more room for cognitive improvement than among higher literacy level older adults. Public efforts aimed at increasing participation in leisure activities may therefore target particularly older adults with low basic cognitive level.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hefziba Lifshitz-Vahav
- a Department of Special Education, School of Education , Bar-Ilan University , Ramat-Gan , Israel
| | - Amit Shrira
- b Interdisciplinary Department of Social Sciences , Bar-Ilan University , Ramat-Gan , Israel
| | - Ehud Bodner
- b Interdisciplinary Department of Social Sciences , Bar-Ilan University , Ramat-Gan , Israel.,c Department of Music , Bar-Ilan University , Ramat-Gan , Israel
| |
Collapse
|
92
|
Rhodes E, Devlin KN, Steinberg L, Giovannetti T. Grit in adolescence is protective of late-life cognition: non-cognitive factors and cognitive reserve. NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENT, AND COGNITION. SECTION B, AGING, NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND COGNITION 2017; 24:321-332. [PMID: 27428038 PMCID: PMC5785334 DOI: 10.1080/13825585.2016.1210079] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
Various psychological assets have been shown to protect against late-life cognitive impairment by promoting cognitive reserve. While factors such as educational attainment and IQ are well-established contributors to cognitive reserve, noncognitive factors, such as grit, have not been studied in this regard. We examined the contribution of adolescent grit, indexed by high school class rank controlling for IQ, to late-life cognition and its decline among approximately 4000 participants in the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study, a random sample of high school graduates followed from 1957 to 2011. Adolescent grit significantly predicted both immediate and delayed memory at ages 64 and 71, over and above the contribution of IQ. While the relative contributions of IQ and grit to immediate memory were comparable, grit was a stronger predictor of delayed memory. Cognitive reserve has noncognitive, as well as cognitive, components.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emma Rhodes
- a Department of Psychology , Temple University , Philadelphia , PA , USA
| | - Kathryn N Devlin
- a Department of Psychology , Temple University , Philadelphia , PA , USA
| | - Laurence Steinberg
- a Department of Psychology , Temple University , Philadelphia , PA , USA
| | - Tania Giovannetti
- a Department of Psychology , Temple University , Philadelphia , PA , USA
| |
Collapse
|
93
|
Pertl MM, Hannigan C, Brennan S, Robertson IH, Lawlor BA. Cognitive reserve and self-efficacy as moderators of the relationship between stress exposure and executive functioning among spousal dementia caregivers. Int Psychogeriatr 2017; 29:615-625. [PMID: 28067184 DOI: 10.1017/s1041610216002337] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND A substantial literature has reported that stress negatively impacts on cognitive processes. As dementia caregiving can be stressful, it has been hypothesized that the challenges of dementia care may increase caregivers' own vulnerability to cognitive decline. Prefrontal processes are thought to be most vulnerable to stress; however, few studies have examined whether greater caregiver stress predicts poorer executive dysfunction, and no previous research has considered potential moderators of this relationship. We examined (1) whether greater psychological stress mediated a relationship between caregiver stress exposure and executive functioning and (2) whether greater self-efficacy and cognitive reserve (CR) moderated this relationship. METHOD Spousal dementia caregivers (n = 253) completed the Neuropsychiatric Inventory Questionnaire (stress exposure), the Perceived Stress Scale, the National Adult Reading Test (CR), the Fortinsky dementia-specific caregiver self-efficacy scale, and the Color Trails Test (executive functioning). Moderated mediation was tested using the PROCESS macro. Age, gender, and dementia risk factors were included as covariates. RESULTS Greater stress exposure indirectly predicted executive functioning through psychological stress. Stronger relationships between greater psychological stress and poorer executive functioning were observed among caregivers with lower CR; there was no evidence that self-efficacy moderated the relationship between stress exposure and psychological stress. CONCLUSIONS Our findings are in line with the idea that greater psychological stress in response to challenges associated with dementia care predicts poorer caregiver executive functioning, particularly among caregivers with low CR. However, these findings are cross sectional; it is also possible that poorer executive functioning contributes to greater caregiver stress.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M M Pertl
- Neuro-Enhancement for Independent Lives (NEIL) Programme,Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience (TCIN),Trinity College Dublin,Dublin 2,Ireland
| | - C Hannigan
- Neuro-Enhancement for Independent Lives (NEIL) Programme,Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience (TCIN),Trinity College Dublin,Dublin 2,Ireland
| | - S Brennan
- Neuro-Enhancement for Independent Lives (NEIL) Programme,Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience (TCIN),Trinity College Dublin,Dublin 2,Ireland
| | - I H Robertson
- Neuro-Enhancement for Independent Lives (NEIL) Programme,Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience (TCIN),Trinity College Dublin,Dublin 2,Ireland
| | - B A Lawlor
- Neuro-Enhancement for Independent Lives (NEIL) Programme,Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience (TCIN),Trinity College Dublin,Dublin 2,Ireland
| |
Collapse
|
94
|
Pettigrew C, Soldan A, Zhu Y, Wang MC, Brown T, Miller M, Albert M. Cognitive reserve and cortical thickness in preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Brain Imaging Behav 2017; 11:357-367. [PMID: 27544202 PMCID: PMC5743433 DOI: 10.1007/s11682-016-9581-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 51] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/19/2022]
Abstract
This study examined whether cognitive reserve (CR) alters the relationship between magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) measures of cortical thickness and risk of progression from normal cognition to the onset of clinical symptoms associated with mild cognitive impairment (MCI). The analyses included 232 participants from the BIOCARD study. Participants were cognitively normal and largely middle aged (M age = 56.5) at their baseline MRI scan. After an average of 11.8 years of longitudinal follow-up, 48 have developed clinical symptoms of MCI or dementia (M time from baseline to clinical symptom onset = 7.0 years). Mean thickness was measured over eight 'AD vulnerable' cortical regions, and cognitive reserve was indexed by a composite score consisting of years of education, reading, and vocabulary measures. Using Cox regression models, CR and cortical thickness were each independently associated with risk of clinical symptom onset within 7 years of baseline, suggesting that the neuronal injury occurring proximal to symptom onset has a direct association with clinical outcomes, regardless of CR. In contrast, there was a significant interaction between CR and mean cortical thickness for risk of progression more than 7 years from baseline, suggesting that individuals with high CR are better able to compensate for cortical thinning that is beginning to occur at the very earliest phase of AD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Corinne Pettigrew
- Department of Neurology, School of Medicine, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, 21205, USA.
| | - Anja Soldan
- Department of Neurology, School of Medicine, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, 21205, USA
| | - Yuxin Zhu
- Department of Biostatistics, Bloomberg School of Public Health, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, 21205, USA
| | - Mei-Cheng Wang
- Department of Biostatistics, Bloomberg School of Public Health, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, 21205, USA
| | - Timothy Brown
- Center for Imaging Science, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, 21218, USA
| | - Michael Miller
- Center for Imaging Science, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, 21218, USA
- Institute for Computational Medicine, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, 21218, USA
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Whiting School of Engineering, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, 21218, USA
| | - Marilyn Albert
- Department of Neurology, School of Medicine, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, 21205, USA
| |
Collapse
|
95
|
Hahn EA, Magasi SR, Carlozzi NE, Tulsky DS, Wong A, Garcia SF, Lai JS, Hammel J, Miskovic A, Jerousek S, Goldsmith A, Nitsch K, Heinemann AW. Health and Functional Literacy in Physical Rehabilitation Patients. Health Lit Res Pract 2017; 1:e71-e85. [PMID: 31294252 PMCID: PMC6607848 DOI: 10.3928/24748307-20170427-02] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/15/2016] [Accepted: 04/05/2017] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND People with disabilities, who face multiple barriers to care, experience health disparities, yet few studies have measured health literacy in this population. OBJECTIVE This study evaluated functional literacy, health literacy, fluid cognitive function, and self-reported health in people who live in community dwellings with spinal cord injury, stroke, or traumatic brain injury. METHODS Participants with a traumatic spinal cord injury, stroke, or traumatic brain injury, one-year postinjury, and age 18 to 85 years, completed a battery of instruments at three medical centers in the Midwestern U.S.: functional literacy (word recognition, vocabulary knowledge), health literacy (comprehension of prose, document, and quantitative health information), fluid cognitive function (memory, executive function, and processing speed), and patient-reported outcomes (mobility, fatigue, sadness, anxiety, social function, and overall health). KEY RESULTS There were strong correlations between functional literacy, health literacy, and fluid cognitive function. After adjustment for sociodemographic and clinical characteristics, higher health literacy was associated with better mobility, less anxiety, and better overall health; higher functional literacy was associated with less anxiety and better overall health; and higher fluid cognitive function was associated with better mobility, less sadness, better social function, and better overall health. CONCLUSIONS To effectively address limited health literacy among people with spinal cord injury, stroke, and traumatic brain injury, and ensure that they are able to be informed partners in their health care, intervention is required at the level of patients, providers, and health care delivery systems. A special consideration is to ensure that health information is both well-targeted to people's health literacy levels and accessible for people with a range of physical, cognitive, and sensory limitations. The multimedia self-administered health literacy measure used in this study could be useful to rehabilitation providers and designers of health information and interfaces. [Health Literacy Research and Practice. 2017;1(2):e71-e85.]. PLAIN LANGUAGE SUMMARY Health literacy represents people's abilities to obtain, understand, and use health information to make informed decisions about their health and health care. People with disabilities face physical, attitudinal, economic, and structural barriers to care. Consideration of health literacy in rehabilitation practice can enhance the effectiveness of the patient-clinician relationship and help address the needs of this population.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elizabeth A. Hahn
- Address correspondence to Elizabeth A. Hahn, MA, Department of Medical Social Sciences, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, 633 N. Saint Clair Street, Suite 1900, Chicago, IL 60611;
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
96
|
Soones TN, Lin JL, Wolf MS, O'Conor R, Martynenko M, Wisnivesky JP, Federman AD. Pathways linking health literacy, health beliefs, and cognition to medication adherence in older adults with asthma. J Allergy Clin Immunol 2017; 139:804-809. [PMID: 27555454 PMCID: PMC5239763 DOI: 10.1016/j.jaci.2016.05.043] [Citation(s) in RCA: 52] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/23/2015] [Revised: 05/04/2016] [Accepted: 05/13/2016] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Limited health literacy is associated with low adherence to asthma controller medications among older adults. OBJECTIVE We sought to describe the causal pathway linking health literacy to medication adherence by modeling asthma illness and medication beliefs as mediators. METHODS We recruited adults aged 60 years and older with asthma from hospital and community practices in New York, New York, and Chicago, Illinois. We measured health literacy and medication adherence using the Short Test of Functional Health Literacy in Adults and the Medication Adherence Rating Scale, respectively. We used validated instruments to assess asthma illness and medication beliefs. We assessed cognition using a cognitive battery. Using structural equation modeling, we modeled illness and medication beliefs as mediators of the relationship between health literacy and adherence while controlling for cognition. RESULTS Our study included 433 patients with a mean age of 67 ± 6.8 years. The sample had 84% women, 31% non-Hispanic blacks, and 39% Hispanics. The 36% of patients with limited health literacy were more likely to have misconceptions about asthma (P < .001) and asthma medications (P < .001). Health literacy had a direct effect (β = 0.089; P < .001) as well as an indirect effect on adherence mediated by medications concerns (β = 0.033; P = .002). Neither medication necessity (β = 0.044; P = .138) nor illness beliefs (β = 0.007; P = .143) demonstrated a mediational role between health literacy and adherence. CONCLUSIONS Interventions designed to improve asthma controller medication adherence in older adults may be enhanced by addressing concerns about medications in addition to using communication strategies appropriate for populations with limited health literacy and cognitive impairments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tacara N Soones
- Department of Geriatrics and Palliative Medicine, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, NY.
| | - Jenny L Lin
- Division of General Internal Medicine, Mount Sinai School of Medicine, New York, NY
| | - Michael S Wolf
- Division of General Internal Medicine, Feinberg School of Medicine, Northwestern University, Chicago, Ill
| | - Rachel O'Conor
- Division of General Internal Medicine, Feinberg School of Medicine, Northwestern University, Chicago, Ill
| | - Melissa Martynenko
- Division of General Internal Medicine, Mount Sinai School of Medicine, New York, NY
| | - Juan P Wisnivesky
- Division of General Internal Medicine, Mount Sinai School of Medicine, New York, NY; Division of Pulmonary, Critical Care and Sleep Medicine, Mount Sinai School of Medicine, New York, NY
| | - Alex D Federman
- Division of General Internal Medicine, Mount Sinai School of Medicine, New York, NY
| |
Collapse
|
97
|
Summers MJ, Thow ME, Ward DD, Saunders NL, Klekociuk SZ, Imlach AR, Summers JJ, Vickers JC. Validation of a Dynamic Measure of Current Cognitive Reserve in a Longitudinally Assessed Sample of Healthy Older Adults: The Tasmanian Healthy Brain Project. Assessment 2017; 26:737-742. [PMID: 28043160 DOI: 10.1177/1073191116685806] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Cognitive reserve (CR) is a theoretical construct describing the underlying cognitive capacity of an individual that confers differential levels of resistance to, and recovery from, brain injuries of various types. To date, estimates of an individual's level of CR have been based on single proxy measures that are retrospective and static in nature. To develop a measure of dynamic change in CR across a lifetime, we previously identified a latent factor, derived from an exploratory factor analysis of a large sample of healthy older adults, as current CR (cCR). In the present study, we examined the longitudinal results of a sample of 272 older adults enrolled in the Tasmanian Healthy Brain Project. Using results from 12-month and 24-month reassessments, we examined the longitudinal validity of the cCR factor using confirmatory factor analyses. The results of these analyses indicate that the cCR factor structure is longitudinally stable. These results, in conjunction with recent results from our group demonstrating dynamic increases in cCR over time in older adults undertaking further education, lend weight to this cCR measure being a valid estimate of dynamic change in CR over time.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mathew J Summers
- 1 University of the Sunshine Coast, Birtinya, Queensland, Australia.,2 University of Tasmania, Hobart, Tasmania, Australia
| | - Megan E Thow
- 2 University of Tasmania, Hobart, Tasmania, Australia
| | - David D Ward
- 2 University of Tasmania, Hobart, Tasmania, Australia
| | | | | | | | - Jeffery J Summers
- 2 University of Tasmania, Hobart, Tasmania, Australia.,3 Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
98
|
Buckley RF, Sparks KP, Papp KV, Dekhtyar M, Martin C, Burnham S, Sperling RA, Rentz DM. Computerized Cognitive Testing for Use in Clinical Trials: A Comparison of the NIH Toolbox and Cogstate C3 Batteries. J Prev Alzheimers Dis 2017; 4:3-11. [PMID: 29188853 PMCID: PMC5726304 DOI: 10.14283/jpad.2017.1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/11/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND As prevention trials for Alzheimer's disease move into asymptomatic populations, identifying older individuals who manifest the earliest cognitive signs of Alzheimer's disease is critical. Computerized cognitive testing has the potential to replace current gold standard paper and pencil measures and may be a more efficient means of assessing cognition. However, more empirical evidence about the comparability of novel computerized batteries to paper and pencil measures is required. OBJECTIVES To determine whether two computerized IPad batteries, the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery and Cogstate-C3, similarly predict subtle cognitive impairment identified using the Preclinical Alzheimer Cognitive Composite (PACC). DESIGN, SETTING, PARTICIPANTS A pilot sample of 50 clinically normal older adults (Mage=68.5 years±7.6, 45% non-Caucasian) completed the PACC assessment, and the NIH Toolbox and Cogstate-C3 at research centers of Massachusetts General and Brigham and Women's Hospitals. Participants made 3-4 in-clinic visits, receiving the PACC first, then the NIH Toolbox, and finally the Cogstate-C3.>= 0.5SD), versus subtle cognitive impairment (<0.5SD). Composites for each computerized battery were created using principle components analysis, and compared with the PACC using non-parametric Spearman correlations. Logistic regression analyses were used to determine which composite was best able to classify subtle cognitive impairment from typical performance. RESULTS The NIH Toolbox formed one composite and exhibited the strongest within-battery alignment, while the Cogstate-C3 formed two distinct composites (Learning-Memory and Processing Speed-Attention). The NIH Toolbox and C3 Learning-Memory composites exhibited positive correlations with the PACC (ρ=0.49, p<0.001; ρ=0.58, p<0.001, respectively), but not the C3 Processing Speed-Attention composite, ρ=-0.18, p=0.22. The C3 Learning-Memory was the only composite that classified subtle cognitive impairment, and demonstrated the greatest sensitivity (62%) and specificity (81%) for that subtle cognitive impairment. CONCLUSIONS Preliminary findings suggest that the NIH Toolbox has the advantage of showing the strongest overall clustering and alignment with standardized paper-and-pencil tasks. By contrast, Learning-Memory tasks within the Cogstate-C3 battery have the greatest potential to identify cross-sectional, subtle cognitive impairment as defined by the PACC.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- R F Buckley
- Dorene M. Rentz, Harvard Medical School, Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women's Hospital, 60 Fenwood Road, Boston, MA 02115, Phone 617-732-8235,
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
99
|
Grotz C, Seron X, Van Wissen M, Adam S. How should proxies of cognitive reserve be evaluated in a population of healthy older adults? Int Psychogeriatr 2017; 29:123-136. [PMID: 27745564 DOI: 10.1017/s1041610216001745] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND While some tools have been developed to estimate an individual's cognitive reserve (CR), no study has assessed the adequacy of the method used for assessing these CR proxy indicators. Therefore, we aimed to determine the most appropriate method to estimate CR by comparing two approaches: (1) the common assessment of CR proxies in the literature (e.g. years of education) and (2) the calculation of a comprehensive index based on most significant parameters used in the estimation of CR. METHODS Data on CR proxies (i.e. education, occupation, and leisure activities) were obtained in a sample of 204 older adults. Regression analyses were used to develop the two indices of CR (i.e. ICR-standard and ICR-detailed) and to determine which index best represented the level of one's CR. RESULTS The ICR-standard was calculated using a combination of the three most common measures of reserve in the literature: number of schooling years, complexity of the primary occupation, and amount of current participation in stimulating activities. The ICR-detailed was calculated using the most significant parameters (established in initial analyses) of CR: highest level of education combined with the number of training courses, last occupation, and amount of current participation in social and intellectual activities. The comparison of both indices showed that higher levels of ICR-standard and ICR-detailed were associated with a greater minimization of the effects of age on cognition. However, the ICR-detailed was more strongly associated to this minimization than the ICR-standard, suggesting that the ICR-detailed best reflect one's CR. CONCLUSIONS This study is the first to show that it is of great importance to question methods measuring CR proxies in order to develop a clinical tool allowing a comprehensive and accurate estimation of CR.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Xavier Seron
- Cognitive Neuroscience Unit,Louvain Catholic University,Belgium
| | | | - Stéphane Adam
- Psychology of Aging Unit,University of Liège,Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
100
|
Dual-task costs in aging are predicted by formal education. Aging Clin Exp Res 2016; 28:959-64. [PMID: 26006256 PMCID: PMC5014893 DOI: 10.1007/s40520-015-0385-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/13/2015] [Accepted: 05/14/2015] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
The capacity to manage different concurrent tasks at the same time decays in older adults. There is however a considerable amount of inter-individual variability in this capacity even in healthy aging. The purpose of this empirical study is to investigate which factors help explaining this variability. A dual-task paradigm was administered to 64 older adults and 31 younger controls. In this paradigm, a primary simple response time task had to be carried out either by itself (single-task condition) or while concurrently performing a secondary subtraction task (dual-task condition). Dual-task costs were operationalized by comparing dual-task and single-task conditions. Older adults showed higher dual-task interference than younger controls. Within the older group, the influence of age, general cognitive abilities, performance on the secondary task, and years of formal education was assessed with a multiple regression analysis. The results showed that years of formal education in older adults were the best predictor that significantly explained a portion of the variance in dual-task performance. These findings extend previous literature by showing that formal education provides an important dose of cognitive reserve, which is useful to successfully implement cognitive dual-task management despite aging.
Collapse
|